Richard Noll Chamanismo
Richard Noll Chamanismo
Richard Noll Chamanismo
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/
info/about/policies/terms.jsp
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content
in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship.
For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].
University of Chicago Press and Wenner-Gren Foundation for Anthropological Research are collaborating with JSTOR
to digitize, preserve and extend access to Current Anthropology.
http://www.jstor.org
This content downloaded from 194.51.135.196 on Wed, 13 Jan 2016 12:34:00 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
CURRENT ANTHROPOLOGY
1 The inspiration
forthispaper grewout of manylongand exciting
discussionswith Leonard George of the Universityof WesternOntario,who is alwaysthe"beareroflight."NormanE. Whitten,Jr.,of
the Universityof Illinois (Urbana-Champaign)read an earlierdraft
and providedsome much appreciatedcriticismand encouragement.
JeromeBruner, Daniel Reisberg,and Veronique Foti of the New
School for Social Research gave the fascinatingseminaron mental
imageryand mentalrepresentation
that I attendedin the springof
1983 and forwhichthefirstdraftwas prepared.SuzzettePattersonof
the Universityof WesternOntarioprovidedassistanceand warmencouragement.Michael Harnerof the New School forSocial Research
introducedme to theproblemspresentedby shamanismand continues
to be a sourceof encouragementand inspiration.My gratitudemust
again be expressedto him. A shortertreatment
of theideas contained
in this paper was presentedat the internationalconferenceentitled
"RecentDevelopmentsin Researchon Shamanism"at the Esalen InRICHARD NOLL is a doctoralcandidatein clinicalpsychology
at the
New School for Social Research in New York City (his mailing
address:206 Azalea, Stratford,
N.J. 08084, U.S.A.). Bornin 1959,
he receivedhis B.A. fromtheUniversity
ofArizonain 1979and his
M.A. fromtheNew Schoolin 1982. He is just completing
a predoctoral internshipat Ancora PsychiatricHospital in Hammonton,
N.J., and at the AtlanticMental Health Centerin AtlanticCity,
N.J. His researchinterestsincludethephenomenology
of shamanism,the cross-cultural
utilizationof alteredstatesof consciousness
and mentalimagery,conceptsofdisease and healing,and primitive
religion.He has published "Shamanism and Schizophrenia:A
State-specific
Approach to the 'SchizophreniaMetaphorof Shamanic States" (AmericanEthnologist10:443-59) and "Reply to
Lex" (AmericanEthnologist11:192). The presentpaper was submittedin finalform1 xII 84.
Vol. 26
No. 4
1985
August-October
This content downloaded from 194.51.135.196 on Wed, 13 Jan 2016 12:34:00 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
443
laT%A7-k.
ncvh
7
veI"nm
{(?nPva
1 0'7k
14AThek
' Oftenthedistinction
between"dreams"and "visions"is notclear.
"Dream incubation,"as I use it here,refersto thedeliberateinduction
and/ormanipulationof mentalimageryduringREM sleep. The controlofthesupernatural
throughdreamsin simplersocietiesis discussed
by D'Andrade (1961), and anthropological
studiesof dreamsin these
societiescan be foundin the worksof Lincoln (1935), Eggan (1949),
Seligman(1924, 1932),and Kilborne(1981). The ancientGreekdream
incubationtraditionconnectedwith the templesof Asclepiusis discussed by Edelstein and Edelstein (1945), Meier (1966, 1967), and
Ellenberger(1970:32-34).
5In his fascinatingaccount of ancient Greek theurgy,Dodds
(1951:292) notes that one of its two main brancheswas "concerned
mainlywiththe consecrating. . . and animatingof magic statuesin
orderto obtainoraclesfromthem."These oraclesweretransmitted
to
thedivinerin auditoryimagery.In his discussionofthevisionquestin
NorthAmerica,Lowie (1937:272)observes:"As a matterof fact,'visions'are sometimesnothingof the sortbut hallucinationsof an auditorynature,or at least are associated with non-visualcomponents.
This is not a trivialpointconsideringthe incrediblefrequencyof sacred,i.e., revealed,songs."
444
This content downloaded from 194.51.135.196 on Wed, 13 Jan 2016 12:34:00 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
ANTHROPOLOGY
No. 4
Noll:THE
ROLE OF VISIONS
IN SHAMANISM
August-October
1985
This content downloaded from 194.51.135.196 on Wed, 13 Jan 2016 12:34:00 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
445
. .
. to possess the
446
This content downloaded from 194.51.135.196 on Wed, 13 Jan 2016 12:34:00 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
/nllbTT-TF ROTE
OF VTSTONS TN
SHTAMANTSM
reinforced
This "responsetraining,"says
by theexperimenter.
Lang (1979:506),"appears to act like an amplifier,increasing
447
The "controllability"
ofmentalimagery-"theextentto which
a personcan evoke and change images at will" (Tower and
Singer1981:131)-is a second importantdimensionof mental
imageryexaminedin theliterature(see, e.g., Gordon1949;A.
Richardson 1972; White, Sheehan, and Ashton 1977). The
abilityto controlimageryhas been positivelycorrelatedwitha
varietyof variables,e.g., the frequencyof dreamrecall(Hiscock and Cohen 1973)and thesuccessfulmentalrehearsalofa
of the
characteristic
motorskill. An importantcross-cultural
visioncultivationprocessis thedevelopmentofrelativecontrol
of the
over "the initiation,the guidance,and the termination
(Casey 1976:73). Casey
imaginative act-cum-presentation"
and spontaneity
considersthe"eidetictraits"of controlledness
mutuallyexclusivebut complementary.
oftheshaman'smentalimagerycan only
The controlledness
be relativebecause the spontaneityof the imaginalcontents
with which the shaman must activelyengage is a dominant
characteristicof shamanic accounts. Shamans reportbeing
surprisedand challengedby the unexpectedappearance of
448
This content downloaded from 194.51.135.196 on Wed, 13 Jan 2016 12:34:00 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
ANTHROPOLOGY
Ataboutmidmorning
eachmandrinks
threesmallcupsofthisyaje,all
andaftera whilethepayewillaskhis
thetimesinging
andsmoking,
visionsareblurred;
itislike
"Whatdo yousee?"Thefirst
companions:
from
smokearising
nowhere,
ascending
slowlylikea wallbefore
their
tubesandabsorbthevih6mixture.
eyes.Theynowtaketheirsnuffing
tothe
Lyingintheirhammocks
theynowfeelthattheyareascending
Milky Way.
. .
to
plished.Anapprentice
willhardly
everbe abletoriseimmediately
willlearnto do so onlyafter
thissuperluminary
regionbut,rather,
hewillbarelyriseoverthehorizon;
thenexttime,
manytrials.Atfirst
totheposition
ofthesun
perhaps,
hewillreacha pointcorresponding
at 9 A.M., thenat 10A.M., andso on,untilat last,ina singlesoaring
he willrisetothezenith.
flight,
The shaman mastersspiritsfor didactic reasons as well.
"Duringtheperiodofinitialcontactsthespiritsfunctionabove
all as teachers"(Siikala 1978:228).Jochelson(1905:47)mettwo
Koryak shamans who reportedthat, while in solitude,"the
spiritsappear to them in visible form,endow them with
power,and instructthem."
The shamanis a specialistwhose expertisederivesfromthe
unique contactwith and manipulationof forcesor agencies,
experiencedin visions as autonomous or semiautonomous,
which generallycannot be contactedand manipulatedin his
ordinarystate of waking consciousness.Culturally,whether
theseare interpreted
as exogenousforcesor agenciesexhibiting
a certain"intentionality"
(such as spiritsor gods)or as endogenous ones such as nonanthropomorphized
sources of occult
powerthatresidewithinthe shaman'sbody (e.g., the healing
energyn/um of the Kalahari Kung [Katz 1982]),theyare all
experiencedfromthe perspectiveofthe shamanas originating
fromoutsidehim.To theshaman,theexperienceof"spirits"is
in everysense of the word "real." To the "cognicentrist"
(see
Harner 1980:xiv; Noll 1983:447-50) observer,such experiences can be reduced to explanationsthat account for their
but not theirontology.
phenomenology
Noll: THE
ROLE OF VISIONS
IN SHAMANISM
449
shaman,whichtheshamanmaynotuse,is interesting.
tional processes of memory.In nonliteratesocieties large
Everyshaman
fromlandmarks.
wouldthushavehisownways,whichhe followed
amountsof culturalinformation
mustbe memorizedand conas signposts,
The tentsofvariousspiritbeingsact in themselves
for
tinuallyrehearsed.Retentionof thisinformation
will depend
is drawnin thevisionto
theyareall different,
and specialattention
on how well the materialis organized.The importanceof orsymbolism.
ganizationalstrategiesin memorywas strikingly
demonstrated theirouterformandassociated
by the experimentalstudies of the Gestalt psychologists
The shaman's masteryof mentalimageryis an important
(Katona 1940; Koffka 1935:423-528; Kohler 1947:279-319)
mnemonist.
assetinhisculturalroleas mythological
Eliade (1958:
and Bartlett(1932) and continuesto be supportedby contem102) lauds the "outstandingrole of memory"in shamanism,
porarycognitivepsychologists(see Tulving and Donaldson
forin nonliterate
societies"theshamanis theman who knows
1972). As Cole and Gay (1972:1068)pointout, "thereis good
and remembers."L. G. Peters (1981:62) relates a Tamang
reasonto expectmembersof a pre-literate,
traditionalsociety myth that explains why the shamanic bombo tradition,
to have developedmnemonicskillsdifferent
fromthoseofliter- representedby Nara Bon Chen, the firsthuman shaman, is
ate, technologicalsocieties."I contendthat the most salient
memorizedand maintainedorally,whereasthe"inferior"
lama
mnemonicstrategiesemployed in nonliteratesocieties are
traditionofGuruRinpoche(Padma Sambhava)mustrelyupon
based on theuse ofmentalimagery.Recentresearchhas demtexts:
onstratedthatmentalimageryplays a keyorganizationalrole
One dayGuruRinpochesetoutto foolNara and causehimto lose
in memory.Neisser (1972:243) assertsthat "we cannot help
thathe had burnedall hisbooks
someofhispowers.So he bragged
noticingthatvisual representation
is amongthemostcommon
andnolonger
neededthem.Notwanting
becausehekneweverything
and the mostpowerfulof the [mnemonic]strategiesused" (see
to be shownup, Nara threwhis booksintoa fire.Whentheywere
also Neisser1982). Indeed, in his comprehensive
reviewofthe
and
GuruRinpoche
reachedintohisjackettorevealhistexts,
aflame,
experimentalevidence linkingmental imageryand human
intothefireandate
laughedatNaraforbeingso stupid.Narareached
as you,butI do notneedtexts
all theashes,saying"I amas powerful
memoryJ. T. E. Richardson(1980:82) concludesthat"under
forI havecommitted
to memory."
everything
laboratoryconditionsinstructions
to use mentalimagerymay
in memoryperformance....
lead to substantialimprovements
This tale demonstratesthe respect accorded the shaman's
theseeffectscome about by meansofincreasedorganisationof
superiorpowersof memoryby his community.It is the shathe materialto be remembered."
man's responsibility
to rememberforthe others.
I further
arguethattheshaman'svisionary"travels"to variThe shaman may have a superiormemorybecause of his
ous "levels,"each pregnantwithspecifically
organizedcultural
superioruse of mental imageryskills. If this conclusionis
meanings,operatesas an imagerymnemonicfortheretention valid, thenitforcefully
illustratestheadaptivevalue ofmental
of mythological
beliefsand otherculturallyrelevantmaterials
imagery.Mental imagerycultivationin nonliteratesocieties
in a nonliteratesociety.Siikala (1978:191)reportsthatan immay perhaps best be viewed fromthe culturalevolutionary
portantprofessionalskilldevelopedearlyin thetrainingofthe
standpointas an adaptive mechanismforthe survivalof culnovice shaman is "the transferof traditionand above all its
ture.
in visions."Each individualjourneythata shainternalization
man embarksupon equally servesas a collectiverehearsalof
MENTAL IMAGERY IN HEALING AND DIVINATION
16
culturalmemories.
The visionaryjourney of the shaman to various levels is
As I have pointedout, an importantculturalrole of mental
analogous to followinga "cognitivemap" (Neisser 1976:108imagerycultivationis to be foundin itsstrongassociationwith
53; Casson 1983:444-46)similarto the mnemonic"methodof
magico-religious
traditions.In primitivesocieties,as well as in
loci" inventedby Greek oratorsto retainlarge amountsof
the West in classical times(see Dodds 1951, 1971), two inexin memory(see Yates 1966). "In thismethod,one
information
tricablyrelated functionsof magico-religioustraditionsare
establishesa cognitivemap ofa seriesofparticularlocationsor
healingand divination.What is oftenoverlookedis theuse of
places, which one can mentally'visit' in a particularorder.
enhancedvisual mentalimageryin the performance
of these
listofitemsto memorize,it is onlynecesGiven any arbitrary
functions.
saryto imagineeach depositedon a particularlocation;nearJustas the Greek god Apollo and his son Asclepiuswere
perfectrecallwill followifone then'revisits'the places in the
physiciansand seers(Meier 1967:32),the shaman uses visual
usual order" (Neisser 1972:243). Justas the ancient Greeks
mentalimageryto diagnose and treatdisease. For example,
memorizedsizable blocksof theirorationsby associativelyorHandelman(1967:451)reportsthata Washo shamanwould"at
ganizingthem along a path of consecutiveloci in theirimsome point during the course of the ritual . . . receive visions
mediateenvironment,
so do shamansretainand reviewtheir
relatingto both the cause of the illnessand the prognosis."
culture'scosmologyby journeyingto successive "levels" in
Shamans induce enhancedvisual mentalimageryin orderto
theirvisions.This use oforientation
schemataby "primitives" "see" and identifydisease entitieswithinthe ill client'sbody
was similarlycommentedupon by Galton (1883:103), who
and thenperform
specificimagery-based
techniquesto remove
wrotethat"the Eskimos are geographersby instinct,and apthem(see Harner 1980).17 Mental imagerytherefore
plays an
pear to see vast tractsof countrymapped out in theirheads."
importantrole in shamanichealing.
Siikala (1978:186)strikingly
illustrates
theimportanceofcogniDivinationhas also been an important
goal ofvisioncultivativemaps in shamanismin heranalysisoftheinitiatory
vision
tion.The use ofunstructured
visual stimuli(e.g., crystalballs,
of a Nganasan Samoyedshaman:
pans of water,etc.) to induce mentalimageryfordivinatory
To be abletooperateintheotherworldtheshamanmustbe familiar purposesextendsfarback intopagan antiquity(Daiches 1913;
withitstopography
and theroadscrossing
it. The guiderepeatedly Dodds 1951, 1971).A fertilearea in experimental
parapsycholimpresses
on thenovicetheimportance
of knowing
theroadsand
ogy is the explorationof the relationshipof imageryto psi
to thewayofanother (George 1981, 1982; George and Krippner1984). Shamans
pathsof variousspiritbeings.The reference
16 Anisimov(1963:112)seemsto describethe shaman'sjourneyand
performanceas group trainingin imagerymnemonicskillswhen he
reportsthatamongthe Evenks "such activity. . . was carriedout by
the shamanduringa mass performance
at whichhis fellowclansmen
werenotonlyactiveparticipantsbut also an inspiredaudience,capable of seeingin wakinghallucinationeverything
the shamansaw and
said."
450
This content downloaded from 194.51.135.196 on Wed, 13 Jan 2016 12:34:00 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
ANTHROPOLOGY
Comments
byJEANNE ACHTERBERG
Departmentof Rehabilitation Science, School of Allied
Health Sciences, University
ofTexas Health Science Center
at Dallas, Dallas, Tex. 75235, U.S.A. 15 IV 85
It is withgreatdifficulty
thatI fashionan objectivecomment
on Noll's scholarlyarticle.When thearticlewas sentto me for
review, my own book Imageryin Healing: Shamanismand
Modern Medicine was about to be releasedby New Science
Library(Boulder: Shambhala Publications).It shouldbe clear
fromthe titlethatNoll and I independently
soughtthe same
data base, albeitfromdifferent
perspectives.He offersan exlitercellentsummaryofthepsychologicaland anthropological
ature, meldingit togetherwith the conceptof imagery.My
intentwas to demonstratethe ancient,ubiquitousrole of the
imaginationin medicine,focusingon theshamanas themaster
healerin the imaginaryrealmsand supportingthe thesiswith
biologicalfactwheneverpossible.
We have cometo a singularconclusionregardingimageryas
theessenceofmanyshamanicpracticesand as a cross-cultural
resourcefordiagnosticand therapeutic
information.
However,
I would argue thatthe researchon imageryhas actuallybeen
quite paltry,despite a flurryof recentinterest.The subject
remainsout of favor in the "hard-nosed"psychologicaland
medical communities,despite its 10,000-20,000-yearhistory
as a healingmodality.Even bringingtogetherthe two bodies
ofinformation,
imageryand shamanism,is a reasonablynovel
conjecture.
Certainly,continueddescriptionof the historyand ethnographic aspects of mental imageryis appropriate,as Noll
suggests.In orderto reinstateimageryas a legitimate,even
indispensable,aspect of medicineand psychology,additional
researchmust answer two difficultquestions:What are the
psychobiologicalmechanismsinvolved in the phenomenon?
Does it workforany of thepurposesitstenacioushistorysuggests?
byERIKABOURGUIGNON
by LEONARD
GEORGE
DepartmentofPsychology,Universityof WesternOntario,
London, Ont., Canada N6A 5C2. 22 III 85
Noll's paper exemplifiesinterdisciplinary
scholarshipat its
finest.The perspectivepresentedin his paper highlightsthe
importanceof enhancedimaginalprocessesin manycultures
and suggeststhat, within every population, there exists a
groupof individualswho potentiallyhave a special relationship withthe imaginaldimensionsof experience.If thisis so,
one importantquestionconcernsthe role of such personsin
modernWesternculture. Psychologicalresearchhas shown
in oursociety
thatimaginallyorientedpersonscan be identified
in such formsas fantasy-prone
personalitiesand hypnotically
talentedsubjects(Barber 1984). Systematicimageryenhancementis occurringamonga proportionof thesepersons,in the
formof occult practices,meditation,and, as Noll mentions,
someformsof psychotherapy
(see Georgen.d.). Because there
equivalent to shamanismin
is no modern institutionalized
whichimaginallyorientedpeople can exercisetheircapacities,
cultural
theymayexistunnoticedin a varietyofcontemporary
ofimaginal
niches.One wondersiftherelativenonrecognition
realmsby our societyis an optimalstateofaffairs.If imaginal
experienceis vitalto spiritualexploration,as Noll's perspective
of"mere"imaginationand the
implies,perhapsthedenigration
epidemiccrisisof meaningin the 20thcenturyare morethan
coincidental.
How did the moderndevaluation of imaginal experience
comeabout? Some scholarshave traceditsrootsas farback as
the 8th-century
Council of Nicaea (Avens 1980); othershave
emphasized the rise of Cartesian dualism as a major contributor
(McMahon and Sheikh1984). Generallyspeaking,the
the imagination,when most
modernattitudeof marginalizing
culturesgrantimaginalentitiesa
premodernand non-Western
primaryrolein theirconceptionsoftheworld,musthave had a
multidetermined
and unusual evolution.There are signsthat
the postmodernattitudewill be morebalanced. A varietyof
thinkershave begunto discussthe possiblevalue of revoking
assignedto concretereality
theontologicalpriority
customarily
over the imaginalsphere (e.g., Buchler 1966, Epstein 1981,
Hillman 1983),and seriousnoteis beingtakenoftheimaginal
This content downloaded from 194.51.135.196 on Wed, 13 Jan 2016 12:34:00 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
ANTHROPOLOGY
man findshimselfthe object-not the subject-of supranormal activity.The "visionquest"foundin shamanismand other
contextswould not, then,belongto the mainstreamof ritual
behaviorand would indeedrequirein its ritualizedformtechniquesforan activationor, as Noll putsit, cultivationofmental imagery.The extensivepreparatory
ritualthattheshaman
performsin solitudebeforeeach seance, which has escaped
Noll's attention,representsa concentration
of mentalpower
and a shiftfromone cognitivemap to anotherwiththeaim of
becominga shaman once more. I have describedthis roletakingoftheshaman(1969:26-55) as thepsychologicalcoreof
the ritualprocessin question:mostof what Noll describesas
mentalimagerymaybe further
analysedas counter-roles
ofthe
shaman. The conceptof rolenotonlyorganisestheperceptual
materialsand revealsthestructure
oftheritualbutalso relates
images to their significances.The learningof supranormal
role sets, and role sequences and theirsignificounter-roles,
cance in theideographicsystemis theessenceoftheprocessof
initiationthat everynovice must undergo.Initiationvisions
are of the utmostimportance:it is here that the models of
perceptionand concretementalimagesare createdin a system
thatwill be reproducedin everyseance. The systemis traditional but also containsindividualcreationand choice, as is
beautifullydepicted by Popov (1963:149-59; cf. Siikala
1978:175-87).
The concept of "mental imagery"tends to misrepresent
thingsbybeingtoo comprehensive
and, paradoxically,toonarrow. If all mentalimagesand theircultivationare includedin
the analysis,one tendsto lose sightof thespecial characterof
supranormalor othermarginalphenomena(i.e., phenomena
marginalalso fromthe pointof view of the experiencingand
perceivingperson). The limitationto the visual modalityof
perceptionis detrimentalto the understanding
of the processing of mentalimages, which includesnot onlywhat the shaman sees but also what he hears, touches,smells,and tastes.
Paraphernaliasuch,as the costume,the pipe, the drum,etc.,
are instrumental
in reactivatingmodelsofexperiencefromthe
past, fromthe initiationand the previousseances of the shaman in question.The stimuluswhichreleasesa mentalimage
may come fromany modalityof perceptionor froman interplay of two or moresensorymodes.
by AKE HULTKRANTZ
InstituteofComparativeReligion,University
ofStockholm,
106 91 Stockholm,Sweden. 7 IV 85
Noll's paperis a welcomecontribution
to theanalyticliterature
on ''visions"in societiesin whichshamanismand visionquests
occur. His discussionof the cultivationof visionsis of major
importance,and mostofhis argumentsare convincing.There
are to myunderstanding
some limitationsin his presentation,
however,thatsometimesreduce the value of his argumentation.
I am referring
herein particularto Noll's arbitrary
use ofthe
conceptof shamanism.It seems to be a vague, all-inclusive
concept,takingin both well-developedSiberian shamanism
and the mostlynonshamanicvision quest in North America. The old Americantraditionof equating "shaman" and
"medicineman"-two not necessarilyidenticalconceptsifwe
retaintheoriginalmeaningoftheformerterm-is also noticeable. Afterthecriticisms
by severalauthors,includingmyself,
of Eliade's definitionof soul flightas the essential manifestationof shamanic ecstasy,it is a bit exasperatingto find
thatNoll, withoutdiscussion,accepts thisinterpretation.
He
claims that the shamanic soul flightis a universalfeatureof
shamanismand that shamanismis primarilyconcernedwith
its visual cultivation.This is not correct.Shamans also have
seances in which the spiritsappear (withoutnecessarilypossessingthem:the situationis usuallyone of spiritualinspiration). The main qualityof the shaman is his abilitythrough
Noll: THE
ROLE OF VISIONS
IN SHAMANISM
ecstasyto createcontactwiththesupernaturalworld;whether
he makes a soul flightor calls on the spiritsis a questionof
professionalchoice and culturalways.
Noll's suggestionthatsomeformofimaginalstimulustrainingis essentialto thetrainingofthenoviceshamancan onlybe
consideredjustifiedifwe have a full-fledged
shamanicteacherpupil relationshipand a specialized characterization
of shamanhood.Many oftheNorthAmerican"shamans"would fall
away here.
To Noll, caughtin a psychologicalthoughtpattern,theshaman'sgoal is enhancedmentalimagery.Again,itseemspreferable to be more precise and, using the believer'sown emic
approach,to say clearlythatthegoal is liberatinga soul, counsellingwiththepowersoftheotherworld,diviningthefuture,
and so on. The enhancedcapacityforimageryis onlya prerequisite for realizingthese goals. This confusionbetween visionarycapacity and visionarycontentsaffectsthe author's
interpretation
when he definesspiritmasteryas controlled
mental imagery. Now, spirit masteryratherthan passive
mediumismseems to be characteristic
of mostshamanism,as
we can learnfroma vivid discussionin thejournalEthnos 25
yearsago (cf. Hultkrantz1978:43-47). Still,the shaman'sattitudeto thespiritsvarieswiththeirroles:he is forcedto obey
thespiritsthatcall himintoofficebutcan commandthespirits
that assist him. In some instancesthe obeyed and the commandedspiritsare separateentities.As Backman has pointed
out, data fromLapp (Saami) shamanismshow that we are
dealing with two differentspirit categories(Backman and
Hultkrantz1978:42-43). As faras is knownthereis, however,
no evidence of different
mentalimageryfor the two sets of
spirits.
453
by CHRISTIE W. KIEFER
by RICHARD J. PRESTON
DepartmentofAnthropology,
McMaster University,
Hamilton, Ont., Canada L8S 4L9. 11 III 85
Noll's synthesisis both good science and refreshing
reading.
My majorchallengeis to thehypothesis
thatthemorevividthe
CURRENT
This content downloaded from 194.51.135.196 on Wed, 13 Jan 2016 12:34:00 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
ANTHROPOLOGY
Noll: THE
ROLE OF VISIONS
IN SHAMANISM
455
byMICHAEL WINKELMAN
School ofSocial Sciences, UniversityofCalifornia,Irvine,
Calif. 92717, U.S.A. 9 iv 85
Noll's considerationof psychophysiological
statesinducedby
mentalimagerycultivationis important,but concernwithvisions to the exclusionof auditoryimageryhas limitedhis perceptionoftheimportanceofmentalimagery.In differentiating
visual-based trance states or altered states of consciousness
fromthose involvingauditoryphenomena,Noll has overlooked the psychophysiological
similaritiesof tranceswithvisual and auditoryexperiences(see Lex 1979, Mandel 1980,
Winkelman 1984b) and the importanceof altered states of
consciousnessin traditionswhich cultivatemental imagery.
He suggeststhat the relationshipbetween mental imagery
and magico-religious
traditionsappears to hold regardlessof
societalcomplexity.
Althoughshamanic-type
visionsare found
in manysocieties,theyare typicalof simplersocieties,while
possessiontrance,whichtendsto involveauditoryperceptions,
is typicalofmorecomplexones (Bourguignon1968).Whysimilar traditions
shouldspecializewithrespectto sensorymodality
is an importantresearchquestion.
Noll contendsthatalteredstatesofconsciousnessare means
forenhancingmentalimagery,notends in themselves.He suggeststhattheadaptivevalue ofmentalimagerylies in improvingmnemonicfunction,providingorganizationin retention
of
mythologicalbeliefsand otherculturallyrelevantmaterials.
Mentalimageryin shamanictraditions
to improve
mayfunction
memory,but it has a moreimportantrole in inducingtrance
statesto facilitatehealingand divination.
Evans-Wentz(1978[1935])points out that yogic traditions
use visualizationto induce alteredstatesof consciousnessand
reversaldiscussed
develop psychicabilities.The field-ground
by Noll as a mentalimagerytechniqueprovidesan internal
focus of attentionwhich is characteristicof altered states,
theirinduction.Noll suggeststhat techniquesfor
facilitating
enhancingmentalimageryactivateunconsciousnonvolitional
levels of psychophysiology
and that shamans are selectedon
thebasis of theirabilityto experiencementalimages.He does
not, however,considerthe physiologicalchangesor the temthat accountforindividualdifferperamentalpredispositions
ences in mentalimagery.Researchsuggeststhatmanyfactors
contributeto visionaryexperiencesthroughinductionof hyslow-wavedischargesfromthe hippocampalpersynchronous
septalareas ofthetemporallobe (see Mandel 1980,Winkelman
1984b).These factorsincludethedeliberateinductionofvisual
imageryand unifyvisual and auditory-basedalteredstatesof
consciousnessin termsofa commonpsychophysiological
basis.
Noll pointsto two inextricably
relatedfunctionsof magicoreligioustraditions,healingand divination,and suggeststhat
mental imageryis used firstdiagnosticallyand secondlyin
healing. Cross-culturalresearch on magico-religiouspractitioners(Winkelman1984b)confirms
the centralroleof healingand divinationin shamanicpracticeand illustratesthatall
practitionersengagingin trainingfor altered states of consciousnessdo healingand divination,suggestinga functional
relationshipbetweenalteredstatesand theseactivities.Since
healing and divination are performedby magico-religious
practitioners
usingalteredstatesofconsciousnessbut notmental imagery,mentalimagerycannotbe central.
Noll correctlysuggeststhatmentalimagerycultivationhas
adaptiveconsequencesas a mnemonicdevice,buthe overlooks
moreimportantaspects. Why would the shaman, whose primaryfunctionsare healingand divination,engage in mental
imagerytrainingto improvememoryskills,whichappearto be
The centralroleofhealingand divination
secondaryfunctions?
functions
suggeststhatmentalimagerytraininghas important
otherthan enhancingthe shaman's memory.Productionof
mentalimageryapparentlyhas functional
effects
in facilitating
healingand divination.Noll pointsout thatenhancedvisual
CU RR E NT
This content downloaded from 194.51.135.196 on Wed, 13 Jan 2016 12:34:00 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
A NT HR OP OL OG Y
Reply
by RICHARD NOLL
Stratford, N.J., U.S.A. 13 v 85
I am gratefulto theinternational
scholgroupofdistinguished
ars who have commentedon my paper. For the mostpart I
findtheircriticisms
stimulating,
as mostare derivedfromconfrommy
ceptual contextswhose assumptionsare different
own. For example, the commentsof Bourguignon,Harner,
Honko, Hultkrantz,and Siikala arise froma primarilyethnologicalcontext,whereasVasquez and Lex and Winkelman
offera materialistic
critique.I am indeed"caughtin a psychologicalthoughtpattern,"as Hultkrantzobserves,and deliberatelyso.
In my analysisof shamanismas a formof mentalimagery
cultivationI have been guildedby one prevailingpassion: to
uncoverand describethe phenomenology
of shamanicexperience. I deliberatelylimitmy discussionto the shamans who
employ"imagery-oriented
visionarytrance,"as Siikala putsit,
in orderto draw attentionto theimportanceofmentalimagery
in the practiceof certainshamanictechniques.I do not deny
theimportancein shamanismof possessiontranceor theother
experiencedalteredstatesof consciousness.Nor do I denythe
betweenpsychoimportanceof the complexinterrelationship
logical,biological,social,and culturalvariablesin shamanism.
I examinein depththe major dimensionof shamanicexperience that is least understoodand perhaps most speculated
about and misinterpreted-visions.Far morescholarlyattention has been givento spiritpossessionand spiritmasteryin
termsofalteredstatesofconsciousness,roleplaying,etc., than
to visionaryexperience.Perhapsthisis because thepsychological literaturehas onlyrelativelyrecentlyprovidedtherichexperimentaldescriptionof mentalimagerythatcan serveas a
languageforexplicatingthatexperience.Whateveroperational
definition
is eventually(ifever)agreedupon, "mentalimagery"
is a usefulconceptforexaminingthephenomenology
ofhuman
experience.
kindcommentscame as a welcomesurprise,as
Achterberg's
her work with Frank Lawlis on the clinical applicationsof
in the
imagery-based
techniquesis amongthemostinteresting
field.I am lookingforwardto readingher new book, which,
fromherdescription,would appear to elaborateon theancient
traditionof healingwithimagerythatreceivesmodestattention in my paper. Bourguignon'scomplimentsare also much
appreciated,as are thoseof Krippner,Preston,and Kiefer.
Georgeis correctin assumingthatthehypothesized
relationship betweenmentalimagerycultivationand magico-medico-
Noll: THE
ROLE OF VISIONS
IN SHAMANISM
religioustraditionsimpliesthat"imaginalexperienceis vitalto
" In thisregardI have beeninfluenced
spiritualexploration.
not
references
to this
onlyby themanyhistoricaland ethnographic
relationship
but also by theworkofHenriCorbin,JamesHillman, and, in particular,C. G. Jung.Jung,himselfa "fantasy" is most appropriatelyread as a pheprone personality,
nomenologist-"imageis psyche"(Jung1967[1929]:50).Jung
feltthattheverypulse oflifewas somehowrepresented
by the
natural,spontaneousprocessesof mentalimagery:"So faras
our presentknowledgeextends,it would seem that the vital
and individualform
urgewhichexpressesitselfin thestructure
of thelivingorganismproducesin theunconscious,or is itself
such a process,whichon becomingpartiallyconsciousdepicts
itselfas a fugue-like
sequenceofimages"(Jung1966[1935]:1
1).
In Jung'sview the vibrantreportsof imageryin shamanic
accounts revealed an underlyingarchetypalstructurethat
confirmedthe "psychologicalinferencethat may be drawn
fromshamanisticsymbolism,namelythatit is a projectionof
theindividuationprocess,"and in thisregardhe compareditto
"philosophical"alchemy(Jung 1967[1945-54]:341).His conceptionof individuationwas one of a naturaltransformative
process nurturedby imagery-basedpsychotherapeutic
techniques (activeimagination)-in myterms,by mentalimagery
cultivation.
Harner's influence on my work is considerable. His
phenomenologicalapproach to shamanism-which I have
in understandadopted-is, I believe,ofparamountimportance
ing the ethnographicliterature.Indeed, his presentationon
shamanismbeforethe SovietAcademyof Sciencesin Moscow
in the summerof 1984 atteststo the growinginternational
interestin his work.That "left-brain
specialists"are poorvisualizerswas firstnoticedby Galtona centuryago in themental
imagerydeficitsof the "men of science":"They had no more
notionof itstruenaturethana colour-blindman, who has not
discernedhis defect,has of the natureof colour. They had a
mentaldeficiency
of whichtheywere unaware,and naturally
enough supposed that those who affirmedtheypossessed it,
were romancing"(Galton 1883:85).
Honko's interesting
proposalthatwhatI describeas mental
imagery"may be further
analysedas counter-roles
of the shaman" deservesfurther
study.In someways thisremindsme of
the largelyintrojected"internalobject world"of the Kleinian
object-relationstheorists.I stronglydisagreewith the statement, however, that "the concept of role . . . organises the
perceptualmaterials"ifHonko is implying
thatperceptualprocesses are essentiallystructured
by learning.This is a mistake
also made by Freud (1960[1923])in suggesting
thattheemerging ego is the organizationof the perceptualworld through
language. The experimentalstudiesof perceptionby the Gestaltpsychologists
thatorganizationis an a
have demonstrated
prioriconditionof consciousexperienceand not an effectof
learning(K6hler1947,Koffka1935). Honko, alongwithother
commentators
(Harner,Vasquez and Lex, Winkelman),criticizes theemphasison thevisual modalityat theexpenseofthe
othermodalities,particularly
audition.This emphasisis deliberate.Visual mentalimageryis byfarthemostcommontypeof
and historimageryexperiencementionedin theethnographic
ical literature,and the mentalimagerycultivationof magicoconcerned
religiouscomplexesseems to be disproportionately
withthevisual. In addition,thevast majorityofexperimental
studiesofmentalimageryis concernedwiththismodality.I do
not denythat auditoryimageryis important(see my n. 5) or
that it or any of the othermodalitiescan occur with visual
withtheethnographic,
imagery,butto be consistent
historical,
and experimental
literatures
I limitmydiscussionto thevisual
modality.
Hultkrantzand Siikalacriticizemyuse oftheword"shaman-
457
ism." Siikalacorrectly
takes"shamanism"in mypaperto mean
essentially"a homogeneousphenomenoncharacterisedby viin thecourseoftrance,"and
sionaryexperiencesand soul flight
Hultkrantzfindsthis"exasperating."I deliberatelyadopt this
(anthropologically)
extremepositionin orderto draw attention
to theprevalenceof mental-imagery-based
techniquesamong
many(notall) magico-religious
practitioners,
manyofwhomfit
thetraditional
"shaman"or "medicineman"labels. That imagery-basedtechniquesare used by ecstaticspecialistsamong
suchwidelydistributedpeoples as the Washo and theLakota
Sioux of North America, the Tukano of Colombia, the
Tamang of Nepal, the classic Siberian groups,the Kalahari
!Kungof Africa,and the AustralianAboriginespointsto the
valid generalizability
oftheconceptofshamanismas a formof
mentalimagerycultivation.It is truethatI sometimesblurthe
boundariesbetween the traditionalNorth Americanvisionquest complexesand classical Siberian shamanism,but this
the universality
blurringis aimed at demonstrating
of mental
imagerycultivationas a culturalphenomenon.In thisrespectI
submita humble mea culpa to my more seasoned scholarly
colleagues.However,althoughit is notclearfrommypaper,I
do not want to conveythe impressionthatI believe the only
visual mentalimageryexerciseconductedby shamansis soul
flight.The literatureis clear, as both Hultkrantzand Siikala
pointout, that a shaman can invoke spiritsand have them
"appear" (withoutpossession)and not embarkon a shamanic
journeyto do so. Spiritscan indeedbe summonedfordidactic
and divinatory
reasonswithouta "soul flight"scenario.If I am
guiltyofany scholarlycrime,it is theadoptionofan approach
thatsacrificessome specificity
forgeneralizability.
Hultkrantzchargesthat I have "omittedto discuss the influenceof drugs on the exerciseof mentalimagery."As my
intention
was to be as phenomenological
as possible,I did not
feel it appropriateto discuss specificneurophysiological
or
biochemicalfactorsin imageryproduction.However,relevant
references
are citedin n. 14.
viewVasquez and Lex representa "neuromythological"
point. For thema psychologicalprocessor experienceis not
"legitimate"
or "real"unlessneurophysiological
or biochemical
hypothesescan be formulatedto accountforit. Such analyses
can be of value. The problemis thatin thisrelativelyyoung
fieldof studydata are oftenprematurely
citedin an explanatorymode (hence,"neuromythologically".)
The richnessofhuman experienceis oftenlostin theseanalyses,and phenomena
may be ignoredfordecades untileven a tenuousmaterialistic
hypothesiscan be posited to explain them. Noam Chomsky
once observedthat the progressof science can be viewed as
simplythe translationof more and more metaphorsof the
mentalintothemetaphorsof thematerial.The problemspresentedby the phenomenaof humanexperiencedo notchange
overtime;what does changeis theculturallyderived,consensuallyacceptablebaselinemetaphoriclanguagethatis used to
describethem.In ourtimeand place causal explanationsbased
on materialistic-particularly
or biochemneurophysiological
ical-metaphors are grantedprimaryvalidity.I chosetheterm
"mentalimagery"forits utilityin phenomenological
analysis.
do I use it in a quasi-causal sense. Finke's
Only peripherally
(1980) use of it is similar-open-endedenoughnot to violate
the phenomenological
oftheconceptand yetcapable
integrity
of reductionto neurophysiological
function.Precisionforits
own sake is meaningless.Whetherthis rendersmy paper's
purpose"opaque" is a judgmentthatI can onlyleave to others.
Winkelmanlikewiseinterprets
my lack of materialisticreductionas a deficiency,
butI see thismoreas a misunderstanding producedby a conflictof perspectives.Winkelmansees
mentalimageryas facilitatingthe inductionof alteredstates
of consciousness,whereasI argue that the inductionof such
statesis a meansofproducingvividmentalimagery.(Krippner
agrees with me.) As evidence Winkelmancites his studies
of magico-religiouspractitionersand the parapsychological
458
ReferencesCited
J., and G. F. LAWLIS. 1984. Imageryof disease: A
diagnostictoolforbehavioralmedicine.Champaign,Ill.: Institute
[SK]
forPersonalityand AbilityTesting.
PsychoAL-ISSA,I. 1977. Social and culturalaspectsofhallucinations.
[LG]
logicalBulletin84:570-87.
ANIsIMov, A. F. 1963. "The shaman's tentof the Evenks and the
originof the shamanisticrite,"in Studies in Siberian shamanism.
Edited by H. N. Michael. Toronto: Universityof TorontoPress.
ARBMAN, E. 1968. Ecstasy or religioustrance. Vol. 2. Stockholm:
[AH]
ScandinavianUniversityBooks.
AVENS, R. 1980. Imaginationis reality: WesternNirvana in Jung,
[LG]
Hillman,Barfield,and Cassirer.Irving,Tex.: Spring.
1978. Studies in Lapp shamanBACKMAN, L., and A. HULTKRANTZ.
[AH]
ism. StockholmStudiesin ComparativeReligion16.
BARBER, T. X. 1984. "Changing'unchangeable'bodilyprocessesby
(hypnotic)suggestions:A new look at hypnosis,cognitions,imagining, and the mind-bodyproblem,"in Imaginationand healing.
Edited by A. A. Sheikh, pp. 7-34. Farmingdale,N.Y.: Bay[LG]
wood.
and
A studyof experimental
F. C. 1932. Remembering:
BARTLETT,
social psychology.London: CambridgeUniversityPress.
R. F. 1922. The vision in Plains culture.AmericanAnBENEDICT,
24:1-23.
thropologist
. 1923. The conceptof the guardianspiritin NorthAmerica.
Association29.
Memoirsof the AmericanAnthropological
B. M. 1978. "Visionand auditionin Biblical prophecy,as
BENNETT,
illuminatedby recentresearchon humanconsciousness,"in Psi and
statesofawareness.Edited by B. Shapin and L. Coly. New York:
Foundation.
Parapsychology
BLOCK, N. J. Editor. 1981. Imagery.Cambridge:M.I.T. Press.
BOGORAS,W. G. 1909. The Chuckchee.Memoirsof the American
Museum of Natural History11.
and patternsof possesE. 1968. "Worlddistribution
BOURGUIGNON,
sion states,"in Trance and possessionstates. Edited by R. Prince.
Montreal:R. M. Bucke MemorialSociety.
forthe comparativestudy
A framework
. 1973. "Introduction:
ofalteredstatesofconsciousness,"in Religion,alteredstatesofconsciousness,and social change. Edited by E. Bourguignon.Columbus: Ohio State UniversityPress.
. 1974. "Cross-culturalperspectiveson the religioususes of alin contempoteredstatesofconsciousness,"in Religiousmovements
raryAmerica.Edited by I. I. Zaretskyand M. P. Leone. Princeton:
PrincetonUniversityPress.
1976. Possession. CorteMadera, Calif.: Chandlerand Sharp.
to human
An introduction
1979. Psychologicalanthropology:
New York: Holt,Rinehartand Winnatureand culturaldifferences.
ston.
ACHTERBERG,
CURRENT
This content downloaded from 194.51.135.196 on Wed, 13 Jan 2016 12:34:00 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
ANTHROPOLOGY
1982.
chokinesis:A pilot study,"in Research in parapsychology
Edited by W. G. Roll and J. Beloff.Metuchen,N.J.: Scarecrow.
BRETT, R. L. 1969. Fancy and imagination.London: Methuen.
[RJP]
BRIERRE DE BOISMONT, A. 1853. Hallucinations: or, The rational
historyof apparitions,visions, dreams, ecstasy,magnetism,and
somnambulism.
Philadelphia:Lindsay and Blakiston.
BUCHLER, J. 1966. Metaphysicsof natural complexes.New York:
[LG]
Columbia UniversityPress.
of the image," in Imagery:
BUGELSKI, B. R. 1971. "The definition
Currentcognitiveapproaches.Edited by S. J. Segal. New York:
AcademicPress.
BUNDY, M. W. 1927. The theoryof imaginationin classical and
medieval thought.Urbana: Universityof Illinois Studies in Language and Literature.
BUTLER, E. M. 1949. Ritual magic. Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress.
CAESARIUS OF HEISTERBACH. 1929. Dialogue on miracles. 2 vols.
Translated and edited by H. von E. Scott and C. C. S. Bland.
London: Routledge.
study.BloomingCASEY, E. S. 1976. Imagining:A phenomenological
ton: Indiana UniversityPress.
Annual
CASSON, R. W. 1983. Schemata in cognitiveanthropology.
12:429-62.
Review ofAnthropology
CHRISTIAN, W. A. 1981. Apparitionsin late medieval and Renaissance Spain. Princeton:PrincetonUniversityPress.
COLE, M., and J. GAY. 1972. Culture and memory.AmericanAn74:1066-84.
thropologist
CORBIN, H. 1960. Avicennaand the visionaryrecital.Translatedby
W. Trask. New York: Pantheon.
in The
. 1966. "The visionarydream in Islamic spirituality,"
dreamand humansocieties. Edited by G. E. Grunebaumand R.
Callois. Berkeley:Universityof CaliforniaPress.
. 1969. Creativeimaginationin theSufismofIbn 'Arabi.Translated by R. Manheim. Princeton:PrincetonUniversityPress.
. 1972. Mundus Imaginalis,or The imaginaryand the imaginal. Spring: An Annual of ArchetypalPsychologyand Jungian
Thought,pp. 1-19.
psyCRONBACH, L. J. 1975. Beyond the two disciplinesof scientific
chology.AmericanPsychologist30:116-34.
in ritualhealing.
oftransformation
CSORDAS, T. J. 1983. The rhetoric
[EB]
Culture,Medicine, and Psychiatry7:333-75.
CZAPLICKA, M. A. 1969 (1914). AboriginalSiberia: A studyin social
Oxford:ClarendonPress.
anthropology.
DAICHES, S. 1913.Babylonianoil magicin theTalmudand in thelater
Jewishliterature.London: Jews'College.
D'ANDRADE, R. G. 1961. "Anthropologicalstudies of dreams," in
Approachesto cultureand personality.
Psychologicalanthropology:
Edited by F. L. K. Hsu. Homewood: DorseyPress.
DENNETT, D. C. 1981. "The nature of images and the introspective trap," in Imagery.Edited by N. Block. Cambridge:M.I.T.
Press.
DEVEREUX, G. 1956. "Normal and abnormal:The key problemof
in Some uses of anthropology:
Theoretpsychiatricanthropology,"
ical and applied. Edited by J. B. Casagrande and T. Gladwin.
Societyof Washington.
Washington,D.C.: Anthropological
. 1961. Shamans as neurotics. American Anthropologist
63:1088-90.
Chicago: University
. 1980. Basic problemsofethnopsychiatry.
of Chicago Press.
DODDS, E. R. 1951. The Greeksand theirrational.Berkeley:Universityof CaliforniaPress.
. 1971. Supernormalphenomenain classical antiquity.ProceedingsoftheSocietyfor PsychicalResearch55:189-237.
DOOB, L. W. 1972. "The ubiquitousappearance of images,"in The
functionand nature of imagery.Edited by P. W. Sheehan. New
York: AcademicPress.
EDELSTEIN, E., and L. EDELSTEIN. 1945. Asclepius. 2 vols. Baltimore:JohnsHopkins UniversityPress.
reof dreamsforanthropological
EGGAN, D. 1949. The significance
51:171-98.
search.AmericanAnthropologist
of
ELIADE, M. 1958. Rites and symbolsof initiation:The mysteries
birthand rebirth.Translatedby W. R. Trask. New York: Harper
Colophon.
. 1964. Shamanism:Archaictechniquesof ecstasy.Translated
by W. R. Trask. Princeton:PrincetonUniversityPress.
ELKIN, A. P. 1977 (1945). 2d edition.Aboriginalmenofhighdegree.
New York: St. Martin'sPress.
in
ELLENBERGER,H. F. 1967. "The evolutionof depthpsychology,"
Historic derivationsof modernpsychiatry.Edited by I. Galdston.
New York: McGraw-Hill.
459
Press.
[AH]
in
KROLL,J.,and B. BACHRACH.1982. Visionsand psychopathology
the Middle Ages. JournalofNervousand Mental Disease 170:4149.
460
CURRENT
This content downloaded from 194.51.135.196 on Wed, 13 Jan 2016 12:34:00 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
ANTHROPOLOGY
Noll: THE
ROLE OF VISIONS
IN SHAMANISM
SHIROKOGOROFF,
S. 1935. PsychomentalcomplexoftheTungus.London: Routledge.
SHOR,RONALDE. 1959. Hypnosisand theconceptof the generalized
reality-orientation.
AmericanJournalofPsychotherapy
13.
[AS]
SIIKALA,A.-L. 1978. The ritetechniqueoftheSiberianshaman.Helsinki:SuomalainenTiedeakatemia.
SILVERMAN,
J. 1967. Shamanismand acute schizophrenia.American
Anthropologist
69:21-31.
SINGER,J. I., and K. S. POPE. Editors. 1978. The power of human
imagination:New methodsin psychotherapy.
New York: Plenum.
SWANSON,G. E. 1973. The searchfora guardianspirit:A processof
in simplersocieties.Ethnology12:359-78.
empowerment
TART,C. T. 1975. States ofconsciousness.New York: Dutton.
THOMASAQUINAS,ST. 1956 (ca. 1258-64). Summa contragentiles.
Book 3. Providence, Pt. 2. Translated by V. J. Bourke. Notre
Dame: Universityof Notre Dame Press.
TOWER,R. B., and J. L. SINGER.1981. "The measurement
of imagery: How can it be clinicallyuseful?"in Assessmentstrategies
for
cognitive-behavioral
interventions.Edited by P. C. Kendall and
S. D. Hollon. New York: Academic Press.
TULVING,E., and W. DONALDSON.Editors.Organizationofmemory.
New York: Academic Press.
TVERSKY,B., and K. HEMENWAY.1984. Objects, parts,and categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 113:16993.
[LG]
TYLOR,E. B. 1958 (1871). Religion in primitiveculture.Pt. 2. New
York: Harper and Row.
VASILEVICH,G. M. 1963. "Early conceptsabout the universeamong
the Evenks," in Studies in Siberian shamanism.Edited by H. N.
Michael. Toronto:Universityof TorontoPress.
WAGNER,M. B. 1983. Metaphysicsin MidwesternAmerica.Columbus: Ohio State UniversityPress.
[EB]
WATKINS,M. M. 1976. Waking dreams. New York: Harper Colophon.
WEIL, A. T. 1972. The naturalmind. Boston: HoughtonMifflin.
WHITE, K. D., P. W. SHEEHAN,and R. ASHTON. 1977. Imagery
assessment:A surveyof self-report
measures.Journal of Mental
Imagery1:145-70.
WIERZBICKA,
A. 1984. Apples are not a "kind of fruit":The semantics of human categorization.American Ethnologist 11:313-28.
[LG]
WILSON, S. C., and T. X. BARBER.1981. "Vivid fantasyand hallucinatoryabilitiesin thelifehistoriesofexcellenthypnoticsubjects
(',sonambules'):Preliminaryreportwith femalesubjects,"in Imagery:Concepts,results,and applications.Edited by E. Klinger.New
York: PlenumPress.
. 1982. "The fantasy-prone
personality:
Implicationsforunderstandingimagery,hypnosis,and parapsychological
phenomena,"in
Imagery:Currenttheory,research,and applications.Edited by A.
Sheikh.New York: Wiley.
WINKELMAN,M. 1982. Magic: A theoreticalreassessment.CURRENT
ANTHROPOLOGY23:37-66.
SPrialms
1985
Vol.26 * No. 4 * August-October
This content downloaded from 194.51.135.196 on Wed, 13 Jan 2016 12:34:00 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
461