Ventromedial prefrontal cortex
This article includes a list of general references, but it lacks sufficient corresponding inline citations. (April 2013) |
Ventromedial prefrontal cortex | |
---|---|
Details | |
Identifiers | |
Latin | Cortex praefrontalis ventromedialis |
Anatomical terms of neuroanatomy |
The ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) is a part of the prefrontal cortex in the mammalian brain. The ventral medial prefrontal is located in the frontal lobe at the bottom of the cerebral hemispheres and is implicated in the processing of risk and fear. It also plays a role in the inhibition of emotional responses, and in the process of decision making and self control. It is also involved in the cognitive evaluation of morality.
Anatomy
While the ventromedial prefrontal cortex does not have a universally agreed on demarcation, in most sources, it is equivalent to the ventromedial reward network of Ongur and Price.[2] This network includes Brodmann area 10, Brodmann area 14, Brodmann area 25, and Brodmann area 32, as well as portions of Brodmann area 11, Brodmann area 12, and Brodmann area 13.[3] However, not all sources agree on the boundaries of the area. Different researchers use the term ventromedial prefrontal cortex differently. Sometimes, the term is saved for the area above the medial orbitofrontal cortex, while at other times, 'ventromedial prefrontal cortex' is used to describe a broad area in the lower (ventral) central (medial) region of the prefrontal cortex, of which the medial orbitofrontal cortex constitutes the lowermost part. This latter, broader area, corresponds to the area damaged in patients with decision-making impairments investigated by António Damásio and colleagues (see diagram, and below).
To get a rough idea of where the ventromedial prefrontal cortex is, recall that the left and right hemispheres of the brain are separated [by the longitudinal cerebral fissure]. Now imagine you could take your hand and starting at the anterior brain (where your forehead is) insert it into this gap until you reached the insula (a structure that is beneath the frontal lobe). Your palm would be touching a part of the prefrontal cortex, which is in the front part of the brain and the lower part of your palm would be touching the ventral medial prefrontal cortex.
The ventromedial prefrontal cortex is connected to and receives input from the ventral tegmental area, amygdala, the temporal lobe, the olfactory system, and the dorsomedial thalamus. It, in turn, sends signals to many different brain regions including; The temporal lobe, amygdala, the lateral hypothalamus, the hippocampal formation, the cingulate cortex, and certain other regions of the prefrontal cortex.[4] This huge network of connections affords the vmPFC the ability to receive and monitor large amounts of sensory data and to affect and influence a plethora of other brain regions, particularly the amygdala.
Function
Functional differences between the orbitofrontal and ventromedial areas of the pre-frontal cortex have not yet been clearly established, although the areas of the ventromedial cortex superior to the orbitofrontal cortex are much less associated with social functions and more with pure emotion regulation. Research in developmental neuroscience also suggested that neural networks in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex are rapidly developing during adolescence and young adulthood supporting emotion regulation through the amygdala,[5] being associated with a decrease in cortisol levels.
There are only a few reports of people with early-onset vmPFC damage during childhood, but these individuals tend to have severe antisocial behavior and impaired moral judgment. Compared to individuals with damage later in life, their behavior pattern is similar but more severe.[6]
Decision making
Patients with bilateral lesions of the vmPFC develop severe impairments in personal and social decision-making[4][7] even though most of their intellectual ability is preserved.[7][8] For instance, they have difficulties in choosing between options with uncertain outcomes, whether the uncertainty is in the form of a risk or of an ambiguity.[9] After their lesion, these patients have an impaired capacity to learn from their mistakes, making the same decisions again and again even though they lead to negative consequences. These patients choose alternatives that give immediate rewards, but seem to be blind to the future consequences of their actions.[7] However, the underlying mechanisms of this behaviour are not yet fully understood.[7]
Damage to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (especially in the right hemisphere) has been connected with deficits in detecting irony, sarcasm, and deception.[10] Subjects with damage in this area have been found to be more easily influenced by misleading advertising.[11] This has been attributed to a disruption of a "false tagging mechanism" which provides doubt and skepticism of new beliefs.
People with damage to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex still retain the ability to consciously make moral judgments without error, but only in hypothetical situations presented to them. There is a gap in reasoning when applying the same moral principles to similar situations in their own lives. The result is that people make decisions that are inconsistent with their self professed moral values.[4] People with early damage to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex are more likely to endorse self-serving actions that break moral rules or cause harm to others. This is especially true for patients whose damage occurred the earliest in life.[12]
Emotions and an understanding of social norms are used to provide reasoning of the moral nature on our behaviors, beliefs, and the people around us. The vmPFC works as the neural basis in allowing emotion to influence moral judgement. The trolley dilemma is a classic situation testing emotional involvement in moral judgment. In the trolley dilemma, participants imagine a runaway trolley heading toward five people who will be killed if the trolley is to continue. The participant is to decide whether to allow the trolley to run its course, or to shunt the trolley off to another track where it will only kill one person. Most people choose to throw the switch, concluding that it is moral to save five by sacrificing the life of one. In a variation, there is no switch at hand to shunt the trolley, instead the participant has the option to push someone on the trolley tracks to stop it from moving. Most people are repulsed at the idea of pushing the man to his death, even though outcome is the same as the first dilemma, with five people surviving and one person dying. The more personal nature of pushing someone onto the tracks discourages this response, and the role of emotion is proposed to be the difference in the reasoning. In functional imaging studies, increased activity in the vmPFC is associated with thinking of these personal moral situations, while making harmless decisions does not.[13] Patients with vmPFC lesions made the same decision in non-moral and moral personal dilemmas. Dysfunction of the vmPFC causes failure in using correct moral emotion, which explains why these patients showed less emotional responses when facing these dilemmas.[14]
Regulation of emotion
The vmPFC plays an important role in regulating and inhibiting our response to emotions. VmPFC seems to use our emotional reactions to model our behavior and control emotional reactions in certain social situations. The inputs of the vmPFC provide it with information from the environment and the plans of the frontal lobe, and its outputs allow the vmPFC to control different physiological responses and behaviors. The role of the vmPFC is especially highlighted in people with damage to this region. A damaged vmPFC causes impairments of behavioral control and decision making, consequences which are rooted in emotional dysregulation. The first and most famous case of someone with defects to this region was Phineas Gage, a railroad construction foreman who had his vmPFC bilaterally destroyed in an accident in 1848. Before his accident, Gage was described as “serious, industrious and energetic. Afterward he became childish, irresponsible, and thoughtless of others.”[15] Another patient with vmPFC damage wasted away his life savings on foolish investments and failed to make appropriate decisions in his personal life. In patients with vmPFC damage, evidence shows that there is a correlation between emotional dysregulation and dysfunction in real world competencies.[15]
The amygdala plays a significant role in instigating the emotional reactions associated with anger and violence. With the vmPFC’s outputs to the amygdala, the vmPFC plays a part in preventing such behavior. Evidence has shown that impulsive murderers have decreased activity in the prefrontal cortex and increased activity in subcortical areas such as the amygdala. This imbalance can enhance actions that are created by negative emotions and limit the ability of the prefrontal cortex to control these emotions. Lower activation in the prefrontal cortex is also correlated with antisocial behavior. The dysfunction of the ventromedial cortex seems to, in part, be caused by lower levels of serotonin release.[15]
The vmPFC also is involved in courage. In experiments with participants allowing snakes to come near or away from them, acts of courage correlated with activation in the vmPFC, specifically the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex.[15][16]
Activation of the vmPFC is associated with successful suppression of emotional responses to a negative emotional signal.[17] Patients with vmPFC lesions show defects both in emotional response and emotion regulation.[8] Their emotional responsivity is generally diminished and they show markedly reduced social emotions such as compassion, shame and guilt. These are emotions that are closely associated with moral values.[8] Patients also exhibit poorly regulated anger and frustration tolerance in certain circumstances.[8]
Patients with focal lesions in the vmPFC show personality changes such as lack of empathy, irresponsibility, and poor decision making. These traits are similar to psychopathic personality traits.[18] In addition, a correlation between individuals with a history of physical violence and decreased grey matter density in the vmPFC has been evidenced.[19]
The right half of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex was associated with regulating the interaction of cognition and affect in the production of empathic responses. Hedonic (pleasure) responses were also associations to orbitofrontal cortex activity level by Morten Kringelbach. This finding contributes findings suggesting ventromedial prefrontal cortex being associated with preference judgement, possibly assigning the ventromedial prefrontal cortex a key role in constructing one's self. Studies with Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) also supported the idea that the ventromedial prefrontal cortex is an important component for reactivating past emotional associations and events, therefore essentially mediating pathogenesis of PTSD.[20][21] Dysfunction of the vmPFC has also been identified as playing a role in PTSD-affected parents' response to their own children's mental states.[22] Treatments geared to the activation of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex were therefore suggested for individuals and parent-child relationships affected by PTSD. The right half of the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, being active during emotion regulation, was activated when participants were offered an unfair offer in a scenario. Specific deficits in reversal learning and decision-making have led to the hypothesis that the ventromedial prefrontal cortex is a major locus of dysfunction in the mild stages of the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia.[23]
The capacity for mature defense mechanisms such as intellectualization, compensation, reaction formation, and isolation has been tied to proper functioning of the right ventromedial prefrontal cortex, while more primitive defense mechanisms such as projection, splitting, verbal denial, and fantasy have been found to rely on other regions, primarily in the left hemisphere .[24]
Somatic marker hypothesis
One particularly notable theory of vmPFC function is the somatic marker hypothesis, accredited to António Damásio. By this hypothesis, the vmPFC has a central role in adapting somatic markers—emotional associations, or associations between mental objects and visceral (bodily) feedback—for use in natural decision making. This account also gives the vmPFC a role in moderating emotions and emotional reactions because whether the vmPFC decides the markers are positive or negative affects the appropriate response in a particular situation.[citation needed] However, a critical review of this hypothesis concluded that there is a need for additional empirical data to support the somatic marker theory.[17]
Extinction
Another role that the vmPFC plays is in the process of extinction, the gradual weakening and eventual cessation of a conditioned response, as studies have shown increased activation of the vmPFC after extinction training.[25] The specific role played by the vmPFC concerning extinction is not well understood, but it is believed that it plays a necessary role in the recall of extinction learning after a long period of time. Studies show that it aids in the consolidation of extinction learning.[26] A separate study has implicated the correlation between the cortical thickness of the vmPFC and the degree of extinction memory. Patients with larger vmPFCs tended to have lower responses to the extinct conditioned stimulus, therefore suggesting a superior extinction memory.[27] In general, the ventromedial prefrontal cortex plays a major role in the later stages of memory consolidation.[28]
Gender specific social cues
Ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions were also associated with a deficit in processing gender specific social cues. One experiment tested the ability of patients with vmPFC lesions to categorize gender-specific names, attributes, and attitudes compared to patients with dorsolateral prefrontal cortex lesions and control subjects. Whereas the patients with dorsolateral prefrontal cortex lesions performed similarly to the control subjects on tests indicating gender stereotypes, patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions demonstrated impaired stereotypic social knowledge.[29]
Cocaine abuse
Frequent cocaine users have been shown to have lower than normal activity in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. When asked to perform certain tasks that rely heavily on activation of this area of the brain, the cocaine users perform worse and have less prefrontal cortex activation than the control subjects.[30] The quantity of cocaine used was found to be inversely proportional to the level of activation.[31]
The prefrontal cortex is also physically affected by cocaine use. Chronic use has been shown to lead to a decrease in the amount of gray matter in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. The decrease in gray matter and effect on behavior is analogous to a person having lesions throughout their medial prefrontal cortex.[30] Specifically, the pyramidal cells of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex are known to be linked with drug seeking behaviors.[32] Both an increased and decreased level of activity in these pyramidal cells has shown to lead to extinction of cocaine-seeking behaviors depending on when the activation takes place. Inactivation of these cells was needed to inhibit cocaine-seeking behavior after a longer duration of time, whereas activation was required to reduce the behavior soon after using cocaine.[33]
References
- ^ Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Anderson SW (January 1998). "Dissociation Of working memory from decision making within the human prefrontal cortex". J. Neurosci. 18 (1): 428–37. PMID 9412519.
- ^ Ongur, D. (2000). "The Organization of Networks within the Orbital and Medial Prefrontal Cortex of Rats, Monkeys and Humans". Cerebral Cortex. 10 (3): 206–219. doi:10.1093/cercor/10.3.206. ISSN 1460-2199.
- ^ Finger, E. C.; Marsh, A. A.; Mitchell, D. G.; Reid, M. E.; Sims, C.; Budhani, S.; Kosson, D. S.; Chen, G.; Towbin, K. E.; Leibenluft, E.; Pine, D. S.; Blair, J. R. (2008). "Abnormal Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Function in Children with Psychopathic Traits During Reversal Learning". Archives of General Psychiatry. 65 (5): 586–594. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.65.5.586. PMC 3104600. PMID 18458210.
- ^ a b c Carlson, Neil R. Physiology of Behavior. 11th ed. Boston: Pearson, 2013. Print.
- ^ Decety, J; Michalska, K (2010). "Neurodevelopmental changes in the circuits underlying empathy and sympathy from childhood to adulthood". Developmental Science. 13 (1): 886–899. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00940.x. PMID 20977559.
- ^ Aaron D Boes; et al. (2011). "Behavioral effects of congenital ventromedial prefrontal cortex malformation". BMC Neurology. 11 (151). doi:10.1186/1471-2377-11-151.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link) - ^ a b c d Bechara, A; Tranel, D; Damasio, H (2000). "Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions" (PDF). Brain. 123 (11): 2189–202. doi:10.1093/brain/123.11.2189. PMID 11050020.
- ^ a b c d Michael Koenigs; et al. "Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements" (PDF). Nature. 446: 908–911. doi:10.1038/nature05631.
- ^ Fellows, L. K.; Farah, M. J. (2007). "The Role of Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex in Decision Making: Judgment under Uncertainty or Judgment Per Se?". Cerebral Cortex. 17 (11): 2669–674. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhl176.
- ^ Zald, D. H.; Andreotti, C. (2010). "Neuropsychological assessment of the orbital and ventromedial prefrontal cortex". Neuropsychologia. 48 (12): 3377–3391. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.08.012.
- ^ Asp, E.; Manzel, K.; Koestner, B.; Cole, C. A.; Denburg, N. L.; Tranel, D. (2012). "A neuropsychological test of belief and doubt: damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex increases credulity for misleading advertising". Frontiers in Neuroscience. 6. doi:10.3389/fnins.2012.00100.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link) - ^ Taber-Thomas, B. C.; Asp, E. W.; Koenigs, M.; Sutterer, M.; Anderson, S. W.; Tranel, D. (2014). "Arrested development: early prefrontal lesions impair the maturation of moral judgement". Brain. 137 (4): 1254–1261. doi:10.1093/brain/awt377. PMC 3959552. PMID 24519974.
- ^ Nicolle, A. & Goel, V. (2013). What is the role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in emotional influences on reason? In I. Blanchette (Ed.), Emotion and Reasoning. Psychology Press.
- ^ Hu, C; Jiang, X (2014). "An emotion regulation role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in moral judgment". Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8: 873. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00873.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link) - ^ a b c d Carlson, N. (2012). Physiology of Behavior (11th ed.). Harlow: Prentice Hall.
- ^ Nili, Uri; Goldberg, Hagar; Weizman, Abraham; Dudai, Yadin (2010). "Fear Thou Not: Activity of Frontal and Temporal Circuits in Moments of Real-Life Courage". Neuron. 66 (6): 949–962. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.06.009. ISSN 0896-6273. PMID 20620879.
- ^ a b Alexander Hänsel; Roland von Känel (2008). "The ventro-medial prefrontal cortex: a major link between the autonomic nervous system, regulation of emotion, and stress reactivity?" (PDF). BioPsychoSocial Medicine. 2 (21). doi:10.1186/1751-0759-2-21.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|lastauthoramp=
ignored (|name-list-style=
suggested) (help)CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link) - ^ Motzkin, Julian C.; Newman, Joseph P.; Kiehl, Kent A.; Koenigs, Michael (2011). "Reduced prefrontal connectivity in psychopathy". The Journal of Neuroscience. 31 (48): 17348–48. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.4215-11.2011.
- ^ Chester, David S.; Lynam, Donald R.; Milich, Richard; DeWall, C. Nathan (2017-12-01). "Physical aggressiveness and gray matter deficits in ventromedial prefrontal cortex". Cortex. 97 (Supplement C): 17–22. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2017.09.024.
- ^ Insel, TR (2009). "Disruptive Insights in Psychiatry: Transforming a Clinical Discipline". Journal of Clinical Investigation. 119 (4): 700–705. doi:10.1172/jci38832. PMC 2662575. PMID 19339761.
- ^ Koenigs, M., & Grafman, J. (2009). Post-traumatic stress disorder: The role of medial prefrontal cortex and amygdala. The Neuroscientist : A Review Journal Bringing Neurobiology, Neurology and Psychiatry, 15(5), 540–548. http://doi.org/10.1177/1073858409333072.
- ^ Schechter DS, Moser DA, Giacobino A, Stenz L, Gex-Fabry M, Adouan W, Cordero MI, Suardi F, Manini A, Sancho-Rossignol A, Merminod G, Aue T, Ansermet F, Dayer AG, Rusconi-Serpa S. (epub May 29, 2015) Methylation of NR3C1 is related to maternal PTSD, parenting stress and maternal medial prefrontal cortical activity in response to child separation among mothers with histories of violence exposure. Frontiers in Psychology. http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00690/abstract
- ^ Grossman, M.; Eslinger, P. J.; Troiani, V.; Anderson, C.; Avants, B.; Gee, J. C.; Antani, S. (2010). "THE ROLE OF VENTRAL MEDIAL PREFRONTAL CORTEX IN SOCIAL DECISIONS: CONVERGING EVIDENCE FROM fMRI AND FRONTOTEMPORAL LOBAR DEGENERATION". Neuropsychologia. 48 (12): 3505–3512. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.07.036. PMC 2949451. PMID 20691197.
- ^ Northoff, G (2010). "Region-based approach versus mechanism-based approach to the brain". Neuropsychoanalysis: An Interdisciplinary Journal for Psychoanalysis and the Neurosciences. 12 (2): 167–170. doi:10.1080/15294145.2010.10773640.
- ^ Madsen, Heather B.; Guerin, Alexandre A.; Kim, Jee Hyun. "Investigating the role of dopamine receptor- and parvalbumin-expressing cells in extinction of conditioned fear". Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 145: 7–17. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2017.08.009.
- ^ Quirk, Gregory J.; Russo, Gregory K.; Barron, Jill L.; Lebron, Kelimer (2000). "The Role of Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex in the Recovery of Extinguished Fear". The Journal of Neuroscience. 20 (16): 6225–231.
- ^ Milad Mohammed, R.; Quinn, Brian T.; Pitman, Roger K.; Orr, Scott P.; Fischl, Bruce; Rauch, Scott L. (2005). "Thickness of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in humans is correlated with extinction memory". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102 (30): 10706–30. doi:10.1073/pnas.0502441102. These results are a possible factor for explaining why different people show different degrees of controlling their fear.
- ^ Nieuwenhuis, I. L.; Takashima, A. (2011). "The role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in memory consolidation". Behavioural Brain Research. 218 (2): 325–334. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2010.12.009.
- ^ Milne, Elizabeth; Grafman, Jordan (2001). "Ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions in humans eliminate implicit gender stereotyping". The Journal of Neuroscience. 21 (12): RC150.
- ^ a b Carlson, N. (1977). Physiology of Behavior (11th ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon. pp. 621–622. ISBN 0-205-05706-3.
- ^ Bolla, K.; Ernst, M.; Kiehl, K.; Mouratidis, M.; Eldreth, D.; Contoreggi, C.; London, E. (2004). "Prefrontal Cortical Dysfunction in Abstinent Cocaine Abusers". The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 16 (4): 456–464. doi:10.1176/appi.neuropsych.16.4.456.
- ^ Kalivas, P.W.; Volkow, N.; Seamans, J. (2005). "Unmanageable Motivation in Addiction: A Pathology in Prefrontal-Accumbens Glutamate Transmission". Neuron. 45 (5): 647–650. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.005. PMID 15748840.
- ^ Van; den Oever, M. C.; Rotaru, D. C.; Heinsbroek, J. A.; Gouwenberg, Y.; Deisseroth, K.; Stuber, G. D.; Smit, A. B. (2013). "Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Pyramidal Cells Have a Temporal Dynamic Role in Recall and Extinction of Cocaine-Associated Memory". The Journal of Neuroscience. 33 (46): 18225–18233. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2412-13.2013. PMC 3828471. PMID 24227731.
External links
- The Brain from Top to Bottom: Brain Abnormalities Associated With Anxiety Disorders
- Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H (November 2000). "Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions". Brain. 123 (Pt 11): 2189–202. doi:10.1093/brain/123.11.2189. PMID 11050020.
- Quirk GJ, Russo GK, Barron JL, Lebron K (August 2000). "The role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in the recovery of extinguished fear". J. Neurosci. 20 (16): 6225–31. PMID 10934272.
- "Scientists Draw Link Between Morality And Brain's Wiring". Wall Street Journal. 2007-05-11.
- Carey, Benedict (2007-03-22). "Brain Injury Said to Affect Moral Choices". New York Times.
- Shamay-Tsoory, S. G.; Tomer, R.; Berger, B. D.; Aharon-Peretz, J. (2003). "Characterization of Empathy Deficits following Prefrontal Brain Damage: The Role of the Right Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 15 (3): 324–37. doi:10.1162/089892903321593063. PMID 12729486.
- Hooper CJ, Luciana M, Conklin HM, Yarger RS (November 2004). "Adolescents' performance on the Iowa Gambling Task: implications for the development of decision making and ventromedial prefrontal cortex". Dev Psychol. 40 (6): 1148–58. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1148. PMID 15535763.
- Elliott R, Rees G, Dolan RJ (April 1999). "Ventromedial prefrontal cortex mediates guessing". Neuropsychologia. 37 (4): 403–11. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00107-9. PMID 10215087.
- Rahman, S.; Sahakian, B.J.; Hodges, J.R.; Rogers, R.D.; Robbins, T.W. (1999). "Specific cognitive deficits in mild frontal variant frontotemporal dementia". Brain. 122 (8): 1469–93. doi:10.1093/brain/122.8.1469. PMID 10430832.
- Jacques Mehler; Susana Franck (1995). Cognition on cognition. MIT Press. pp. 3–. ISBN 978-0-262-63167-9.
Intensity to Future Consequences Following Damage to Human Prefrontal Cortex
- Urry HL, van Reekum CM, Johnstone T, et al. (April 2006). "Amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex are inversely coupled during regulation of negative affect and predict the diurnal pattern of cortisol secretion among older adults". J. Neurosci. 26 (16): 4415–25. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3215-05.2006. PMID 16624961.
- Paulus MP, Frank LR (July 2003). "Ventromedial prefrontal cortex activation is critical for preference judgments". NeuroReport. 14 (10): 1311–5. doi:10.1097/01.wnr.0000078543.07662.02. PMID 12876463.
- Zald DH, Mattson DL, Pardo JV (February 2002). "Brain activity in ventromedial prefrontal cortex correlates with individual differences in negative affect". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99 (4): 2450–4. doi:10.1073/pnas.042457199. PMC 122385. PMID 11842195.
- Tabibnia G, Satpute AB, Lieberman MD (April 2008). "The sunny side of fairness: preference for fairness activates reward circuitry (and disregarding unfairness activates self-control circuitry)". Psychol Sci. 19 (4): 339–47. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02091.x. PMID 18399886.
- The Orbitofrontal Cortex: Linking Reward to Hedonic Experience
- Milne E, Grafman J (June 2001). "Ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions in humans eliminate implicit gender stereotyping". J. Neurosci. 21 (12): RC150. PMID 11404442.
- Koenigs M, Huey ED, Raymont V, et al. (February 2008). "Focal brain damage protects against post-traumatic stress disorder in combat veterans". Nat. Neurosci. 11 (2): 232–7. doi:10.1038/nn2032. PMC 2693133. PMID 18157125.
- Sanchez-Navarro, Juan P.; Driscoll, David; Buchanan, Tony W.; Bechara, Antoine; Anderson, Steven W.; Tranel, Daniel (September 2011). "Ventromedial prefrontal cortex regulation of prepulse inhibition and emotional modulation of the startle reflex: effect of lesion onset". Front. Hum. Neurosci. Conference Abstract: XI International Conference on Cognitive Neuroscience (ICON XI). doi:10.3389/conf.fnhum.2011.207.00165