Jump to content

Koinophilia: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Line 21: Line 21:
A major evolutionary problem has been how the continuous process of [[evolution]] produces the morphologically discontinuous groups labeled [[species]]. Although various processes of [[speciation]] have long been recognized (e.g. [[allopatric speciation|allopatric]], [[peripatric speciation|peripatric]], [[parapatric speciation|parapatric]], [[sympatric speciation|sympatric]]),<ref name= "Coyne, 1992">COYNE, J.A. (1992). Genetics and speciation. ''Nature (Lond)'' '''335''', 511-515</ref> they do not explain why the process is almost universal and very prominent among sexual creatures, while it is often conspicuously absent among asexual creatures. Asexual organisms very frequently show the continuous variation in form (often in many different directions) that evolution is expected to produce, making their classification into "species" (more correctly "morphospecies") very difficult.<ref name="Koeslag, 1990">KOESLAG, J.H. (1990). Koinophilia groups sexual creatures into species, promotes stasis, and stabilizes social behaviour. ''J. theor. Biol.'' '''144''', 15-35</ref><ref name="Maynard Smith, 1983">MAYNARD SMITH, J. (1983) The genetics of stasis and punctuation. ''Ann. rev. Genet.'' '''17''', 11-25</ref><ref name="Clapham et al. 1952">CLAPHAM, A.R., TUTIN, T.G., WARBURG, E.F. (1952). ''Flora of the British Isles.'' Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.</ref><ref name="Grant, 1971">GRANT, V. ''Plant speciation'' New York:Columbia University Press</ref> The above named processes also do not explain the phenotypic canalization of the entire outward appearance of both genders, regardless of each individual trait's relative contribution to fitness. Thus, a deer's tail does not contribute nearly as much to the animal's fitness as does its fur coat, the shape of its ears or the position of its eyes, yet all vary as little in form and appearance as do the others, as if selection were acting equally strongly on all of them. (It is a general principle in evolution that features that are neither particularly advantageous, or disadvantageous, can be expected to be subject to greater individual variation than, say, the camouflage coloring of the coat which is under strong selective pressure causing it to be the same in all individuals in the same environment.)
A major evolutionary problem has been how the continuous process of [[evolution]] produces the morphologically discontinuous groups labeled [[species]]. Although various processes of [[speciation]] have long been recognized (e.g. [[allopatric speciation|allopatric]], [[peripatric speciation|peripatric]], [[parapatric speciation|parapatric]], [[sympatric speciation|sympatric]]),<ref name= "Coyne, 1992">COYNE, J.A. (1992). Genetics and speciation. ''Nature (Lond)'' '''335''', 511-515</ref> they do not explain why the process is almost universal and very prominent among sexual creatures, while it is often conspicuously absent among asexual creatures. Asexual organisms very frequently show the continuous variation in form (often in many different directions) that evolution is expected to produce, making their classification into "species" (more correctly "morphospecies") very difficult.<ref name="Koeslag, 1990">KOESLAG, J.H. (1990). Koinophilia groups sexual creatures into species, promotes stasis, and stabilizes social behaviour. ''J. theor. Biol.'' '''144''', 15-35</ref><ref name="Maynard Smith, 1983">MAYNARD SMITH, J. (1983) The genetics of stasis and punctuation. ''Ann. rev. Genet.'' '''17''', 11-25</ref><ref name="Clapham et al. 1952">CLAPHAM, A.R., TUTIN, T.G., WARBURG, E.F. (1952). ''Flora of the British Isles.'' Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.</ref><ref name="Grant, 1971">GRANT, V. ''Plant speciation'' New York:Columbia University Press</ref> The above named processes also do not explain the phenotypic canalization of the entire outward appearance of both genders, regardless of each individual trait's relative contribution to fitness. Thus, a deer's tail does not contribute nearly as much to the animal's fitness as does its fur coat, the shape of its ears or the position of its eyes, yet all vary as little in form and appearance as do the others, as if selection were acting equally strongly on all of them. (It is a general principle in evolution that features that are neither particularly advantageous, or disadvantageous, can be expected to be subject to greater individual variation than, say, the camouflage coloring of the coat which is under strong selective pressure causing it to be the same in all individuals in the same environment.)


This canalization of the entire [[phenotype]] (i.e. the uniformity of all the individuals' appearances within a species) is extraordinary. Most people if they saw an Okapi, for instance, and then, a few days later a second Okapi in the same general vicinity, then they would not know whether they were seeing the same animal again, or, in fact, another member of the same species. Similarly, an ornithologist studying individual behavior in a flock of birds can only distinguish between the individuals if they have been fitted with individualized combinations of colored rings on their legs. In fact, for most persons, if you have seen one member of a species, then you have seen them all. So much so, that an illustration of a just single individual (or a single male and a single female) suffices to describe, in minute detail, every member of a species in a field guide or encyclopedia. Consider, on the other hand, the wide variety of dogs that humans have bred over the past 100 -200 years or so. There are breeds with no fur at all, and others with extra thick coats; there are long-legged greyhounds and stubby-legged dachshund; there are long snouted Afghan hounds and stub nosed pugs and bull dogs; and so it goes on - great danes and chihuahuas; loose skinned dogs and tight skinned dogs. That all of these breeds could have been derived in such a short space of time implies that all of this enormous variation was already latently present in the original domestic dog population. This, in turn, implies that evolution has an enormous amount of raw material on hand to work with. The slightest change of circumstances would, therefore, be expected to produce a change in [[phenotype]]. This is indeed what happens in asexual creatures.<ref name="Maynard Smith, 1983">MAYNARD SMITH, J. (1983) The genetics of stasis and punctuation. ''Ann. rev. Genet.'' '''17''', 11-25</ref><ref name="Clapham et al. 1952">CLAPHAM, A.R., TUTIN, T.G., WARBURG, E.F. (1952). ''Flora of the British Isles.'' Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.</ref><ref name="Grant, 1971">GRANT, V. ''Plant speciation'' New York:Columbia University Press</ref> Sexual creatures, however, seem to vigorously resist these changes, down to even the most trivial of phenotypic features.
This canalization of the entire [[phenotype]] (i.e. the uniformity of all the individuals' appearances within a species) is extraordinary. Most people if they saw an Okapi, for instance, and then, a few days later a second Okapi in the same general vicinity, then they would not know whether they were seeing the same animal again, or, in fact, another member of the same species. Similarly, an ornithologist studying individual behavior in a flock of birds can only distinguish between the individuals if they have been fitted with individualized combinations of colored rings on their legs. In fact, for most persons, if you have seen one member of a species, then you have seen them all. So much so, that an illustration of a just single individual (or of a single male and a single female) in a field guide or encyclopedia suffices to describe, in minute and absolute detail, all the adult members of a species. Consider, on the other hand, the wide variety of dogs that humans have bred over the past 100 -200 years or so. There are breeds with no fur at all, and others with extra thick coats; there are long-legged greyhounds and stubby-legged dachshund; there are long snouted Afghan hounds and stub nosed pugs and bull dogs; and so it goes on - great danes and chihuahuas; loose skinned dogs and tight skinned dogs. That all of these breeds could have been derived in such a short space of time implies that all of this enormous variation was already latently present in the original domestic dog population. This, in turn, implies that evolution has an enormous amount of raw material on hand to work with. The slightest change of circumstances would, therefore, be expected to produce a change in [[phenotype]]. This is indeed what happens in asexual creatures.<ref name="Maynard Smith, 1983">MAYNARD SMITH, J. (1983) The genetics of stasis and punctuation. ''Ann. rev. Genet.'' '''17''', 11-25</ref><ref name="Clapham et al. 1952">CLAPHAM, A.R., TUTIN, T.G., WARBURG, E.F. (1952). ''Flora of the British Isles.'' Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.</ref><ref name="Grant, 1971">GRANT, V. ''Plant speciation'' New York:Columbia University Press</ref> Sexual creatures, however, seem to vigorously resist these changes, down to even the most trivial of phenotypic features.


This is, however, only one aspect of what is almost certainly a two-dimensional problem. The "horizontal" dimension refers to the almost complete absence of transitional forms between present-day species (e.g. lions, leopards, cheetahs and tigers). The "vertical" dimension concerns the fossil record. Palaeontological species are frequently remarkably stable over extremely long periods of geological time, despite continental drift and major climate changes. When phenotypic change does occur, it tends to be abrupt in geological terms, again producing phenotypic gaps, but now between successive species, which often co-exist for considerable periods of time.
This is, however, only one aspect of what is almost certainly a two-dimensional problem. The "horizontal" dimension refers to the almost complete absence of transitional forms between present-day species (e.g. lions, leopards, cheetahs and tigers). The "vertical" dimension concerns the fossil record. Palaeontological species are frequently remarkably stable over extremely long periods of geological time, despite continental drift and major climate changes. When phenotypic change does occur, it tends to be abrupt in geological terms, again producing phenotypic gaps, but now between successive species, which often co-exist for considerable periods of time.

Revision as of 07:43, 22 July 2007

Koinophilia is a term used in genetics, meaning that sexual creatures prefer mates with a preponderance of common or average features. Stated differently, sexual creatures avoid mates with unusual, uncommon or deviant features.

Because natural selection results in beneficial (or "fit") features replacing their disadvantageous counterparts, the beneficial features become increasingly more common with each generation, while the disadvantageous features become increasingly rare. A sexual creature, therefore, wishing to mate with a fit partner, would be expected to avoid individuals sporting unusual features, while being especially attracted to those individuals with a predominance of common or average features. Koinophilia has, as an important side effect, that mates displaying mutant features (the result of a genetic mutations) are also avoided. This, in itself, is also advantageous, because the vast majority of mutations are disadvantageous. Because it is impossible to judge whether a new mutation is beneficial or not, koinophilic creatures will avoid them all with equal determination, even if this means avoiding the very occasional beneficial mutation. Thus, koinophilia, although not perfect or infallible in its ability to distinguish fit from unfit mates, on average, remains the best strategy when choosing a mate: it will be right far more often than it will be wrong. Even when it is wrong, a koinophilic choice always ensures that the offspring will inherit a suite of functional features.

Introduction

Koinophilia provides very simple and obvious explanations for such evolutionary conundrums as the process of speciation,[1] evolutionary stasis and punctuated equilibria,[1][2] sex and the affordability of males,[3][4] and the evolution of cooperation[5] (see Contents, above). Most of the answers to these evolutionary problems stem from the evolutionary conservatism (i.e., resistance to change) that is the inevitable result of koinophilia. Evolutionary change can still take place, but generally not in large groups. It is presumably only when small groups of individuals become isolated from the main group that genetic drift will induce changes, which, as the isolated population grows, become the new set of norms according to which the desirability of potential mates will be judged. If the new set of norms is inherently disadvantageous the isolate will die out. If, on the other hand, the isolate survives and comes into contact with other isolates with other norms, competition between the groups will determine which set of norms survives, and therefore what the new "species" will look like,[1] and how it will behave.[5]

This mating strategy, was first referred to as koinophilia by Johan H. Koeslag, from the Greek, koinos, meaning "the usual" or "common", and philos, meaning "fondness" or "love".[2] It was tested in humans by Judith Langlois,[6] who found that the average of two human faces was more attractive than either of the faces from which that average was derived. The more faces (of the same gender and age) that were used in the averaging process the more attractive and appealing the average face became.

The koinophilia model assumes that sexual creatures have senses with which they evaluate the various individual features of potential mates; and that they are capable of judging which features are common or usual, and which are unusual. Commonness is the only universally appropriate sign of fitness, or, at the very least, of an evolutionarily tried and tested phenotype. The link between all other mate attractants and fitness is both cirumstance-dependent, and ultimately "fakable" (i.e., the link between the mate attractant and fitness fades, or might even reverse). Commonness cannot be faked, nor is it circumstance-dependent in the way that the other mate attractants are. (Thus, rats and peacocks can both use commoness to judge the desirability of a potential mate, but a gaudy tail cannot be used likewise.) Fitness, by definition, causes commonness, and drives less fit traits into rarity. Koinophilia is therefore evolutionarily robust and likely to be widespread amongst sexual creatures.

Physical attractiveness

In keeping with these theoretical considerations, humans clearly find young average faces the most attractive.[6][7][8][9] However, Perrett et al.[9] found that both men and women found a slightly off-average female face the most attractive from a wide range of women's faces with neutral expressions and identical hairstyles. When the non-average features were slightly exaggerated the face was judged more attractive still. Close examination of the photos in Perrett, May and Yoshikawa's paper[9] shows, in fact, that the exaggerated face looks younger than the average face (composed of women's faces aged 22-46 years). The differences are, however, very small, and, to many people, not immediately obvious. Since the same results were obtained with Japanese subjects, these findings are probably culture independent, and would indicate that people generally find young average female faces sexually the most attractive,[6] as expected.

Speciation and "punctuated equilibria"

The evolutionary problem

A major evolutionary problem has been how the continuous process of evolution produces the morphologically discontinuous groups labeled species. Although various processes of speciation have long been recognized (e.g. allopatric, peripatric, parapatric, sympatric),[10] they do not explain why the process is almost universal and very prominent among sexual creatures, while it is often conspicuously absent among asexual creatures. Asexual organisms very frequently show the continuous variation in form (often in many different directions) that evolution is expected to produce, making their classification into "species" (more correctly "morphospecies") very difficult.[2][11][12][13] The above named processes also do not explain the phenotypic canalization of the entire outward appearance of both genders, regardless of each individual trait's relative contribution to fitness. Thus, a deer's tail does not contribute nearly as much to the animal's fitness as does its fur coat, the shape of its ears or the position of its eyes, yet all vary as little in form and appearance as do the others, as if selection were acting equally strongly on all of them. (It is a general principle in evolution that features that are neither particularly advantageous, or disadvantageous, can be expected to be subject to greater individual variation than, say, the camouflage coloring of the coat which is under strong selective pressure causing it to be the same in all individuals in the same environment.)

This canalization of the entire phenotype (i.e. the uniformity of all the individuals' appearances within a species) is extraordinary. Most people if they saw an Okapi, for instance, and then, a few days later a second Okapi in the same general vicinity, then they would not know whether they were seeing the same animal again, or, in fact, another member of the same species. Similarly, an ornithologist studying individual behavior in a flock of birds can only distinguish between the individuals if they have been fitted with individualized combinations of colored rings on their legs. In fact, for most persons, if you have seen one member of a species, then you have seen them all. So much so, that an illustration of a just single individual (or of a single male and a single female) in a field guide or encyclopedia suffices to describe, in minute and absolute detail, all the adult members of a species. Consider, on the other hand, the wide variety of dogs that humans have bred over the past 100 -200 years or so. There are breeds with no fur at all, and others with extra thick coats; there are long-legged greyhounds and stubby-legged dachshund; there are long snouted Afghan hounds and stub nosed pugs and bull dogs; and so it goes on - great danes and chihuahuas; loose skinned dogs and tight skinned dogs. That all of these breeds could have been derived in such a short space of time implies that all of this enormous variation was already latently present in the original domestic dog population. This, in turn, implies that evolution has an enormous amount of raw material on hand to work with. The slightest change of circumstances would, therefore, be expected to produce a change in phenotype. This is indeed what happens in asexual creatures.[11][12][13] Sexual creatures, however, seem to vigorously resist these changes, down to even the most trivial of phenotypic features.

This is, however, only one aspect of what is almost certainly a two-dimensional problem. The "horizontal" dimension refers to the almost complete absence of transitional forms between present-day species (e.g. lions, leopards, cheetahs and tigers). The "vertical" dimension concerns the fossil record. Palaeontological species are frequently remarkably stable over extremely long periods of geological time, despite continental drift and major climate changes. When phenotypic change does occur, it tends to be abrupt in geological terms, again producing phenotypic gaps, but now between successive species, which often co-exist for considerable periods of time.

The solution

Koinophilia could explain both the horizontal and vertical manifestations of speciation, and why it usually involves the entire external phenotype.[1] Since, by definition, fit traits replace less fit traits, each fit trait tends to become more common, and ultimately the dominant phenotype, while the maladaptive traits become increasingly rare. Sexual creatures would therefore be expected to prefer mates sporting predominantly common features, while avoiding mates with unusual or unfamiliar attributes. This is termed koinophilia. It causes common features to become more common still, and at a rate that exceeds that which would be driven by natural selection alone. Since it affects the entire external phenotype, the members of an interbreeding group will soon all begin to look alike, and noticeably different from other interbreeding groups. Any individual from one interbeeding group who wanders into another interbreeding group will now be immediately recognizable as morphologically different, and will, therefore, be discriminated against during the mating season. This koinophilia-induced reproductive isolation might thus be the first crucial step in the development of, ultimately, molecular biological, physiological, behavioral, and anatomical barriers to hybridization, and thus, ultimately, full specieshood. Koinophilia will thereafter defend that species phenotype against invasion by unusual or unfamiliar forms (which might arise by immigration or mutation), and thus be a paradigm of punctuated equilibrium (or the "vertical" aspect of the speciation problem.[1]).

The evolution of cooperation

Definition of cooperation

Cooperation is any group behavior that benefits the individuals more than if they were to act as independent agents. There is however a second, very important, corollary to cooperation: it can always be exploited by selfish individuals who benefit even more by not taking part in the group activity, yet reaping its benefits. It is for this reason that cooperation poses an evolutionary problem. An extreme example was described by Wynne-Edwards[14] [15] in the 1960s. He described a gannetry on Cape St Mary on the Newfoundland coast. It consisted of two adjacent cliffs on which the gannets roosted at night. The birds that roosted on Cliff 1 mated, built nests and raised chicks. The birds that roosted on Cliff 2, despite being adult, and of both sexes, did not mate, build nests or raise chicks, unless a vacancy arose on Cliff 1. A pair of birds would then move from Cliff 2 to Cliff 1, and start breeding. The obvious benefit of this behavior was that it limited population size, thereby ensuring that everyone had enough to eat, even in the long-term. It had the additional benefit that should an epidemic or inclement weather wipe out a large portion of the colony, there were always enough spare birds to fill the vacancies created by the disaster on Cliff 1. The population could thus be restored within a very short space of time.

The evolutionary problem

This observation caused a major controversy in biological circles because it seemed evolutionarily impossible. Imagine a mutant bird on Cliff 2 which, because of its mutation, was blind to the convention that (s)he should not breed on Cliff 2. Imagine that it found a mate equally unconcerned about the convention, and that the two of them set about raising chicks either on Cliff 2, or on a convenient ledge nearby. Since this unusual behavior is due to a genetic mutation, the resultant chicks would feel equally unbound by the convention that only birds that can be accommodated on Cliff 1 may breed. Thus the grand-offspring of the first mutant pair would behave similarly, as would the great-grand-offspring. If, for ease of calculation, only half of the colony is normally accommodated on Cliff 1, then each normal bird has, on average, a 50% chance of breeding. The mutants, on the other hand, have a 100% chance of breeding. They are therefore twice as "fit" as the normal birds, meaning that the mutation will spread extremely rapidly. Within a very short space of time, it would replace its normal counterpart. In biological jargon, the normal cooperative behavior is "evolutionarily unstable"; it has no evolutionary defense against selfish mutants.

Although this is an extreme example, it illustrates the evolutionary conundrum posed by any form of cooperative behavior. The selfish individuals who do not join the hunting pack and its incumbent dangers but nevertheless share the spoils, might be only 5-10% fitter than the cooperative individuals, but that does not matter. That extra 5% fitness will eventually result in the selfish mutation replacing its cooperative counterpart. Once the mutation has taken over from the normal gene, everyone is at a disadvantage compared to the original (cooperative) condition. Though this might seem extraordinary, it is nevertheless an inevitable evolutionary trap from which there is no obvious way of escape.

Game Theory solutions

The problem has been widely discussed in biology, because, despite these theoretical predictions (that cooperation is evolutionarily unstable), cooperative behavior is widespread in nature. The problem is most commonly addressed in "Game Theory" terms, which have led to important insights and understanding of cooperative behavior and how it might be evolutionarily stabilized. An example is the following. The males of many species defend and fight for territories during the mating season. If all these encounters were full scale battles, then it might turn out that, for the average male, the loss in fitness due to injuries is greater than the gains in fitness from securing a territory and therefore a mate. If this is the case, then Game Theory predicts that inter-male squabbles should be settled through the observance of one or other non-violent convention, while reserving full scale battles only for those occasions when the convention is broken by one or other party. In other words it is tit-for tat. The convention then becomes an "Evolutionarily Stable Strategy" because the individuals who disregard the convention suffer, on average, heavier losses in fitness than the individuals who adhere to the convention.

This is indeed what is observed in nature. The rule that is most commonly followed in most species is that squabbles are settled by the convention that the current owner of the territory always wins, usually without a physical fight. This, indeed, solves the original problem, but raises a new one. If the owner always wins when challenged, then why challenge? The "game" has now degenerated into the tick-tack-toe level of pointlessness. (The Game Theory prediction is that tick-tack-toe is a game not worth pursuing as it has only one stable end-point: an endless series of draws.)

A "tit-for-tat" strategy does not always lead to pointless "games".[16] In some situations, where the same pair of individuals regularly meet, it confers evolutionarily stability to cooperation, unless one of the players makes a mistake (e.g. mistakenly rewards cooperation with a selfish response), in which case a long series of selfish tit-for-tat acts ensues, which can only be broken by a second mistake (e.g. a cooperative response to a selfish act).[17] Different variations, however, on the tit-for-tat theme[18][19] can outperform pure tit-for-tat, and therefore take over from it, but some are, in turn, unstable in the presence of selfishness. Thus, if the different strategies arise by random mutations, then the affected population will cycle through all of the strategies in an endless series of chaotic cycles.[20] Game Theory, on its own, therefore does not seem to provide an answer to why cooperation is as common as it is (and apparently evolutionarily stable). It certainly does not provide an explanation as to why Wynne-Edwards' gannets behaved the way he observed them to behave.

The Koinophilic solution

Koinophilia, because it discriminates against any rare or unusual form of behavior is capable of stabilizing any strategy in any of the cooperation versus selfishness games. Any individual who behaves abnormally (as a result of a mutation, or through immigration) will not easily find a mate, and will thus not be able to pass that mutation on into the next generation. Koinophilia therefore has the effect of increasing the fitness of whatever happens to be the common strategy.[18] Different groups, practicing different strategies will thus become evolutionarily trapped in these different behaviors. A group that happens, by chance, to follow a cooperative strategy will, by definition, be fitter, as a group, than groups that consist of selfish individuals. Competition between such groups will ultimately result in the replacement of the selfish groups by cooperative groups.[5] Thus, Wynne-Edwards' gannets can be expected to continue practicing population control well into the future, without fear of being driven into extinction by selfishly reproducing mutants, because those mutants will experience difficulties in finding mates. And, even if one of them did happen to find a mate, then the offspring would be discriminated against, as their behavior would still stand out as being unusual, or deviant, in a large colony of several hundred birds.

  • The concept is poignantly summarized in a poem by Richard Fein entitled Koinophilia.
  • Why Sex? discusses the origin of sex, and the evolutionary problem of the affordability of males, together with its koinophilic solution.

References

  1. ^ a b c d e KOESLAG, J.H. (1995). On the engine of speciation. J. theor. Biol. 177, 401-409 Cite error: The named reference "Koeslag, 1995" was defined multiple times with different content (see the help page).
  2. ^ a b c KOESLAG, J.H. (1990). Koinophilia groups sexual creatures into species, promotes stasis, and stabilizes social behaviour. J. theor. Biol. 144, 15-35 Cite error: The named reference "Koeslag, 1990" was defined multiple times with different content (see the help page).
  3. ^ KOESLAG, P.D., KOESLAG, J.H. (1994). Koinophilia stabilizes bi-gender sexual reproduction against asex in an unchanging environment. J. theor. Biol. 166, 251-260
  4. ^ KOESLAG, J.H., KOESLAG, P.D. (1993). Evolutionarily stable meiotic sex. J. Heredity 84, 396-399
  5. ^ a b c KOESLAG, J.H. (2003). Evolution of cooperation: cooperation defeats defection in the cornfield model. J. theor. Biol. 224, 399-410
  6. ^ a b c LANGLOIS, J.H. & ROGGMAN, L. (1990). Attractive faces are only average. Psychol. Sci. 1, 115-121
  7. ^ ETCOFF, N. (1994). Beauty and the beholder. Nature (Lond) 368, 186-187
  8. ^ ENQUIST , M & GHIRLANDA, S. (1998). The secret of faces. Nature (Lond) 394, 826-827
  9. ^ a b c PERRETT D.I. et al. (1998). Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness. Nature (Lond) 394, 884-887 Cite error: The named reference "Perrett et al. 1998" was defined multiple times with different content (see the help page).
  10. ^ COYNE, J.A. (1992). Genetics and speciation. Nature (Lond) 335, 511-515
  11. ^ a b MAYNARD SMITH, J. (1983) The genetics of stasis and punctuation. Ann. rev. Genet. 17, 11-25
  12. ^ a b CLAPHAM, A.R., TUTIN, T.G., WARBURG, E.F. (1952). Flora of the British Isles. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  13. ^ a b GRANT, V. Plant speciation New York:Columbia University Press
  14. ^ WYNNE-EDWARDS, V.C. (1962). Animal Dispersion in Relation to Social Behaviour Edinburgh:Oliver & Boyd
  15. ^ WYNNE-EDWARDS, V.C. (1964). Population control in animals.Scientific American 211 (2), 68-74
  16. ^ AXELROD, R. HAMILTON, W.D. (1981). The evolution of cooperation. Science 211, 1390-1396
  17. ^ SIGMUND, K. (1993). Games of Life. Oxford: Oxford University Press
  18. ^ a b KOESLAG, J.H. (1997). Sex, the prisoner's dilemma game, and the evolutionary inevitability of cooperation. J. theor. Biol. 189, 53--61
  19. ^ NOVAK, M. SIGMUND, K. (1993). A strategy of win stay, lose-shift that outperforms tit-for-tat in the Prisoner's Dilemma Game. Nature (Lond.) 364, 56-58
  20. ^ BOYD, R. LORBERBAUM, J.P. (1987). No pure strategy is evolutionarily stable in the repeated Prisoner's Dilemma Game. Nature (Lond.) 327, 58-59