Resolute Ignorance On Race and Rushton
Resolute Ignorance On Race and Rushton
Resolute Ignorance On Race and Rushton
a r t i c l e
i n f o
Article history:
Available online 26 November 2012
Keywords:
Evolution
Intelligence
Life-history theory
Mob science
Race differences
Resolute ignorance
a b s t r a c t
I review Rushtons research on the evolutionary divergence of the three major human lineages. His lifehistory theory predicts, and his multiple analyses document, a consistent three-way patterning of mean
differences among blacks, whites, and East Asians on coevolved sets of morphological, physiological,
developmental, psychological, and behavioral traits. I then analyze a typical example of how critics evaluate his work, including the rate at which they cast his scientic hypotheses, methods and conclusions in
politically charged language. The set of articles in question, although authored by well-known academics
and appearing in a major, peer-reviewed journal, illustrate how mob science works to discredit valid
research and enforce collective ignorance about entire bodies of evidence. Rushton is a scholar and gentleman but it appears that his critics often act like neither.
2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
Philippe (Phil) Rushton has contributed important works to
evolutionary psychology, intelligence, and personality psychology.
I focus here on his work receiving the most attention. That is his
life history theory of how the major races, or geographic lineages,
evolved in somewhat different directions as humans spread out of
Africa about 50100 kya (Rushton, 1995). He has documented a
large suite of morphological, physiological, developmental, psychological, and behavioral differences among these groups, whose
most recent common ancestors are from Africa, Europe and East
Asia. These various traits cohere evolutionarily and are consistent
with his life history explanation of the wide array of mean group
differences that persist over generations.
I rst assess Rushtons research contributions. Then I analyze a
typical example of the scornful commentary on the man and his
worka target article and eight comments published together in
a major, peer-reviewed journal. I look especially at the nature of
evidence and argument used by Rushton and the authors asked
to evaluate his work.
Rushton is proudly of the London School of thought in psychology. It rejects the separation of mind from body and of culture from
its genetic substrate, preferring instead to probe their connections.
It was an outpost of biological realism during the long reign of
behaviorism, whose founder James B. Watson famously assured
Life history is a population-level concept. It refers to the coordinated suite of traits and behaviors that characterizes a particular
species or subspecies (its shared life), which evolved in response
to the recurring adaptive challenges its members faced (its evolutionary history). The life-history concept highlights an important
empirical phenomenon. A species distinctive traitssuch as humans large brain, slow maturation, and pair bondingdo not
evolve independently, one by one, but as a constellation of
219
brains information processing. East Asians have the fastest decision reaction times (measured in milliseconds) on elementary cognitive tasks, which are so simple that virtually everyone can
perform them correctly, and they also have the highest average
levels of general intelligence (g) on validated, unbiased tests of
intelligence.
The pattern of cranial vs. sub-cranial group differences illustrates a second insight from life-history theory: evolution imposes
tradeoffs. For example, the populations with larger brains have
(had to evolve) smaller bodies; and those with slower maturing
offspring have (had to evolve) more socially and sexually constrained adults.
Note that none of the aforementioned biological traits is a social
marker of race, either singly or collectively, as would be skin color
and hair type. Moreover, as Rushton and others have shown, within-race variation in all these characteristics is moderately to highly
heritable, so we cannot dismiss the possibility that mean group differences in body and behavior are also somewhat genetically
rooted. In fact, these mean differences in non-racial traits appear
to shift in tandem, as a coordinated set, from one human lineage to
another. This implicates a consistent deep inuence linked more
tightly to distant genetic heritage than to current circumstance.
The great variation within racial groups is entirely consistent with
genetic divergence, because within-population variation is the
grist for evolution. Also consistent is the systematic overlap among
groups, because mean differences in genotype will emerge from
the same ancestral genotype, slowly but surely, when the groups
adaptive demands diverge and consistently pull selection in somewhat different directions.
Rushtons results reect a third insight of life history theory:
individuals do not evolve, populations do. A populations social
organizationits culturenecessarily co-evolves with the distribution of its members attributes. Humans, for example, are not just
an exceptionally brainy primate, but also an especially social one.
We have a special penchant for pair bonding (even if serial), living
and working in groups, forming networks and coalitions, trading,
teaching, gossiping, reading others minds, and befriending nonkin. Our big-brained, slow-maturing, vulnerable and care-intensive
children would not survive without such enduring social networks
and bonds of long-term reciprocity.
Rushtons version of life-history theory proposes that reproductive strategies drive (cause) differences in social organization.
Although all humans are K-oriented (relatively few offspring with
much parenting), his rK theory posits that the somewhat less Koriented human populations will tend more strongly toward social
relations guided by self-interest, relative physical strength, and a
tolerance for interpersonal conict, with the result that, from a K
perspective, they produce cultures with less stable families, less organized institutions, more transgressions of person and property,
and therefore higher rates of mental and physical illness. In contrast, more K-oriented lineages produce societies whose members
tend to exercise more self-control, social control, and mutual coordination in the pursuit of longer-term shared goals, with the result
that such groups will create somewhat better organized, more productive, personally secure andfrom an r perspectivemore rigid
societies that tightly constrain what their members may do. Nonevolutionary explanations have been offered for these differences
in social organization but none, to my knowledge, can account
forlet alone has predictedthe nexus of physical, mental, and
behavioral mean differences that Rushton foundand had
predicted.
2.2. Forensic anthropology
Rushton has also tested his life-history theory by following
hominoids back down their evolutionary tree. Rushton and
220
Rushton (2004) examined progressive changes in 76 musculo-skeletal traits across seven hominoid populations, listed here by evolutionary age: Pan troglodytes (chimpanzee), Australopithecines, Homo
habilis, Homo erectus, Africans, Europeans, and East Asians (for humans: Rosenberg et al., 2005). The aim was to test the hypothesis
that a cascade of skeletal changes accompanied the evolution of
brain size in hominoids, ranging from humans most distant relative (the chimpanzee, 5 mya) to the youngest human lineage (East
Asians). Standard texts on evolutionary anatomy provided data on
76 skeletal traits for the one ape and the three fossil species, and
standard forensic anthropology textbooks provided data on 42
traits for the three human populations.
The skeletal data for the seven hominoid groups did, in fact, differentiate them in the same order as did their evolutionary distance from the youngest human lineage (East Asians). Of the 42
traits available for the three racial groups, 38 were measured in
absolute terms14 cranial traits (including cranial capacity), 8
teeth and mandibular traits, 3 neck, 3 pelvic, and 12 upper and
lower limb traits. All but one (nasal bone prominence) t the predicted pattern.
The 6 other traits, measured as body proportions, uniformly did
not (e.g., leg length as a % of height, weight of upper limbs as % of
body weight). Ratios and percentages of a trait are hard to interpret
because they have different measurement properties than do absolute measures of a trait, such as length, area, volume and weight.
Nor is it clear that rK theory makes any predictions for body proportions. If we set the body proportions data aside for now, it appears that the increase in cranial capacity from 380 to 1364 cm3
across the 7 hominoid groups was accompanied by systematic
and pervasive changes across the musculo-skeletal system, including cranial traits (e.g., size and shape of the skull, jaw, teeth, eye
sockets, brows, muscle attachments) as well as post-cranial traits
(e.g., dimensions, shape and orientation of particular bones, joints,
and pelvis). The most general change is that the musculo-skeletal
system became less robust as brain size increased. Perhaps there
is a non-evolutionary explanation for the progressive and pervasive skeletal differences among blacks, whites, and East Asians that
are so well known to forensic investigators, but I am not aware of
any plausible ones having been offered.
221
To justify his premise, Lieberman describes 19th century research on cranial size and its social context which, he says, was
the need by Caucasians to justify their domination and exploitation
of other races. He discredits that research and Rushtons own primarily by appealing to authority in 20th century anthropology:
Franz Boass theorizing (no link between culture and genes), ofcial statements on race from the UN and the American Anthropological Association (no biological races, no meaningful innate
racial differences), Goulds critiques of research on intelligence,
brain size, and heritability (none is valid), and anthropologys disavowal of hierarchical and racist thinking. Lieberman also
draws from stock concerns, long-since resolved, about possible
methodological aws in twin research, brain research, behavior
genetics, and mental testing. He says nothing about the explosion
of research in the 1990s using the Y chromosome and mtDNA to
trace the evolution of human lineages as they migrated across
the globe. He says nothing about Rushtons many other 3-way results on reproductive behavior, sex hormones, twinning rate,
speed of physical maturation, personality, family stability, lawabidingness, and social organization (p. 74), except to summarily
dismiss them as a faulty blizzard of data (p. 78).
Zeroing in on Rushtons IQ, race, and brain size analyses,
Lieberman details his own list of 6 major errors. Briey, Rushton
uses race despite decades of ndings that invalidate it, his conclusions about racial differences in cranial capacity are contradicted by evolutionary anthropology, he did not account for
environmental factors that surely inuence cranial capacity and
intelligence, his measurements tell us nothing because they are
confounded or the differences they reveal are trivial in size, he cannot claim to explain a vast array of human behaviors because
some of his measures and concepts may be faulty, and his principle of aggregation (e.g., grouping diverse populations into races,
averaging results from different studies) is invalid. Aggregation is
Liebermans single most frequent complaint of the 6 (38 times in
14 pages), even though it is a well-known principle that averaging
non-comparable samples would work against Rushtons nding
consistent patterns.
The rst two errors are, again, appeals to authority. The next
two disallow drawing conclusions until an innite regress of alternatives has been considered, and the last two demand uniformly
Table 1
Terms used to impugn Rushton without rebutting his evidence.
Category
Lieberman (2001)
(12 pages article,
2 of reply)
Associated 6 hostile
commentaries
(7 pages total)
Evil ideology
Hierarchical thinking
4.9
4.4
Racist thinking
2.8
7.1
0.7
1.3
0.3
10.0
0.9
2.6
1.1
16.1
Unacceptable science
Incompetent
8.4
6.0
Pseudoscientic
2.0
5.7
Dangerous
0.4
0.9
Subtotal
Total per page
(N instances)
10.8
20.8
(291)
12.6
28.7
(201)
222
Table 2
Yes-but gambits commonly used to ignore evidence on mean racial differences in intelligence (g).
Yes-but gambits:
Racial gaps in
intelligence are. . .
1. Nonexistent
2. Mismeasured
3. Unimportant
4. Malleable
5. Environmental
6. Disproved
7. Unthinkable
223