Prevalence and Predictors of Internet Bullying: Kirk R. Williams, Ph.D. and Nancy G. Guerra, Ed.D
Prevalence and Predictors of Internet Bullying: Kirk R. Williams, Ph.D. and Nancy G. Guerra, Ed.D
Original article
a
Presley Center for Crime and Justice Studies, University of California at Riverside, Riverside, California
Southern California Center of Excellence on Youth Violence Prevention, University of California at Riverside, Riverside, California
Manuscript received June 19, 2007; manuscript accepted August 28, 2007
Abstract
Purpose: With the Internet quickly becoming a new arena for social interaction, it has also become
a growing venue for bullying among youth. The purpose of the present study was to contrast the
prevalence of Internet bullying with physical and verbal bullying among elementary, middle, and
high school boys and girls, and to examine whether key predictors of physical and verbal bullying
also predicted Internet bullying.
Methods: As part of an ongoing, statewide bullying prevention initiative in Colorado, 3,339 youth
in Grades 5, 8, and 11 completed questionnaires in 78 school sites during the fall of 2005, and
another 2,293 youth in that original sample participated in a follow-up survey in 65 school sites in
the spring of 2006. Questionnaires included measures of bullying perpetration and victimization,
normative beliefs about bullying, perceptions of peer social support, and perceptions of school
climate.
Results: The highest prevalence rates were found for verbal, followed by physical, and then by
Internet bullying. Physical and Internet bullying peaked in middle school and declined in high
school. Verbal bullying peaked in middle school and remained relatively high during high school.
Males were more likely to report physical bullying than females, but no gender differences were
found for Internet and verbal bullying. All three types of bullying were significantly related to
normative beliefs approving of bullying, negative school climate, and negative peer support.
Conclusions: Preventive interventions that target school bullying by changing norms about bullying and school context may also impact Internet bullying, given the shared predictors. 2007
Society for Adolescent Medicine. All rights reserved.
Keywords:
weaker victim within the context of an ongoing social interaction [5]. As such, research examining the prevalence,
predictors, and prevention of bullying largely has examined
this behavior as it unfolds within a specific social context.
For children, schools have been the primary context for
studying bullying behavior [2,6 9].
Over the last decade, interest in understanding and preventing bullying among school children in the U.S. and
internationally has surged [6,10]. Such interest coincides
with a growing awareness of the detrimental consequences
of being bullied on childrens well-being as well as the
recognition that bullying is a significant problem in schools
[1]. Still, prevalence rates vary as a function of how bullying is measured, what type of bullying (e.g., physical vs.
verbal) is assessed, the age of respondents, and the country
1054-139X/07/$ see front matter 2007 Society for Adolescent Medicine. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.08.018
K.R. Williams and N.G. Guerra / Journal of Adolescent Health 41 (2007) S14 S21
S15
S16
K.R. Williams and N.G. Guerra / Journal of Adolescent Health 41 (2007) S14 S21
Methods
Data for the present research were collected as part of a
larger study evaluating a statewide initiative in Colorado to
strengthen the skills and willingness of youth and adults to
intervene in bullying situations. The Bullying Prevention
Initiative (BPI) is a 3-year, $8.6-million initiative funded by
The Colorado Trust, a private grant-making foundation in
Denver, Colorado. The grantees funded by this initiative
represent school districts, individual schools, or communitybased organizations, evenly split between rural and urban
areas of the state and responsible for implementing bullying
prevention programming in 78 schools across 40 of Colorados 64 counties.
The larger BPI evaluation will provide an empiricallybased understanding of bullying and bystander behavior
among youth, including an increased awareness of this
behavior, social cognitive processes involved in the prevention of bullying, the social context surrounding bullying
incidents, and the involvement of adults and youth in preventing such incidents. These issues are being addressed by
collecting survey data from youth and adults in schools,
collecting data from grantees concerning program implementation, and conducting a supplemental qualitative study
seeking to acquire in-depth information from adults about
challenges and successes in program implementation and
from youth in terms of their awareness of the bullying
prevention programs and their perceived effectiveness. A
prepost survey design collects data from youth in the fall
and the spring of 3 academic years (20052006, 2006
2007, and 20072008) within the 78 schools. This design
allows the assessment of single year changes in individual
youth and contextual (school level) changes over the full 3
years of the BPI. All instruments developed to collect data
from youth were piloted in the summer of 2005 before full
implementation in the fall of that year, with all indices having
acceptable reliabilities (i.e., alpha coefficients .70).
Participants in the present study
The first year of the BPI was a start-up period for both
the prevention programming as well as the evaluation study
in refining its data collection instruments and procedures.
During this year, 3,339 youth completed questionnaires in
the 78 school sites during the fall of 2005, and another 2,293
youth in that original sample participated in a follow-up
survey in 65 school sites in the spring of 2006. Data were
collected in 5th, 8th, and 11th grades, representing transition
years in elementary, middle, and high schools. All data
collection was conducted in compliance with the protocol
approved by the human subjects review board, including
acquiring informed parental consent and youth ascent. To
ensure the quality of data and that school samples are
representative, a subsample of these first-year participants
was selected for the present analysis based on two criteria:
(1) schools must have successfully completed both fall and
Table 1
Demographic characteristics of the total and subsample
Demographic characteristic
Subsample
Total sample
Rural
Urban
5th Grade
8th Grade
11th Grade
Male
Female
White, Non-Latino
Latino
African-American
Asian-American
Native-American
Reduced-cost or free lunch
61.0%
39.0%
43.5%
48.2%
08.3%
45.0%
55.0%
62.0%
26.9%
06.4%
03.5%
01.2%
49.0%
46.0%
54.0%
31.3%
49.7%
19.0%
46.0%
54.0%
60.5%
28.1%
05.9%
04.0%
01.5%
53.1%
K.R. Williams and N.G. Guerra / Journal of Adolescent Health 41 (2007) S14 S21
S17
proximately equal distributions. High scorers indicate participants who approve bullying perpetration and negative
bystander behavior in general, not behavior-specific approval. Participants scoring low on this index disapprove of
bullying and negative bystander behavior in general. This
measure is appropriate given the emphasis on capturing the
normative orientation of students about bullying overall, not
behavior-specific moral beliefs.
School climate. Student perceptions of school climate were
assessed using the California School Climate Scale [23].
This measure contains nine items about teachers, school
staff and administrators, school policy, and a students perceived personal connection to the school. For example,
participants were asked whether they disagree or agree with
statements like My teachers respect me, My teachers are
fair, or Teachers at my school are nice people. Other
items addressed whether the principal in their school listens
to the ideas of students, whether students who break school
rules are treated fairly, and whether teachers and staff are
doing the right things to prevent bullying in general, not
specific forms of bullying in the school. The nine items were
summed to form an additive index (alpha coefficient .84),
with scores ranging from 9 to 36, given the response categories of the individual items. Respondents with high scores
perceived a positive school climate, whereas those with low
scores perceived a more negative school climate.
Perceived peer support. This four-item scale focused on
positive and negative qualities of peers as a source of social
support. It was adapted from the Generalized Perception of
Peers Scale [24]. Regardless of social context (e.g.,
schools), participants were asked to assess whether students their age care about what happens to them, will help
them in time of need, can be trusted, and are sensitive to
their feelings. Response options range from no, not at all
to yes, completely. The four items were summed to form
an additive index, with scores ranging from a low of 4 to a
high of 16, given the response categories for each of the
individual items. High scores indicated higher perceptions
of peers as supportive, whereas low scores indicated the
opposite view. The alpha coefficient for this scale was .79.
Results
The results presented below are arranged according to
the two primary research objectives of this analysis: (1) to
determine the prevalence of Internet compared with verbal
and physical bullying perpetration in this sample of youth,
and (2) to determine whether predictors of Internet bullying
perpetration are similar to predictors of verbal and physical
bullying perpetration. The first objective is addressed simply by tabulating the distributions of the three forms of
bullying and examining whether these distributions vary by
gender and grade level (5th, 8th, and 11th grades). The
empirical examination by gender and grade was done by
S18
K.R. Williams and N.G. Guerra / Journal of Adolescent Health 41 (2007) S14 S21
estimating their effects on each form of bullying perpetration through logistic regression, given the dichotomous bullying measures used.
The second objective is addressed by estimating the
bivariate relations between each of the three predictors and
the three forms of bullying perpetration. As noted above,
these behavioral measures are dichotomized, differentiating
between participants who report never perpetrating such
behavior and those reporting they did so one or more times
during the school year. Hence, the empirical relations were
estimated using logistic regression, similar to the estimation
of gender and grade level effects. The results of these
analyses are presented in the text and in line graphs, where
the predicted probabilities of each bivariate equation are
plotted against the predictor variable respective to each
equation [25]. Statistical tests for the equality of estimated
coefficients across equations are also conducted. Such tests
determine whether the estimated effects for Internet bullying
perpetration are significantly different from those of physical
and verbal bullying perpetration (i.e., are such effects similar or
different for the three forms of bullying). The following formula was applied for these statistical tests [26]:
Z
b1 b2
SEb12 SEb22
Prevalence of bullying
Table 1 shows the distribution of Internet, physical, and
verbal bullying perpetration for the total sample of youth.
Verbal bullying is clearly most prevalent for the total sample, followed by physical bullying and then bullying via
e-mail or instant messaging. In short, Internet bullying was
a part of the behavioral repertoire of only a minority of
youth in this sample during the last school year, but its
prevalence is nontrivial. These three types of bullying perpetration are clearly interrelated, with ordinal associations
(gamma coefficients) ranging from .66 for the relation between Internet and physical bullying to .87 for the relation
between Internet and verbal bullying. However, distinctions
remain, as suggested by 24.8% of the sample refraining
from any type of bullying, 37.9% engaging in only one type,
30.7% perpetrating two types, and only 6.6% self-reporting
involvement in all three types.
Logistic regression was used to estimate the effects of
gender and grade on each of the three forms of bullying
perpetration. No gender differences were found for Internet
and verbal bullying, although such differences were pronounced for physical bullying, with males being more than
twice as likely as females to report perpetrating such behavior (b .79, odds ratio [OR] 2.21, p .00). Grade
was significantly related to all forms of bullying perpetration, with the estimated effect being literally identical between Internet and verbal bullying (b .20, OR 1.22,
Table 2
Percentage of youth self-reporting verbal, physical, and Internet bullying
perpetration, by grade
Type of
bullying
5th Grade
8th Grade
11th Grade
Total sample
Verbal
Physical
Internet
32.6%
34.8%
04.5%
78.5%
44.6%
12.9%
72.3%
37.8%
09.9%
70.7%
40.3%
09.4%
K.R. Williams and N.G. Guerra / Journal of Adolescent Health 41 (2007) S14 S21
S19
Figure 1. Empirical relations between moral beliefs and the predicted probability of verbal, physical, and Internet bullying perpetration.
Figure 2. Empirical relations between school climate and the predicted probability of verbal, physical, and Internet bullying perpetration.
S20
K.R. Williams and N.G. Guerra / Journal of Adolescent Health 41 (2007) S14 S21
Figure 3. Empirical relations between perceived peer support and the predicted probability of verbal, physical, and Internet bullying perpetration.
reported participation in verbal, physical, and Internet bullying. Again, these empirical relations were anticipated. A
single unit increase on the four- to 16-point index of perceived peer support (see description of measure above) is
associated with a 7% decline in the odds of physical bullying (b .08, OR .93, p .00), with the estimated
effects being greatest for Internet (b .14, OR .87, p
.00) and verbal (b .15, OR .86, p .00) bullying.
Similar to the empirical relations between perceived school
climate and these three forms of bullying perpetration, the
estimated effects for Internet bullying are not significantly
different from those of verbal or physical bullying.
Summary and conclusion
The findings on the prevalence of bullying perpetration
suggest that distributions vary by type, with verbal being
most prevalent, followed by physical and then by Internet
bullying. Physical and Internet bullying peaked in 8th grade
and declined in 11th grade, whereas verbal bullying peaked
in 8th grade and remained relatively high in the 11th grade.
Males were more likely than females to report physical
bullying perpetration. Consistent with the expectation that
Internet bullying and verbal bullying would share common
features, no gender differences were found for prevalence of
Internet and verbal bullying.
Three predictors of bullying were empirically examined.
One reflects an individuals normative orientation about the
moral acceptability of bullyingthat is, whether such behavior is right or wrong. However, the other two predictors
K.R. Williams and N.G. Guerra / Journal of Adolescent Health 41 (2007) S14 S21
S21
[10] Espelage DL, Swearer S, eds. Bullying in American schools. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2004.
[11] Craig WM, Pepler DJ. Observations of bullying and victimization in
the schoolyard. Can J School Psychol. 1997;13:41 60.
[12] Demaray MK, Malecki CK. Perceptions of the frequency and importance of social support by students classified as victims, bullies, and
bully/victims in an urban middle school. School Psychol Rev 2003;
23:47190.
[13] Willard NE. Cyberbullying and cyberthreats. Eugene, OR: Center for
Safe and Responsible Internet Use; 2006.
[14] Kowalski R. et al. 2005. Electronic bullying among school-aged
children and youth. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
American Psychological Association. Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
[15] Huesmann LR, Guerra NG. Social norms and childrens aggressive
behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol 1997;72:408 19.
[16] Bentley KM, Li AKF. Bully and victim problems in elementary
school and students beliefs about aggression. Can J Sch Psychol
1995;22:153 65.
[17] Espelage DL, Swearer S. Research on bullying and victimization:
What have we learned and where do we go from here? Sch Psychol
Rev 2003;23:365 85.
[18] Kasen S, Berenson K, Cohen P, Johnson JG. The effects of school
climate on changes in aggressive and other behaviors related to
bullying. In: Espelage DL, Swearer S, eds. Bullying in American
Schools. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2004:187210.
[19] Craig WM, Pepler DJ, Atlas R. Observations of bullying in the
playground and in the classroom. Sch Psychol Int 2000;2:2236.
[20] Brand S, Felner R, Shim M, et al. Middle school improvement and
reform: Development and validation of a school-level assessment of
climate, cultural pluralism, and school safety. J Educ Psychol 2003;
95:570 8.
[21] Salmivalli C, Huttunen A, Lagerspetz KM J. Peer networks and
bullying in schools. Scand J Psychol 1997;38:30512.
[22] Espelage DL, Holt M, Henkel RR. Examination of peer group contextual effects on aggression during early adolescence. Child Dev
2003;74:20520.
[23] Furlong MJ, Bates MP, Smith DC, Kingery P, eds. Appraisal and
Prediction of school violence: Methods, Issues and Contents. Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science; 2004.
[24] Salmivalli C, Ojanen T, Haanpaa J, Peets K. Im OK but Youre
Not and other peer relational schemas: Explaining individual differences in childrens social goals. Dev Psychol 2005;41:36375.
[25] Hamilton LC. Regression with Graphics. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/
Cole Publishing; 1992.
[26] Paternoster R, Brame R, Mazerolle P, Piquero A. Using the correct
statistical test for the equality of regression coefficients. Criminology
1998;36:859 66.
[27] Raskauskas J, Stolz AD. Involvement in traditional and electronic
bullying among adolescents. Dev Psychol 2007;43:564 75.