Pro-drop language
This article has multiple issues. Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page. (Learn how and when to remove these messages)
|
Linguistic typology |
---|
Morphological |
Morphosyntactic |
Word order |
Lexicon |
A pro-drop language (from "pronoun-dropping") is a language in which certain classes of pronouns may be omitted when they can be pragmatically or grammatically inferable. The precise conditions vary from language to language, and can be quite intricate. The phenomenon of "pronoun-dropping" is part of the larger topic of zero or null anaphora.[1] The connection between pro-drop languages and null anaphora relates to the fact that a dropped pronoun has referential properties, and so is crucially not a null dummy pronoun.
Pro-drop is a problem when translating to a non-pro-drop language such as English, which requires the pronoun to be added, especially noticeable in machine translation.[2] It can also contribute to transfer errors in language learning.[citation needed]
An areal feature of some European languages is that pronoun dropping is not, or seldom, possible (see Standard Average European); this is the case for English, French, German,[3] and Emilian dialect, among others.[4] In contrast, Japanese,[5] Mandarin Chinese, Slavic languages,[6] Finno-Ugric languages, Assyrian Neo-Aramaic, Kra-Dai languages, Italian, Spanish, and Portuguese[7] exhibit frequent pro-drop features. Some languages, such as Greek and Hindi[8] also exhibit pro-drop in any argument.
Usage of term
In Noam Chomsky's "Lectures on Government and Binding", the term is used for a cluster of properties of which "null subject" was one (for the occurrence of pro as a predicate rather than a subject in sentences with the copula see Moro 1997).[citation needed]
Thus, a one-way correlation was suggested between inflectional agreement (AGR) and empty pronouns on the one hand and between no agreement and overt pronouns, on the other. In the classical version, languages which not only lack agreement morphology but also allow extensive dropping of pronouns—such as Japanese, Chinese, Korean, and Vietnamese—are not included, as is made clear in a footnote: "The principle suggested is fairly general, but does not apply to such languages as Japanese in which pronouns can be missing much more freely."[9] (Chomsky 1981:284, fn 47).
The term pro-drop is also used in other frameworks in generative grammar, such as in lexical functional grammar (LFG), but in a more general sense: "Pro-drop is a widespread linguistic phenomenon in which, under certain conditions, a structural NP may be unexpressed, giving rise to a pronominal interpretation."[10] (Bresnan 1982:384).
The empty category assumed (under government and binding theory) to be present in the vacant subject position left by pro-dropping is known as pro, or as "little pro" (to distinguish it from "big PRO", an empty category associated with non-finite verb phrases).[11]
Cross-linguistic variation
It has been observed that pro-drop languages are those with either rich inflection for person and number (Persian, Polish, Czech, Portuguese, etc.) or no such inflection at all (Japanese, Chinese, Korean, etc.), but languages that are intermediate (English, French) are non-pro-drop.
While the mechanism by which overt pronouns are more "useful" in English than in Japanese is obscure, and there are exceptions to this observation, it still seems to have considerable descriptive validity. As Huang puts it, "Pro-drop is licensed to occur either where a language has full agreement, or where a language has no agreement, but not where a language has impoverished partial agreement."[12]
In pro-drop languages with a highly inflected verbal morphology, the expression of the subject pronoun is considered unnecessary because the verbal inflection indicates the person and number of the subject, thus the referent of the null subject can be inferred from the grammatical inflection on the verb.[13]
Barbosa defines these typological patterns as null-subject languages (NSL), expressing that the term itself, pro-drop, can be subcategorized into categories such as: topic (discourse) pro-drop, partial NSL (partial pro-drop) and consistent NSL (full pro-drop).[14]
Topic pro-drop languages
In everyday speech there are instances when who or what is being referred to — namely, the topic of the sentence — can be inferred from context. Languages which permit the pronoun to be inferred from contextual information are called topic-drop (also known as discourse pro-drop) languages: thus, topic pro-drop languages allow referential pronouns to be omitted, or be phonologically null. (In contrast, languages that lack topic pro-drop as a mechanism would still require the pronoun.) These dropped pronouns can be inferred from previous discourse, from the context of the conversation, or generally shared knowledge.[15] Among major languages, some which might be called topic pro-drop languages are Japanese,[5][16] Korean,[16] and Mandarin.[17] Topic prominent languages like Korean, Mandarin and Japanese have structures which focus more on topics and comments as opposed to English, a subject-prominent language.[18] It is this topic-first nature that enables the inference of omitted pronouns from discourse.
Korean
The following example from Jung (2004:719) Korean shows the omission of both pronouns in the subject and object position.
너
Neo
you
이것
igeot
this
필요하니?
pilyohani?
need
Do you need this?
필요해
pilyohae
need
(I) need (it).[15]
Japanese
Consider the following examples from Japanese:[2]
この
Kono
This
ケーキ
kēki
cake
は
wa
TOP
美味しい。
oishii.
tasty-PRS
誰
Dare
Who
が
ga
SUBJ
焼いた
yaita
bake-PAST
の?
no?
Q
This cake is tasty. Who baked (it)?
知らない。
Shiranai.
know-NEG.
気に入った?
Ki ni itta?
like-PAST
(I) don't know. Did (you) like (it)?
The words in parentheses and boldface in the English translations (it in the first line; I, you, and it in the second) appear nowhere in the Japanese sentences but are understood from context. If nouns or pronouns were supplied, the resulting sentences would be grammatically correct but sound unnatural. Learners of Japanese as a second language, especially those whose first language is non-pro-drop like English or French, often supply personal pronouns where they are pragmatically inferable, an example of language transfer.
Mandarin
The above-mentioned examples from Japanese are readily rendered into Mandarin:
这
Zhè
This
块
kuài
piece
蛋糕
dàngāo
cake
很
hěn
DEGREE
好吃。
hǎochī.
tasty.
谁
Shéi
Who
烤
kǎo
bake
的?
de?
MODIFY
This cake is tasty. Who baked (it)?
不
Bù
Not
知道。
zhīdào.
know.
喜欢
Xǐhuan
like
吗?
ma?
Q
(I) don't know. Do (you) like (it)?
Unlike in Japanese, the inclusion of the dropped pronouns does not make the sentence sound unnatural.
Partial pro-drop languages
Languages with partial pro-drop have both agreement and referential null subjects that are restricted with respect to their distribution.[14] The partial null-subject languages include most Balto-Slavic languages, which allow for the deletion of the subject pronoun. Hungarian allows deletion of both the subject and object pronouns.
Slavic languages
The following table provides examples of subject pro-drop in Slavic languages. In each of these examples, the 3rd person masculine singular pronoun 'he' in the second sentence is inferred from context.
language | ||
---|---|---|
Belarusian | Бачу [яго]. | Ідзе. |
Bulgarian | Виждам го. | Идва. |
Czech | Vidím ho. | Jde. |
Macedonian | Го гледам. | Доаѓа. |
Polish | Widzę go. | Idzie. |
Russian | Вижу [его]. | Идёт. |
Serbo-Croatian | Vidim ga. / Видим га. | Dolazi. / Долази. |
Slovene | Vidim ga. | Prihaja. |
Ukrainian | Бачу [його]. | Іде. |
'(I) see [him].' | '(He) is coming.' |
In the East Slavic languages even the objective pronoun "его" can be omitted in the present and future tenses (both imperfect and perfective). In these languages, the missing pronoun is not inferred strictly from pragmatics, but partially indicated by the morphology of the verb (Вижу, Виждам, Widzę, Vidim, etc...). However, the past tense of both imperfective and perfective in modern East Slavic languages inflects by gender and number rather than the person due to the fact that the present tense conjugations of the copula "to be" (Russian быть, Ukrainian бути, Belarusian быць) have practically fallen out of use. As such, the pronoun is often included in these tenses, especially in writing.
Finno-Ugric languages
In Finnish, the verb inflection replaces first- and second-person pronouns in simple sentences: menen "I go", menette "all of you go". Pronouns are typically left in place only when they need to be inflected, e.g. me "we", meiltä "from us". There are possessive pronouns but possessive suffixes, e.g. -ni as in kissani "my cat", are also used, as in Kissani söi kalan ("my cat ate a fish"). A peculiarity of Colloquial Finnish is that the pronoun me ("we") can be dropped if the verb is placed in the passive voice (e.g. haetaan, Standard "it is fetched", colloquial "we fetch"). Estonian, a close relative of Finnish, has a tendency that is less clear. Literary Estonian generally uses explicit personal pronouns in the literary language, but they are often omitted in colloquial Estonian.
Hungarian is also pro-drop, and subject pronouns are used only for emphasis: (Én) mentem "I went". Because of the definite conjugation, object pronouns can be often elided as well. For example, the question (Ti) látjátok a macskát? "Do (you pl.) see the cat?" can be answered with just látjuk "(We) see (it)" because the definite conjugation renders the object pronoun superfluous.
Hebrew
Modern Hebrew, like Biblical Hebrew, is a "moderately" pro-drop language. In general, subject pronouns must be included in the present tense. Since Hebrew has no verb forms expressing the present tense, the present tense is formed by using the present participle (somewhat like English I am guarding). The Hebrew participle , as is the case with other adjectives, declines only in grammatical gender and number (like the past tense in Russian), thus:
- I (m.) guard (ani shomer) = אני שומר
- You (m.) guard (ata shomer) = אתה שומר
- He guards (hu shomer) = הוא שומר
- I (f.) guard (ani shomeret) = אני שומרת
- We (m.) guard (anachnu shomrim) = אנחנו שומרים
Since the forms that are used for the present tense lack the distinction between grammatical persons, explicit pronouns must be added in most cases.
In contrast, the past tense and the future tense the verb form is inflected for person, number, and gender. Therefore, the verb form itself indicates sufficient information about the subject. The subject pronoun is therefore normally dropped, except in third-person.[19]
- I (m./f.) guarded (shamarti) = שמרתי
- You (m. pl.) guarded (sh'martem) = שמרתם
- I (m./f.) will guard (eshmor) = אשמור
- You (pl./m.) will guard (tishm'ru) = תשמרו
Many nouns can take suffixes to reflect the possessor in which case the personal pronoun is dropped. In daily usage, the inflection of Modern Hebrew nouns is common only for some nouns. In most cases, inflected possessive pronouns are used. In Hebrew, possessive pronouns are treated mostly like adjectives and follow the nouns which they modify. In Biblical Hebrew, inflection of more sophisticated nouns is more common than in modern usage.
Full pro-drop languages
Full pro-drop languages, also known as consistent NSLs, are languages that are characterized by rich subject agreement morphology where subjects are freely dropped under the appropriate discourse conditions.[14] In some contexts, pro-drop in these languages is mandatory and also occurs in contexts in which pro-drop cannot happen for partial pro-drop languages.[14] The following languages exhibit full pro-drop in their own distinct ways.
Hindi
South Asian languages such as Hindi, in general, have the ability to pro-drop any and all arguments.[8] Hindi is a split-ergative language and when the subject of the sentence is in the ergative case (also when the sentence involves the infinitive participle, which requires the subject to be in the dative case[20]), the verb of the sentence agrees in gender and number with the object of the sentence, hence making it possible to drop the object since it can be contextually inferred from the gender of the verb.
In the example below, the subject is in the ergative case and the verb agrees in number and gender with the direct object.
In the example below, the subject is in the dative case and the verb agrees in number and gender with the direct object.
In the example below, the subject is in the nominative case and the verb agrees in number, gender, and also in person with the subject.
Greek
Subject pronouns are usually omitted in Greek, but the verb is inflected for the person and number of the subject. Example:
Βλέπεις
see.2sg
εκείνο
that
το
the
κούτσουρο;
log?
Θα
Would
ήταν
be.3sg
καλό
good
για
for
τη
the
φωτιά.
fire.
Είναι
be.pres.3sg
τελείως
completely
ξερό.
dried
(You) see this log? (It) would be good for the fire. (It) has completely dried.
Romance languages
Like their parent Latin, most Romance languages (with the notable exception of French) are categorised as pro-drop as well, though generally only in the case of subject pronouns. Unlike in Japanese, however, the missing subject pronoun is not inferred strictly from pragmatics, but partially indicated by the morphology of the verb, which inflects for person and number of the subject. Spanish, Italian, Romanian, Catalan and Occitan can elide subject pronouns only (Portuguese sometimes elides object pronouns as well), and they often do so even when the referent has not been mentioned. This is helped by person/number inflection on the verb. The 3rd person singular and plural subject pronouns are often kept to denote and differentiate male and female subjects/genders.
Spanish
In Spanish, the verb is inflected for both person and number, thus expression of the pronoun is unnecessary because it is grammatically redundant.[13] In the following example, the inflection on the verb ver, 'see', signals informal 2nd person singular, thus the pronoun is dropped. Similarly, from both the context and verbal morphology, the listener can infer that the second two utterances are referring to the log, so the speaker omits the pronoun that would appear in English as "it."
¿Ves
See
este
this
tronco?
log?
Sería
Would be
bueno
good
para
for
la
the
fogata.
campfire.
Está
Is
completamente
completely
seco.
dry
(Do) (you) see this log? (It) would be good for the campfire. (It) is completely dry.
Although Spanish is a predominantly pro-drop language, not all grammatical contexts allow for a null pronoun. There are some environments that require an overt pronoun. In contrast, there are also grammatical environments that require a null pronoun. According to the Real Academia Española, the expression or elision of the subject pronoun is not random. Rather there are contexts in which an overt pronoun is abnormal, but in other cases, the overt pronoun is possible or even required.[21] Further, the examples below illustrate how overt pronouns in Spanish are not constrained by inflectional morphology. The pronoun nosotros can be either present or absent, depending on certain discourse conditions:[22]
Salimos
left
“We left.”
Nosotros
We
salimos.
left
“We left.”
The third person pronouns (él, ella, ellos, ellas) in most contexts can only refer to persons. Therefore, when referring to things (that are not people) an explicit pronoun is usually disallowed.[21]
Subject pronouns can be made explicit when used for a contrastive function or when the subject is the focus of the sentence. In the following example, the first person explicit pronoun is used to emphasize the subject. In the next sentence the explicit yo, stressed that the opinion is from the speaker and not from the second person or another person.
Yo
I
creo
think
que
that
eso
that
estuvo
was
mal.
wrong.
Subject pronouns can also be made explicit in order to clarify ambiguities that arise due to verb forms that are homophonous in the first person and third person. For example, in the past imperfect, conditional, and the subjunctive, the verb forms are the same for first person singular and third person singular. In these situations, using the explicit pronoun yo (1st person singular) or él, ella (3rd person singular) clarifies who the subject is, since the verbal morphology is ambiguous.[21]
Italian
Vedi
See
questo
this
tronchetto?
log?
Andrebbe
Would go
bene
well
per
for
il
the
fuoco.
campfire.
È
Is
completamente
completely
secco.
dry.
Do (you) see this log? (It) would be fit for the campfire. (It) is completely dry.
Italian further demonstrates full pro-drop by allowing for the possibility of a salient, referential, definite subject of finite clauses. With respect to the Null subject parameter (NSP), this will be analyzed using the phrase 'S/he speaks Italian.'[23]
Italian has a [+] value:
- Parla italiano. (Italian, +NSP)
A non pro-drop language, such as English, has a [-] value for NSP and thus does not allow for that possibility:
- *Speaks Italian. (English, -NSP)
Portuguese
Portuguese displays full pro-drop by allowing subjects of finite clauses to be phonetically null:[24]
Chegaram.
arrived-3PL
‘They have arrived.’
Provided this example, it is important to note that variations of Portuguese can differ with respect to their pro-drop features. While European Portuguese (EP) is a full pro-drop language, Brazilian Portuguese (BP) exhibits partial pro-drop. The two are compared below, respectively:
Examples of omitted subject:
Estás
Are
a
to
ver
see
este
this
tronco?
log?
Seria
Would be
bom
good
para
for
a
the
fogueira.
campfire.
Secou
Dried
completamente.
completely
(European Portuguese)
(Do) (you) see this log? (It) would be good for the campfire. (It) has completely dried.
Está(s)
Are
vendo
seeing
esse
this
tronco?
log?
Seria
Would be
bom
good
pra
for-the
fogueira.
campfire.
Secou
Dried
completamente.
completely
(Brazilian Portuguese)
(Do) (you) see this log? (It) would be good for the campfire. (It) has completely dried.
Omission of object pronouns is likewise possible when the referent is clear, especially in colloquial or informal language:
Acho
Think
que
that
ele
he
vai
goes
rejeitar
(to-)reject
a
the
proposta,
proposal,
mas
but
pode
may
aceitar.
accept.
(I) think he is going to turn down the proposal, but (he) may accept (it).
Ainda
Still
tem
is there
macarrão?
pasta?
Não,
No,
papai
daddy
comeu.
ate.
Is there pasta left? No, daddy ate (it).
The use of the object pronoun in these examples (aceitá-la, comeu-o) is the default everywhere but Brazil.
Ela
She
me
me
procurou
sought
ontem
yesterday
e
and
não
not
achou.
found.
She looked for me yesterday and didn't find (me).
Here não me achou would also be possible.
A:
A:
Eu
I
te
you
amo;
love;
você
you
também
too
me
me
ama?
love?
B:
B:
Amo,
Love-1sg,
sim.
yes.
A: I love you; do you love me too? B: I do.
Omission of the object pronoun is possible even when its referent has not been explicitly mentioned, so long as it can be inferred. The next example might be heard at a store; the referent (a dress) is clear to the interlocutor. In both Brazilian and European Portuguese the pronoun is omitted.
Viu
Saw
que
how
bonito?
beautiful?
Não
Don't
gosta?
like?
Pode
Can
comprar?
buy?
(BP)
(using polite 2nd person) (BP)
Viste
Saw
que
how
bonito?
beautiful?
Não
Don't
gostas?
like?
Podes
Can
comprar?
buy?
(EP)
(using informal 2nd person) (EP)
Have you seen how beautiful it is? Do you like it? Can you buy it?
Pro-drop with locative and partitive
Modern Spanish and Portuguese are also notable amongst Romance languages because they have no specific pronouns for circumstantial complements (arguments denoting circumstance, consequence, place or manner, modifying the verb but not directly involved in the action) or partitives (words or phrases denoting a quantity of something).[clarification needed] However, both languages had them during the Middle Ages: Portuguese hi and ende.
Compare the following examples in which Spanish, Portuguese, Galician, and Romanian have null pronouns for place and partitives, but Catalan, French, Occitan, and Italian have overt pronouns for place and partitive.
language | locative | partitive | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Spanish | ¡Voy! | Tengo cuatro. | ||
Portuguese | Vou! | Tenho quatro. | ||
Galician | Vou! | Teño catro. | ||
Romanian | Mă duc! | Am patru. | ||
Catalan | Hi | vaig! | En | tinc quatre. |
French | J'y | vais ! | J'en | ai quatre. |
Occitan | I | vau! | N' | ai quatre. |
Italian | Ci | vado! | Ne | ho quattro. |
'I'm going [there]!' | 'I have four (of them).' |
Other examples
Arabic
Arabic is considered a null-subject language, as demonstrated by the following example:
sāʻid
help.PRES.3SG you
ghayrak,
other,
yusāʻiduk.
help.PST.3SG you
Help another, (he) helps you.
Turkish
Sen-i
2SG-ACC
gör-dü-m
see-PAST-1SG
(I) saw you.
The subject "I" above is easily inferable as the verb gör-mek "to see" is conjugated in the first person simple past tense form. The object is indicated by the pronoun seni in this case. Strictly speaking, pronominal objects are generally explicitly indicated, although frequently possessive suffixes indicate the equivalent of an object in English, as in the following sentence.
Gel-diğ-im-i
come-NMLZ-POSS.1SG-ACC
gör-dü-n
see-PAST-2SG
mü?
Q
Did you see me coming?
In this sentence, the object of the verb is actually the action of coming performed by the speaker (geldiğimi "my coming"), but the object in the English sentence, "me", is indicated here by the possessive suffix -im "my" on the nominalised verb. Both pronouns can be explicitly indicated in the sentence for purposes of emphasis, as follows:
Sen
2SG
ben-im
1SG-POSS
gel-diğ-im-i
come-"ing"-POSS.1SG-ACC
gör-dü-n
see-PAST-2SG
mü?
Q
Did you see me coming?
Swahili
In Swahili, both subject and object pronouns can be omitted as they are indicated by verbal prefixes.
Ni-
SUBJ.1SG-
-ta-
-FUT-
-ku-
-OBJ.2SG-
-saidia.
-help
(I) will help (you).
English
English is not a pro-drop language, but subject pronouns are almost always dropped in imperative sentences (e.g., Come here! Do tell! Eat your vegetables!), with the subject "you" understood or communicated non-verbally.[25]
In informal speech, the pronominal subject is sometimes dropped. The ellipsis has been called "conversational deletion" and "left-edge deletion",[26][27][28] and is common in informal spoken English as well as certain registers of written English, notably diaries.[29] Most commonly, it is the first person singular subject which is dropped.[30]
Some other words, especially copulas and auxiliaries, can also be dropped.
- [Have you] ever been there?
- [I'm] going shopping. [Do you] want to come?
- [I] haven't been there yet. [I'm] going later.
- Seen on signs: [I am/We are] out to lunch; [I/we shall be] back at 1:00 [P.M].
- What do you think [of it]? – I like [it]! (the latter only in some dialects and registers)
- [Do you] want a piece of cake?
- [You] are not! – [I] am too! This pattern is also common with other tenses (e.g., were, will) and verbs (e.g., do/did, have/had).
In speech, when pronouns are not dropped, they are more often reduced than other words in an utterance.
Relative pronouns, provided they are not the subject, are often dropped in short restrictive clauses: That's the man [whom] I saw.
The dropping of pronouns is generally restricted to very informal speech and certain fixed expressions, and the rules for their use are complex and vary among dialects and registers. A noted instance was the "lived the dream" section of George H. W. Bush's speech at the 1988 Republican National Convention.[31][32][33][34]
German
Colloquial and dialectal German, unlike the standard language, are also partially pro-drop and typically allow deletion of the subject pronoun only in main clauses without inversion. German has personal inflections of verbs, which makes pro-drop sentences easier to understand.
Other language families and linguistic regions
Among the Indo-European and Dravidian languages of India, pro-drop is the general rule though many Dravidian languages do not have overt verbal markers to indicate pronominal subjects. Mongolic languages are similar in this respect to Dravidian languages, and all Paleosiberian languages are rigidly pro-drop.
Outside of northern Europe, most Niger–Congo languages, Khoisan languages of Southern Africa and Austronesian languages of the Western Pacific, pro-drop is the usual pattern in almost all linguistic regions of the world. In many non-pro-drop Niger–Congo or Austronesian languages, like Igbo, Samoan and Fijian, however, subject pronouns do not occur in the same position as a nominal subject and are obligatory even when the latter occurs. In more easterly Austronesian languages, like Rapa Nui and Hawaiian, subject pronouns are often omitted even though no other subject morphemes exist. Pama–Nyungan languages of Australia also typically omit subject pronouns even when there is no explicit expression of the subject.
Many Pama–Nyungan languages, however, have clitics, which often attach to nonverbal hosts to express subjects. The other languages of Northwestern Australia are all pro-drop, for all classes of pronoun. Also, Papuan languages of New Guinea and Nilo-Saharan languages of East Africa are pro-drop.
Among the indigenous languages of the Americas, pro-drop is almost universal, as would be expected from the generally polysynthetic and head-marking character of the languages. That generally allows eliding of all object pronouns as well as subject ones. Indeed, most reports on Native American languages show that even the emphatic use of pronouns is exceptionally rare. Only a few Native American languages, mostly language isolates (Haida, Trumai, Wappo) and the Oto-Manguean family are known for normally using subject pronouns.
Yahgan, an extinct language isolate from Tierra del Fuego, had no pro-drop when it was still spoken widely in the late 19th century, when it was first described grammatically and had texts translated from English and other languages (three biblical New Testament texts: Luke, John, and Acts of the Apostles). In fact, emphatic pronouns and cross-reference pronouns on the verb commonly appeared together.
Pragmatic inference
Classical Chinese exhibits extensive dropping not only of pronouns but also of any terms (subjects, verbs, objects, etc.) that are pragmatically inferable, which gives a very compact character to the language. Note, however, that Classical Chinese was a written language, and such word dropping is not necessarily representative of the spoken language or even of the same linguistic phenomenon.
See also
- Null-subject language – Class of language where a sentence subject is not required (NSL)
- Null subject parameter – Parameter that determines whether the subject can be dropped from a sentence (NSP) – The parameter which determines if languages are pro-drop, marking them as either positive (+) or negative (-) NSP.[35]
- Zero copula – Lacking or omission of a "to be" verb, common in some languages and stylistic in others; many languages such as Arabic and Hebrew lack a "to be" verb which is implicit in the subject.
References
- ^ Welo, Eirik (24 September 2013). "Null Anaphora". Encyclopedia of Ancient Greek Language and Linguistics. doi:10.1163/2214-448x_eagll_com_00000254.
- ^ a b Wang, Longyue; Tu, Zhaopeng; Zhang, Xiaojun; Liu, Siyou; Li, Hang; Way, Andy; Liu, Qun (2017-06-01). "A novel and robust approach for pro-drop language translation". Machine Translation. 31 (1): 65–87. doi:10.1007/s10590-016-9184-9. hdl:1893/24678. ISSN 1573-0573. S2CID 10567431.
- ^ Martin Haspelmath, The European linguistic area: Standard Average European, in Martin Haspelmath, et al., Language Typology and Language Universals, vol. 2, 2001, pp. 1492-1510
- ^ Fabio Foresti, Dialetti emiliano-romagnoli, Enciclopedia Treccani
- ^ a b Zushi, Mihoko (2003-04-01). "Null arguments: the case of Japanese and Romance". Lingua. Formal Japanese syntax and universal grammar: the past 20 years. 113 (4): 559–604. doi:10.1016/S0024-3841(02)00085-2. ISSN 0024-3841.
- ^ Kordić, Snježana (2001). Wörter im Grenzbereich von Lexikon und Grammatik im Serbokroatischen [Serbo-Croatian Words on the Border Between Lexicon and Grammar]. Studies in Slavic Linguistics ; 18 (in German). Munich: Lincom Europa. pp. 10–12. ISBN 978-3-89586-954-9. LCCN 2005530313. OCLC 47905097. OL 2863539W. CROSBI 426497. Summary.
- ^ Shlonsky, Ur (2009). "Hebrew as a partial null-subject language*". Studia Linguistica. 63 (1): 133–157. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9582.2008.01156.x. ISSN 1467-9582.
- ^ a b Butt, Miriam (2001-01-01). "Case, Agreement, Pronoun Incorporation and Pro-Drop in South Asian Languages". ResearchGate.
- ^ Chomsky, Noam (2010-12-14). Lectures on Government and Binding. De Gruyter Mouton. doi:10.1515/9783110884166. ISBN 978-3-11-088416-6.
- ^ Bresnan, Joan (1982). The Mental Representation of Grammatical Relations. MIT Press. p. 384. ISBN 9780262021586.
- ^ R.L. Trask, A Dictionary of Grammatical Terms in Linguistics, Routledge 2013, p. 218.
- ^ Huang, C.-T. James. "On the distribution and reference of empty pronouns". Linguistic Inquiry 15: 531-574. 1984.
- ^ a b Flores-Ferrán, Nydia (2007-11-01). "A Bend in the Road: Subject Personal Pronoun Expression in Spanish after 30 Years of Sociolinguistic Research". Language and Linguistics Compass. 1 (6): 624–652. doi:10.1111/j.1749-818X.2007.00031.x. ISSN 1749-818X.
- ^ a b c d Barbosa, Pilar P. (1 June 2019). "pro as a Minimal nP: Toward a Unified Approach to Pro-Drop". Linguistic Inquiry. 50 (3): 487–526. doi:10.1162/ling_a_00312. S2CID 62520202. Retrieved 12 December 2021.
- ^ a b Jung, Euen Hyuk (Sarah) (2004). "Topic and Subject Prominence in Interlanguage Development". Language Learning. 54 (4): 713–738. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9922.2004.00284.x. ISSN 1467-9922.
- ^ a b O'Grady, William; Yamashita, Yoshie; Cho, Sookeun (2008). "Object Drop in Japanese and Korean". Language Acquisition. 15 (1): 58–68. doi:10.1080/10489220701774278. ISSN 1048-9223. JSTOR 20462508. S2CID 143578926.
- ^ Li, Yen-Hui Audrey (2014-11-01). "Born empty". Lingua. Structural Approaches to Ellipsis. 151: 43–68. doi:10.1016/j.lingua.2013.10.013. ISSN 0024-3841.
- ^ Li, Charles & Thompson, Sandra. (1976). Subject and Topic: A New Typology of Language. Subject and Topic.
- ^ Hacohen, Gonen; Schegloff, Emanuel A. (2006-08-01). "On the preference for minimization in referring to persons: Evidence from Hebrew conversation". Journal of Pragmatics. Focus-on Issue: Discourse and Conversation. 38 (8): 1305–1312. doi:10.1016/j.pragma.2006.04.004.
- ^ Bhatt, Rajesh (2003). Experiencer subjects. Handout from MIT course “Structure of the Modern Indo-Aryan Languages”.
- ^ a b c "Pronombre Personales Tónicos". Diccionario Panhispánico de Dudas. Real Academia Española. 2005.
- ^ Camacho, Jose A. (2013). Null Subjects. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/cbo9781139524407. ISBN 978-1-139-52440-7.[permanent dead link ]
- ^ Sessarego, Sandro; Gutierrez-Rexach, Javier (2017). "Revisiting the Null Subject Parameter: New Insights from Afro-Peruvian Spanish". Open Journal of Romance Linguistics. 3 (1): 43–68. doi:10.5565/rev/isogloss.26. hdl:10256/15298. Retrieved 12 December 2021.
- ^ Barbosa, Pilar P. (2011). "Pro-drop and theories of pro in the minimalist program part 1: Consistent null subject languages and the pronominal-agr hypothesis". Language and Linguistics Compass. 5 (8): 551–570. doi:10.1111/j.1749-818X.2011.00293.x.
- ^ Geoffrey K. Pullum, Rodney Huddleston, A Student's Introduction to English Grammar, 2005, ISBN 1139643800, p. 170
- ^ Waldman, Katy (May 4, 2016). "Why Do We Delete the Initial Pronoun From Our Sentences? Glad You Asked". Slate.
- ^ Randolph H. Thrasher, Shouldn't ignore these strings: A study of conversational deletion, PhD dissertation, 1974, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor (not seen)
- ^ Randolph H. Thrasher, "One way to say more by saying less: A study of so-called subjectless sentences", 1977, Kwansei Gakuin University Monograph Series 11 Tokyo: Eihosha (not seen)
- ^ Andrew Weir, "Left-edge deletion in English and subject omission in diaries", English Language & Linguistics 16:1:105-129 (March 2012) doi:10.1017/S136067431100030X
- ^ Susanne Wagner, "Never saw one – first-person null subjects in spoken English", English Language and Linguistics 22:1:1-34 (March 2018)
- ^ Bush, George H. W. (18 August 1988). "Address Accepting the Presidential Nomination at the Republican National Convention in New Orleans". American Presidency Project. University of California, Santa Barbara. Archived from the original on 22 July 2015. Retrieved 22 July 2015.
Those were exciting days. Lived in a little shotgun house, one room for the three of us. Worked in the oil business, started my own. In time we had six children. Moved from the shotgun to a duplex apartment to a house. Lived the dream - high school football on Friday night, Little League, neighborhood barbecue.
- ^ Didion, Joan (27 October 1988). "Insider Baseball". The New York Review of Books. Retrieved 22 July 2015.
as Bush, or Peggy Noonan, had put it in the celebrated no-subject-pronoun cadences of the "lived the dream" acceptance speech.
- ^ Greenfield, Jeff (September 2008). "Accepting the Inevitable: What McCain can learn from the acceptance speeches of Reagan, Bush, and Gore". Slate: 2.
Note how, as he tells his story, the pronouns drop out, underscoring the idea that this was more a conversation than a speech
- ^ Winant, Gabriel (21 December 2006). "When the Going Gets Tough". Leland Quarterly. Retrieved 23 July 2015.
Bush projects an image as a forthright Westerner who has no truck with fancy language or personal pronouns.
- ^ Sessarego, Sandro; Gutierrez-Rexach, Javier (2017). "Revisiting the Null Subject Parameter: New Insights from Afro-Peruvian Spanish". Isogloss. 3 (1): 43–68. doi:10.5565/rev/isogloss.26. hdl:10256/15298.
Further reading
- Bresnan, Joan (ed.) (1982) The Mental Representation of Grammatical Relations, MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts.
- Chomsky, Noam (1981) Lectures on Government and Binding: The Pisa Lectures. Holland: Foris Publications. Reprint. 7th Edition. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter, 1993.
- Graffi, Giorgio (2001) 200 Years of Syntax. A critical survey, John Benjamins, Amsterdam, the Netherlands.
- Moro, Andrea (1997) The raising of predicates. Predicative noun phrases and the theory of clause structure, Cambridge Studies in Linguistics, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England.
- Rizzi, Luigi (1982) Issues in Italian Syntax, Foris, Dordrecht.
- Krivochen, Diego and Peter Kosta (2013) Eliminating Empty Categories. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang.
- Jaeggli, Osvaldo, and Ken Safir (1989) The Null Subject Parameter. Dordrecht: Kluwer.
External links
- List of languages Archived 2009-04-06 at the Wayback Machine including pro-drop (PD) or non-pro-drop (NPD) status.