Jump to content

Origin of language: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
m revert - vandalism
(One intermediate revision by one other user not shown)
Line 20: Line 20:
===Archaic hominids===
===Archaic hominids===
:''See also [[Neanderthal#Language|Neanderthal language]]''
:''See also [[Neanderthal#Language|Neanderthal language]]''

Based upon the [[assumptions]] which form the foundation of [[Darwinistic]]-based [[biological evolution]], the following [[hypotheses]], for which there is no [[physical evidence]], have been derived.


There is considerable speculation about the language capabilities of ancient hominids. Some scholars believe the advent of hominid bipedalism around 3.5 million years ago would have brought changes to the human skull, allowing for a more L-shaped [[vocal tract]]. The shape of the tract and a [[larynx]] positioned relatively low in the neck are necessary prerequisites for many of the sounds humans make, particularly vowels. Other scholars believe that, based on the position of the larynx, not even the Neanderthals had the anatomy necessary to produce the full range of sounds modern humans make.<ref name="linguisticshandbook"/><ref name="comparativereview">{{cite web |url=http://www3.isrl.uiuc.edu/~junwang4/langev/localcopy/pdf/fitch00speech.pdf |title=The Evolution of Speech : A Comparative Review |author=Fitch, W. Tecumseh |format=PDF |accessdate=2007-09-09}}</ref>
There is considerable speculation about the language capabilities of ancient hominids. Some scholars believe the advent of hominid bipedalism around 3.5 million years ago would have brought changes to the human skull, allowing for a more L-shaped [[vocal tract]]. The shape of the tract and a [[larynx]] positioned relatively low in the neck are necessary prerequisites for many of the sounds humans make, particularly vowels. Other scholars believe that, based on the position of the larynx, not even the Neanderthals had the anatomy necessary to produce the full range of sounds modern humans make.<ref name="linguisticshandbook"/><ref name="comparativereview">{{cite web |url=http://www3.isrl.uiuc.edu/~junwang4/langev/localcopy/pdf/fitch00speech.pdf |title=The Evolution of Speech : A Comparative Review |author=Fitch, W. Tecumseh |format=PDF |accessdate=2007-09-09}}</ref>
Line 53: Line 51:
The gestural theory states that human language developed from [[gesture]]s that were used for simple communication.
The gestural theory states that human language developed from [[gesture]]s that were used for simple communication.


Two types of evidence may be assumed to support this theory.
Two types of evidence support this theory.
# Gestural language and vocal language depend on similar neural systems. The regions on the [[Cerebral cortex|cortex]] that are responsible for mouth and hand movements border each other.
# Gestural language and vocal language depend on similar neural systems. The regions on the [[Cerebral cortex|cortex]] that are responsible for mouth and hand movements border each other.
# Nonhuman [[primates]] can use gestures or symbols for at least primitive communication, and some of their gestures resemble those of humans, such as the "begging posture", with the hands stretched out, which humans share with chimpanzees.
# Nonhuman [[primates]] can use gestures or symbols for at least primitive communication, and some of their gestures resemble those of humans, such as the "begging posture", with the hands stretched out, which humans share with chimpanzees.{{fact|date=November 2007}}


Research found strong support for the idea that verbal language and sign language depend on similar neural structures. Patients who used sign language, and who suffered from a left-[[cerebral hemisphere|hemisphere]] [[lesion]], showed the same disorders with their sign language as vocal patients did with their spoken language.<ref>{{cite book |title=Neuromotor Mechanisms in Human Communication |author=Kimura, Doreen |year=1993 |publisher=Oxford University Press |location=Oxford |id=ISBN 978-0-19-505492-7}}</ref> Other researchers found that the same left-hemisphere brain regions were active during sign language as during the use of vocal or written language.<ref>{{cite journal |title=A Critical Period for Right Hemisphere Recruitment in American Sign Language Processing |author=Newman, A. J., et al. |date=2002 |journal=Nature Neuroscience |volume=5 |pages=76-80}}</ref>
Research found strong support for the idea that verbal language and sign language depend on similar neural structures. Patients who used sign language, and who suffered from a left-[[cerebral hemisphere|hemisphere]] [[lesion]], showed the same disorders with their sign language as vocal patients did with their spoken language.<ref>{{cite book |title=Neuromotor Mechanisms in Human Communication |author=Kimura, Doreen |year=1993 |publisher=Oxford University Press |location=Oxford |id=ISBN 978-0-19-505492-7}}</ref> Other researchers found that the same left-hemisphere brain regions were active during sign language as during the use of vocal or written language.<ref>{{cite journal |title=A Critical Period for Right Hemisphere Recruitment in American Sign Language Processing |author=Newman, A. J., et al. |date=2002 |journal=Nature Neuroscience |volume=5 |pages=76-80}}</ref>


The important question for gestural theories is why there was a shift to vocalization. There are three likely explanations (Based upon the [[assumptions]] which form the foundation of [[Darwinistic]]-based [[biological evolution]], the following [[hypotheses]], for which there is no [[physical evidence]], have been derived.):
The important question for gestural theories is why there was a shift to vocalization. There are three likely explanations:
# Our ancestors started to use more and more tools, meaning that their hands were occupied and could not be used for gesturing.
# Our ancestors started to use more and more tools, meaning that their hands were occupied and could not be used for gesturing.
# Gesturing requires that the communicating individuals can see each other. There are many situations in which individuals need to communicate even without visual contact, for instance when a predator is closing in on somebody who is up in a tree picking fruit.
# Gesturing requires that the communicating individuals can see each other. There are many situations in which individuals need to communicate even without visual contact, for instance when a predator is closing in on somebody who is up in a tree picking fruit.
Line 107: Line 105:
Religions and ethnic mythologies often provide explanations for the origin and development of language. Most mythologies do not credit humans with the invention of language, but know of a [[language of the gods]] (or, language of God), predating human language. Mystical languages used to communicate with animals or spirits, such as the [[language of the birds]], are also common, and were of particular interest during the [[Renaissance]].
Religions and ethnic mythologies often provide explanations for the origin and development of language. Most mythologies do not credit humans with the invention of language, but know of a [[language of the gods]] (or, language of God), predating human language. Mystical languages used to communicate with animals or spirits, such as the [[language of the birds]], are also common, and were of particular interest during the [[Renaissance]].


One of the best known examples in the West is the [[Tower of Babel]] passage from [[Genesis]] in the [[Bible]] or [[Torah]]. The passage, common to all [[Abrahamic faith]]s, tells of [[God]] punishing [[Human beings|man]] for the tower's construction by means of the [[confusion of tongues]] ([[Genesis]] 11:1-9, [[KJV]]).
One of the best known examples in the West is the [[Tower of Babel]] passage from [[Genesis]] in the [[Bible]] or [[Torah]]. The passage, common to all [[Abrahamic faith]]s, tells of [[God]] punishing [[Human beings|man]] for the tower's construction by means of the [[confusion of tongues]] ([[Genesis]] 11:1-9). Local variations of this passage are found to have followed [[Christian missionaries]] on their journeys across the world, although the extent to how much of the tradition existed prior to the arrival of the missionaries is still discussed.

:1. AND the whole earth was of one [[language]], and of one [[speech]].
:2. And it came to pass, as they journeyed from the east, that they found a plain in the land of [[Shinar]]; and they dwelt there.
:3. And they said one to another, Go to, let us make [[brick]], and burn them [[throughly]]. And they had brick for stone, and [[slime]] had they for [[morter]].
:4. And they said, Go to, let us build us a city and a tower, whose top ''may reach'' unto [[heaven]]; and let us make us a [[name]], lest we be scattered abroad upon the face of the whole earth.
:5. And the [[LORD]] came down to see the city and the tower, which the children of men builded.
:6. And the LORD said, Behold, the people ''is'' one, and they have all one language; and this they begin to do: and now nothing will be restrained from them, which they have imagined to do.
:7. Go to, let us go down, and there [[confound]] their language, that they may not understand one another’s speech.
:8. So the LORD scattered them abroad from thence upon the face of all the earth: and they left off to build the city.
:9. Therefore is the name of it called [[Babel]]; because the LORD did there confound the language of all the earth: and from thence did the LORD scatter them abroad upon the face of all the earth.

Local variations of this passage are found to have followed [[Christian missionaries]] on their journeys across the world, although the extent to how much of the tradition existed prior to the arrival of the missionaries is still discussed.


A group of people on the island of [[Hao (French Polynesia)|Hao]] in [[Polynesia]] tell a similar story to the Tower of Babel, speaking of a God who "in anger chased the builders away, broke down the building, and changed their language, so that they spoke diverse tongues."{{ref|williamson}}
A group of people on the island of [[Hao (French Polynesia)|Hao]] in [[Polynesia]] tell a very similar story to the Tower of Babel, speaking of a God who, "in anger chased the builders away, broke down the building, and changed their language, so that they spoke diverse tongues".{{ref|williamson}}


== See also ==
== See also ==

Revision as of 14:50, 26 March 2008

The origin of language (glottogony) is a topic that has attracted considerable speculation throughout human history. The use of language is one of the most conspicuous and diagnostic traits that distinguish Homo sapiens from other species. Unlike writing, spoken language leaves no trace. Hence linguists have to resort to indirect methods in trying to decipher the origins of language.

Linguists agree that there are no existing primitive languages, and all modern human populations speak languages of comparable complexity. While existing languages differ in the size of and subjects covered in their lexicons, all possess the grammar and syntax needed, and can invent, translate, or borrow the vocabulary necessary to express the full range of their speakers' concepts.[1][2] All humans possess similar linguistic abilities, and no child is born with a biological predisposition favoring any one language or type of language.[3]

Speech versus language

It is necessary to make a minor distinction between speech and language. Speech involves producing sounds from the voicebox. Talking birds, such as some parrots, are able to imitate human speech with varying ability. However this ability to mimic human sounds is very different from the acquisition of syntax. On the other hand, the deaf generally do not use speech but are able to communicate effectively using sign language, which is considered a fully-developed, complex, modern language. What this implies is that the evolution of modern human language required both the development of the anatomical apparatus and also neurological changes in the brain.

Animal communication

Though all animals use some form of communication, researchers generally do not classify their communication as language. However, the communication systems of a few animal species do share some attributes in common with modern human language. Dolphins, for example, are able to communicate like humans by calling each other by name.[4][5]

Primate language

Not much is known about great ape communication in the wild, but in captivity they have been taught rudimentary sign language and to use lexigrams (keyboards with symbols). Some apes such as Kanzi have reportedly been able to learn several hundred words. However, they do lack grammar or syntax. Furthermore the anatomical structure of their larynx does not enable apes to make many of the sounds that humans do.

In the wild, the communication of vervet monkeys has been the most studied.[5] They are known to make up to ten different vocalizations. Many of these are used to warn other members of the troupe about approaching predators, and include a "leopard call", a "snake call", and an "eagle call". Each alarm triggers a different defensive strategy. Scientists were able to elicit predictable responses from the monkeys using loudspeakers and prerecorded sounds. Other vocalizations may be used for identification. If an infant monkey calls, its mother turns toward it, but other vervet mothers turn instead toward that infant's mother to see what she will do.[6]

Archaic hominids

See also Neanderthal language

There is considerable speculation about the language capabilities of ancient hominids. Some scholars believe the advent of hominid bipedalism around 3.5 million years ago would have brought changes to the human skull, allowing for a more L-shaped vocal tract. The shape of the tract and a larynx positioned relatively low in the neck are necessary prerequisites for many of the sounds humans make, particularly vowels. Other scholars believe that, based on the position of the larynx, not even the Neanderthals had the anatomy necessary to produce the full range of sounds modern humans make.[3][7] The recent discovery of a Neanderthal hyoid bone suggests that Neanderthals may have been anatomically capable of producing sounds similar to modern humans, and studies indicate that by 400,000 years ago the hypoglossal canal of living hominids had reached the size of that in modern humans. The hypoglossal canal transmits nerve signals to the brain and its size is said to reflect speech abilities. Hominids who lived earlier than 300,000 years ago had hypoglossal canals more akin to those of chimpanzees than of humans.[8][9][10]

However, although Neanderthals may have been anatomically able to speak, many scholars doubt that they possessed a fully modern language. They largely base their doubts on the fossil record of archaic humans and their stone tool kit. For 2 million years following the emergence of Homo habilis, the stone tool technology of hominids changed very little. Richard G. Klein, who has worked extensively on ancient stone tools, describes the crude stone tool kit of archaic humans as impossible to break down into categories based on their function, and reports that Neanderthals seem to have had little concern for the final form of their tools. Klein argues that the Neanderthal brain may have not reached the level of complexity required for modern speech, even if the physical apparatus for speech production was well-developed.[11][12] The issue of the Neanderthal's level of cultural and technological sophistication remains a controversial one.

Anatomical features such as the L-shaped vocal tract have been continuously evolving as opposed to appearing suddenly. [13][citation needed] Even though archaic humans used crude stone technology, it was still more advanced than that of chimpanzees or gorillas. Hence it is most likely that archaic humans possessed some form of communication intermediate between that of modern humans and that of other primates.[14][citation needed]

Modern humans

Anatomically modern humans first appear in the fossil record 200,000 years ago in Ethiopia. But while modern anatomically, these humans continued to behave just like the hominids who existed before. They used the same crude stone tools and hunted inefficiently.[15] However, beginning about 100,000 years ago, there is evidence of more sophisticated behaviour, and by 50,000 years ago fully modern behaviour is thought to have developed in various parts of Africa.[16][10] After this point, stone tools show regular patterns that are reproduced or duplicated with more precision, and tools made of bone and antler appear for the first time. The artifacts are also now easily sortable into many different categories based on their function, such as projectile points, engraving tools, knife blades, and drilling and piercing tools.[11] Teaching offspring how to manufacture such detailed tools may have required complex language.[citation needed]

The greatest step in language evolution would have been the progression from primitive, pidgin-like communication to a creole-like language with all the grammar and syntax of modern languages.[5] Many scholars believe that this step could only have been accomplished with some biological change to the brain, such as a mutation. It has been suggested that the a gene such as FOXP2 may have undergone a mutation allowing humans to communicate. Evidence suggests that this change took place somewhere in Africa around 50,000 years ago, which rapidly brought about significant changes that are apparent in the fossil record.[5] There is still some debate as to whether language developed gradually over thousands of years or whether it appeared suddenly.

According to the Out of Africa hypothesis, around 50,000 years ago[17] a group of humans left Africa and proceeded to colonize the rest of the world, including Australia and the Americas, which had never been populated by archaic hominids. Some scientists[15] believe that Homo sapiens did not leave Africa before that, because they had not yet attained modern cognition and language, and consequently lacked the skills or the numbers required to migrate.

Monogenesis

Linguistic monogenesis (the "Mother Tongue Theory") is the hypothesis that there was one single protolanguage (the "Proto-World language") from which all other languages spoken by humans descend. All human populations from the Australian aboriginals to the Fuegians living at the Southern tip of Argentina possess language. This includes populations, such as the Tasmanian aboriginals or the Andamanese, who may have been isolated from the old world continents by as long as 40,000 years. Thus, the multiregional hypothesis would entail that modern language evolved independently on all the continents, a proposition considered implausible by proponents of monogenesis.[18][19]

All humans alive today are descended from Mitochondrial Eve, a woman estimated to have lived in Africa some 150,000 years ago. This raises the possibility that the Proto-World language could date to approximately that period.[20] There are also claims of a population bottleneck, notably the Toba catastrophe theory which postulates human population at one point some 70,000 years ago was as low as 15,000 or even 2,000 individuals.[21] If it indeed transpired, such a bottleneck would be an excellent candidate for the date of Proto-World, which also illustrates the fact that Proto-World would not necessarily date to the first emergence of language.

Some proponents of a Proto-World hypothesis, such as Merritt Ruhlen, have attempted to reconstruct the Proto-World language. However, most mainstream linguists reject these attempts and the methods they use (such as mass lexical comparison) for a number of reasons.[22][23]

Scenarios for language evolution

Gestural theory

The gestural theory states that human language developed from gestures that were used for simple communication.

Two types of evidence support this theory.

  1. Gestural language and vocal language depend on similar neural systems. The regions on the cortex that are responsible for mouth and hand movements border each other.
  2. Nonhuman primates can use gestures or symbols for at least primitive communication, and some of their gestures resemble those of humans, such as the "begging posture", with the hands stretched out, which humans share with chimpanzees.[citation needed]

Research found strong support for the idea that verbal language and sign language depend on similar neural structures. Patients who used sign language, and who suffered from a left-hemisphere lesion, showed the same disorders with their sign language as vocal patients did with their spoken language.[24] Other researchers found that the same left-hemisphere brain regions were active during sign language as during the use of vocal or written language.[25]

The important question for gestural theories is why there was a shift to vocalization. There are three likely explanations:

  1. Our ancestors started to use more and more tools, meaning that their hands were occupied and could not be used for gesturing.
  2. Gesturing requires that the communicating individuals can see each other. There are many situations in which individuals need to communicate even without visual contact, for instance when a predator is closing in on somebody who is up in a tree picking fruit.
  3. The need to co-operate effectively with others in order to survive. A command issued by a tribal leader to 'find' 'stones' to 'repel' attacking 'wolves' would create teamwork and a much more powerful, co-ordinated response.

Humans still use hand and facial gestures when they speak, especially when people meet who have no language in common.[26]

Pidgins and creoles

A pidgin is a simplified language that develops as a means of communication between two or more groups who do not share a common language, in situations such as trade, whose vocabulary is generally derived from languages of the various groups. The manner in which pidgins develop is of interest in understanding the origin of human language.

Pidgins are significantly simplified languages with only rudimentary grammar and a restricted vocabulary. In their early stage pidgins mainly consist of nouns, verbs and adjectives with few or no articles, prepositions, conjunctions or auxiliary verbs. The grammar consists of words with no fixed word order[citation needed] and the words have no inflectional endings.

If contact is maintained between the groups speaking the pidgin for long periods of time, the pidgins may become more complex over many generations. If the children of one generation adopt the pidgin as their native language it develops into a creole language, which becomes fixed and acquires a more complex grammar, with fixed phonology, syntax, morphology, and syntactic embedding. The syntax and morphology of such languages may often have local innovations not obviously derived from any of the parent languages.

Studies of creole languages around the world have suggested that they display remarkable similarities in grammar and are developed uniformly from pidgins in a single generation. These similarities are apparent even when creoles do not share any common language origins. In addition creoles share similarities despite being developed in isolation from each other. Syntactic similarities of creoles include Subject Verb Object word order. Even when creoles are derived from languages with a different word order they often develop the SVO word order. Creoles tend to have similar usage patterns for definite and indefinite articles, and similar movement rules for phrase structures even when the parent languages do not.[5]

Universal grammar

Since children are largely responsible for creolization of a pidgin, scholars such as Derek Bickerton and Noam Chomsky concluded that humans are born with a Universal grammar hardwired into their brains. This universal grammar consists of a wide range of grammatical models that include all the grammatical systems of worlds' languages. The default settings of this universal grammar are represented by the similarities apparent in creole languages. These default settings are overridden during the process of language acquisition by children to match the local language. When children learn a language they first learn the creole-like features more easily than the features that conflict with creole grammar.[5]

Another issue that is often cited as support for the Universal grammar theory is the recent development of Nicaraguan Sign Language. Beginning in 1979, the recently installed Nicaraguan government initiated the country's first widespread effort to educate deaf children. Prior to this there was no deaf community in the country. A center for special education established a program initially attended by 50 young deaf children. By 1983 the center had 400 students. The center did not have access to teaching facilities of any of the sign languages that are used around the world; consequently, the children were not taught any sign language. The language program instead emphasized spoken Spanish and lipreading, and the use of signs by teachers limited to fingerspelling (using simple signs to sign the alphabet). The program achieved little success, with most students failing to grasp the concept of Spanish words.

The first children who arrived at the center came with only a few crude gestural signs developed within their own families. However, when the children were placed together for the first time they began to build on one another's signs. As more younger children joined the language became more complex. The children's teachers, who were having limited success at communicating with their students, watched in awe as the kids began communicating amongst themselves.

Later the Nicaraguan government would solicit help from Judy Kegl, an American sign-language expert at Northeastern University. As Kegl and other researchers began to analyze the language, they noticed that the young children had taken the pidgin-like form of the older children to a higher level of complexity, with verb agreement and other conventions of grammar.[27]

Idioglossia

There have also been accounts of twins who spoke an unintelligible language that only their sibling understood. These cases are better documented; in the 1970s, the Kennedy twins, whose given names were "Grace" and "Virginia", called each other Poto and Cabengo; it was determined that their idiosyncratic speech was a deeply altered form of English, with some influence from their grandmother's German. It appeared to be a well-formed language, with rules governing grammar and syntax. Similarly idiosyncratic speech patterns were reported from the twin writers June and Jennifer Gibbons.

Even in the absence of the unusual social lives of twins, many people have found it relatively easy and natural to construct new languages, with lexicons either derived from pre-existing languages, or wholly imagined. The author J. R. R. Tolkien and his several languages of Middle-earth is one well known creator; there are many others.

History

The search for the origin of language has a long history, rooted in mythology.

History of research

Late 18th to early 19th century European scholarship assumed that the languages of the world reflected various stages in the development from primitive to advanced speech, culminating in the Indo European family seen as the most advanced. Modern linguistics does not begin until the late 18th century, and the romantic or animist theses of Johann Gottfried Herder and Johann Christoph Adelung remained influential well into the 19th century. The question of language origins proved inaccessible to methodical approaches, and in 1866 the Linguistic Society of Paris famously banned discussion of the origin of language, deeming it to be an unanswerable problem. A systematic approach to Historical linguistics became only possible with the Neogrammarian approach of Karl Brugmann and others from the 1890s, but scholarly interest in the question has only been gradually re-kindled from the 1950s (and then controversially) with ideas such as Universal grammar, mass lexical comparison and glottochronology. "Origin of language" as a a subject of its own emerges out of studies of neurolinguistics, psycholinguistics and human evolution in general. The Linguistic Bibliography introduces "origin of language" as a separate heading in 1988, as a sub-topic of psycholoinguistics, with dedicated research institutes of evolutionary linguistics emerging in the 1990s.

Historical experiments

History contains a number of anecdotes about people who attempted to discover the origin of language by experiment. The first such tale was told by Herodotus, who relates that Pharaoh "Psamtik" (probably Psammetichus I) caused two children to be raised by deaf-mutes; he would see what language they ended up speaking. When the children were brought before him, one of them said something that sounded to the pharaoh like bekos, the Phrygian word for bread. From this, Psamtik concluded that Phrygian was the first language. King James V of Scotland is said to have tried a similar experiment; his children were supposed to have ended up speaking Hebrew. Both the medieval monarch Frederick II and Akbar, a 16th century Mughal emperor of India, are said to have tried a similar experiment; the children involved in these experiments did not speak.[28][29]

In religion and mythology

According to Genesis, the observed variety of human languages originated at the Tower of Babel with the confusion of tongues. (Image from Gustave Doré's Illustrated Bible).

Religions and ethnic mythologies often provide explanations for the origin and development of language. Most mythologies do not credit humans with the invention of language, but know of a language of the gods (or, language of God), predating human language. Mystical languages used to communicate with animals or spirits, such as the language of the birds, are also common, and were of particular interest during the Renaissance.

One of the best known examples in the West is the Tower of Babel passage from Genesis in the Bible or Torah. The passage, common to all Abrahamic faiths, tells of God punishing man for the tower's construction by means of the confusion of tongues (Genesis 11:1-9). Local variations of this passage are found to have followed Christian missionaries on their journeys across the world, although the extent to how much of the tradition existed prior to the arrival of the missionaries is still discussed.

A group of people on the island of Hao in Polynesia tell a very similar story to the Tower of Babel, speaking of a God who, "in anger chased the builders away, broke down the building, and changed their language, so that they spoke diverse tongues".[1]

See also

Footnotes

  1. ^ "Primitive languages". Language Miniatures. Retrieved 2007-02-27.
  2. ^ Pinker, Steven (2000). The Language Instinct: How the Mind Creates Language. New York: Harper Perennial Modern Classics. pp. 13–14. ISBN 0-060-95833-2.
  3. ^ a b (2001). The Handbook of Linguistics, eds. Mark Aronoff & Janie Rees-Miller. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers, pp. 1-18. ISBN 1405102527
  4. ^ "Dolphins 'Have Their Own Names'". BBC News online. 2006-05-08. Retrieved 2007-09-09. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  5. ^ a b c d e f Diamond, Jared (1992, 2006). The Third Chimpanzee: The Evolution and Future of the Human Animal. New York: Harper Perennial. pp. 141–167. ISBN 0060183071. {{cite book}}: Check date values in: |year= (help)CS1 maint: year (link)
  6. ^ Wade, Nicholas (2006-05-23). "Nigerian Monkeys Drop Hints on Language Origin". The New York Times. Retrieved 2007-09-09. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  7. ^ Fitch, W. Tecumseh. "The Evolution of Speech : A Comparative Review" (PDF). Retrieved 2007-09-09.
  8. ^ Jungers, William L.; et al. (2003). "Hypoglossal Canal Size in Living Hominoids and the Evolution of Human Speech" (PDF). Human Biology. 75 (4): 473–484. Retrieved 2007-09-10. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  9. ^ DeGusta, David; et al. (1999). "Hypoglossal Canal Size and Hominid Speech". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 96 (4): 1800–1804. Retrieved 2007-09-10. Hypoglossal canal size has previously been used to date the origin of human-like speech capabilities to at least 400,000 years ago and to assign modern human vocal abilities to Neandertals. These conclusions are based on the hypothesis that the size of the hypoglossal canal is indicative of speech capabilities. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help)
  10. ^ a b Johansson, Sverker (2006). "Constraining the Time When Language Evolved" (PDF). Evolution of Language: Sixth International Conference, Rome. Retrieved 2007-09-10. Hyoid bones are very rare as fossils, as they are not attached to the rest of the skeleton, but one Neanderthal hyoid has been found (Arensburg et al., 1989), very similar to the hyoid of modern Homo sapiens, leading to the conclusion that Neanderthals had a vocal tract similar to ours (Houghton, 1993; Bo¨e, Maeda, & Heim, 1999). {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  11. ^ a b Klarreich, Erica (April 20, 2004). "Biography of Richard G. Klein". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 101 (16): 5705–5707. Retrieved 2007-09-10.
  12. ^ Klein, Richard G. "Three Distinct Human Populations". Biological and Behavioral Origins of Modern Humans. Access Excellence @ The National Health Museum. Retrieved 2007-09-10.
  13. ^ Olson, Steve (2002). Mapping Human History. Houghton Mifflin Books. ISBN 0618352104. Any adaptations produced by evolution are useful only in the present, not in some vaguely defined future. So the vocal anatomy and neural circuits needed for language could not have arisen for something that did not yet exist
  14. ^ Ruhlen, Merritt (1994). Origin of Language. ISBN 0471584266. Earlier human ancestors, such as Homo habilis and Homo erectus, would likely have possessed less developed forms of language, forms intermediate between the rudimentary communicative systems of, say, chimpanzees and modern human languages
  15. ^ a b Klein, Richard. "Three Distinct Populations". Retrieved 2007-11-10. You've had modern humans or people who look pretty modern in Africa by 100,000 to 130,000 years ago and that's the fossil evidence behind the recent "Out of Africa" hypothesis, but that they only spread from Africa about 50,000 years ago. What took so long? Why that long lag, 80,000 years?
  16. ^ Perlman, David (2002-01-11). "Cave's Ancient Treasure: 77,000-Year-Old Artifacts Could Mean Human Culture Began in Africa". San Francisco Chronicle. Retrieved 2007-09-10. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  17. ^ Minkel, J. R. (2007-07-18). "Skulls Add to "Out of Africa" Theory of Human Origins: Pattern of skull variation bolsters the case that humans took over from earlier species". Scientific American.com. Retrieved 2007-09-09. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  18. ^ Wade, Nicholas (2003-07-15). "Early Voices: The Leap to Language". The New York Times. Retrieved 2007-09-10. {{cite news}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  19. ^ Sverker, Johansson. "Origins of Language - Constraints on Hypotheses" (PDF). Retrieved 2007-09-10.
  20. ^ Ruhlen, Merritt (1996). "Language Origins". National Forum. Retrieved 2007-11-10. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  21. ^ Whitehouse, David (2003-06-09). "When Humans Faced Extinction". BBC News Online. Retrieved 2007-11-10. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  22. ^ Rosenfelder, Mark. "Deriving Proto-World with Tools You Probably Have at Home". Zompist.com. Retrieved 2007-11-10.
  23. ^ Salmons, Joseph (1997). "'Global Etymology' as Pre-Copernican Linguistics". California lɪŋ gwɪs tɪk Notes. 25 (1). Program in Linguistics, California State University: 1, 5–7, 60. ISSN 1548-1484. {{cite journal}}: External link in |journal= (help)
  24. ^ Kimura, Doreen (1993). Neuromotor Mechanisms in Human Communication. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-505492-7.
  25. ^ Newman, A. J.; et al. (2002). "A Critical Period for Right Hemisphere Recruitment in American Sign Language Processing". Nature Neuroscience. 5: 76–80. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help)
  26. ^ Kolb, Bryan, and Ian Q. Whishaw (2003). Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology (5th edition ed.). Worth Publishers. ISBN 978-0716753001. {{cite book}}: |edition= has extra text (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  27. ^ A Linguistic Big Bang
  28. ^ Re: Did hitler experiment with babies
  29. ^ Linguistics 201: First Language Acquisition

References