Toward A Phenomenology of Emotion in Film - Randall Halle PDF
Toward A Phenomenology of Emotion in Film - Randall Halle PDF
Toward A Phenomenology of Emotion in Film - Randall Halle PDF
%U\QQWUXSDQG7KH)DFHRI*D\6KDPH
5DQGDOO+DOOH
MLN, Volume 124, Number 3, April 2009 (German Issue), pp. 683-707
(Article)
3XEOLVKHGE\7KH-RKQV+RSNLQV8QLYHUVLW\3UHVV
DOI: 10.1353/mln.0.0157
Toward a Phenomenology
of Emotion in Film:
Michael Brynntrup and
The Face of Gay Shame
Randall Halle
1
For an overview discussion see Klaus Scherer, Profiles of emotion-antecedent
appraisal, Cognition and Emotion 11:2 (1997): 11350; Klaus Scherer, Universality of
emotional expression, Encyclopedia of Human Emotions, ed. D. Levinson, J. Ponzetti,
and P. Jorgenson, vol. 2 (New York: Macmillan, 1999) 66974.
MLN 124 (2009): 683707 2009 by The Johns Hopkins University Press
684
RANDALL HALLE
MLN
685
this area marks the beginning of the affective turn. It has led to an
expansion of new research and new models of perception, emotion,
and affect, and it will be of central consideration in this essay.6
There is a second turn of central interest to this essay and that
is the turn to cognitive film studies. While cognitive scientists have
carefully constructed experiments with visual materials to trace out
how perception functions, they have not extensively considered different modes of viewing, different media of seeing.7 Cognitive film
studies holds the potential to offer to cognitive scientists a wealth
of information and an elaborated language developed over years to
describe various aspects of image and viewing, frame and positionality, the embodiment of viewing as it interacts with the constructed
viewing space and directed viewing material. Cognitive film studies is
a new development, having emerged in the span of the last decade.8
While posing new productive questions, the attempt to develop an
interdisciplinary relationship between cognitive science and film studies up until now has resulted mainly in a rather one-sided direction
of application.9 Cognitive film studies have primarily sought to adopt
begun to consider the possible primacy of emotions and affective states in establishing
in Nietzschean termsa reactive filter or an active constituting mechanism within the
process of cognition. Indeed, seeing and perception as part of cognition are increasingly
understood as part of a process that shapes, forms, and constitutes what is seen.
6
See Patricia Ticineto Clough and Jean Halley eds., The Affective Turn: Theorizing the
Social (Durham: Duke UP, 2007); Sara Ahmed, The Cultural Politics of Emotions (New
York: Routledge, 2004); Brian Massumi, Parables for the Virtual: Movement, Affect, Sensation (Durham: Duke UP, 2002).
7
Terence Horgan and John Tienson did initiate what is known as the frame problem
in cognitive science, but this problem is largely about individual circumstances and
social positioning and has never taken on the quality of exploring the visual frame or
the embodiment of seeing. See Terence Horgan and John Tienson, Representations
Without Rules, Philosophical Topics 17 (1989): 14774.
8
Seeking to draw on the insights of cognitive science and analytic philosophy, cognitive film studies were initiated with some controversy by David Bordwell and Nol
Carroll, and have led to productive studies that chart out new directions for the discipline. See David Bordwell and Nol Carroll, eds., Post-Theory: Reconstructing Film Studies
(Madison: U of Wisconsin P, 1996). See also David Bordwell, Figures Traced in Light: On
Cinematic Staging (Berkeley: U of California P, 2005), and also his own webpage with
extensive discussions and blogs: http://www.davidbordwell.net/essays/zizek.php. Warren Bucklands excellent synthetic study of cognitive and semiotic approaches and Per
Perrsons attempt to develop a psychological theory of moving imagery have helped
reinvigorate areas of investigation, like spectator studies, that had reached impasses
in existing critical methodologies. See Warren Buckland, The Cognitive Semiotics of Film
(Cambridge: Cambridge UP, 2000); Per Persson, Understanding Cinema: A Psychological
Theory of Moving Imagery (Cambridge: Cambridge UP, 2003).
9
Bordwell and Carroll, xiii.
686
RANDALL HALLE
MLN
687
688
RANDALL HALLE
MLN
689
690
RANDALL HALLE
17
MLN
691
692
RANDALL HALLE
MLN
693
694
RANDALL HALLE
24
25
MLN
695
696
RANDALL HALLE
MLN
697
698
RANDALL HALLE
in contradistinction to EAT and the pornographic source of the footage, that these images come from a private and even intimate sphere
and were not intended or released for appropriation into a public
and sexualized sphere. It further provokes with Warning title cards
and at two points either delays or stops the review of names and faces
by instructing the spectator in 1950s stylized lurid language of the
social problem film that the viewer is also responsible for images
made public. Be aware that when you watch the following film, you
will infringe upon the privacy of those persons who have been photographed. You have one minute to decide if you really want to see
this film. Dont hesitate to leave the theater with a clear conscience
if that be your choice.
The face here is the face of the lover with personal information
attached. The emotional register explored, however, is not that of a
melodramatic register, or even that of flipping through a friends photo
album. In contradistinction to EAT, the face is identified and the question of permission draws up a sense of ownership and (failed) agency
over the image. This is an unusual state to describe as emotional, and
again with Brynntrup we reach the limits of the expressible in words.
The cognitive theories all work to describe emotion within narrative.
How does this non-narrative focus on the face require an expanded
understanding of emotional systems?
In their work, Grodal, Smith, and Plantinga all rely on a downstream
flow of perception, cognition, emotional processing in narrative film.30
It is a uni-directional flow; the viewers see, they comprehend, they
experience emotion. However, underlying all of their work are Silvan
Tomkinss foundational studies of affect from the 1960s. Tomkinss
analyses make possible a more complicated multi-directional understanding of affect, an understanding that better describes Brynntrups
work. Tomkins explored affect as located in the voice, skin, autonomic
nervous system, hand, body, and most extensively, the face. Rather
than perceive affect and emotion as developing outward from the
inner organs as Henri Bergson, William James, or Carl Lange had
suggested, Tomkins and his colleagues Carrol Izard and Paul Ekman
focused mostly on the face as an organ for the maximal transmission
of information, to the self and to others and concluded that the
information it transmits is largely concerned with affects.31 This is the
Grodal 132.
Silvan Tomkins, What and Where are the Primary Affects? Some Evidence for a
Theory, Exploring Affect: The Selected Writings of Silvan S. Tomkins, ed. E. Virginia Demos
(Cambridge: Cambridge UP, 1995) 218.
30
31
MLN
699
700
RANDALL HALLE
MLN
701
702
RANDALL HALLE
in front of his face and shoots into a mirror. All the men, presenting
themselves, take self-portraits in a mirror, holding the camera in front
of their faces. The body appears but the eyes, nose, mouth, the specific
facial features, are obscured. Sometimes it is simply a camera that is
positioned in front of the face. Often, however, it is a flash from the
camera. In place of the face alluded to in the title, FACE IT, the viewer
sees a bright blast, a flare, a sort of radiant halo.
This motif of the flash is new. The other and older strategy of choice
to preserve a form of anonymity while presenting the self as incitement
is to cut the head out of the frame. This strategy establishes an aesthetic connection to antiquity: the men display themselves as a classic
Greek torso. The flash, however, belongs to the aesthetic aspects of
the technological possibilities opened up by the cheap digital cameras
and ubiquitous hosting platforms of Web 2.0. This flash that effaces
is a new form of self-representation.
Brynntrup edited these images together so that they pass review at
various speeds to the rhythm of an electronic soundtrack. Periodically
there is a countdown of the type that usually appears in a film trailer;
this acts as a sort of coda or refrain. Toward the end, the montage
accelerates, taking on the quality of collage. The final sequence
occludes the men and shows instead only the cameras, repeating the
entire sequence again, as if to suggest that it was always only about the
cameras. In the absence of the bodies the erotic aspect of the images
dissipates. Plantinga, Smith, or Grodal offer little that can address
these non-narrative faceless pieces. Where Plantinga considered the
close-up and the face, Brynntrup offers the body without a head (and
the camera without the body without the head). Nevertheless, clearly,
the piece can affect its viewers.
The significance of the images grew as FACE IT began to circulate.
During the Berlinale, the closet was positioned in the Atrium of the
Filmhaus on Berlins Potsdamer Platz. Starting the following August,
it traveled as a projected piece through a large festival circuit from
Berlin to Budapest, Moscow, Istanbul, So Paulo, Seoul, the Hague,
and Paris. And then in a new form, as a return of the project to the
web, Brynntrup loaded FACE IT (Cast your self ) as streaming video
onto YouTube. In spite of its success on the festival circuit, the debut
of the piece on YouTube lasted only a half hour before it was flagged
as inappropriate content. Here the piece returned the images to the
web, their original medium, albeit in a different forum. Nevertheless
the YouTube viewer had the opportunity to experience the images in
that illusory private space in which they originally appeared, while
bringing them into a decidedly less gay, more public space.
MLN
703
704
RANDALL HALLE
MLN
705
706
RANDALL HALLE
MLN
707