Basics of Genetics (Betsey Dexter Dyer)

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 105

THE BASICS

OF GENETICS
COURSE GUIDE

Professor Betsey Dexter Dyer


WHEATON COLLEGE
The Basics of Genetics
Professor Betsey Dexter Dyer
Wheaton College

Recorded Books is a trademark of


Recorded Books, LLC. All rights reserved.
The Basics of Genetics
Professor Betsey Dexter Dyer


Executive Editor
Donna F. Carnahan

RECORDING
Producer - David Markowitz
Director - Matthew Cavnar

COURSE GUIDE
Editor - James Gallagher
Design - Robin White

Lecture content 2009 by Betsey Dexter Dyer


Course guide 2009 by Recorded Books, LLC

72009 by Recorded Books, LLC


Cover image: Scott Bowlin/shutterstock.com
#UT132 ISBN: 978-1-4361-6994-3
All beliefs and opinions expressed in this audio/video program and accompanying course guide
are those of the author and not of Recorded Books, LLC, or its employees.
Course Syllabus

The Basics of Genetics

About Your Professor/Introduction ...............................................................................4

Lecture 1 The Long History of Practical Genetics .................................................6

Lecture 2 What Did Gregor Mendel Do? .............................................................11

Lecture 3 What Did Mendel Do Next? .................................................................21

Lecture 4 A Cookie Factory in a Black Box Designed by Rube Goldberg ..........31

Lecture 5 How Mammals Get Their Colors..........................................................37

Lecture 6 The Importance of Environment: In the Short Term ............................44

Lecture 7 Environment Over the Long Term: The Genetics


of Populations ......................................................................................50

Lecture 8 DNA (Deoxyribonucleic Acid)...............................................................57

Lecture 9 Transcription and Translation: The Metaphor of an Error-Prone


Medieval Monk in a Scriptorium...........................................................61

Lecture 10 Chromosomes, Mitosis, and Meiosis: How DNA Is


Packaged for Moving ...........................................................................66

Lecture 11 Mutations..............................................................................................74

Lecture 12 Regulating the Genes and the Dramatic Consequences


of Regulatory Mutations .......................................................................81

Lecture 13 Viruses, Jumping Genes, and Symbionts: Whose DNA Is It


and What Are the New Philosophical Questions? ..............................86

Lecture 14 Conclusions..........................................................................................93

Glossary......................................................................................................................95

Isolating DNA............................................................................................................102

Course Materials.......................................................................................................103

3
Photo courtesy of Betsey Dexter Dyer

About Your Professor


Betsey Dexter Dyer
Betsey Dexter Dyer is a biology professor at Wheaton College in Norton,
Massachusetts, where her courses include bacteriology, genetics, parasitol-
ogy, and invertebrate evolution. She earned her Ph.D. in biology at Boston
University in 1984. Dyers research interests include DNA sequence analysis,
cell evolution, symbiosis, and field microbiology. Dyer considers herself to be
a curious naturalist and a generalist, with lots more to learn. She has written
three books: Perl for Exploring DNA (with coauthor Mark LeBlanc, Oxford
University Press, 2007), A Field Guide to Bacteria (Cornell University Press,
2003), and Tracing the History of Eukaryotic Cells (with coauthor Robert
Obar, Columbia University Press, 1994).
Dyer grew up on a family farm in Rehoboth, Massachusetts, which was a
great influence on her development as a biologist and naturalist. She had
many opportunities to be puzzled about why the offspring of animals (such
as a new litter of kittens in the barn) might look so different from their parents.
She lives in Walpole, Massachusetts, with husband Robert Obar, a protein
chemist, two children Alice and Sam, and a Brittany, Genevieve. She loves
reading, writing, cooking, and dancing.

Introduction
Genetics is the study of the hereditary information of organisms, how it
is used, and how it is transferred through generations. Genetics has deep,
practical roots in animal and plant husbandry and has its scientific beginnings
in natural questions (such as the following) asked over and over by our
human ancestors:
Why do the offspring of those particular parents look and act the way they do?
Recently, genetics has become quite sophisticated and seemingly far afield
of such basic questions. Now that scientists seem to know so much about
DNA (the hereditary information) and are able to manipulate DNA in so
many astounding ways, the nature of the questions seems to have changed.
We are more and more called upon to tackle such enormous philosophical
conundrums as the following:

4
Should I be carrying an identification card containing all of the sequences
of my genes?
Is it okay to clone a beloved, deceased pet? A human? An organ of a
human with the plan to transplant it? And what IS cloning anyway?
Indeed, to be a well-educated, conversant human is to be able to pull togeth-
er some sort of coherent response concerning the latest genetic develop-
ments. However, I would argue that there is still a great deal of mystery in the
fundamental question:
Why do the offspring of those particular parents look and act the way they do?
The goal of this course is to create a foundation in the practical, everyday
genetics that fascinated our ancestors and which is still fascinating. The plan
is to not skip quickly over it but to linger on examples that will leave you feel-
ing more empowered to think about and talk about what genes do. Many
examples will be drawn from easily visualized traits of domestic animals and
plants. As a curious naturalist myself, that sort of information alone gives me
a great deal of satisfactionhaving that insight about my everyday world.
Indeed, it was exactly the sort of information that was missing from my life as
a young naturalist growing up on a farm. I had access to the results of all
sorts of matings: new kittens, new calves, new chicks, new guinea pigs, but
almost no understanding of any mechanisms of genetics and no particular
confidence in learning it from the few complicated-looking diagrams I could
find on the topic.
How about the big, important philosophical problems for which we are asked
more and more often to have at the ready some sort of well-informed
response? I think those follow naturally (as they have in the history of genet-
ics) right after the foundations are set out. First I will tell you all about kittens
and puppies and other familiar animals (including zebras, birds, and goldfish)
and all about vegetables and flowers, such that the grocery or florist shop
could be your laboratory and seed catalogues could be a sort of textbook. I
do not intend to pass too quickly over any of those basics. Throughout those
lectures I will interweave terminology, concepts, and topics from current prob-
lems in genetics. For example, in the lectures about Gregor Mendel, I will
include some information on cloning.
In this course we also will make our way toward an understanding of the
amazing properties of DNA sequences themselves and how those properties
lend themselves to such things as genetic engineering or genetic modifica-
tion, viruses, genetic diseases and conditions (cancers, birth defects), genet-
ic testing, and therapies. All that? you might be wondering. Well, the foun-
dations for all that. Those topics may appear to be springing up as complete
entities unto themselves, each with its own thicket of questions, but all have
deep roots in practical genetics and all are themes of a grander overall
scheme of DNA structure and function.

5
Lecture 1:
The Long History of Practical Genetics

The Suggested Reading for this lecture is Juliet Clutton-Brocks


A Natural History of Domesticated Mammals.

This course focuses on the basics of genetics:


The rules and patterns by which genes work
The many fascinating exceptions to those rules and patterns
Genes: what they are, how they function
DNA (deoxyribonucleic acid): what it is, how it functions
A goal for this course is to give you a firmer foundation in those basics of
genetics so that you can be that much more conversant and thoughtful about
some major current topics in genetics. Another goal is to make genetics
accessible to you, no matter what your background in science; indeed, you will
be encouraged throughout to explore and investigate at home or in gardens or
pet shops or other venues, where genetic variations are readily observed.
How accessible is genetics? It is so accessible and so practical that our
human ancestors for thousands of years have been cleverly manipulating and
evaluating genetic crosses without any particular scientific training in the
modern (seventeenth-century) sense of science. Genetics is a foundation of
agriculture from its deepest roots ten thousand years ago and without the
practice of genetics, agriculture would not have evolved to become such an
efficient and abundant supplier of animals and plants for our use. The transi-
tion from gathering and hunting food to cultivating and breeding plants and
animals must have been accompanied by a close scrutiny and manipulation
of the reproduction of those plants and animals. Arranging matings, monitor-
ing the results (the offspring), and adjusting future matings were the obvious
activities of the most successful early farmers. Thus wild species of animals
and plants were brought into domestication, usually becoming more
enhanced in ways useful to humans. The evidence of these ancient endeav-
ors in genetics is all around us.
Wild apples (crab apples) are tiny,
hard, and sour. Domesticated apples
(the products of agricultural practices)
are enormous, tender, and sweet. Wild
wheat has fewer and smaller grains than
domesticated wheat. The original, wild
wheat, a version of which still grows
wild, is hypothesized to have had just
one grain per spiklet and therefore just a
2008 G. Volker
LECTURE ONE

few grains per spike, thus the name


einkorn, meaning one seed. Further-
more, wild wheat is more difficult to A male mouflon, the species that is thought
harvest because the grains are more to be one of the ancestors for all modern
domestic sheep breeds.
fragile and more likely to drop off before
6
harvesting. Domestic wheat has been bred for
efficient, abundant processing, whether by
ancient methods such as thrashing and win-
nowing or by their mechanical counterparts.
Domestic egg-laying, milk-producing, wool-pro-
ducing, and meat-producing animals of all sorts
are doing so on a far more extravagant scale

Photos.com
than any of their wild counterparts.
However, domesticated dogs provide some of
the very best evidence of the skill of our ances- Canis lupus, North American Grey Wolf
tors worldwide in manipulating genetics. Humans
paid close attention to the behaviors of wolves
and chose the tamest and most cooperative of
wolf puppies to produce what would become the
many lineages of dogs. Dogs have been bred for

Eric Issele/shutterstock.com
a great variety of work (guarding, hunting) but
also for aesthetics perhaps more so than any
other domestic animal. Get a large picture book
of dogs (or go to a dog show) and begin by just
looking at place names (from all over the world)
associated with particular breeds, indicative of A harlequin-coated Great Dane
widespread and enormous success in getting
dogs to look and act pretty much any way that
the human breeders could imagine. Thus we

Nataliya Kuznetsova/shutterstock.com
have from an ancestral wolf-like wild canine, the
most extravagant ranges of sizes (Chihuahua to
Great Dane), colors (brindle, piebald, golden,
chocolate), and shapes (bull dogs, Pekinese, cor-
gis, greyhounds), not to mention diverse disposi-
tions and other behavioral tendencies such as
pointing and fetching. If youd like to see more
evidence of extravagant genetics, get picture A long-haired Chihuahua
books from the library of breeds of fancy gold-
fish, fancy pigeons, fancy mice, poultry, sheep, or almost any domestic animal
that has been bred for thousands of years by imaginative humans. The word
fancy usually implies that the breeding has gone far beyond mere practicali-
ties and into some of the more individual tastes in aesthetics.
Next take a field trip to the grocery store and be amazed at the varieties of
plants all in the genus Brassica (also called mustards or crucifers) and
which abound in the produce section. They all derive from small, spindly,
seemingly inedible wild mustards and include the following:
Broccoli Broccoli rabe
Brussels sprouts Cabbage
Canola (or rape) Cauliflower
Chinese cabbage Collard
Kale Mustard (as in mustard seed)
Swiss chard Turnip

7
Broccoli Brussels Sprouts Cabbage

Broccoli rabe Cauliflower


Brassica rapa,
wild mustard

Turnips Chinese cabbage Kale All images: Photos.com

Note that many of these are closely associated with regional cuisines and
are indicative of preferences for one or another plant part. For example, flow-
ers (as in cauliflower) were selected, or unopened flower buds (as in broc-
coli), or oil (as in canola), or roots (as in turnips). You probably have some
version of wild mustard growing in your area for comparison. Get a field guide
to wild flowers and look up wild mustard or wild radish and find some.
So how much of the underlying genetics did our amazing ancestors
understand? They understood almost none of it! They were persistent and
observant and made some good guesses. Most of their mistakes no longer
exist. Practical genetics is all about making choices: allowing this organism to
breed, but not that organism; allowing this organism to live to reproductive
age, but not that organism; saving the seeds of this organism, but not that
one and so on through many generations.
Our ancestors also must also have pondered many surprise outcomes rang-
ing from the mild (such as the unexpected colors in a litter of new kittens) to
the horrifying or disturbing (such as birth defects of all kinds that used to be
classified as monstrous). Furthermore, our ancestors became clever at ani-
mal and plant husbandry, monitoring reproductive cycles, carefully taking sea-
sons into account, and experimenting with means of propagation such as the
grafting of trees and vines. The practices by which the domestication and
LECTURE ONE

specific enhancements of animals and plants occurred were mostly about


common sense. No one had any knowledge of genetics per se, that is, the
study of how genes (the hereditary information of organisms) are transferred
and how they function.

8
Now and then superstitions must have arisen due to the unpredictable
nature of some heritable traits. Humans being humans, there were attempts
(in a pre-science world) to rationalize and find causes, and thus superstitions
and religious beliefs were often evoked as explanations. For example, there
was a notion that a baby might have a short life if the pregnant mother cut
her hair. (Many babies of our ancestors had short lives and many pregnant
women cut their hair. Such correlations can be difficult to sort out.) However,
superstition must have played a relatively small part overall. The rudiments of
scientific logic seem to have prevailed. The evidence is in the many success-
ful results with domesticated plants and animals that could only have come
by repeated and thoughtful experimentation and not by wild guesses or magi-
cal spells. The most effective (most logical) breeders of animals and plants
most likely were also the most successful. For important lineages such as
racehorses and special dogs, humans developed ways to keep records and
thereby to establish pedigrees.
By the way, there still are plenty of surprises in genetics, even among knowl-
edgeable breeders. For example, striving to produce a faster and faster lin-
eage of race horses might inadvertently produce some with overly long, brit-
tle, breakable bones or some other skeletal or muscular defect.
Humans are also prodigious manipulators of their own reproduction and thus
the world over there are distinctive regional differences in human populations
based on hundreds of generations of some lineages being able to reproduce
more often and abundantly than others. Medical doctors are particularly inter-
ested in regional differences among humans because along with the distinc-
tive positive traits of a location and lineage such as skin color, height, and
shape of face there may be surprise negative traits such as a tendency to a
particular eye problem or blood disease.
Our human ancestors considered this important enough that pedigrees of
humans or genealogies are one of the most ancient forms of record keeping,
whether by oral or written methods, and are the foundation of history. Our
ancestors developed and used elaborate vocabularies to indicate relatedness
as in second cousin, great-great-aunt, and so on. There was even an
aspect of manipulation of genetics through arranged marriages not only for
religious, economic, or other cultural reasons, but also from the perception
that certain traits such as royalty or strength or wisdom might be passed
down through generations. It was difficult then as it still is now to sort out
accidental effects of good fortune versus the effects of hard work or good
teachers versus the effects of heritable traits passed down from a parents
genes. The analysis still goes under the rubric of nurture versus nature,
although a simple dichotomy like that does not do justice to the problem.
After thousands of years of good work in genetics, a few humans began to
understand some of the science behind it in the last part of the nineteenth
century. Then it was a major project of the twentieth century to establish the
fundamental principles and mechanisms with a major turning point being the
discovery of the structure of DNA in 1953. Genetics is still a work in progress,
with new and amazing things being discovered every year.

9

FOR GREATER UNDERSTANDING

Questions and Activities

1. How does domesticated wheat differ from wild wheat?


2. What is the evidence of the enormous success of our human ancestors in
practical genetics?
3. Develop a repertoire of vivid examples from among your own favorite
domesticated animals and plants.

Suggested Reading

Clutton-Brock, Juliet. A Natural History of Domesticated Mammals. 2nd ed.


Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1999.

Other Books of Interest

Brown, Jack, and Peter Caligari. An Introduction to Plant Breeding. Oxford:


Wiley-Blackwell, 2008.
LECTURE ONE

10
Lecture 2:
What Did Gregor Mendel Do?

The Suggested Reading for this lecture is James Schwartzs In Pursuit


of the Gene: From Darwin to DNA.

Before Mendel
Although the history of genetics typically
begins with the investigations of Gregor
Mendel in the 1850s, the actual practical
beginnings of genetics already were deeply
rooted worldwide. Humans had been ask-
ing questions about genetics for many thou-
sands of years, probably ever since they
were able to put into words puzzles like
Why does this child from these parents
seem to have some traits or mixture of
traits in common with the parents and yet

Public Domain
some traits that seem different from either
parent? By the time Mendel was doing his
experiments on peas in the 1850s, many
humans worldwide were experts many Gregor Johann Mendel
(18221884)
times over in the practices of genetics.
They were simply missing the mechanisms.
They knew the practicalities of how but they didnt know why.
So what did Mendel do that was such a turning point in genetics? He decid-
ed to look at one simple trait at a time for an organism that could be made to
reproduce quickly and abundantly, the pea, Pisum sativum. It might seem
obvious now, but it was not obvious at the time. Practical genetics in the
1850s had advanced to the point of being an essential part of any compre-
hensive breeding program by which any animal or plant might be improved.
Racehorses are a good example from that time period. Their reproduction
was carefully controlled and followed closely and analyzed with detailed pedi-
gree charts. Horse breeders wanted as much as possible from their expen-
sive, potentially lucrative, and relatively slow-reproducing animals. They want-
ed speed, stamina, strength, intelligence, and determination. They wanted
winners. If that wasnt enough, they wanted beauty: lovely coat colors, flowing
tails and manes, nicely shaped heads and bodies. Thats why none of it was
ever successfully sorted out, so that the hereditary mechanisms could be
understood. Genetics seemed to be something like mixing various colors and
amounts of paints from a paint box and then trying to understand how the
shades and gradations of resulting colors might have come about. Indeed,
paint mixing was a popular metaphor for what was going on. The traits of the
stallion and mare were somehow mixing in the colt, but exactly how was not
understood. Furthermore there must have been occasional inexplicable sur-
prises as there still are today in animal and plant breeding (especially when

11
breeders are overeager to possess all valuable traits at once). These surpris-
es include amazingly fast and winning horses from a couple of improbably
slow parents and (at the other end of good fortune) unpredictable, catastroph-
ic fractures of delicate leg bones in horses of noble racing lineages.
In contrast to the elaborate, high-stakes schemes of horse breeders,
Mendels endeavors were quite modest and that seems to have been key to
his success. Not only did Mendel look at only one trait of his peas at a time,
but he chose trivial traits of relatively minor interest to pea growers. He did not
look at flavor or aroma or shelf life in storage or any other trait that might be
valued in a food plant. Instead, he chose traits that were easily visualized, eas-
ily counted and recorded. These included flower color (purple or white), seed
color (yellow or green), plant height (tall or short), and seed shape (wrinkled or
smooth). These particular varieties of peas (different flower colors, seed col-
ors, and so forth) already existed and were available from seed companies
and were the results of many generations of pea breeding, just as different col-
ors of horses or dogs were the results of generations of breeding. Mendel set
out to understand the basic mechanisms by which plant and animal varieties
occurred and interacted using those simple pea traits, one at a time.
True-Breeding Peas
Mendels simplest mating (or cross) between two pea plants demonstrated
true breeding. This is also one of the crosses of great interest to any breed-
er of animals or plants who has found a set of desirable traits in an organism
and would like those traits to be passed down as completely as possible to all
subsequent generations. Mendel simply took each of his varieties and
crossed like traits with like: purple flower with purple flower, yellow seeded
with yellow seeded, and found perhaps to no great surprise that purple-flow-
ered parents yielded purple offspring and yellow-seeded parents yielded yel-
low offspring. Indeed, that is what true breeding means. Lets represent one
of those crosses like this:

Purple Purple
x
Female Male

All offspring
are purple

So true breeding is not much more than the phenomenon of similar parents
yielding offspring similar to themselves. This tendency to breed true is one
that a seed company might advertise as one of the reliable attributes of a par-
ticular seed. It is also the foundation of the idea of pedigree, thoroughbred,
purebred, and pureblood lineages of animals. Humans even refer proudly
to themselves with adjectives like pureblood, dating from a time when the
blood was thought to confer genetic traits. According to the Oxford English
LECTURE TWO

Dictionary, the concept of true breeding dates from at least as far back as the
time of William Shakespeare. It has come to mean that identical, or as nearly
identical as can be managed, organisms mate with each other and produce
identical or nearly identical offspring. It can also imply true breeding for a par-
ticular set of traits but not all of the traits. For example, a breeder of miniature

12
poodles might take great pride in producing a consistent lineage of desirable
miniature traits, generation after generation of carefully chosen matings.
However, the dogs also come in a large variety of coat colors, which are not
necessarily true breeding. Surprises can occur too, such as shapes and sizes
that are not quite of dog-show quality, temperaments that may be problematic
and even genetic disabilities such as hip dysplasia. In this and in subsequent
lectures, I will explain more about how such surprises might occur in seem-
ingly stable lineages of supposedly true breeding genetic traits.
Inbreeding (the breeding of like organisms) is a way to get a true (or near-
ly true) breeding lineage, but inbreeding can have negative connotations.
Indeed, before our human ancestors understood exactly what might go
wrong, they had some deep misgivings, taboos, and prohibitions (as well as
natural inclinations) against incest or mating with their closest relatives.
However, despite taboos against inbreeding, our human ancestors nonethe-
less were probably quite inbred. This would simply be a consequence of
being in a small population, with little mobility in or out, isolated from other
populations for many generations. Geneticists studying human variation have
concluded that for most of the one million years of evolution of our species,
weve existed mostly in small, inbred populations. For example, church
records for tiny villages in England reveal inbred genealogies, unavoidable
because people rarely traveled more than several miles from their birthplaces
to find a spouse. There are plenty of cousin and second-cousin marriages in
all of our ancestries. For the most part things went well in our ancestral,
small, partly inbred villages. However, rare genetic diseases sometimes
appear in higher frequencies in particular lineages of humans because of that
tendency to inbreed.
Meanwhile, in some human lineages, especially ones that considered them-
selves royal, a certain amount of inbreeding (presumably to maintain the
purebred lineage of royalty) was considered positive and was deliberately
arrangedand still is to some extentto this day among some royals. As a
result, some royal diseases can arise, such as the prevalence of hemophilia
(a blood-clotting disorder) in the inbred royal families of Europe.
Similar to our human ancestors, many wild populations of animals and plants
are inbred to some degree, a natural result of little migration with the advan-
tages of mostly good traits being perpetuated in an environment that remains
relatively stable. On the other hand, for domestic animals and plants, there
was probably considerable experimentation with close inbreeding, for the pur-
pose of establishing thoroughbred or purebred lineages. Lineages of domes-
tic animals and plants are often inbred because of the conservative breeding
practices of their human facilitators. Some breeders in an attempt to perpetu-
ate desirable traits may have bred siblings. Sometimes the consequences
were fine. In other cases rare genetic disorders appeared as they still do
today. Hip dysplasia, epilepsy, and cataracts in dogs are just a few examples.
Some of the negative connotations of inbreeding come about because some
deleterious genes function only when there are double copies of them. For
example, animals typically have two copies of each gene. If a gene q is
deleterious in double copies but I have only one copy, I am merely a carrier
for q, but will not suffer any consequences of q myself. However, if two

13
organisms (perhaps relatives or perhaps just coincidently each bearing a
copy of q) get together, then there will be possibilities for offspring that are
qq with negative consequences. The Oxford English Dictionary provides
some insight in our ancestors understanding of undesirable aspects

Inbreeding |'in brd ing|


of inbreeding:

' '
Breeding from animals of the same parentage or closely related; breed-
ing in-and-in. c1842 E. J. LANCE Cottage Farmer, An in-breeding soon
breaks up the inattentive keeper of domestic animals. 1881 J. P. SHEL-
DON Dairy Farming 4/2 In-breeding, that is, the breeding for a time
amongst near relations generally results mischievously on the systems
and on the fertility of the stock. 1882 Standard 23 Aug. 2/1 Over-pre-
serving and in-breeding are deteriorating the herds.
Laboratory white rats bred for generations are a good example of a true-
breeding lineage developed via inbreeding for the purpose of having as many
genetic traits identical as possible. In that lineage, even very close relatives
(twin-like) are mating and presumably the rat breeders are getting away with
it because so many deleterious effects of inbreeding were culled early on in
the lineage. Furthermore, white rats are maintained in controlled laboratory
conditions with plenty of food and little competition. In the wild, some of their
gene combinations might be detrimental. However, in spite of the extraordi-
nary uniformity of white rats, they still have differences in their genomes
because of important complications concerning chromosomes (lecture 10),
gene regulation (lecture 12), and the surprisingly unstable structure and
arrangement of genetic material that will have to be explained in lecture 13 on
transposons (jumping genes) and viruses.
So how about Mendels peasthe ones from the original seed packets he
purchased? Were they true breeding? Were they inbred? Yes, we could use
those words for them and that is what Mendel confirmed in his first sets of
crosses. We dont know whether they were perfectly true breeding for all of
their thousands of genes, but certainly they were true breeding for purple
flower, white flower, yellow seed, and so forth, as well as a host of other traits
considered desirable by the original seed grower. Furthermore, being plants,
they were probably much more true breeding and inbred than any mammal
with which they might be compared.

Cloning
The idea of true breeding is also relevant to understanding identical twins
and clones, which genetically are the same phenomenon, albeit with different
mechanisms. Identical twins or clones have the same (or very nearly the
same) complement of genes (hereditary material). Essentially, those true-
breeding purple-flowered plants of Mendel were twins of each other,
although we dont usually use the word twin for plants. Some true-breeding
LECTURE TWO

plants are even clones of each other, although now we typically reserve the
word clone for particular results of procedures in modern genetics labs.
In order to properly sort out the word clone, I again went to the Oxford
English Dictionary to get a better idea about the evolving use of clone,
including the development of emotionally charged connotations. It turns out

14
that a discussion of cloning presents an excellent opportunity to present
some crucial (although sometimes overlooked) differences between animals
and plants that have profound effects on their genetics. Actually, our human
ancestors were acutely aware of these differences because they involve so
many practical aspects of plant and animal breeding. It matters very much
that Mendel chose plants and not animals for his experiments. He would not
have gotten the same clear results otherwise. The problem comes in when
we use a term like cloning in general as though it could be universally
applied to any organism with identical results. Thats not the case. In a sense,
even when trying to understand modern genetics in a theoretical way, it is dif-
ficult to get away from the everyday practicalities of handling the organisms
themselves and understanding their differences. Lets sort it out.
It turns out that plants have many natural mechanisms of propagation by
which they can bypass the complications of having parents all together.
These mechanisms include some that gardeners will recognize, such as
the following:
1. Taking cuttings and rooting them in moist sand or water.
2. Taking cuttings and grafting them onto other plants.
3. Dividing roots, bulbs, and tubers.
4. Establishing new plants from runners.
It is actually extremely easy to get a set of identical plants and thereby (if
one chooses to mate them) to develop a true-breeding (even clonal) lineage.
Plants in the wild make great use of their asexual (non-sexual, clonal) mecha-
nisms for reproduction, often to the exclusion of the relatively transient oppor-
tunities for sexual reproduction via flowers and seeds. For example, a stand
of aspen trees may be identical, all connected underground having sprouted
up from a common root system. Another way to think about it is that plants
easily can avoid the hassle of having two parents and do so regularly.
In contrast, mammals (for example, humans and dogs) have no asexual
reproduction by which to avoid the necessity of having two parents. Mammals
are obligately sexual and from the point-of-view of a plant, strangely limited in
their reproductive repertoires. There are no naturally occurring mechanisms
to get a perfectly true breeding lineage of mammals. Mating identical twins is
not an option, as identical twins are (by definition) of the same sex. Very
recently some techniques in the laboratory have been developed to attempt
to bypass the step of having two parents in mammals and thereby to accom-
plish an asexual, clonal means of propagation, that which plants do on their
own routinely. The genetic material from a single individual (one parent) is
implanted into an egg and, if all goes well, the offspring that develops from
the egg is an identical twin of its sole parent.
An additional characteristic of plants allows them to self. Their sexual
reproduction (which occurs less often than their asexual, clonal methods)
involves pollen (the equivalent of sperm) fertilizing an ovum (the equivalent of
an egg). The pollen can come from almost anywhere via the wind or a polli-
nating animal such as a honeybee. It can be any pollen: pollen from a com-
pletely different species (and no fertilization), pollen from a completely differ-
ent species (yet a surprise fertilization), pollen from a distant plant of the

15
same species, pollen from a nearby, identical plant, or pollen from the very
same plant, which is called selfing. Many plants have both male and female
parts on the same flower or at least on the same plant and many have no
mechanisms against selfing (although some do). One term for having both
male and female organs is hermaphrodism, but more typically plants in that
condition are called monecious.
In contrast, mammals have little or no such flexibility as to how and where
their sperm and eggs are joined. Fertilization always requires two partners, is
internal, and generally only is successful if those partners are of the same
species. Indeed, species for mammals and some other animals (with similar
limitations in reproduction) is often defined by the ability to mate and produce
offspring who in turn can mate, and so on.
There is another complication and intriguing difference between mammals
and plants: plants seem to have a much greater tolerance for the results of
true breeding. Their indeterminate growth (for example, with widely varying
numbers and shapes of branches) is in contrast to the more determined body
plan of mammals, in which (for example) extra limbs would be considered a
birth defect.
All of this means that a breeding pair of pedigree black Labrador retrievers is
never true breeding in the strictest sense, that one could use for plants. A
looser definition of true breeding describes what most breeders attempt to
do with just a small subset of genetic traits that define a particular breed,
such as size, shape, color, and behavior.
But there is more, especially with regard to plants: Plant breeders must have
noticed early on that plants were capable of a great variety of propagation
methods (including asexual mechanisms) and were tolerant of a much wider
range of inbreeding and true breeding practices. For some of these cases
the word clone was coined. Currently, this word has been taken over by sci-
ence fiction writers and modern geneticists alike, as well as becoming a
metaphor for a host of phenomena involving imitation. However, clone (as
either a noun or a verb), has deep roots in the Oxford English Dictionary. It
was first applied to plants in the early twentieth century.

Clone |kln|
noun
1. Botany
a. A group of cultivated plants the individuals of which are transplant-
ed parts of one original stock, the propagation having been carried
out by the use of grafts, cuttings, bulbs, etc.
b. In wider use in Biol. Any group of cells or organisms produced
asexually from a single sexually produced ancestor.
2. a. fig. A person or animal that develops from one somatic cell of its
parent and is genetically identical to that parent. Also (colloq.), a
person who imitates another, esp. slavishly.
LECTURE TWO

b. A thing produced in imitation of, or closely resembling, another;


spec. a microcomputer designed to simulate the functions of
another (usu. more expensive) model.

16
verb
a. To propagate or cause to reproduce so as to form a clone. (And
as of 2004 in the OED) a new verb definition: To make copies of
(a DNA molecule, base sequence, gene, etc.).
b. (loosely) To propagate or reproduce (an identical individual) from
a given original; to replicate (an existing individual). Chiefly fig.
1903 H. J. WEBBER in Science 16 Oct. 502/2
Clon[e]s . . . are groups of plants that are propagated by the use
of any form of vegetative parts.
[Professor Dyer: Such as most commercially grown fruit trees.]

1929 Bibliographia Genetica V. 234


In Bacillus coli communis . . . a biotype was also found having lower
motility than the remainder of the clone from which it came.
[Professor Dyer: This is the first use as applied to bacteria
(Bacillus coli, which is now called Escherischia coli.) At the
time, bacteria were often classified together with plants.]

1958 New Scientist 20 Feb. 13/1


Various techniques have been devised for producing these clone
cultures from single cells.
[Professor Dyer: The British biologist Honor Fell developed
methods of taking single cells from mammalian tissues and
growing them on Petri dishes. She is one of the founding
researchers of what is now thought of as stem cell research.
Note that plant stem cell research can be done by almost any
clever gardener or botanist. Plants lend themselves well to such
manipulations because asexual propagation is normal for most
plants. The logistics for animals are more challenging.]

1959 Nature 22 Aug. 648 (heading)


A New Technique for Isolating and Cloning Cells of Higher Plants.
[Professor Dyer: In addition to typical gardeners techniques for
asexual propagation of plants (a legacy of eons), there was still
much to be done with getting the techniques refined for labora-
tory use.]

1959 Genetics XLIV. 1259


A number of variants were obtained from two recently cloned lines of
strain HeLa S3.
[Professor Dyer: In this groundbreaking work cancer cells isolat-
ed from a woman named Henrietta Lacks who died of cancer in
1951 were established on Petri dishes. This became the first (of
many) immortal lineages of cloned human cells.]

1968 Observer (Colour Suppl.) 10 Mar. 9/1


One of the most extraordinary of the possibilities now being explored
. . . is referred to as cloning people the creation of genetically
identical individuals from body cells.

17
[Professor Dyer: This may be the first written mentions of this
possibility. It must have caught the imaginations of popular sci-
ence writers, social commentators, and science fiction writers,
given the flurry of subsequent reference, many from the popular
press in the OED.
[Note that the Ultimate Science Fiction Web Guide
(http://www.magicdragon.com/UltimateSF/clone.html) has ana-
lyzed this topic. The idea of cloning (such as making multiple
copies of humans) is a popular theme but predates the actual
use of the word clone in that context. They cite A. E. Van
Vogts 1945 serial and later novel The World of Null-A.]

1970 Nature 19 Apr. 210/2


The Jockey Club was . . . understandably cool when asked to com-
ment on the possibility of a dozen cloned Arkles thundering neck and
neck round the course at Epsom.
1970 A. TOFFLER Future Shock ix. 197
Those most likely to replicate themselves will be those who are most
narcissistic, and . . . the clones they produce will also be narcissists.
1974 Proc. National Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 71 1747/2
The procedure reported here offers a general approach utilizing bac-
terial plasmids for the cloning of DNA molecules from various
sources. AND Proc. National Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 71 3459/1 ColE1 has
been shown to serve as an effective molecular vehicle for cloning
and amplifying specific regions of unrelated DNA.
[Professor Dyer: This is groundbreaking work by which cloning
went from being bounded and constrained by the organism to
being about any genetic information. Previously, it was known
that plants and bacteria were easy to clone and animals much
more difficult and limited. In this work, animal genes were
inserted into bacteria and behold, suddenly animal genes were
easy to clone. This launched genetic engineering.]

1979 Whig-Standard (Kingston, Ontario) 13 Nov. 23/1


The 32-year-old is not one of a myriad of Elvis clones who came out
of the woodwork when the King died two years ago.
1982 Sci. Amer. May 112/1
Individual organisms that arise asexually from the somatic, or body,
cells of the parent rather than from the specialized sexual cells are
called clones.
[Professor Dyer: A new, more general scientific definition is
established. Note, it no longer matters which organism.]

1993 D. SHAY & J. DUNCAN Making of Jurassic Park p. ix


LECTURE TWO

The research seems to echo Jurassic Park, the novel about scien-
tists who bring dinosaurs back to life by cloning their DNA.

18
2000 U.S. News & World Rep. 20 Mar. 52/2
With the biotech revolution of the 1960s and 1970s, scientists cloned
the relevant gene and inserted that DNA into nonhuman cells.

What about famous cloned animals such as


Dolly the sheep and other various well-publi-
cized attempts to clone whole mammals from
single cells? These will be discussed in subse-
quent lectures. (See the discussion of a cloned
calico cat in lecture 10, as well as the complexi-
ties of environment and gene regulation in lec-
tures 6 and 12.) The technical challenges are
many and fascinating. (Again for plants, this is
relatively easy and has been for decades.)
So how about Mendels peas. Were they
clones of each other? That is, were all the

Public Domain
purple-flower peas in his original true breeding
packets of seeds clones of each other? If
Mendel (who by the way did not know the word Dolly the sheep, 1998
clone) had chosen a different plant to work
withone that had a history of being propagat-
ed asexually, such as most domesticated fruit treesthen yes, he could have
been working with clones! However, peas generate quickly by sexual repro-
duction (a major reason Mendel chose them) and are not commonly propa-
gated by asexual manipulations by breeders. Therefore, although Mendel
would have been fascinated to learn all about cloning (if he could time-travel
to the present), his own plants were just true breeders.
So to reiterate, Mendels first crosses confirmed the well-established true-
breeding nature of his plants. What he did next was outbreeding (outcrossing,
crossbreeding) in as many ways as he could imagine, keeping careful notes
of the outcomes.

19

FOR GREATER UNDERSTANDING

Questions

1. What is true breeding and what are some examples?


2. Why does it matter sometimes whether a genetic subject is a plant or
an animal?
3. What are some of the early uses of the word clone?

Suggested Reading

Schwartz, James. In Pursuit of the Gene: From Darwin to DNA. Cambridge,


MA: Harvard University Press, 2008.

Other Books of Interest


Mawer, Simon. Gregor Mendel: Planting the Seeds of Genetics. New York:
Harry N. Abrams, Inc., 2006.

Websites to Visit

1. The Compact Oxford English Dictionary is available free online


http://www.askoxford.com
2. The full version of the Oxford English Dictionary is available by subscrip-
tion online http://www.oed.com
LECTURE TWO

20
Lecture 3:
What Did Mendel Do Next?

The Suggested Reading for this lecture is James Schwartzs In Pursuit


of the Gene: From Darwin to DNA.

Crossbreeding or Outcrossing
Simply put, Mendel crossbred or outcrossed his plants and monitored the
results carefully.
Lets continue perusing the Oxford English Dictionary for selected defini-
tions to see that Mendels next set of experiments with his pea varieties
were also straight from the annals of plant and animal breeding practices.
In this case, I will concatenate the OED historical records for various forms
of two relevant, interchangeable concepts: outcross and crossbred to pro-
duce one chronological record of use. All together it reads like an outline for
the history of practical genetics. Here and there Ive added annotations. Try
looking up the words hybrid and hybridization yourself to get a chronology of
their rich history.
Notice the mixture of references to humans and to their domestic animals.
Notice too the many familiar words that come up in the definitions and
descriptions, such as mongrel, blood, race, and hybrid. Feel free to look

Crossbreed |'krs brd|


up those words and more in the OED to get a more complete picture.

'
To breed across the lines which separate varieties or races; to breed
(animals or plants) from individuals of different species or races. Hence
cross-breeding vbl. n. A breed of animals (or plants) produced by cross-

Outcross | out'krs|
ing; a mongrel or hybrid breed.

'
An introduction of unrelated breeding stock to an established, usually
somewhat inbred or homogeneous, line; a mating between unrelated

Hybrid |'h brid|


individuals; the result or offspring of such a mating.

'
The offspring of two animals or plants of different species, or (less strictly)
varieties; a half-breed, cross-breed, or mongrel.
And here are a few selected uses of crossbreed and outcross to show you
their antiquity, with commentary.
1675 WYCHERLEY Country Wife II. i,
They are come to think cross breeding for themselves best, as well
as for their dogs and horses.
1774 WILKES Corr. (1805) IV. 185
The family of monsieur Louvet . . . emigrated to England; and made
a cross-breed with those who [etc.].

21
1882 J. MACDONALD & J. SINCLAIR Hist. Polled Aberdeen or Angus
Cattle 259 In President 3rd 246,
. . . a very judicious outcross was taken. This animal had in his veins
an almost equal proportion of Keillor and Ardovie blood.
1844 DISRAELI Coningsby III. v,
It seems to me a barren thing, this Conservatism, an unhappy cross-
breed; the mule of politics that engenders nothing.
Here Disraeli refers to a mule that is an offspring (unable itself to reproduce) of
an extreme crossbreed between a horse and a donkey.
1918 Genetics 3 475
When double-throwing Matthiola is used as egg parent in an
outcross to ordinary singles, half the offspring receive a factor
for doubleness.
Wow, theres a bit of very early technical jargon from a genetics research
paper. As scientific jargon often does, this suffers from having been coined
before a full understanding of the phenomena had been settled. Matthiola is a
plant. Double and single here refer to shapes of chromosomes as viewed
under the microscope. Chromosomes were suspected to contain the heredi-
tary material (and they do) but in 1918 nobody knew how that worked.
1928 D.F. JONES Selective Fertilization vi. 117
The chromosome situation in these flies is not known, and it is not
clear what the reduced fertility in outcrosses is due to.
At this point in the history of genetics, the fruit fly Drosophila was well on its
way to being a favorite model organism for geneticists. There will be more
about fruit flies in subsequent lectures.
Mendels Methods
Here is some notation we will be using to sort this out as Gregor Mendel did
with his outcrosses. I will be using a deck of cards analogy:
Most of the organisms (animals and plants) to be described in this course
have two of each of their genes. Bacteria presented only briefly in these lec-
tures are an exception. For animals and plants, one of each gene pair came
from its mother, one from its father. Therefore, the genes of a true-breeding
pea plant will be shown here as pairs. Simple genetics problems often sym-
bolize genes with a single letter code, either upper or lower case. (That
means for the twenty-six-letter alphabet we can describe fifty-four genes with
a single letter code. That isnt nearly enough, but its a start. Humans and
most other animals have about twenty-five thousand genes and several to
many versions of each of those genes.)
Here are some of the genes of a true-breeding organism such as a pea plant
or a black Labrador retriever:
LECTURE THREE

GG ff PP TT rr ww QQ NN vv ll xx ZZ...
The dots at the end are important. They imply that all the rest of the twenty-
five thousand genes continue along in the same manner. Note that no matter
which symbol we are using (whether uppercase QQ or lower case ff) the
gene pairs are identical to each other because the organism is true breeding.

22
If we perform a true breeding cross (of identical parents) it looks like this:
Parents:
GGffPPTTrrwwQQNNvvllxxZZ... X GGffPPTTrrwwQQNNvvllxxZZ...

Offspring:
GGffPPTTrrwwQQNNvvllxxZZ...
Note the genetically identical parents and identical offspring.
Now here is a technicalitya sort of dichotomy between the ideal and the
practical. Think about those black Labrador retrievers once more. If we were
trying to understand just a few of their genes (and not the entire set of twenty-
five thousand) and if those genes just happened to be some of the genes
represented as letters (GGffPP...) in the cross above (with each gene pair
identical), then for those genes, the dogs would be true breeding. Most of
genetics is practiced on a practical, functional level with just a few genes at a
time, not the entire idealized set. In the case of the black Labradors, the dog
breeder might think of the pedigree dogs as being true breeding just for the
specific sets of genes for black coat color and a big chunky body and head
and other pedigree Lab traits. If we were to look at the entire set of dog
genes, we would find that many were not true breeding. It is impossible to
mate identical twin dogs, as they would have to be of the same gender.
For the majority of problems that I will be presenting, we will consider one
gene pair or at most two gene pairs at a time, as Mendel did. However, now
and then I will remind you that there are in fact twenty-five thousand genes. It
is our choice to simplify down to one or two genes. In doing so we are not
declaring that genes are simple; however, by simplifying, we are getting a
glimpse at some basic rules for how genes function. That we deliberately are
choosing to simplify (to one or two genes at a time) may seem too obvious to
even say, but I think it is the source of a common misconception about genet-
ics. If you read about a new scientific development that sounds like this:
They just discovered the gene for autism or They are looking for the gene
for homosexuality, then you are encountering the misconception that genes
function as simple, isolated individual entities and can be understood com-
pletely or sufficiently that way. In subsequent lectures we will return to think-
ing about many genes at once with intricate, convoluted interactions.
Meanwhile, here is a simplified true-breeding cross for a dog:
Lets say that all individuals bearing the genes BB (one from each of
their parents) all have black coat color and lets say the bb animals all
have brown coat colors.
BB x BB (two black dogs)
BB (black offspring)
Here is another.
bb x bb (two brown dogs)
bb (brown offspring)
So the two true-breeding crosses show that black dogs beget black dogs,
brown dogs beget brown dogs, and so on forever and ever until we decide to
outcross black with brown. Lets do that:

23
BB x bb
black x brown
Bb
All offspring are black

Notice that we need just one copy of that B gene to get a black dog. Thats
what Mendel noticed too, except that he was working with pea colors and
other pea traits. Sometimes a gene functions just fine with one copy, as with
B for black, and sometimes two copies are needed, as in bb for brown.
Here is what did not happen: The two colors did not mix like paint, which
was a common misconception about heredity previous to Mendels analyses.
We did not get a blackish-brown color. We got black.
Card dealing is a useful analogy here. Each dog was holding two cards in its
hand (or paw.) Each dog contributed one card to the future offspring. For this
out cross, there was not a lot of choice as to which card each dog contributed.
To continue the card-playing analogy, clearly these cards are not quite
equal. In fact, one seems to trump the other. If we have B, it doesnt seem
to matter what that second card is. We get black.
Indeed, there are three combinations of gene pairs we can observe from this
round of play:
BB black
Bb black
bb brown

Rather than use the word trump, geneticists use the terms dominant for B
and recessive for b. A dominant gene is one that needs only one copy in
order to cause a particular trait (blackness). A recessive gene needs two
copies to show its trait (brownness).
Surely somebody had noticed this before, that inherited traits sorted like
playing cards and did not mix like paint. After all, we have thousands of years
of serious dog and horse breeding before Mendels time. I think it must have
been noticed over and over, but apparently nobody had thought to apply a set
of symbols by which nearly every possible combination could be figured out
and the amazing regularity revealed. (And recall too that dog and horse
breeders were and continue to be generally uninterested in examining exactly
one trivial trait at a time). To confound things a bit, not all traits sort them-
selves out as crisply and obviously as the ones in this example. Mendel may
also have been fortunate in his choice of organism and choice of traits. In
fact, much later Mendel continued his investigations with different plants and
different traits and was considerably less successful.
Mendel must have been eager to try out every possible combination of
crosses that he could using just this pair of genes. The symbols applied here
LECTURE THREE

either to peas or dogs; it doesnt matter.


Here are all the combinations that are not just true breeding:
BB x Bb
Offspring could be either BB or Bb

24
bb x Bb
Offspring could be either Bb or bb
Bb x Bb
Offspring could be either BB or Bb or bb

Recall we are dealing out these genes as though they were cards that would
make up a new hand. The offspring can be one or another of all the possi-
ble combinations that could be made from the parents hands.
Just as in card playing, it is fun to know the odds of getting those combina-
tions. If I am dealing out cards blindfolded and my fellow card player is doing
the same, what are the odds (or probabilities or chances) of us each dealing
a B card such that we get a BB pairing? And what are the odds of our
cards producing a mismatched pair Bb. It is convenient to set up the prob-
lem on a Punnett square (named for Reginald Punnett, who devised the
method in the early twentieth century).

BB x Bb yields all black puppies

B b

B BB Bb
black black

Bb x bb yields a 1:1 ratio of black to brown puppies.


That means there is a one-half chance of getting a black or a brown puppy.

B b

b Bb bb
black brown

Bb X Bb yields a 3:1 ratio of black to brown puppies.


That means there is one-quarter chance brown and a three-quarter chance
of black.

B b

B BB Bb
black black

b Bb bb
black brown

25
So does that mean that in a litter of four puppies with parents Bb and Bb that I
will get exactly one brown and three black? No, it does not mean that. Those
are ideal ratios that one might see if the dogs were to have dozens to hundreds
of offspring. Each puppy represents a sort of independent hand of cards. Notice
this phenomenon in families. There is a 50 percent chance of getting either a
boy or a girl and yet plenty of families do not show a 1:1 ratio of boys and girls.
It is entirely possible to have a lucky run of six boys or eight girls in a row. This
is an important concept for genetic counselors explaining to parents the proba-
bility of some deleterious gene being passed to offspring. Take Tay Sachs dis-
ease as an example.

TT normal recycling of wastes from neurons


Tt carrying but not showing the trait of failing to recycle wastes
tt Tay Sachs disease: a progressive, incurable deterioration of
muscles and nerves due to a failure to recycle wastes in
neurons, resulting in death by the age of three

Lets say the parents (Tt and Tt) have already three normal children
(either T T or Tt) and are discussing plans for a fourth child with their
genetic counselor.
Are they due for a child with Tay Sachs (tt) after that run of good fortune
with their first three? No, the probability is exactly the same as with their first
child. Likewise, parents who have already lost three children in a row to Tay
Sachs are not due to have a TT or Tt child. One of the reasons this can be a
challenge to explain is because the popular misconceptions about probabili-
ties are pervasive. For example, many people do consider themselves due
for a win, after a string of losses. How about you? Answer these two ques-
tions honestly, to find out whether you are vulnerable to magical thinking
about numbers:
1. Which of these lottery numbers is more likely to be a winner:
2222 or 7569?
2. If 7569 won a million dollars yesterday, is it therefore unlikely to
win today?
Answers: Both 2222 and 7569 are equally likely (or unlikely) to win. And
7569 is as likely (or unlikely) to win today as it was yesterday. If you got
those wrong, you are in good company with billions of people. Mistakes of
this sort about numbers seem to be deeply ingrained.
Mendelian Ratios in Nature
Can you see 1:1 or 3:1 ratios of particular traits in natural populations of
organisms? For reasons that will be described in lectures 6 and 7 about envi-
ronment, it is not typical to be able to do so. Rather, if you can spot ratios (by
LECTURE THREE

doing lots of counting and evaluating of a trait) they are likely to be highly
skewed away from Mendelian ideals. But they will be interesting nonetheless.
Here are some suggestions for investigations.
1. Look through a guidebook to wild flowers in your area, watching for ones that
are described as varying in color, typically in the range of blues-pinks-whites
(rather than yellows and oranges). Then set out to find a large stand of your
26
plant and see if you notice any variations. If you do not, you may have found
a true-breeding population for that trait or maybe there is some environmen-
tal reason for only one color. If you do, have fun counting and recording and
seeing whether any interesting ratios emerge.
2. Watch for reports of unusual albino or melanistic mutants of wild animals,
such as white deer or black squirrels. Sometimes there will be just one
sighting. However, there are interesting cases of populations with excep-
tionally high ratios of color mutations, for example, the population of black
squirrels in some areas of northeastern United States. If you are in the
right area, try counting them at your bird feeder.
3. Go to the grocery store during harvest season and purchase colorful, deco-
rative corn. These are not from natural populations and therefore are likely
to display Mendelian ratios or some variation of a more complex ratio. The
more decorative the corn, the more likely it is to have genetic ratios that
are more complex than 1:1 and 3:1. Expect them especially in ears with
three or four or even more different colors of kernels. Sometimes, though,
you can analyze an ear of corn such that you ignore subtle variations in
color but just declare everything to be either pigmented (all of the reds,
purples, blues) or not pigmented (all of the yellows and whites). That is, do
as Mendel did, ignore everything except for one trait at a time.
Here in brief are a few somewhat advanced additional topics, typically part of
a genetics course. You may want to consider these further, once you are
quite comfortable with more basic principles:
Dominant and recessive relationships between genes can be complicated.
Here are some possibilities:
The variations of genes are called alleles. Each gene has many potential
alleles, way too many to account for with a system like B, b, which is why
we are resorting to superscripts for some of these. Note that in any given
organism, we have only two alleles, one from the mother, one from the father.
It is when we consider a population of organisms that we need to bring in
more complex notation to name all the different possibilities.

Bb Simple dominance. The B trait is expressed as in black.

Bb Dosage dependent dominance. We have just one copy of B,


so the animal has a somewhat lighter coat color than it would
if it were BB.

B1B2 Co-dominance, which is more likely to appear in a structure.


It is almost as though B1 and B2 were two different types of
bricks being used equally to build a wall. Structures attached
to the outside of red blood cells are often co-dominant. An
example is the AB blood group, where the two co-dominant
alleles are IAIB.

B1B2 B1 is dominant to B2.

B2B3 B2 is no longer recessive because the relationships are


relative. In this pairing, B2 is dominant to B3.

27
I have not yet provided a definition for gene. That will come later. It turns
out that throughout his career as our official first geneticist, Mendel did not
know what a gene was. Indeed, throughout the first part of the twentieth
century, geneticists worked on genes as concepts, but did not know what
they were. We will follow that same trajectory and in the first few lectures
treat genes as though they were some sort of hereditary information with
many as yet unknown properties and functions. Meanwhile, here are two
more useful terms:
Genotypes: A genotype is the string of symbols representing all of the
genes under consideration for an organism, such as:
Bb
or:
GGffPPTTrrwwQQNNvvllxxZZ

Phenotypes: Phenotypes are the description (as long and detailed


as they need to be) of the traits associated with a par-
ticular genotype:
black
or:
long tailed, floppy eared, short snout, brindle coat

It is possible to analyze two genes at a time, three genes at a time and


more, except that each increment becomes much more complicated with
potential combinations. Such analyses (in search of Mendelian ratios) work
best when genes are acting as independently and separately as individual
cards in a deck. However, there are some circumstances in which the genes
are linked together (positioned right next to each other) and therefore tend to
be sorted out as though they were like playing cards glued together. That is,
they do not sort independently. Mendel didnt know about that and he would
have been confused if he had encountered it. For this next example, well
consider two genes that are independent and provide you with some more
ratios to watch for.
Lets try various crosses involving two traits relevant to the flavor and texture
of peas.
AA or Aa sweet

BB or Bb early

aa bland

bb late

AaBb (sweet early pea) x AABb (sweet early pea)


LECTURE THREE

Notice the result (next page) is a 6:2, which reduces to a 3:1 ratio
of sweet-early to sweet-late peas.

28
AB Ab aB ab

AB AABB AABb AaBB AaBb


sweet- sweet- sweet- sweet-
early early early early

Ab AABb AAbb AaBb Aabb


sweet- sweet- sweet- sweet-
early late early late

Try AaBb x AaBb (both are sweet early peas). The result is a 9:3:3:1 ratio
of sweet-early (the nine not labeled below): bland-early; sweet-late; bland-
late. It is one of the more complex ratios from a two-gene cross.

AB Ab aB ab

AB AABB AABb AaBB AaBb

Ab AABb AAbb AaBb Aabb


sweet- sweet-
late late

aB AaBB AaBb aaBB aaBb


bland- bland-
early early

ab AaBb Aabb aaBb aabb


sweet- bland- bland-
late early late

29

FOR GREATER UNDERSTANDING

Questions

1. What are examples of outcrossing, especially ones you can observe yourself?
2. What is an allele and what makes an allele either dominant or recessive?

Suggested Reading

Schwartz, James. In Pursuit of the Gene: From Darwin to DNA. Cambridge,


MA: Harvard University Press, 2008.

Other Books of Interest

Mawer, Simon. Gregor Mendel: Planting the Seeds of Genetics. New York:
Harry N. Abrams, Inc., 2006.
LECTURE THREE

30
Lecture 4:
A Cookie Factory in a Black Box
Designed by Rube Goldberg

The Suggested Reading for this lecture is James Schwartzs In Pursuit


of the Gene: From Darwin to DNA.

This lecture introduces an extensive metaphor that I believe will help you to
envision some of the more complex genetics processes that go beyond sim-
ple Mendelian ones. In subsequent lectures I will say something like, recall
that cookie factory from lecture 4 to transport you briefly back to being able
to visualize it, and then I will layer on some bit of genetics. So please, even
though the relevance may not seem perfectly apparent now, read through
and prepare to use this as a sort of memorization system. It is similar to a
method loci, which is a classical mnemonic system sometimes based on
visualizing a temple or palace in order to keep track of a large number of
items and relationships.
One of the main reasons that Mendel and other early geneticists found so
many confusing exceptions to their ratios (such as the 3:1 ratio) is because
most genes can not be examined in isolation from their relationships or inter-
actions with other genes. Those interactions make up the functions (the
chemical reactions) of cells and therefore of organisms. Lets say there is an
automated cookie factory with assembly lines arranged in a logical order like
the following:
1. A dough-mixing machine for combining flour, sugar, butter, and so on.
2. The first fork in the path: some cookies receive chocolate chips, some
do not.
3. A shaping machine by which cookies are formed from the dough.
4. A baking machine.
5. A cooling machine.
6. A second fork in the path: sorting machine (to
utterstock.com

remove broken and burned cookies).


7. A third fork in the path: some cookies receive
an /sh

frosting, some do not.


om
Th

8. A counting machine to set out the right number


ry
Co

of cookies for each package.


9. A packaging machine.
10. A labeling machine.
Each of the ten steps require at least one machine. Each of the machines
requires its own little set of instructions for assembly and function.
And now, lets put it all into a black box so that you cannot see what is going
on inside. You can only look at the output: cookies of one sort or another. You
wait patiently and observantly for some little mistake to be made inside the
factory and then you diligently record exactly what happened to the cookies.

31
Here is how your analysis goes if you are trying to decipher this as a
geneticist would.
Some mistakes (mutations to the instructions for the machines) seem trivial
and have little effect on the important aspect of the cookies (their edibility
and deliciousness). For example:
The labeling machine spells cookie wrong: bookie.
There are three cookies in a package instead of two.

Other mistakes seem to cause a complete absence of an expected cookie


(but we do get a default cookie). For example:
There are no chocolate chip cookies at all, just those without chips.
There are no frosted cookies at all, only unfrosted.

Still other mistakes have dramatic effects. For example:


The cookies are raw and therefore cannot be sorted by the sorting
machine, cannot be frosted, cannot be counted, cannot be packaged. Only
horribly deformed and inedible cookie dough pours out of the factory.
No cookies emerge at all. Could there be something wrong with the
dough mixer? Perhaps no ingredients were measured in.

Things to notice:
The hierarchy of seriousness of the mutations has something to do with
the order of events. Certain early-acting machines can have a cascading
effect on the subsequent machines. Some early-acting machines can
even prevent the entire pathway from functioning.
At the places where paths fork, we can have mutations that eliminate
one possibility but not the other
(frosted versus not-frosted).
Events at the end of this path-
way seem to be less important
to the overall product.

This is what geneticists think


aboutmetaphoricallywhen they
are using the results of mutations to
try to understand and reconstruct
the order of events of a hidden cell
pathway. In addition to watching for
single mistakes (mutations) they
also watch for combinations of
mutations (double mutations and
triple mutations) and use those to
LECTURE FOUR

decipher the pathway.


Notice that some double or triple
mutations might have interesting
and not especially disastrous
results. How about cookies that are
miscounted and mislabeled? Clipart.com

32
However, any double or triple mutation that involves a broken dough mixer
may not be observable at all. If one combines a broken dough mixer with a
broken cookie counter, the effects of the broken counter disappear.
Over the years, geneticists have come up with a multitude of jargon by
which to analyze pathways hidden in (metaphoric) black boxes by examining
the end-products. I think having a good picture of an assembly-line analysis
as Ive just described is useful and will help you with subsequent lectures.
Memorizing a lot of jargon that goes with such an analysis is less useful.
However, I will introduce the jargon and etymologies here for another pur-
pose: to demystify and deconstruct some of the struggles and challenges that
geneticists have had and continue to have in their analyses.
Epistasis
When a mutation at the beginning of a pathway completely obliterates your
view of any subsequent parts of the pathway, we call that epistasis. If we
observe such a mutation (like uncooked dough), we declare it to be important
and early in the pathway we are deciphering. The etymology of the word
stands upon implies the early confusion as to what was going on with
epistatic mutations. I still have my undergraduate genetics text book, written
in 1969. Epistatic genes are described there as covering up or hiding (or
standing upon) other genes, but there is not a word in that chapter about
understanding genes as parts of pathways. It was still too early in 1969 for
that sort of analysis to emerge from the black box.
Modifier
The frosting machine and the chocolate chip machine are modifiers. Notice
that they most likely are parts of their own pathways that we have not
described. (That is, there must be a frosting-making machine somewhere.)
But from the point-of-view of the main pathway, the frosting machine is just
adding a little extra to a cookie that otherwise will turn out just fine. So we
use this word modifier for those genes that (in our judgement) cause seem-
ingly optional, and only slightly different variations.
Pleiotropy
A single mistake may have far-ranging and diverse consequences. How
many different cookie types are we getting from the factory? I count at least
six, each pertaining to a particular branching section of the pathway:
Unfrosted, no chocolate chips
Unfrosted, chocolate chips
Frosted, no chocolate chips
Frosted, chocolate chips
Broken without chocolate chips
Broken with chocolate chips
A geneticist would analyze this by looking for how many different results a
particular pathway is producing. If there is more than one result we call that
pleiotropy and we assume that branching pathways are the cause of it. And
of course we can use epistasis to figure out whether some of those branching
nodes are more important (near the beginning of the pathway) than others.

33
An example would be the chocolate chip machine node. If that machine is
broken, no chips will appear in any subsequent cookie regardless of the
cookies other attributes; for example, frosting or wholeness.
Penetrance
And now we are entering the realm of weasel words that are used when
geneticists are not exactly sure what is going on in the black box, but are
pretty sure its complicated and needs to be described somehow. Lets say I
expect a particular machine to behave a certain way and every now and then
it doesnt. Lets say that 10 percent of the time, the cookie-wrapping machine
fails to wrap some cookies. Id love to know why. Its probably something fas-
cinating that will give me a more complete understanding of the cookie facto-
ry. However, right now I am forced to declare the cookie-wrapping machine to
be 90 percent penetrant, which means it wraps cookies about 90 percent of
the time. Interestingly, this word gets used most often for important genetic
traits, the ones that are most difficult to sort out. A geneticist might say that a
particular birth defect such as polydactyly (extra digits on fingers or toes) is
incompletely penetrant. That is, a certain percentage of people who have the
mutated gene that causes extra digits will have normal numbers of digits.
Expressivity
This is another vague word, used when we do not have a full understanding
of a pathway. Lets say the cookie-shaping machine sometimes made extra-
large cookies and sometimes tiny cookies, in addition to those of normal
size. If we could figure it out, we would have a better understanding of the
cookie-shaping function. Meanwhile, we are obliged to say that it has vari-
able expressivity. It turns out that polydactyly also may be used as an
example of variable expressivity. Sometimes it affects one hand, sometimes
both, sometimes just one foot, and sometimes both.
Predisposition
This is a useful word often in conjunction with penetrance and expressivity.
For example, breast cancer is complex. If I think I have identified a mutation in
one of many genes in the complex pathway by which breast tissue develops, I
might say I have found a putative breast cancer allele. People who have the
allele are most likely not guaranteed to get breast cancer. However, I could
say they are predisposed to it. How predisposed? Thats where I would bring
in an analysis of penetrance and expressivity if I had enough data to do so.
Cascade
Biologists sometimes use this word evocative of a multilevel tumbling water-
fall to describe complex multibranching pathways.
Alleles
LECTURE FOUR

There are many possible different ways to modify (including break) a


machine. Those are all of its different variants or alleles.
Environment
This is so important that it will be addressed in lectures 6 and 7. It is impor-
tant to never forget that genes operate within the contexts of environments.

34
The factory may seem complex enough at this point, but there is one more
layer of detail to add. Lets say this factory was designed by Rube Goldberg
(or Heath Robinson, or Dr. Seuss) using baling wire, duct tape, clothespins,
and other odds and ends from their inventors workshop. In spite of the seem-
ingly linear (or cascading) logic described above, the factory is actually quite
convoluted, loaded with redundant processes, baroque functions, and seem-
ingly nonfunctional (at least for cookie-baking) cowbells and horns. And this is
actually a truer picture of any pathway in any cell. The picture is wildly non-
linear, convoluted, and seemingly pieced together from unrelated items as
though from the corner of an eccentric inventors basement. This is what
geneticists must deal with when they set about to decipher genes. As usual
(in the tradition of Mendel) the most important first step is to simplify, to look
at a few parts at a time and to assume none of it has any sort of logically
designed, carefully, linearly assembled structure.

Rube Goldberg & of Rube Goldberg, Inc.

Professor Goldberg walked in his sleep, strolled through a cactus field in his bare feet, and
screamed out an idea for a self-operating napkin:
As you raise spoon of soup (A) to your mouth it pulls string (B), thereby jerking ladle (C)
which throws cracker (D) past parrot (E). Parrot jumps after cracker and perch (F) tilts,
upsetting seeds (G) into pail (H). Extra weight in pail pulls cord (I), which opens and lights
automatic cigar lighter (J), setting off sky-rocket (K) which causes sickle (L) to cut string
(M) and allow pendulum with attached napkin to swing back and forth thereby wiping off
your chin. After the meal, substitute a harmonica for the napkin and youll be able to
entertain the guests with a little music.

Rube Goldberg (roob goldberg) n. a comically involved, complicated invention, laboriously


contrived to perform a simple operationWebsters New World Dictionary

35

FOR GREATER UNDERSTANDING

Questions

1. Can you explain the cookie factory metaphor to someone and use it to
explain genes and mutations in the context of pathways?
2. What is epistasis?
3. What are modifiers?

Suggested Reading

Schwartz, James. In Pursuit of the Gene: From Darwin to DNA. Cambridge,


MA: Harvard University Press, 2008.

Other Books of Interest


Goldberg, Rube. Rube Goldberg: Inventions! New York: Simon &
Schuster, 2000.
Robinson, Heath. Contraptions. New York: The Overlook Press, 2007.

Website of Interest

Rube Goldberg, Inc. provides a website dedicated to the late engineer-


turned-cartoonist http://www.rubegoldberg.com
LECTURE FOUR

36
Lecture 5:
How Mammals Get Their Colors

The Suggested Reading for this lecture is Juliet Clutton-Brocks


A Natural History of Domesticated Mammals.

All mammals are very closely related (being relatively recently evolved) and
all use the same complex pathway to make their melanin pigments by which
their hair (or coats) and skins are colored. The pathway that I will describe
here was worked out mostly for mouse coat colors and dog coat colors, but
pertains to any mammalian pigmentation, including humans. A major differ-
ence is that human pigmentation is mostly quite limited to a range of dark
brown pigments to pale pigments. Humans evolved such that they are not
using the full potential of the melanin pathway; for example, humans tend not
to come in stripes and spots and multicolors (or if they do, it is declared to be
an exceptional genetic condition such as Waardenburg syndrome, in which
patches of white appear against a background of darker hair). Note too that
many wild animals (whether they have stripes or spots or just plain grayish-
brown coats) also are quite limited in their normal range of pigments.
In contrast dog breeders and fancy mouse breeders have delighted in
achieving as wide a range of colors and combinations as possible. Therefore,
to see the full capabilities of the melanin pigment pathway we turn to fancy
mice and dogs.
Recall the cookie factory. I am going to lead you step by step through a set
of activities and functions just as I did with cookie making. Only here it will be
the relatively unfamiliar melanin pigment-making pathway. The goal is not to
memorize the pathway, but rather it is to
appreciate its intricacy
see the possibilities for how it was analyzed (as we did in the
hypothetical analysis of the cookie factory)
be intrigued by the connectedness of the melanin pathway to
other pathways
get a little glimpse of the potential effects of environment
never look at dog (or cat or other mammalian) coat colors the
same way again
The problem of exactly where to begin and where to end and which other
auxiliary pathways and branching pathways to include is difficult. That is
intrinsic to any biological pathway. I will make a choice to begin arbitrarily
right outside of a specialized cell called a melanocyte that is destined (or
not) to become filled with the pigment melanin. This is a simplified pathway,
but it features all nine of the major pigment genes that are fairly well under-
stood. In case youd like to read more (for example, at a genetics for dog
breeders website), I am using the typical letter symbols (K, A, E, C, B, and
D, S, T, and M) used for genes of the melanin pathway.

37
1. The K gene product binds (if it can) to the outside of the melanocyte
(the specialized cell that is destined [or not] to become full of pigment). If
K does bind, we will be on our way to dark pigmentationproducing the
dark pigment eumelanin.
2. But here is our first branch. There is another gene product A. Some
forms (some alleles) of A prevent K from binding. If that happens, we are
on our way to making various degrees of pale pigmentationproducing
the pigment phaeomelanin.
3. But actually none of this happens without the gene product E, which
forms the place on the melanocyte where K would like to bind.
You might be noticing something right about now. There is a sort of nursery
rhyme or even Dr. Seuss-like quality to the description. It is very difficult to put
it all into a neat straight line. Indeed, very little in the cell is linear, no matter
how hard we might try to depict it as linear. In this sense, the cookie factory
metaphor is much simpler.
4. Okay, so lets say we made it through those first three nonlinear steps and
the following happened: K and E both were functioning and therefore K
did bind to E and, by the way, the form of A that was available did not
prevent this from happening.
Were there any other options? Sure, lots. E might have been nonfunctional,
not allowing E to bind. Or K might have been nonfunctional, unable to bind E.
Or A might have prevented K from binding E. We have scarcely begun and
we already have lots of possibilities.
5. Just inside of the cell (the one on which K just successfully bound) is a
gene product called C waiting for the signal to go ahead with its function.
Successful binding by K is that signal. C triggers a multibranching pathway.
Well just look at two of the branches to start with. One version of C is very
efficient at converting the chemical tyrosine to dopaquinone. If it does, we
are well on our way to making the dark pigment eumelanin with whatever B
genes are available: If B, well get black; if b, well get brown.
6. Furthermore, we can modify that eumelanin (made by B or b) a bit.
Recall modifiers do not dramatically change a product but just cause little
conversions and tweaks for slightly different products. The D genes do
that modification, producing either a richer or more diluted version of black
or brown.
There was another completely different option, though. There are many ver-
sions of C, some of which send us down the other fork in the path to making
the paler pigment phaeomelanin.
So where are we so far?
In order to get a completely black Labrador retriever dog, I need a minimum
of the following:
LECTURE FIVE

K bindng to E
E allowing K to bind
the lower-case version (recessive) allele a, which does not interfere
with K and E
38
C, which allows reactions to move strongly ahead to making eumelanin
B (the upper case, dominant version) makes black eumelanin pigment (if
b, it would be brown)
D helps make full, rich, undiluted black eumelanin
Now notice this. Almost any variation to the pathway will result in a lack of
black eumelanin and the possibility of defaulting to the paler pigmentation
pathway. There is one major branch of the pathway to get black and several
different modifications to get all of the non-black (paler) pigmentations. Take,
for example, the following potential modifications, taken individually:
1. A (upper case, dominant A) interferes with binding. This is one way
to get a yellowish-colored dog. (There is actually a range of varieties
of A and, depending upon which are present, there will be a range of
yellowish colorations or markings against a darker background), or
2. e (lower case e) does not allow K to bind. We get a cream colored
dog, or
3. c (almost any version of C besides the maximum-strength upper
case C version) gives us a range of pale coat colors, often seen in
Siamese cats.
Consider what I explained in the first two lectures of this course about true
breeding and outbreeding:
A purebred black Labrador retriever that is true breeding for its rich black
coat color (and most likely true breeding for a host of other black Lab traits)
will have the following genes in double copies:
KK EE aa CC BB DD
However, if the dog is not exactly truebreeding for black coat color but rather
carries a gene for brown (or chocolate, as Labrador breeders call it), it will
have the following genes:
KK EE aa CC Bb DD
Lets isolate the only pair of genes with any possibility of giving us variation
in color and ignore the rest:
Bb
And next lets mate two dogs with the Bb combination:
Bb x Bb
B b

B BB Bb
black black

b Bb bb
black brown

And that is how we get a 3:1 ratio.

39
How about a cream- or yellow-colored retriever? There are lots of ways to
do that by deviating just a bit from the black pathway. In fact, for some breeds
of yellowish or cream-colored dogs it is not yet known exactly which gene is
causing the deviation from black to a pale color.
Here are a few of the possibilities varying just one set of genes at a time:
KK EE aa CC BB DD: black
KK ee aa CC BB DD: pale color
KK EE aa cc BB DD: pale color
KK EE AA CC BB DD: pale (often yellow) color
But we are not done yet. I have not explained patches of white, including
spots and stripes. For that we need to consider the packaging gene M and
transport genes S and T for moving pigments. Again, it is difficult to put
these into a simple linear pathway. In a sense, M, S, and T are more impor-
tant than K, E, A, C, B, and D in that we will not be able to distribute pig-
ment all over the animal if we cannot package and transport the pigment uni-
formly. The results of nonuniform packaging and transport are fascinating. It
is how we get striping and spotting and in some cases completely white coat
colors. The crucial activities take place very early in the development of the
embryonic mammal, when it has just a few thousand cells. Along the future
backbone of the embryonic animal, melanocytes either produce eumelanin or
phaeomelanin (according to the genes present). Here is what happens next.
1. The recessive m gene product packages the pigment into specialized
compartments in the cell. Alternatively, it does not package the pigment
quite right. If we have one copy of upper case M, we will get some pale
cells (with incomplete packaging), even if the pigment pathway is produc-
ing black. In addition, we may get odd pigmentations such as one blue
eye and one brown and in some cases a tendency to deafness in one or
both ears. If we have two copies of M, it is lethal. Clearly, something pret-
ty dramatic is going on with M. This is a good example of another impor-
tant, complex pathway interacting profoundly with the pathway we are
examining. Why is packaging of pigments so important and with such far-
reaching effects, including deafness and lethality? (Well get back to that.)
2. Meanwhile S and T get to work and cause those melanocytes to
migrate all over the body of the developing embryo, to one degree or
another. Double copies SS efficiently transport pigment all over the body.
Solid black Labs (or any completely solidly colored animal) are SS. There
are lots of alternative, recessive versions of the transport gene S. It does
not suffice to call them all s. Instead, we need to resort to superscripts,
which is typical in genetic nomenclature. Here is a series of some of four
variants (alleles) of s and their consequences (phenotypes).
S solid-colored animal
LECTURE FIVE

si Irish markings: white on feet, legs, and chest


sp piebald spots
sw extreme white (range of completely white to one or two spots
of black, including just one bullseye-like patch around the eye)

40
Eline Spek/shutterstock.com
A Gypsy Vanner Horse
The Gypsy Vanner horse is the preferred wagon and riding horse of the Romany Gypsies. While the
brightly colored horses with profuse feather, mane, and tail are often called Gypsy Cobs, and Irish
Tinkers, the Gypsy Vanner is a designation bestowed upon the best of the best of a half century
breeding program. The program has the intention of producing the most spectacular and well-bred
Gypsy Horse to complement the culture and colorful lifestyle of the Romany Gypsies.

The T genes (also a series of alleles) effect a more subtle pigment migra-
tion, with versions of t giving some mammals little spots on their noses and
paws, called ticking.
In contrast to the extravagant pigmentations of domestic animals, human
evolution has been rather conservative. Most humans are pigmented along a
short gradient of pale to dark. Exceptions are notable. The National Center
for Biotechnology Information (NCBI) hosts the Online Mendelian Inheritance
in Man (OMIM) site. Once there, use the term melanin as the search word
(see Website of Interest at the end of this lecture).
Many of the hits will be for various laboratory mammals, but some will be for
humans. Note that OMIM tends to present some of the most extreme exam-
ples of a particular gene and therefore some of the descriptions may suggest
that pigment anomalies in humans always have very negative consequences.
That is not the case at all. However, for those descriptions at OMIM, notice
that nervous system disorders often accompany the extreme pigment disor-
der. This is a good example of pleiotropy in a very complex pathway. On the
way to making eumelanin and melanin, there are a few other branches that
lead to production of some neurotransmitters and hormones. That is one of
the reasons that the M packaging gene described above is lethal (or
extremely detrimental) in double doses and somewhat detrimental in single
doses. The gene M does not simply affect color, but also development of
the nervous system. In dogs a double dose of the milder alleles of M mm
results in a coat color called merle, which may include heterochromia iridis
(different colored eyes) and some hearing loss too.

41
Search for Waardenburg syndrome at the OMIM website. That is the
human pigment condition in which there may be a blaze of white hair (against
darker hair) and there may be heterochromia iridis, that is, one dark and
one light eye. Sometimes considerably more severe traits may accompany
the pigmentation, including deafness. Although the details are still being
worked out, it appears that Waardenburg syndrome is caused by a version of
the M alleles. That is, Waardenburg seems to be a version of merle.
Albinism (the condition of having little or no melanin pigmentation) may be
searched at OMIM. Note that in humans, it is a genetic condition of some con-
cern, while in many domestic animals (such as laboratory rats) and some arc-
tic wild animals (such as polar bears), a lack of pigment is normal. How does a
lack of pigment occur? There are many ways in the long, complicated pathway
to derail the production of dark eumelanin. Therefore, there are lots of genes
for albinism. That means that two albino humans getting together to have chil-
dren may not have albinos. It depends on which genes each are carrying.
Lets say that having either aa or bb could result in a deviation from melanin
production. One albino individual is AAbb. The other albino is aaBB. They
have children: all of them are AaBb. All of them have normal melanin produc-
tion. The same could happen in a mating of two pale-colored dogs or cats. It
is results like that, that reveal the complexity of the pigment pathway.
One major factor that has not been included yet in all of this discussion of
genes is the environment. The pigment pathways (and any genetic pathways)
occur within a greater context. That will be introduced in the next two lectures.

C. Burnett
LECTURE FIVE

Left: Waardenburg syndrome is usually inherited in an autosomal (non-sex determining) dominant


pattern. Right: Types II and IV Waardenburg syndrome may sometimes have an autosomal reces-
sive pattern of inheritance.

42

FOR GREATER UNDERSTANDING

Questions and Activities

1. How does the evolution of human pigmentation compare to that of


domestic animals?
2. What is Waardenburg syndrome?
3. Make a little pocket guide (that is, some notes on an index card) for yourself
of dog-coat color genotypes and use it to diagnose the possible genotypes
of dogs that you meet.

Suggested Reading

Clutton-Brock, Juliet. A Natural History of Domesticated Mammals. 2nd ed.


Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1999.

Other Books of Interest

Coile, D. Caroline. Encyclopedia of Dog Breeds. 2nd ed. Hauppauge, NY:


Barrons, 1999.
Helgren, J. Anne. Encyclopedia of Cat Breeds. 2nd ed. Hauppauge, NY:
Barrons, 1997.

Website of Interest

The National Center for Biotechnology Information (NCBI) provides the Online
Mendelian Inheritance in Man page; use melanin as a search word
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/sites/entrez?db=OMIM

43
Lecture 6:
The Importance of Environment:
In the Short Term

The Suggested Reading for this lecture is Michael Rutters Genes and
Behavior: Nature-Nurture Interplay Explained.

One of my favorite genome projects is the canine project, because it is


accompanied by a wealth of information from hundreds of years of dog breed-
ing. Even an amateur breeder of dogs with no particular knowledge of genetics
cannot help but notice the natural tendencies of particular breeds for certain
ranges of behavior (both positive and negative) and predispositions to dis-
eases. If you read a detailed description of a particular breed you will find
information concerning behavior under the rubric of trainability and tempera-
ment. These observations are now being paired with gene sequences and, lit-
tle by little, the genetics of behavior is being deciphered. Whatever is learned
from dogs will be applicable in general to any mammal, including humans.
Dog breeders are usually well aware of the effects of environment on dog
behavior. That is, a dog raised in a calm, disciplined, nurturing household will
have a different set of behaviors from its identical twin brought up in a chaot-
ic, undisciplined, unpredictable environment. Behavior is not simply genetic;
the environmental setting is important. However, dog breeders also know that
they are working within a range of natural (genetic) behavioral tendencies.
For example, you do not have to teach a puppy from a lineage of pointers to
point. It does so on its own. Within that range of natural (genetic) pointing
behavior, however, there is plenty of room for a trainer to bring out nuances,
such as holding the point until commanded to do something else, pointing to
the right game, and pointing at the right distance from the game.
What do geneticists mean by environment? Sometimes they are referring to
the quality of air, water, and food, presence of pollutants, and other such
external parameters, commonly associated with environment. However,
more often, environment means the intimate setting in which genes are
expressed. That setting includes the body, the various organs and tissues,

Small Mnsterlnder on Point


The Small Mnsterlnder is a hunt-
Linn Currie/shutterstock.com
LECTURE SIX

ing-pointing-retrieving dog breed


that reached its current form in the
area around Mnster, Germany.
Small Mnsterlnders bear a resem-
blance to both spaniels and setters,
but are more versatile.

44
and the cells. It includes other genes and their particular products. The first
few cell divisions of a new mammal take place within the influential environ-
ment of the uterus. Those early dividing cells have internal environments
influenced by whatever conditions were in the newly fertilized egg. Organs,
tissues, and cells communicate and coordinate with each other via hormones,
neurotransmitters, and other signaling factors. Often the receipt of a signal
results in expressing one or another gene. Interactions with our own species
and with other species are also part of the environment and influence neu-
ronal and hormonal communications. Whatever genes are present represent
only part of the picture. It is all about the complex environmental context in
which the genes are used or expressed.
Visualize the cookie factory. Not only is there an environment external to the
factory itself, but there is also an intricate internal environment of relation-
ships among the machines. Changing one small parameter, such as the tem-
perature of the oven, can have a cascading effect on other functions.
Nature versus nurture is a favorite dichotomy for arguments about which
is more important: the genes (nature) or the environment (nurture). Well
before any understanding of genetics, our human ancestors recognized that
some aspects of traits seemed to be inherited while others seemed to be
malleable products of training and teaching and other external factors. It is
quite an interesting discussion to try to sort it out for any trait. The problem is
that nature versus nurture is a false dichotomy. It is not an either/or situation
but rather an intricate play of many environmental conditions (mostly unob-
served by us) with hundreds of genes and their interactions (mostly undeci-
phered by us). If you find yourself in a nature versus nurture discussion, try
introducing the useful (more nuanced) concept of predisposition, which
allows much more flexibility and a more realistic picture of how a genetic trait
is manifested. For example:
My family is predisposed to diabetes (but we manage to control it
with diet).
That breed of dog is often predisposed to dominant behavior (but weve
trained our dog not to act on her dominant impulses).
A particular breast cancer gene predisposes about 5 percent of the
women who carry it to getting breast cancer. What about the other 95
percent? There are gene and cell interactions in those women that we do
not fully understand yet. The breast cancer gene seems to be expressed
differently in them.
The separated identical twins seemed to be predisposed to having anxi-
etiesbut one was raised in a calm, stable environment, the other in a
hectic, unpredictable environment, and so you can see some differences
in their behaviors.
Note that this is not predetermination or determinism, which is a politically
charged concept that sends us back to the false dichotomy of nurture versus
nature. Predetermination is used simplistically to describe traits that are
determined solely by our genetics, a situation that never turns out to be true
once the particular trait is examined closely. Nonetheless, you may hear of
predetermination in arguments in which one or another party does not fully

45
understand the complexities of genetics. A famous example is when Edward
O. Wilsons Sociobiology: The New Synthesis was first published in 1975.
The book caused great consternation among those who misunderstood and
thought that the message was that all human behavior is predetermined by
genes. For several years after the publication, research in the genetics of
human behavior was discouraged, out of concern that proving genetic prede-
termination was the goal of such research. Indeed, the arguments were so
heated that they are sometimes referred to as the sociobiology wars. For
example, Richard Lewontin and Stephen Jay Gould (both influential Harvard
professors) and others equated a study of the genetics of human behavior to
be eugenics in the most negative sense, as it was practiced in Nazi
Germany. The most extreme arguments, many of which had political roots,
have been mostly dispelled and the genetics of mammalian behavior, includ-
ing that of humans, is now a thriving research area.
One caveat about the genetics of behavior is that it is easy to fall into the trap
of thinking of single gene traits for complex behaviors, sometimes dubbed by
the popular press with eye-catching names. About that, Lewontin, Gould, and
others were right to be concerned. There is no math gene or psychopath
gene or athleticism gene or warrior gene. How might such misconceptions
arise? Here is an example: A gene in mammals codes for the enzyme
monoamine oxidase MAO, which breaks down neurotransmitters after they
have been used to pass along some neuronal communication. Laboratory
rodents that have had their MAO gene switched off have excesses of some
neurotransmitters and are more anxious than control rats. Some researchers
have examined MAO genes in human families, trying to decipher any correla-
tions with behavior. It appears that certain versions of MAO genes are con-
nected to risk-taking behaviors. Is this difficult to sort out? It sure is. Any
behavior is the end result of complex pathways with dozens to hundreds of
genes involved. Environment in all of its many aspects is important and difficult
to control out of a laboratory setting. Against the wishes of most serious scien-
tists working on MAO and other genes, some popular writers have called it the
warrior gene. A good rule of thumb is that if you encounter a simplistic name
for a gene that purports to be the primary explanation for a complicated trait, it
probably isnt. But it makes a good headline.
There is a famous, ongoing study in Russia that was begun in the 1950s with
silver foxes. It continues to be an excellent example of the intricacies of
behavioral genetics. The goal of the experiment was to take an undomesticat-
ed canine and select the tamest animals every generation for as many gener-
ations as are necessary to get a lineage of domesticated animals. One sur-
prise was how quickly domestication of the foxes occurred, suggesting that it
was not all that difficult for our human ancestors to establish their own lin-
eages of domesticated wolves, which became dogs. Another surprise was
that other traits that were not being directly selected by the researchers
nonetheless accompanied the complex behavioral trait of tameness. For
LECTURE SIX

example, researchers were looking for young foxes that would approach an
offered human hand (neither cowering away nor growling) and they were look-
ing for adults that were calm and even sociable around humans, rather than
hypersensitive and anxious. These tamer foxes were allowed to breed the
next generation, and so on. As each generation became more domesticated,

46
coat colors and shapes changed too, although the researchers were not
selecting for those traits. The foxes became piebald (that is, two-colored such
as black and white) with curly tails, floppy ears, and somewhat flattened
(puppy-like) faces. Their rates of maturation and reproductive cycles changed
such that they retained puppy-like characteristics much longer, even into sex-
ual maturity. This is a phenomenon called neoteny, the retention of juvenile
characteristics. It seems to be a trait of
most well-integrated domestic breeds
of dogs. (Exceptions might be some
dog lineages bred deliberately to be
aggressive.) So what does it mean
that docile behavior and certain coat
colors and ear and tail configurations
as well as developmental changes all
occur together. This is a good exam-
ple of the way genes actually work,
never one single gene at a time, but

Photos.com
rather in complex pathways. Actually,
it makes sense that the docile, socia-
ble puppy-like behavior was accompa- Two domesticated silver fox puppies from the
Russian fifty-year breeding program.
nied by neoteny and cute puppy color-
ing, tails, and ears.
Dog breeders have long noticed that certain coat colors of dogs can be cor-
related with behaviors. Some of the explanation for that will allow us to revisit
the dog coat color pathway elaborated in a previous lecture:
Recall the C series of allele products, the ones just inside of the melanocyte
(pigment cell) at which the pathway branches to make either dark eumelanin
or lighter phaeomelanin or perhaps stops right there to make no melanin at
all. It turns out that another branch from the C allele junction leads to the syn-
thesis of certain hormones and neurotransmitters. Therefore, a failure to
make enough or any melanin results not only in a pale animal but also may
be accompanied by deafness, some eye disorders, and even some differ-
ences in temperament.
Furthermore, the C-alleles are a wonderful, accessible example of the role of
environment in the activity of a gene product. The C enzyme (tyrosinase)
works best in slightly cooler-than-core body temperatures. Siamese and
Burmese cats have versions of C that produce pale coat color, but dark
extremities (ears, feet, nose, tail) where temperature is a little cooler. This
effect can be enhanced and can even fluctuate depending on what tempera-
ture environment the cat is experiencing. It is the same phenomenon in
rodents and dogs with Himalayan pigmentation. Some forms of albinism in
humans are due to the C-series alleles and, in some rare cases, an albino
may darken a little, sometimes in the extremities. Note that in general, young
mammals (including humans) are paler than adults and seem to develop
more melanin as time passes. For example, light blond babies often develop
darker hair through maturity, possibly due to variations in the C-series alleles.
Another example involves the S series of allele products that are involved
with the distribution of pigment from the backbone area to the rest of a

47
mammal during development. A nonfunctional version of S results in a fail-
ure to distribute melanin and therefore is one of the many ways to get a
white animal. Various patterns of stripes and spots are due to somewhat
active forms of S and the most dominant form of S yields a fully colored ani-
mal (assuming all other parts of the pathway function). It turns out that neu-
rons use the same S-allele series to grow out from the backbone area at
around the same time in development as pigment is being distributed.
Zebra striping is a good way to envision that pigment migration. Note that
many mammals are darker along the backbone and paler beneath because
of the way pigments migrate. Therefore, once again, it may be no surprise
that interesting pigmentation can go together with particular neuronal condi-
tions, given the interconnectedness of the pathways.
A demonstration of the importance of environment in plant genetics is one
that can be set up in your kitchen. Boil some red cabbage and save the pig-
mented water. The color is from anthocyanin, which is found in various flow-
ers, fruits, and leaves (including bright red autumn leaves). The pathway for
the production of anthocyanin is complex. The pigment itself is susceptible to
changing color depending upon the pH of the plant cells or sap. Pour some of
the cabbage pigment into each of five glasses. Make a solution of baking
soda in water (a few teaspoons in a cup of water) and have on hand some
white vinegar. These are your basic (high pH) and acidic (low pH) solutions
respectively. Add a couple of drops of acid to one glass and a couple of
drops of base to another. Notice the color change. Try getting a slightly differ-
ent color in two of your other glasses by adding more drops. Leave one glass
of pigment untouched, so that you can compare.
So if you are trying to explain the impor-
tance of environment to someone, use the
visual examples of the dark extremities of
Siamese cats (effect of temperature) and the
beautiful range of purples, reds, and pinks of
anthocyanins in plants (effect of pH).
However, also remind your listener that these
are specially chosen examples to be the
most dramatically visible. All genes and gene
products are affected by environment with
varying results, sometimes difficult to deci-
pher. The superficial pigment genes are just
metaphorical for a much greater phenome-
non. Also, if you have a chance to talk to a
serious dog breeder or trainer, you may find
them to be a wealth of information about
behavioral predispositions, the limits of train-
Chepko Danil Vitalevich/shutterstock.com

ability, and even the correlations between


some coat colors and temperaments.
LECTURE SIX

A Siamese kitten exhibiting traits associated with C-alleles


(pale coat, but dark extremities).

48

FOR GREATER UNDERSTANDING

Questions

1. Can you explain the siamese cat example in the context of the influence of
environment on genes?
2. What insights do we have on the domestication of animals from the silver
fox experiment?

Suggested Reading

Rutter, Michael. Genes and Behavior: Nature-Nurture Interplay Explained.


Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell, 2006.

Other Books of Interest

Grandin, Temple, and Catherine Johnson. Animals in Translation: Using the


Mysteries of Autism to Decode Animal Behavior. New York: Harvest
Books, 2006.
Wilson, Edward O. Sociobiology: The New Synthesis. 25th anniversary ed.
Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press/Harvard University Press, 2000.

Article of Interest

Trut, Lyudmila. Early Canid Domestication: The Farm-Fox Experiment.


American Scientist, vol. 87, pp. 160169, 1999.

49
Lecture 7:
Environment Over the Long Term:
The Genetics of Populations

The Suggested Readings for this lecture are the examples of evolution
in Kenneth R. Miller and Joseph S. Levines Biology (or any other intro-
ductory biology textbook).

In lecture 3, when the idea of looking for ratios of genetic traits was intro-
duced, you may have begun looking for ratios in natural populations, some-
thing that Gregor Mendel might have done too. I suggested some places to
look, listed again below. You will see variability if you look carefully; however,
you are not likely to find Mendelian-type ratios. Those are the results of care-
ful breeding experiments. Something else is going on in natural populations
and figuring that out is the topic of population genetics.
1. Look through a guidebook to wild flowers in your area, watching for ones
that are described as varying in color, typically in the range of blues-pinks-
whites (rather than yellows and oranges). Then set out to find a large
stand of your plant and see if you notice any variations. If you do not, you
may have found a true-breeding population for that trait, or maybe there is
some environmental reason for only one color.
2. Watch for reports of unusual albino or melanistic mutants of wild animals
such as white deer or black squirrels. Sometimes there will be just one
sighting. However, there are interesting cases of populations with excep-
tionally high ratios of color mutations; for example, the population of black
squirrels in some areas of the northeastern United States. If you are in the
right area, try counting them at your bird feeder.
3. Or take any organism of interest and observe enough of them and in
enough detail that you begin to notice variations. Focus in on one or
another readily identifiable traits. Examples of natural genetic variability
that I enjoy are variegated (white or reddish and green) markings in some
clover leafs and pink and white water lilies. I also like to look at photos of
wolves and other mammals to see the variations in coat colors. In an
example below, I suggest counting spots in ladybugs. But really, if you are
going to make a project of this, choose an organism you like (and can
work with easily) because you will have to scrutinize a great many of
them, perhaps at close range.
So lets take my example of a wild flower that can be either pink or white. If
I find a large field of them and count one hundred, would I expect to get any
of the ratios Mendel observed, such as 3:1 pinks to whites? Probably I would
not, because out of the controlled conditions of the laboratory or green-
house, environment plays a major role. Some alleles are selected above oth-
LECTURE SEVEN

ers. Some combinations of genes allow an organism to leave more offspring


than others. Skewed ratios of traits are one of the first things to be noticed
by a population geneticist. Then begins the challenging task of figuring out
why. In the case of that pink pigment, which is anthocyanin, it has been
noticed that in drought conditions, pink is favored over white perhaps

50
because the pigment is conferring some drought
protection. In situations in which dryness seems not
to be a factor, there may be less of a skewed ratio
toward pink.
Pigmentation patterns in populations of mammals can
often be interpreted fairly easily in terms of camouflage
coloration. White animals are rare in temperate and
tropical zones. They stand out against the background
of vegetation. In the arctic, white coat color is a com-
mon trait (polar bears, rabbits, and arctic foxes). The
same pigment pathways are present in all mammals.
Which variants of the pathway are present is a conse-
quence of selection by environmental conditions.
Environment in this context (or any genetics context)
should be interpreted broadly. The environment of a
rabbit includes its predators; more white rabbits survive
predation in the arctic, while more brown rabbits sur-
vive predation in the temperate forest.
What if we have a particular trait in a range of varia-
tions and the particular variation does not seem to
matter much to the survival of the organism. Indeed,
that is the case with some traits, which is why popula-
tion geneticists use some restraint when they try to
come up with explanations for their observations. For
example, sometimes when an organism is new to an
environment and does not yet have any natural
predators, camouflage coloring or warning colorations
can become quite variable. That seems to be the
case with Harmonia axyridis, an Asiatic ladybird bee-
tle, introduced as a natural pest-control insect to the
United States. The patterns of orange coloration of
these beetles are probably a warning of non-palatabil-
ity to their natural predators, but there are no preda-
tors in their new environment. Therefore, a wonderful
array of different markings has been occurring with
no particular discernable patterns of selection. In
contrast, native ladybird beetles have many more
constraints in their warning colorations (their numbers
and arrangements of spots) because they do have
natural predators. Note that some Asiatic ladybird
Tom Biegalski/shutterstock.com

beetles like to overwinter in warm places, such as in


your house. So if you happen to have an autumn
infestation of colorful orange and black beetles,
before you sweep them out the door, you might
examine their varied color patterns.
Population genetics is the genetics that Charles Asiatic ladybugs display
Darwin would have loved to have known about. multiple patterns and
Darwin was fascinated with variations in traits. He was shades of orange to
almost red.
a close observer of variability in artificially selected

51
domesticated organisms, and this strongly influenced his theory of natural
selection. He wrote The Variation of Animals and Plants Under Domestication
in 1868 and did some experiments to try to sort out the way in which traits
were being inherited. For example, Darwin bred snapdragons and according
to the data he collected, the traits were present in a 3:1 ratio, but unrecog-
nized by Darwin. Meanwhile, around the same time, but completely unknown
to Darwin, Mendel was doing the same sorts of experiments with peas and
did manage to recognize a 3:1 ratio and its significance. How close was
Darwin to figuring out some simple patterns of inheritance? Possibly he never
would have spotted and correctly interpreted any ratios in his plant experi-
ments. However, he had one more chance. A copy of Mendels publication is
in Darwins library. If Darwin had read it, he might have understood the signifi-
cance. In those days, inexpensive, bound publications sometimes had their
pages doubled over, uncut, and the reader was obliged to cut each one, to
reveal each new page. Darwins copy of Mendel is still uncut. He never read
it. By the way, a copy of Darwins On the Origin of Species is in Mendels
library and Mendel did read it.
Genetics was the major component missing from Darwins otherwise thor-
ough explanation of natural selection. Genetics was not added into the picture
of evolution until the twentieth century. The two major components of evolu-
tion are:
1. Genetic variability, easily observed but less easily interpreted and analyzed.
2. Selection by a host of environmental parameters for particular traits and
combinations of traits. That is, some combinations of genes allow an
organism to leave more offspring than others.
And then there is the component of time. There must be many generations
of reproduction of genetic variants of organisms accompanied by selection for
particular variants, over and over again. Thus originated the extraordinary
richness of species on Earth as well as all the variants within species.
Population genetics is the genetics that medical doctors are beginning to pay
attention to. It begins with human migrations or more likely less purposeful
wanderings, starting with a population of Homo sapiens in east Africa about
two hundred thousand years ago. Their coloring was dark, and therefore pro-
tective, which makes lots of sense for hairless organisms dwelling under a
tropical sun. There was probably little genetic variability between individuals.
This was a small, interbreeding, therefore inbred population, as were most
human populations through most of human history.
Around eighty thousand years ago (a mere four thousand generations if you
let each generation be twenty years), migrations or wanderings out of Africa
began. By about forty to fifty thousand years ago (just two thousand genera-
tions), Europe, Asia, and Australia had their own small inbred populations of
Homo sapiens. Therefore we are all east Africans with just a few variations
LECTURE SEVEN

due to our diverse wanderings. Around fifteen thousand years ago (less than
one thousand generations) during the ice age, some humans made it to the
Americas and wandered all over. And finally, once-separated populations of
Homo sapiens met after coming full circle around the globe when Europeans
first reached the Americas by boat, less than one thousand years ago (less

52
than fifty generations). In the latter part of the twentieth century some Homo
sapiens began to be extraordinarily mobile (at least those with a desire to do
so and who could afford it). However, we have spent many more generations
not being mobile, with pockets or islands (or valleys) of genetically inbred
groupsmade even more so by cultural differences. Inbreeding has resulted
in recognizably different-looking populations of humans with different skin col-
ors, face shapes, statures, and other recognizable characteristics. In general,
populations along the equator have maintained protective skin pigmentation.
That protection was lost in European populations and then apparently lost at
a different place and time by temperate Asian populations. However skin pig-
mentation is a rather superficial trait compared to what your doctor would
really like to know about you. Therefore, it is becoming pass to fill out a sim-
ple form with just four boxes from which to choose, such as:
Caucasian
African descent
Asian/Pacific Island
Native American
Far more important is a
description of you or me
such as:
Most of my ancestors
are from Sicily, but
my grandmother
always said she was

Julie DeGuia/shutterstock.com
half Native American
and I am about to
marry someone
whose ancestors
have lived in Sichuan
Province of China for
A young Californian of Asian-Caucasian descent.
many generations.
Or:
I am approximately half Inuit and half Norwegian and I am about to marry
a Ghanian who can trace extensive ancestry within the Fanti tribe.
Your doctor is interested because your ancestors spent many more genera-
tions being isolated in Sicily or Siberia or Ghana than they did meeting each
other and intermarrying in the nineteenth and twentieth centuries. Along with
distinctive facial characteristics and pigmentations and other visible traits are
predisposition to particular genetic conditions such as diabetes and cancers.
Knowing which small populations are part of your ancestry will help to individ-
ualize medical testing and treatment.
Lets take the Amish of Lancaster County, Pennsylvania, as an example
of an isolated population by their own choice due to their religious beliefs.
They are a favorite example of human population geneticists. Because of
the inbreeding that naturally occurs in such a population, the Amish have
an unusual prevalence of certain rare genetic conditions, including some

53
muscular, neurological, and metabolic genetic diseases. But you can say the
same for any population, whether geographically remote or isolated by
choice. They will have their own particular suite of genetic predispositions.
Oliver Sacks writes wonderfully about an example of exceptional color blind-
ness in two island populations (one in Scandinavia, one in the Pacific) in The
Island of the Colorblind. What is notable is that the physical remoteness of
the island populations was only part of the story. For many genetic condi-
tions, people make conscious choices to live together. The color blindness in
Sackss examples was accompanied by extreme sensitivity to light and there-
fore a life-style that involved being sheltered indoors during the day and being
more active outdoors at night. So color-blind people were doing more social-
izing (and marrying) within their group than with others. This is sometimes the
case with social groups of deaf people and little people who share major life-
style choices.
The concept of eugenics (good genes) was developed in the 1880s
when some of the science behind heredity and natural selection had been
elucidated. Francis Galton, a cousin of Charles Darwin, first developed the
idea that there might be social programs based in scientific fact by which
humans might be improved. The basic idea was far from new. It is the foun-
dation of thousands of years of selective breeding and improvement (by
some human criteria) of domestic animals and plants. What was new was
the possibility of supporting scientific facts. Galtons goal was to lessen
human suffering by disease, to increase intelligence, strength, and other
desireable qualities, and to conserve limited resources by directing them to
the healthiest, fittest individuals. In just a few decades, eugenics became
associated with (indeed a justification for) the genocides of Nazi Germany.
However, extreme policies and political agendas based on eugenics were
far from being unique to Nazi Germany. Other manifestations of eugenics
(throughout the world, including the United States) included strict immigra-
tion quotas to eliminate less desirable people and sterility programs to pre-
vent some less fit individuals, such as those of less-than-average intelli-
gence, from reproducing.
However, before we dismiss eugenics as the root of some of the most mis-
guided social policies of the twentieth century, lets look at the deeper roots.
Presented here in a list format, approximately from the mildest to the most
extreme, are eugenic-type practices, many of which were and still are entirely
routine ways by which human lineages are improved by some criteria.
Where do we draw the line? Perhaps it is somewhere part way down the list,
depending on your own beliefs and choices. When is it genocide, for all
intents and purposes? And what are the criteria for good traits and how are
they measured? You may disagree with the order in which I present these
and with some of the items on this list or some left off. All of that places many
issues of eugenics well into the realm of the ethics of genetics, not just the
LECTURE SEVEN

science of genetics. Furthermore, some of the most extreme public policies of


eugenics come from pseudoscience (intentional or not) rather than science.

54
Choosing mates for various positive qualities (perhaps
subjectively determined)
Arranged marriages for various positive traits
Social policies calling positive attention to strong marriages (an early Nazi
tactic, but could be an innocently framed policy of encouragement)
Premarital genetic testing
Genetic counseling
Sperm banks and egg banks (including from donors with
desireable characteristics)
In vitro fertilization
Birth control for oneself, or imposed on others (Margaret Sanger was part
of an early eugenics movement)
Sterilization chosen for oneself
Prenatal testing and then decisions about terminating a pregnancy
Shunning or ousting from sects
Limiting immigration (by nationality, by abilities)
Marriage laws (preventing certain marriages)
Gene therapies (if it works; there are no routinely used examples)
Designer gene therapy (if it works; there are no routinely used examples)
Cloning (if it works; there are no routinely used examples in humans)
Institutionalizing people with undesirable traits and thus preventing
their marriages
Rationing healthcare by group (that is, not using it excessively in
hopeless cases)
Selective abortionas done to others
Sterility programsas done to others
Forced emigration
Infanticide
Euthanasia
Genocidead hoc or systematic
By the way, some of the more intriguing outcomes of eugenics policies in the
United States in the early twentieth century were public policies to show more
sympathy to people who were deemed to be defective genetically in some
way. That included reform of some practices with disabled people in institu-
tions and adjustments in the education system to accommodate special
needs. It also included reevaluations of the use of capital punishment, espe-
cially in cases where the defendant might have some genetic deficiency that
led to the crime. Clarence Darrow successfully used a defense of genetic dis-
ability in the famous murder trial of Leopold and Loeb.

55

FOR GREATER UNDERSTANDING

Questions and Activities

1. What is indicated by the many different colorations of Asiatic ladybird beetles?


2. What are the two major components of evolution?
3. Try working up a few detailed examples to use in conversations concerning
the natural variability of animals and plants. Focus on your own favorites
and try to get some experience with them, whether observing from nature
or photographs.
4. Do the same for eugenics by coming up with a range of examples from rou-
tine, innocuous practices to more elaborate schemes that require careful
analysis. The extremes of deliberate and methodical genocides are obvious
examples, but it might be more interesting to have on hand some more
subtle ones.

Suggested Reading

Miller, Kenneth R., and Joseph S. Levine. Biology. Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Prentice Hall, 2007.

Other Books of Interest


Peterson, Roger Tory, and Margaret McKenny. A Field Guide to Wildflowers.
New York: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt, 1998.
Sacks, Oliver. The Island of the Colorblind. New York: Vintage, 1998.

Website of Interest

1. Idea.org provides simulations of various types of color blindness


http://www.idea.org/vision-demo.html
2. The University of Missouri, Kansas City School of Law, provides an in-
depth look at the 1924 murder trial of Leopold and Loeb, in which Clarence
Darrow referred at length to the genetic factors of the case
http://www.law.umkc.edu/faculty/projects/FTrials/leoploeb/leopold.htm
LECTURE SEVEN

56
Lecture 8:
DNA (Deoxyribonucleic Acid)

The Suggested Reading for this lecture is James Schwartzs In Pursuit


of the Gene: From Darwin to DNA. (Also see Isolating DNA from
Strawberries activity on page 102.)

I introduce DNA about halfway through my college genetics course because


I think it is valuable to get a good feel for genes as information without the
complexities of discussing the molecule actually carrying that information. It is
a little like the difference between software and hardware for a computer. The
information of genes is software-like. The DNA molecule is the physical reality
of how that information is encoded: hardware-like. For many decades the
chronology of discoveries concerning DNA as a molecule ran parallel to and
independent of the studies of genetics. At first there were no particular strong
connections between the two.
Dr. Frederich Miescher, a Swiss working in Germany, isolated DNA in 1869
from the pus (white blood cells) of the bandages of surgical patients. Actually,
following a set of modern instructions included at the end of this lecture, it is
not all that difficult to isolate DNA. The end result is a bit slimy or mucousy if
you get enough of it. Dr. Miescher described DNA as a white wolley precipi-
tate. Note that the doctor was looking at all sorts of molecules in pus, trying
to get a better understanding of how to treat wounds.
By 1889, the four bases or building blocks that make up DNA had been dis-
covered and were understood to make up DNA. Here they are in order and
with the original materials from which they were first isolated.

Guanine 1850 from bird guano and later from the silvery sheen of
fish skin
Adenine 1885 from glands such as ox pancreas and spleen
Cytosine 1894 from calf thymus gland
Thymine 1894 from calf thymus gland

These bases usually are referred to as A, C, G, and T. In addition, there is a


fifth base uracil U found in RNA instead of T.
Throughout the twentieth century, there was a growing awareness that
there must be a type of information-bearing molecule in the cell and most
likely in the nucleus. However, until the 1940s, DNA was not considered the
most likely candidate because it seemed too simple with only four types of
building blocks.
Erwin Chargaffs research in the 1940s provided some important clues about
the nature of DNA. He cut up DNA molecules into subunits, counted the
bases, and found that the number of As equaled the number of Ts and the
number of Cs equaled the number of Gs.
Motionstream/shutterstock.com

57
The story of how the structure of DNA was discovered has been reiterated
from many different points of view in several books, recommended at the end
of this lecture. In short, three research groups found themselves in competi-
tion for making the discovery.
Linus Pauling at the California Institute of Technology was already an expert
in the necessary technique, X-ray crystallography, and had already discov-
ered the structure of protein, for which he won a Nobel Prize. Pauling came
close to collecting the Nobel Prize for DNA too.
Meanwhile in England were Francis Crick and James Watson at Kings
College, and Rosalind Franklin and Maurice Wilkins at the Cavendish labs. It
would have been a powerful and versatile team if all four had worked togeth-
er. Watson and Crick were imaginative, adventurous builders of models using
X-ray data, while Franklin and Wilkins were experts in X-ray crystallography,
that is, producing the data. However, the two teams had quite different per-
sonalities and worked separately with only occasional, albeit significant inter-
actions. The day-by-day accounts of their work in 1952 and early 1953 are
well worth reading to get a flavor of how politics, friendships, rivalries, and
communication can affect the outcomes of scientific research.
That DNA was a helix of two strands (like a spiral staircase) was revealed by
X-ray crystallography. That DNA fit together such that each step of the stair-
case was A paired with T or C paired with G was revealed by model building.
T and C are both single-ring structures. A and G are both double-ring struc-
tures. By pairing according to Chargaffs numbers, each step was three rings
side by sideA:T and C:G.
Three back-to-back papers appeared in Nature on April 25, 1953, to
announce the nearly simultaneous discoveries of the structure of DNA. First
were Watson and Crick, then Wilkins and his colleagues Stokes and Wilson,
and finally Franklin and her colleague Gosling. At this point Wilkins and
Franklin were no longer working harmoniously together. In 1962 Watson,
Crick, and Wilkins shared the Nobel Prize. Franklin had died of cancer per-
haps from exposure to X-rays.
In 1953, it was already obvious that the structure of DNA was relevant to
genetics. Watson and Crick wrote the following:
We wish to suggest a structure for the salt of deoxyribose nucleic acid.
This structure has novel features which are of considerable biological
interest. . . . It has not escaped our notice that the specific pairing we
have postulated immediately suggests a copying mechanism.
Apparently Crick wanted to include the word beauty in the paper, but it was
struck by a reviewer perhaps as too emotional. But it is a beautiful molecule.
Try to find a model to look at from all angles, perhaps even a large model at
a science museum. The spiral staircase effect is stunning. Look closely at the
chemical structure and notice something of an M.C. Escher quality to the
LECTURE EIGHT

staircase. The two strands are anti-parallel; that is, one strand is in reverse
orientation to the other. Furthermore, DNA in real life almost never gets the
space and conditions to stretch out as a smooth double helix but rather is
stuffed in a kinked and tangled mass into the tiny confines of the cell nucleus.

58
How does DNA work? The structure allows it to replicate so that if we begin
with a double helix (flattened out) like this:
ACGTTACCGTAC
| | | | | | | | | | | |
TGCAATGGCATG
It can be made to split apart down the middle
(splitting each step) like this:
ACGTTACCGTAC

TGCAATGGCATG
And then each separate strand can have its
complementary strand built. And now there
are two DNA molecules, each bearing the
same sequence (and therefore the
same information).
ACGTTACCGTAC
| | | | | | | | | | | |
TGCAATGGCATG

ACGTTACCGTAC
| | | | | | | | | | | | Roberto Sanchez/shutterstock.com

TGCAATGGCATG
What makes DNA interesting and in fact what makes possible its use as a
biological information molecule is that sometimes the replication isnt perfect.
Think of how tediously identical all DNA would be (and all biological informa-
tion would be) if replication were perfect. It is the lack of perfection that
makes the extraordinary diversity of life possible. We (or any organism) are
the products of millions of little mistakes in replication or mutations. Although
the word mutation sometimes has a negative context (the roots of which will
be addressed further in lecture 11), it really is mostly positive in the biological
sense. That is, it is positive if you consider the vast, versatile range of living
beings and our own amazing complexity as positive attributes. Otherwise the
molecule of life would be nothing but a short string of information precisely
copying itself endlessly for generations in the same little warm pond of its ori-
gin. We certainly would not be here to talk about it.
How about the context of the cookie factory? Those blueprints for every
machine are made of DNA. Building a new factory begins with the copying of
the blueprints.
Sometimes there are errors. Whoever or whatever is doing the copying does
not do so precisely. Therefore every new cookie factory, built with its own
newly copied set of blueprints, is a little different from the others.
One of the original metaphors developed by DNA researchers for under-
standing the function of DNA was that of a medieval monk copying a manu-
script in a scriptorium. In the next lecture, watch for the way some of the ter-
minology used to talk about DNA reflects that metaphor.

59

FOR GREATER UNDERSTANDING

Questions and Activities

1. How did Dr. Frederich Miescher isolate DNA?


2. How is mutation positive in the biological sense?
3. Try isolating some DNA from strawberries and other materials using
reagents made from common household chemicals (see Isolating DNA
on page 102 for instructions).

Suggested Reading

Schwartz, James. In Pursuit of the Gene: From Darwin to DNA. Cambridge,


MA: Harvard University Press, 2008.

Other Books of Interest

Crick, Francis. What Mad Pursuit: A Personal View of Scientific Discovery.


New York: Basic Books, 1988.
Judson, Horace F. The Eighth Day of Creation: Makers of the Revolution in
Biology. New York: Touchstone, 1979.
Maddox, Brenda. Rosalind Franklin: The Dark Lady of DNA. New York:
Harper Perennial, 2003.
Watson, James. The Double Helix. New York: Signet Books, 1968.
LECTURE EIGHT

60
Lecture 9:
Transcription and Translation:
The Metaphor of an Error-Prone Medieval Monk in a Scriptorium

The Suggested Reading for this lecture is James Schwartzs In Pursuit


of the Gene: From Darwin to DNA.

Imagine a medieval monk laboring over a manuscript in a scriptorium. He is


somewhat error-prone and therefore introduces little mistakes (mutations)
here and there. Overall the manuscripts produced are mostly readable and
mostly quite functional. That is, in the short run, many mutations may not
matter much. For example, lots of DNA information is redundant so that a
mistake in one section is balanced by the correct information being else-
where. (And remember the nature of the cookie factory. It was designed by
Rube Goldberg. It works well enough in spite of the baroque convolutions.)
Now and then, the monk introduces an enormous error, such as eliminating
a few pages or badly misspelling the most meaningful word of a chapter.
Here is where selection comes in. We do have a wastebasket for truly egre-
gious manuscripts. That is, if DNA is accidently copied so badly as to cause
an organism not to survive and therefore not to reproduce, then that DNA will
not have opportunities to replicate again.
When the information of DNA is actually used as information (not just replicat-
ed with little errors) the first step is transcription. Think of the medieval monk,
copying (tran-
THE GENETIC CODE
scribing) a short U C A G
bit of a
sequence onto UUU Phenyl UCU UAU UGU U
Tyrosine Cysteine
alanine
a piece of scrap U UUC UCC
Serine
UAC UGC C
paper, for imme- UUG
Leucine
UCA UAA
Stop
UGA Stop A
diate use, down UUA UCG UAG UGG Tryptophan G
the hall in a dif-
ferent section of CUU CCU CAU CGU U
Histidine
the monastery. CUC CCC CAC CGC C
It is as though C Leucine Proline Arginine
CUA CCA CAA CGA A
the main manu- Glutamine
CUG CCG CAG CGG G
script (the main
blueprint) is
AUU ACU AAU AGU U
stored safely Asparagine Serine
AUC Isoleucine ACC AAC AGC C
(and copied reg- A Threonine
ularly in its AUA ACA AAA AGA A
Lysine Arginine
entirety), but in AUG Methionine ACG AAG AGG G
order to actually
build something GUU GCU GAU Aspartic GGU U
we dont want GUC GCC GAC acid GGC C
G Valine Alanine Glycine
the entire set. GUA GCA GAA Glutamic GGA A
We just want a GUG GCG GAG acid GGG G
little section

61
actually, we just want a gene. And here at last is an official definition (albeit
one with lots of exceptions) for gene. A gene is that sequence which can be
transcribed (jotted down on a scrap of paper) and sent elsewhere (out of the
scriptorium, out of the nucleus) for use. Scrap paper is a reasonable analogy
here because the format in which the transcript is made is temporary. The
message must be used within a reasonable period of time; otherwise it gets
torn up. The actual DNA transcript is made of a single-stranded RNA molecule
called messenger RNA or mRNA.
Here is the original DNA:
ACGTTACCGTAC
| | | | | | | | | | | |
TGCAATGGCATG
One strand serves as a template. Lets say it is this one:
ACGTTACCGTAC
A complementary mRNA is made using U instead of T:
UGCAAUGGCAUG
And now UGCAAUGGCAUG, freshly transcribed (message-like) is being
rushed down the hall so that it can be used as instructions to build a machine
before it can be torn up.
How does the message get used? It gets translated; that is, the information
is converted from one language to another. Therefore, down the hall is a set
of bilingual translation machinery capable of reading two languages: DNA lan-
guage and protein language. Organisms are made mostly of protein. Proteins
form almost all of the distinctive structures and functions of the body. By anal-
ogy, proteins are the machines and all the infrastructure of the cookie factory.
The deciphering of the DNA code and its correspondence to protein code
was a major accomplishment of the early 1960s. There are four bases (A, C,
G, and T) in DNA; think of a four-letter alphabet. There are twenty amino acid
subunits of proteins; think of a twenty-letter alphabet. Is it possible to say
twenty different things with words comprising only four letters? Here are some
of the possibilities:
If words in DNA language are of length one, then there are just four words
possible: A, C, G, and T, which is much too parsimonious a language. How
long do the words need to be in order to code for twenty amino acids?

Number of Possible Words


Length of Word (if alphabet size is four)

One Letter Four Words


Two Letters Sixteen Words
Three Letters Sixty-four Words
LECTURE NINE

And the answer is that the DNA code comprises three-letter words (or
triplets), which is more than enough to code for twenty protein words. Indeed,
with sixty-four possible triplets, there are lots of synonyms and even four
punctuation words.

62
The laboratory breakthrough in the discovery of the triplet code was a
little like the breakthrough of Helen Keller, who was blind and deaf. Her
teacher Annie Sullivan simultaneously poured water into Helens hands
and signed the word for water and Helen made the connection. In the lab,
an artificial sequence of messenger RNA was concocted of only uracils:
UUUUUUUUUU...
Then the message was sent through the bilingual translational machinery
and the result was a string of the amino acid phenylalanine:
Phe Phe Phe Phe...
And thus the code was broken.
Each of the twenty amino acids had at least one DNA triplet (DNA codon)
and most had many more than that. For example, here are the four synonyms
of the amino acid serine: UCU, UCC, UCA, UCG. Note that there is a great
advantage to having synonyms. It means that the system can tolerate some
mistakes (mutations) quite easily without changing the meaning of the word.
In addition, there are four punctuation codons:
AUG marks the beginning of a gene (that which can be transcribed); it
also codes for the amino acid methionine.
UGA, UAA, and UAG are synonyms for stop, marking the end of
a gene.
So a simplified gene (with spaces inserted between codons) looks like
the following:
AUG UUU UUU UCA UCG UUU UAG
And it is translated to a tiny protein (typical proteins have hundreds of
amino acids):
Met Phe Phe Ser Ser Phe
Returning to the metaphor of the monk in the scriptorium, lets say there are
two major types of mutations he can cause in the manuscript:
1. He can substitute one letter for another or write the letter such that it is
unintelligible. These are point mutations.
2. He can insert or delete letters. Both have the same effect and unless you
know what the original manuscript was supposed to have looked like, it
can be difficult to tell which happened: an insertion or a deletion of a
letter. That is why these are sometimes called indels.
Many point mutations will have no major effect on the final product (a
protein). That is in part because there are so many synonyms in the lan-
guage. It is also because changing just one amino acid in a long string of
hundreds making up a protein in many cases does not result in any major
change in protein function.
However, indels can have dramatic consequences. They cause shifts in
reading frame, frame shift mutations, which might render the code unread-
able. Consider this example using three-letter words in English:
THE CAT SAT

63
Remove the T and read it:
HEC ATS AT*
Or add a letter:
THE BCA TSA T**
Either way, the remainder of the sentence is spoiled. The same happens in
DNA frame shifts, typically resulting in a nonfunctional protein.
So at this point, lets review what a gene is, and then move on to a bit of
surprising complexity that will necessitate invoking a completely different
metaphor than the monk in the scriptorium.
A gene is defined by its functionality. It is a sequence of DNA with a start
(AUG), a sequence of triplets coding for amino acids, and a stop (for exam-
ple, UAG). It is capable of being transcribed (mRNA, the temporary scrap of
paper) and translated (from DNA to protein language).
Genes of complex organisms (like humans) are themselves complex in that
they comprise coding sequences interspersed with non-coding sequences.
An English example (using three-letter words) might look like this:
THE CAT SAT XZFYYH THE MEN BAT LFFFDCG GLOAAAB WET HAT
In order to make this into plain English (or into plain DNA language), first we
transcribe and then we splice, removing all those intervening, unreadable
sequences called introns. The readable parts are called exons.
THE CAT SAT THE MEN BAT WET HAT
And then we translate that spliced sequence.
It seems a bit convoluted, doesnt it? What are those introns doing in there,
except to add a seemingly unnecessary step?
Time to switch metaphors: Lets say that exons and introns were a building
toy and that there were all sorts of combinations that could be built of vari-
ous exons.
Start with this set of building toys:
EXON#1 INTRON EXON#2 INTRON EXON#3
How many ways can I mix and match (splice) those exons?
There are seven possibilities, assuming they stay in order:
EXON#1
EXON#2
EXON#3
EXON#1 EXON#2
EXON#1 EXON#3
EXON#2 EXON#3
EXON#1 EXON#2 EXON#3
And each of those seven combinations will give us a different (albeit related)
protein assembled in a sort of modular fashion. It means that one gene has
LECTURE NINE

the potential to make many different products. This helps to explain why it is
that with only twenty-five thousand genes (typical of complex, multicellular
organisms), there are hundreds of thousands of potential proteins that can be
made. Our coding capacity has something in common with the possibilities
from a large set of building toys in the hands of an imaginative child.
64

FOR GREATER UNDERSTANDING

Questions

1. Why is scrap paper a good analogy for the way a gene functions?
2. What is the difference between introns and exons?
3. Explain transcription and translation using the medieval monk in the
scriptorium analogy.

Suggested Reading

Schwartz, James. In Pursuit of the Gene: From Darwin to DNA. Cambridge,


MA: Harvard University Press, 2008.

Other Books of Interest

Crick, Francis. What Mad Pursuit: A Personal View of Scientific Discovery.


New York: Basic Books, 1988.
Judson, Horace F. The Eighth Day of Creation: Makers of the Revolution in
Biology. New York: Touchstone, 1979.
Maddox, Brenda. Rosalind Franklin: The Dark Lady of DNA. New York:
Harper Perennial, 2003.
Watson, James. The Double Helix. New York: Signet Books, 1968.

65
Lecture 10:
Chromosomes, Mitosis, and Meiosis:
How DNA Is Packaged for Moving

The Suggested Reading for this lecture is James Schwartzs In Pursuit


of the Gene: From Darwin to DNA.

Thus far in the course, we have considered DNA as information (strings of


letters spelling out the sequences for protein) and DNA as a molecule capa-
ble of being imperfectly replicated. There is an additional aspect of DNA hav-
ing to do with the vast quantities of it packed into the tiny nuclei of each cell.
If all of the DNA from just one human cell were set out end-to-end, stretched
out to full length, forming one string, it would be 1.8 meters long. Thats right:
meters. And all of it is packed into a cell of perhaps 20 micrometers in diame-
ter. Within that cell, the DNA is packed into an even tinier compartment, the
nucleus, about 5 micrometers in diameter. To put that into perspective:
1.8 meters of DNA =
180 centimeters =
1,800 millimeters =
1,800,000 micrometers
packed into a 5 micrometer diameter (0.000196850394) space
Is that a problem? It sure is. Enough of a problem that cells have entire sets
of structures and mechanisms to deal with it. These include chromosomes,
mitosis, and (for special occasions) meiosis. Can anything go wrong with
chromosomes, mitosis, and meiosis? Yes, pretty much anything that can go
right in a cell can go wrong too, and thats what can be so interesting. There
are whole categories of DNA mutations on a grand scale having to do with
the necessities of hauling around such vast quantities of it and the mistakes
that can be made.
Chromosomes (colored bodies) are
enormous rope-like cell structures that
have been observed with ordinary light
microscopes since the nineteenth century.
20 micrometers (m)

Thus the name, implying that chromo- 2


somes readily take up stain and become
colorful, highly visible structures at certain
times in the life of a cell under observation.
In particular, it was noticed that chromo-
somes become enlarged and highly visible
and active right before cell division. Here is
what is happening. Notice that the first step
Gejr Dietzel

cannot be observed under the microscope:


}
LECTURE TEN

1
1. All of the DNA of the cell replicates.
Now there are two copies of each A chromosome that has replicated to
make two copies. The copies called chro-
DNA string, non-visible, under matids (1) are held together temporarily
a microscope. with a structure called a centromere (2).

66
2. The DNA begins to coil up into thick rope-like structures stabilized with
proteins, that is, chromosomesvisible under a microscope. The copies
remain held together by a centromere. (Actually, DNA is in some chro-
mosomal form all the time, but usually more loosely coiled and there-
fore not visible.)
3. The centromeres split. The chromosomes move and separate in an
organized fashion such that each of the two copies of the cells DNA
(from step #1) become poised to separate into two cells (visible).
4. The cell divides; if all has gone well each new cell has a complete set
of DNA (the DNA returns to invisibility under the microscope).
What is going on here is a little like moving day. Lets say you and your
roommate own hundreds of books and are about to go your separate ways
and would each like a copy of every book in your library. Step one: copy all
those books. Maybe hire a medieval monk in a scriptorium to do it. Step two
(the highly visible step): package up all those books into large moving boxes
and shuffle them around such that when you go your separate ways, youll
each have a set of boxes. That is what chromosomes are all about. They are
the moving boxes, packaging long stringy DNA such that it can be more effi-
ciently sorted and separated. The process of shuffling and separating is
mitosis, and it contains all of the steps that you might expect:
1. Pack the boxes (coil up the chromosomes).
2. Pair up the boxes so that we can be sure that each roommate will get
one of a pair.
3. Slide the boxes to opposite corners (my boxes versus your boxes).
4. Move boxes into two new apartments (cell division). Done.
What could go wrong? Plenty! Maybe Ill get two boxes of history books but
no cookbooks and youll get all the cookbooks and no history, which wasnt
the intent, but with all that shuffling it could happen by accident. The conse-
quences (for DNA) turn out to be fascinating. There will be more on that later.
Meanwhile, to help you visualize and to add in a few crucial nuances:

What could go wrong?


Clipart.com

67
We humans have in most of our cells, most of the time, two sets of DNA. One
set originally came from our mother, one set from our father. That DNA is bun-
dled up (loosely or tightly) into 23 different chromosomes. Since we have two
sets of each, we actually have 23 pairs of chromosomes or a total of 46 chro-
mosomes. It is a common misunderstanding among people who have not
studied genetics that DNA and genes and chromosomes are all something like
synonyms. As you know, they arent. Chromosomes, DNA, and genes exist on
completely different scales, ranging from visible to conceptual.
Each chromosome consists of a long strand of DNA tightly (or loosely)
wound up and coiled with the help of proteins. Each chromosome (each DNA
strand) has thousands of gene sequences. That is why mistakes on moving
day can have such dramatic consequences. Im not losing just one cookbook.
Im losing hundreds of them.
When the genome project was not yet underway, there was plenty of specu-
lation as to how genes might be organized on chromosomes. If you are think-
ing about books shelved in libraries, you might be thinking of something as
orderly as the metaphor of boxed books on moving day, such as all the cook-
books in one section, the history books in another. That was certainly a
hypothesis about gene organization too. One big surprise from completed
genome projects of complex organisms was that the genes were arranged
mostly in no particular discernable order. That is, the books are scattered all
over the house. Using a particular set of books (genes) in some coordinated
fashion apparently requires first getting them together somehow. There will
be more about this in lecture 12, but for the moment consider this: The linear
book-shelf metaphor begins to fall apart (as most metaphors eventually do)
when you consider the logistics of 1.8 meters of DNA stuffed into that tiny
nucleus. There is nothing linear about it. So rather than orderly rows of
books, think of a dynamic ball of yarn with lots of potential for creative new
combinations. Think of the yarn as capable of coiling and uncoiling such that
in any given fraction of a second, this or that set of genes might be juxta-
posed in the tangle. That may be how genes are being used in coordination,
despite the seemingly (at least to our human perception) lack of library-order.
By the way, right after DNA replication (right after our 46 chromosomes are
replicated), if you were to go in and count up all the chromosomes, you might
expect 92 chromosomes. We just doubled 46: 46 x 2 = 92. And if you and I
were writing the earliest genetics textbooks, that is what we would say: 92
chromosomes. However, for various reasons, mostly due to some early mis-
understandings about what was important about mitosis, we never say 92
chromosomes. It is in the nature of scientific jargon that often early misunder-
standings become nonetheless petrified permanently in the terminology.
Genetics, being such an early topic of investigation, is rife with difficult termi-
nology and concepts, not just because it is complex, but because of the
amazing stability of vast amounts of jargon, coined well before a full under-
standing of the concepts.
LECTURE TEN

Mitosis (that mostly orderly dividing of chromosomes) is happening all the


time, all over your body, every single time you make new tissues (new skin,
new hair, new intestinal linings, and so on). It is also the mechanism of growth.
From infant (a single cell at fertilization) to adult (with ten trillion cells) there are
many millions of mitoses. Mitosis is also the means by which single-celled

68
organisms reproduce. Mitosis is how cancer cells duplicate themselves out of
control. And that is why rapid mitosis is sometimes the Achilles heel of cancer
cells. Some chemotherapies set about to poison those cells that are dividing
rapidly by poisoning certain aspects of mitosis. That is why side effects of
chemotherapies can affect our other cells that divide frequently, such as hair,
skin, and digestive lining.
There is another way to divide up chromosomes, used only in the special cir-
cumstance of making sperm or eggs. It is called meiosis and it goes like this:
Sexual reproduction (as opposed to simple reproduction by just doing mito-
sis) entails some logistical problems with numbers of chromosomes. Sexual
reproduction is the union of two cells (sperm and egg) to make one cell
(zygote)followed by lots of cell divisions (lots of mitoses) to make an
embryo. So I have 46 chromosomes and you have 46 chromosomes. We
have sexa fusion of sperm and egg, fertilization. Our new zygote has 92
chromosomes. And that is a problem. Somehow, we have to begin this whole
process, several steps back, or well be doubling the number of chromo-
somes every new generation. We need to start by reducing the number of
chromosomes (in the sperms and eggs) in half. That requires a special cell
divisionmeiosiswhich is like mitosis, but instead we go from 46 chromo-
somes to 23 chromosomes, a sort of modified moving day.
Meiosis is very specialized, occurring only in the gonads. It requires two cell
divisions. For this sequence I am going to use the typically forbidden number
of 92 to make the point about what is happening:
1. Start with 46 (23 pairs) of chromosomes.
2. Replicate the DNA so now we have 92 (23 quadruples).
3. Do the first division. Now we are down to 46 (23 pairs).
4. Do the second division. Now we are down to 23 single copies of
each chromosome.
So, sperm have 23 chromosomes, eggs have 23, and zygotes (newly fertil-
ized eggs) have 46, or 23 pairs (one set from each parent).
Is that it? Well, no. With genetics there is always something more. The
following are three (of many possible) additional nuances concerning chro-
mosomes and meiosis.
Chromosomal Mutations
All sorts of mistakes (chromosome mutations) are possible in packing and
moving chromosomes, and the consequences are interesting. The severity of
those mutations depends on which organisms we are talking about. Plants
have a great capacity for tolerating chromosomal mutations, while in humans
and other mammals, most major chromosomal mutations are lethal or have
dramatic negative consequences. There are two major types.
Changes in the number of chromosomes. Recall we have 23 pairs of
chromosomes typically referred to by number, as in Chromosome 1,
Chromosome 2, and so on (or in the moving-day analogy, Box 1, Box 2).
If we move those boxes wrong, I might get an extra copy of Chromo-
some (Box) 3 and you might get none. Meiosis that results means an
extra chromosome or missing chromosome in a sperm or egg turns out

69
to be important enough in humans that the majority of cases are lethal
and miscarriage occurs, or even immediate failure of the fertilized egg to
implant. The major exception in humans is Chromosome 21, in which an
individual receives three copies of that chromosome and the result is a
form of mental retardation called Downs syndrome or Trisomy 21. Other
exceptions in humans are for the sex chromosomes refered to not as
any number, but rather as X and Y. Females have XX and males
have XY. Sometimes there are extra or missing copies of Xs and Xs.
X0Turner syndrome. Women with short stature and some fertili-
ty problems; also in many cases lethal in utero.
XXX or XXXX. These may be discovered as a result of investi-
gating a fertility problem in women.
Xyy. Not often noticed because the characteristics are not typical-
ly followed up with chromosome studies: men with taller than aver-
age height, increased acne, and perhaps a tendency to some
developmental delays.
XXy or XXXyKlinefelter syndrome. Men with some ambiguous
sexual characteristics and fertility problems.

The sex chromosomes and mosaics. The sex chromosomes X and y


present an interesting problem and solution in the way that the genes on
those chromosomes are used. In particular, the X is a large chromosome
with thousands of genes on it, including some for color vision and blood-
clotting factors. Indeed, there is no particular association between the X
and any sexual characteristics.
Males have just one X. That means that for color vision or blood clotting and
a host of other characteristics, there is no dominant or recessive charac-
teristics for genes on the X for males. Whatever version of an X chromosome
gene they have manifests itself directly. That means color blindness and
hemophilia are either present or not, but never hidden as recessives in

Meiosis
LECTURE TEN

Major events involving meiosis, showing chromosomal crossover.


National Institutes of Health

70
males. However, females have two copies of each X chromosome gene and
therefore can be carriers of hidden recessive genes. That is why sex-linked
genes are sometimes referred to as skipping a generation. Sometimes (but
not always) they do skip because they may be hidden in the mothers genera-
tion and then appear suddenly in the sons generation.
Another consequence of two X chromosomes versus one is that the cells of
females compensate to prevent an overdose of the genes on the X. That
means that all over the body of females, in every cell (other than those lead-
ing to eggs), one or the other of the two Xs is permanently turned off. Which
X is inactivated is random and it occurs very early in utero. That means that
females are mosaics of characteristics of the X chromosome, especially for
those genes in which there was a real difference in allelespresent, for
example, as Aa or Bb.
It seems not to matter too much in hemophilia (a disorder of blood clotting)
and color-blindness, in which the female has one copy of the normal gene
and one of the mutated gene. All over her body she is either expressing nor-
mal blood clotting or abnormal blood clotting. However, those traits are not
used all over the body but just in the bone marrow. The rest of the mosaic
doesnt matter. Furthermore, in the bone marrow and in the retina there
seems to be typically enough compensation from the normal cells to over-
come the abnormal cells. This seems to be the case for most color-blindness
too. The cells of the retina of the eye compensate so that females with one
copy of a color-blind gene nonetheless see color.
That is why we switch to cats to get a really striking example of X inactivation
and mosaics in action and highly visible. The gene for black or orange pigment
is on the X in cats and are alleles of each other. Normal males can be either
black or orange but never both. Females carrying both black and orange alle-
les are a wonderful mosaic (called tortoise shell) of black and orange splotch-
es, randomly placed all over the body
and of varying sizes. The size
depends on how early the inactivation
of one or another X occurred. If the
cat was in very early development
just 4 to 8 cellsthe splotches will be
bigger than if it occurred later in the
16 to 32 cell stage. Add a gene for
white (not on the X) to tortoise shell,
and you get a three-colored cat called
a calico.
It is because of X inactivation that
identical twin female mammals,
Eric Issele/shutterstock.com

whether human or cat or any other,


are not identical in respect to their X
chromosome genes. Inactivation
occurs randomly in each. This is
especially important for traits in which
mosaic might matter (although may
A tortoise shell calico kitten.
not be visible to our eyes as tortoise

71
shell is). It is X-inactivation that makes the first cloned cat a rather unfortunate
example. For some reason, researchers chose to clone a calico female, set-
ting up a situation in which the cloned offspring could not possibly have the
same coat color patterns. Sure enough, they look quite different, and keep in
mind that coat color is just a visible manifestation (symbolic) of all the other
thousands of genes on the X, also present in mosaic patterns.

Crossovers (recombinations). DNA, that amazing linear sequence of As,


Cs, Gs, and Ts, arranged in gorgeous double helix, is surprisingly, dis-
concertingly unstable. If you have already had a chance to look at a
three-dimensional model (such as at a museum), you probably have the
impression of an amazingly stable construction not unlike a well-built set
of stairs. Actually, DNA is capable of shuffling sequences from one
strand to another (a process called crossover or recombination).
Sometimes recombinations are between similar (homologous) strands,
as in this English-language example:
The cat sat on the hat.
The dog ate on the mat.
Recombine to make:
The cat sat on the mat.
Where mat crossed over.
At other times, DNA can recombine between completely different
sequences, sometimes even between different organisms (part of a natural
process that humans have called genetic engineering). For example:
The cat sat on the hat.
I like ice cream.
Recombining to:
The cat ice cream.
The last is a new combination that may or may not be nonsensical in a par-
ticular context. DNA is quite promiscuous about crossovers and does them
without regard to making sensible combinations.
Although crossovers can happen any time, there is a special time right at the
beginning of meiosis (the division to make sperm and eggs) during which
homologous crossovers are facilitated and therefore occur in abundance. The
consequences are significant. What it means (in terms of moving day with
boxes of books) is that right before you start sliding those boxes to opposite
corners of the room, there is a flurry of activitybook exchanges from box to
box with no particular rhyme or reason except that it tends to occur with simi-
lar books (similar DNA sequences). So imagine at that crucial time, we are
trading furiously for a few minutesmy Keats poetry book for your Yeats, my
pancake cookbook for your waffle cookbook, etc. Within the context of this
introductory course, there will not be much more elaboration on crossovers,
LECTURE TEN

which would lead to some advanced topics. However, I think the image of the
book exchanges is useful even if just to keep in mind that DNA is full of sur-
prises and complexities.

72

FOR GREATER UNDERSTANDING

Questions

1. How are chromosomes like moving boxes?


2. What is the difference between a gene and a chromosome?
3. What is the difference between DNA replication and mitosis?
4. When (or for what purpose) do cells do mitosis and when meiosis?

Suggested Reading

Schwartz, James. In Pursuit of the Gene: From Darwin to DNA. Cambridge,


MA: Harvard University Press, 2008.

Other Books of Interest


Miller, Kenneth R., and Joseph S. Levine. Biology. Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Prentice Hall, 2007. (Students are encouraged to read the sections on
chromosomes. Any up-to-date introductory biology textbook will also serve
for this purpose.)

Website of Interest

YouTube video clip of chromosomes moving through mitosis


http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=s1ylUTbXyWU

73
Lecture 11:
Mutations

The Suggested Reading for this lecture is James Schwartzs In Pursuit


of the Gene: From Darwin to DNA.

The following is a quotation from the fruit fly geneticist Hermann Muler, taken
from a lecture he gave in 1929. It is a celebration of mutations, which is a
major take-home story of this course. The quotation appeared in In Pursuit of
the Gene by James Schwartz:
All types of mutations, large and small, ugly and beautiful, burst upon
the gaze. Flies with bulging eyes or with flat or dented eyes; flies with
white, purple, yellow, or brown eyes, or no eyes at all; flies with curly
hair, with ruffled hair, with parted hair, with fine and with coarse hair,
and bald flies; flies with swollen antennae, or extra antennae, or legs in
place of antennae; flies with broad wings, with narrow wings, with
upturned wings, with downturned wings, with outstretched wings, with
truncated wings, with split wings, with spotted wings, with bloated
wings and with virtually no wings at all. Big flies and little ones, dark
ones and light ones, active and long-lived and short-lived ones. Flies
that preferred to stay on the ground, flies that did not care about the
light, flies with a mixture of sex characteristics, flies that were especial-
ly sensitive to warm weather. The roots of lifethe geneshad indeed
been struck, and had yielded.
Now it is true that in many circumstances, in particular contexts, any one of
the above-named mutations could be considered negative. For example, flies
with virtually no wings at all placed in competition with winged flies to get at
some limited supply of food would probably be at a disadvantage. That is,
unless the circumstances were such that flying was ridiculous. Lets say that
the flies found themselves having to crawl into tiny crevices to get to the food
Nicolas Gompel
LECTURE ELEVEN

Drosophila is a genus of small flies, belonging to the family Drosophilidae. The red-eyed fly (right)
is Drosophila melanogaster, which has been heavily used in research in genetics and is a common
model organism in developmental biology. The more darkly pigmented fly on the left is one of
many that involve mutations to the pigment pathway for eye and body colors. The terms fruit fly
and Drosophila are often used synonymously with D. melanogaster in modern biological litera-
ture. The entire genus, however, contains more than two thousand species and is very diverse in
appearance, behavior, and breeding habitat.

74
and that the richest sources were in the narrowest passages, excluding the
flies with bulky wings. Indeed, plenty of insects use walking as the preferred
method to get to food and flying is more of an occasionally used evasive tac-
tic for avoiding predators. In many documented cases, insects suddenly on a
remote island (having been blown or washed there), and without their natural
predators, gradually become wingless. That is, when wingless mutations
appear accidentally, those insects end up doing a little better than their rela-
tives, still lugging around large useless appendages.
It turns out that fruit fliesand probably nearly any insect that can be cul-
tured in the labare outstanding subjects for the study of mutations. This
may be because insect and other arthropod bodies have a somewhat modu-
lar construction, such that mutations to one part are isolated from other parts.
Thus you can have a fly with misplaced antennae or extra wings or extra seg-
ments but otherwise normal parts. If you take good care of them in the lab
(lots of food and mates and no competition) they do just fine and are easy
subjects for all sorts of investigations. Fruit fly researchers have a long tradi-
tion of rejoicing in their mutants and even eagerly anticipating as yet undis-
covered ones for the privilege of naming them. Some fly labs even save a list
of good names, awaiting assignment to the right mutant. Here are a few of
the examples. Longer lists easily may be found online.
Tin man: Embryos developing (albeit briefly) without hearts.
Methuselah: Long-lived flies.
Amontillado: Referring to the Edgar Allan Poe story; embryos unable to
hatch, that is, walled in.
Van Gogh: Flies having swirly bristle patterns.
Road block, Gridlock, Sunday driver, and Red Tape: All referring to
blocked neuron communication.
Yippee: From the margin of a lab notebook of an exuberant
graduate student.
Here are some things Id like to reinforce before we do a little analysis of
how mutant came to be such a negative word in common parlance. All
genes are mutantsno exception. Maybe somewhere, sometime, four billion
years ago at the origin of life there was some little sequence of DNA that
qualifies to be called the original sequence, the one from which all others
derive. But after that its nothing but mutations all the way. The entire mecha-
nism of replication is set up so that it is impossible to not have mutations. It is
a normal situation. Alleles of genes are mutations; variations of genes (same
as alleles) are mutations. Mutations are the reason we have millions of
species of organisms and why there is so much variation within each species.
Perhaps it is right that the small subset of negative, detrimental mutations get
most of the research money and the attention of the popular press. After all,
those are the mutations that cause cancers, birth defects, and other tragic
genetic conditions that could be cured if we understood them better. However,
those are the exceptions, and a grander more inclusive understanding of
mutations may help to better inform us when investigating the few noteworthy
negative ones.

75
Science fiction writers may be in part responsible for the popular misconcep-
tions about mutations. According to the Oxford English Dictionary, mutation
was used from the fourteenth to the eighteenth centuries to mean change.
In the nineteenth and twentieth centuries, scientists trying to make sense of
genetics began to use mutation to talk about differences in traits of organ-
isms. In 1938 (according to the OED), mutation was first used in science fic-
tion to imply something freakishly abnormal. However, the stage already was
well set for such a use in that fantasy fiction of the nineteenth and twentieth
centuries abounds with images of the grotesque and bizarre, as in Franken-
stein, Dr. Jekyll and Mr. Hyde, and The Island of Dr. Moreau. It was probably
a fairly easy step to begin assigning the word mutant to monsters.
A misunderstanding of the word mutation has helped fuel misunderstandings
about evolution. A common question is, if mutations are so bad, how could
incremental accumulations of mutations lead to anything good? Look back to
lecture 7 to be reminded of the importance of the environment in selecting
mutants, not necessarily the very best ones (if such a thing exists), but rather
the ones that are merely good enough for a particular place and time.
Evolution is not about attaining perfection and mutations are good or bad
only within the context of their use. The majority of truly negative mutations
are selected out, leaving those that work well enough that the organisms con-
taining them can pass along their DNA to the next generation. The proof is in
the diversity of extant organisms, the products of millions of mutations and
relentless selection.
Another misunderstanding of mutation is about the significance of their ran-
dom occurrences. Again, the question is, How could random changes,
whether good or bad or neutral, ever lead to anything like a functional change
in an organism? The idea of selection is usually the missing component in
this question. If you flip a coin, the appearance of heads or tails will be ran-
dom. If you flip one hundred coins, the results will still be random. However, if
you add in selection, such that for some reason in this particular environment,
at this time it is advantageous to be heads and disadvantageous to be tails,
then by and by you would have a pile of coins consisting only of heads.
The naming conventions for most mutations of any organism (including the
whimsically named fruit fly mutations) follow certain conventions that some-
times inadvertently introduce misunderstandings. So in this part of the lecture,
some of those conventions will be deconstructed or demystified, such that
when you encounter mutant names you will be able to place them into greater
context of your understanding of genetics.

Mutants are typically named backwards.


Take a look again at those fly mutant names. None of them sound very
normal. All imply some sort of aberration. Thus you might be led to believe
that all mutations are about deviations from some normal trait. (And this is
LECTURE ELEVEN

yet another source of the idea that mutations are bad). However, the names
of mutants have more to do with the nature of genetic analysis than with the
actual function of the mutant gene. Recall the cookie factory in the black
box. If all is running smoothly (normally) and edible cookies are emerging
from the back door, it is extremely difficult (with ordinary genetic analysis) to

76
figure out what each machine is doing. It is only when the machines mal-
function one at a time or in various combinations that we can get a glimpse
of what the machines were supposed to be doing. So our first look at the
dough-mixing machine is really a look at its mistakebadly mixed dough
by which we infer that when it is operating correctly dough is mixed correctly.
And now that we have had our first look at the dough machine, it is time to
name it. Unfortunately, what we understand best about the machine so far is
going to be reflected in the newly coined name: Unmixed dough. Months or
years later we may acquire a much more nuanced understanding of the
dough-mixing machine, but too late, the original mutant name withstands.
Here is an example from biology. There are many mutant versions of genes
named cancer genes of one sort or another and these names (being for
genes of great interest) tend to appear in the popular press. You might won-
der how it could be that we humans are carrying around such a load of can-
cer genes and may ask, where did they come from? Actually, none of them
typically are cancer genes; they were just named that way because
researchers caught their first glimpse of them going awry in the context of
cancer. Cancer genes tend to be cell-division genes, either facilitating nor-
mal division or controlling the rate of normal division. As long as cell division
is proceeding along correctly, which it does the vast majority of the time, it is
as difficult to decipher as a well-functioning cookie factory in a black box.
Mutants often are named too simply for what they are.
This point may seem obvious, but nonetheless sometimes needs to be point-
ed out to dispel misunderstandings. Jargon is a sort of shorthand code by
which people who understand the jargon can communicate efficiently. Lets
take the example of colon cancer gene, which might appear in the popular
press. The name has the appearance of singularity as though it were solely
responsible for a trait and therefore a sort of lynch pin or keystone by which
an entire disease might be finally understood and cured. However, by now
you are used to my evocation of the cookie factory. Cell division is complicat-
ed. There are many ways for it to go wrong. Any number of errors might
result in one or another cancerous condition. This is not the colon cancer
gene (or mutation), but a colon cancer mutation, one of many in an intricate
pathway, by which normal cell division in the colon is regulated. And it is typi-
cal of the way in which scientific research is published, that the report of a
colon cancer gene will precede by months and years any further understand-
ing of the greater process of which it is a small part.

Mutants often are named with ridiculous acronyms.


Using long acronyms is the other extreme of naming that, unlike cancer
gene or Van Gogh (the fruit fly with swirly bristles), almost completely
excludes lay persons from any discussion. The custom began innocently
enough with bacterial mutations, of which there were just a few for many
decades. Therefore, simple three-letter acronyms worked just fine and we had
not yet run out of letters in the English alphabet. For example, pen was the
acronym for bacteria mutants with some particular response to penicillin.
Adding superscripts made some distinctions: penr means penicillin resistant
and pens means penicillin sensitive. So far, so good. Except that now there

77
are hundreds of thousands of bacterial genes (mutants) named and we have
run out of letters of the alphabet and acronyms are part of an insider system of
arcane knowledge. And the situation is by no means limited to bacteria. For
many other model genetic organisms, the acronym system is in use, extrava-
gantly so. It took me about a minute in a professional database of biological lit-
erature to come up with this mouse mutant: Cftr-/-hCFTR-G542X. To
deconstruct it a bit, Cf means cystic fibrosis and the / in the middle indi-
cates that the name refers to two genes. Recall that in complex organisms
there are two sets of each gene. So that notation is a little like B/b. Also rele-
vant is that this mutant mouse is something of a human construct; that is,
humans put together this particular set of mutations in order to get a closer
look at a phenomenon relative to cystic fibrosis. What to do about this as a
layperson? On one hand it is a wonderful thing that good research is being
done on cystic fibrosis and however the experts want to name their mutants
really is just fine. But if you should find yourself in a conversation with a genet-
ics researcher who begins to sling around acronyms, you are perfectly within
your right to tell the person to define their acronym and then perhaps for the
duration of the conversation to use some simple English phrase to stand in for
the acronym. Scientists often need a reminder of this sort of thing.
The hundreds of completed genomics projects are changing the way we
name mutants (albeit very slowly).
We humans have only about twenty-five thousand genes and so do mice
and fruit flies and tiny worms called Caenorhabditis. Many of the really fun-
damental genes, such as those that control cell division, are in common.
That is why we can learn so much about the basics of human biology by
exploring the same phenomena (often much more easily) in flies or worms or
LECTURE ELEVEN

Linda Bucklin/shutterstock.com

78
mice. The more we get a look at the sequences of genes for these diverse
(yet not so diverse) organisms, the more we know this to be true. However,
for many decades researchers went separate ways in their naming systems
for mutants. Therefore, a set of human mutations will have one set of
names. That very same set of mutations in flies will have another set of
names (amusing ones most likely), and the same set in mice will go by a
completely different set of names. Now imagine wishing to do a comprehen-
sive search to gather together all of the information available on a set of
mutants. You will need to search each mutant under several names. If you
want to set up a database to pull together the information, you will need
many extra columns to accommodate all the redundant jargon. Slowly the
situation is being rectified and perhaps some day there will be a few hundred
thousand logically named genes that can be easily cross-referenced from
one species to another. It would be nice if each gene name reflected some-
thing about the actual work that the particular machine was doing in the
context of all of the rest of the machines in a pathway. One example is the
white eye mutant of fruit flies. Normal, wild fruit flies have brick-red eyes.
White-eyed fly mutants were discovered almost one hundred years ago and
the name is evocative of the trait. However, now we know that white is a
gene that codes for an ATP binding cassette transporter that moves sub-
stances into cells, not just into fruit fly cells but also human, mouse, worm,
and so on. And it is a very important transporter not just for making fruit fly
eye colors, but for all sorts of behavioral traits in many species. Wouldnt it
be nice if we could research ATP binding cassette transporters without hav-
ing to learn dozens of different names for the same phenomenon? That is
the goal of many researchers trying to organize the databases of information
from gene projects, striving for a more unified, more searchable naming sys-
tem that ultimately will facilitate research.

79

FOR GREATER UNDERSTANDING

Questions

1. How does the mechanism of replication make it impossible to not


have mutations?
2. What are the main problems with the naming conventions for mutants?
3. Explain the backwards-naming tradition for many mutations. (Connect this
back to mishaps in the cookie factory.)
4. Explain the problem with declaring a mutation or gene to be the gene and
not a gene.
5. Why are mutations basically a positive phenomenon in spite of the com-
monly held belief that they are negative?

Suggested Reading

Schwartz, James. In Pursuit of the Gene: From Darwin to DNA. Cambridge,


MA: Harvard University Press, 2008.

Other Books of Interest

Venter, Craig. A Life Decoded. New York: Viking, 2007.

Website of Interest

The University of Georgia website provides examples of the creative naming


system used for Drosophila http://jpetrie.myweb.uga.edu/genes.html
LECTURE ELEVEN

80
Lecture 12:
Regulating the Genes and the
Dramatic Consequences of Regulatory Mutations

The Suggested Reading for this lecture is James Schwartzs In Pursuit


of the Gene: From Darwin to DNA.

The topic of how genes are regulated is advanced, complex, and not even
all that well understood by researchers. However, the consequences of gene
regulation (and of gene regulation gone awry) can be highly visible and often
readily interpretable even by laypersons. Therefore, the goal of this lecture is
not to dwell on the jargon-laden, intricate mechanisms of gene regulation,
the background for which might take an entire course. Instead, the goal is to
empower you to talk about the basics of gene regulation using helpful
metaphors and to recognize confidently the traits of gene regulation in
organisms all around you. Gene regulation is one of my favorite topics to
contemplate and the mutations of regulation are among the most interesting.
Many are quite distinctive and (at least for those not involved with disease)
are even surrealistically beautiful. As for the disease-causing regulation
problems, they are among the most important, including some major,
debilitating birth defects.
Recall the cookie factory. It would seem that we have covered nearly every
aspect of it: the machines (functioning or not), the blueprints for the machines,
the replication (with errors of those blueprints), the shuffling of boxes of blue-
prints on moving day so that they are divided equally, and the importance of
environment to the functioning of the factory both short and long term.
Here is what we have not yet considered. These blueprints (those genes)
are actually pretty simple in their function. Recall that a gene is that which
can be transcribed and then translated to make a protein. To switch
metaphors here (from cookie factory metaphor to a grammar metaphor), a
gene is a simple imperative statement like this:
Make this.
Make what? Make whatever protein is encoded. And thats it. However, it
isnt enough. You might remember the Walt Disney movie Fantasia, in
which Mickey Mouse commands the broom to fetch buckets of water,
but does not indicate how much water or when to stop. Therefore
the broom (and then hundreds of brooms) flood the room with water.
That is what is missing when genes are used as simple
commands with none of the extra nuances of grammar.
What we really need is the following:
Make this.
If it is needed.
As many as are needed or until we have
this number.
If it is in the right place.
David Krupa/shutterstock.com

81
If this is the right time.
If there are other genes making their particular products nearby.
And so on, and so on.
In order to use genes precisely and in a coordinated fashion with other
genes and with the environment, there must be grammar of instruction for
use. That is gene regulation. The grammar exists on either side of (upstream
and downstream) nearly every gene. Recall that humans have about three
billion bases of DNA, but only about twenty-five thousand genes. Those
genes account for less than 10 percent of the entire genome. What is the
other 90 percent of DNA doing? We dont know what most of it is doing, but
some of it is providing the grammar for gene use. There are short sequences
in some of that DNA that can be juxtaposed with other short sequences and
with proteins such that they form complete sentences of instructions:
Make hemoglobin, if you are in a red blood cell, and if more
hemoglobin is needed and until we have enough.
The make hemoglobin part is the gene. All the rest resides up or down-
stream of the gene. How does the sentence get put together? Recall that
DNA is linear only in our imaginations. It is really a tightly coiled, dynamic ball
of twists and kinks. At any given moment, short sequences plus associated
genes may find themselves all kinked up in the same tiny area, forming a sort
of nuanced command.
The nature of gene regulatory grammar seems to be as fuzzy as that of any
natural language, like English. For example, there are many more than one
ways to say something, including ungrammatical and misspelled ways to get
a point across. Dont envision it as being as precise as a computer program.
Rather, it is like noisy party chatter, with all sorts of extraneous noise and
overlapping, mostly unintelligible talk. There are lots of redundancies, lots of
ways to say the same thing and more or less get a good enough result. That
is, in part, a source of the variations we see from individual to individual. And
it is why it can be difficult to tease apart and decipher the grammar, because
if one part of a sentence is mutated, there may be other ways to say the
same thing.
Exactly how all this looks on a molecular level is too advanced a topic for a
single lecture. However, the consequences of regulation (whether it is going
right or wrong) are something fairly easily visualized.
Gene regulation in a normal multicellular organism.
Elbow/eye is your mnemonic for this example. You have the very same set
of genes in a cell of your elbow as you do in a cell of your eye. Yet those
genes are being used in different ways to make either an elbow or an eye. It
is as though each cell had an entire dictionary of words (or simple com-
mands) as any other cell, but was writing a different essay, for instance, The
LECTURE TWELVE

Essay of the Eye, using those words. The grammar and syntax are what
make the essays different. Any functional multicellular organism with its body
parts (both internal and external) in the right places and with its functional
pathways operating in the right sequence is testament to the power and
importance of gene regulation. That said, it is through mutations to gene reg-
ulatory sequences that we have a better view of how it was working.

82
Mutations to gene regulatory system in humans and other mammals.
These are birth defects in their most extreme forms or they are just interest-
ing variations in morphologies and placements of structures in the milder ver-
sions. For example, charcot-marie tooth disease is a birth defect of limb
development including muscles and nerves. It is caused by failures to regu-
late large sets of genes crucial in embryonic limb formation.
However, most variation is within some defined range of normal and simply
gives us interesting genetic differences such as in shapes of noses and ears
and faces in general. Some of the most dramatic examples are found in pedi-
gree dogs, in which human breeders have been enormously successful in
getting dog faces throughout the range from truncated (flattened, even con-
cave) to elongated. As always with most extreme dog morphologies, such
mutations are accompanied by negative side effects such as problems with
eating and breathing. Where does aesthetics cross the line into birth defect
(in the most detrimental sense)? It should be kept in mind that many extreme
breeds of dogs would never survive on their own in the wild in competition for
scarce resources with other canines. Those breathing and eating problems
would perhaps be better defined (under those circumstances) as lethal or
nearly so.
Mutations to gene regulatory system in insects, birds, and fish.
Once you know what you are looking for, you will see these everywhere. Go
to a pet store or a poultry show or leaf through any comprehensive picture
book on a particular type of animal. Notice the wonderful diversity of shapes
and positions of structures. Notice especially the extremes of the fancy
domesticated animals. Maybe you cant explain exactly what grammatical ele-
ments are at play. Dont worry. You are in good company. Most researchers
dont have the full explanations yet either. But rest assured you can say with
some confidence when pointing to a chicken with an enormous
powder puff of feathers covering its head or a goldfish with
its eyes perched on periscope-like appendages: That is
not caused by an ordinary gene mutation. That is a
gene regulatory mutation.
Fruit flies are their own special example because they
are the organisms for which many of the fundamentals
of gene regulation have been worked out and in which some
of the first examples of regulatory mutants were identified.
Also, flies in captivity (given plenty of food and little compe-
tition) can survive quite well with some extreme muta-
tions. When some of the most extreme examples, such
as antennapedia (having legs growing out of the head
where the antennae should be), they looked like the
results of little diabolic (Frankenstein-like) surgeries.
Indeed, some early experiments to try to understand
Spruce Hollow Farm, Walton, NY

these mutations were surgical projects that actually did


elucidate some of the mechanisms at work.

A white-crested black Poland chicken perches on a fence at the


Spruce Hollow Farm in Walton, NY.

83
The regulation of fruit fly genes is very similar to that of human genes, so
much so that a grammar gene for the development of eyes in humans can
be placed into a fly embryo and it works. The proof is in a dramatic experi-
ment in which researchers inserted that human gene everywhere in the body
of an embryonic fly and then turned on the gene simultaneously in a dozen
different places to see what would happen. What happened was eyes (albeit
somewhat misshapen ones) popping out all over the fly. By the way, the
wingless flies and the tin man (heartless) flies discussed in lecture 11 are
also regulatory mutants, as are nearly any arthropod with multiplied or modi-
fied body parts. Such mutations have been essential in arthropod evolution,
for example, resulting in a wide range of numbers of legs in millipedes, cen-
tipedes, crustaceans, spiders, and insects.
Mutations to gene regulatory system in plants.
Plants seem to have a great tolerance for regulatory mutations, so much so
that we can easily take them for granted and not even notice how strangely
varied plant parts can be. Think about all those Brassica in the produce sec-
tion of the grocery storecabbage, brussels sprouts, turnipsall derived
from a skinny wild mustard. Regulatory mutations are the key to how such
variation in morphology occurred.
There are many types of regulatory mutations in plants, but one of my
favorites is fasciation, coming from the word fascia, meaning holding togeth-
er a bundle of sticks. (The word fascism also is derived from fascia.) Think of
a single thin plant stem, say of a wild mustard, and multiply it many fold on
one thick stem, looking a little like conjoined twins. That is how we get the
thick stems of broccoli and cauliflower. Multiply the leaves of Brassica so that
they are packed into a dense sphere and you have cabbage. Multiply small
leaf-spheres more subtlely and in a more spaced-out manner along a stem
and you have brussels sprouts. An entire grammar of multiplications are dis-
played in the produce bins of your grocery store.
Have you ever noticed that the fruits of some large heirloom tomatoes and
peppers look like two or three fruits melded together? And when you cut them
open, there appears to be more than one central core of seeds? Thats fasci-
ation too. Now and then, if you are examining a field of dandelions quite
closely you may notice multiplied stems and flowers: fasciation. And fascia-
tion is your one-word answer to How do we get four-leafed clovers?
Daffodils are one of my favorite plant regulatory mutants and often many dif-
ferent types are featured in the spring at botanical gardens (although any
domesticated flower is likely to have just as dramatic variations). Look
through a comprehensive picture book of your favorite flowers and be
amazed at multiplications and juxtapositions of petals. In the case of daffodils,
they can range all the way from pom-pom-like multiplications of dozens of
LECTURE TWELVE

petals to petite, subtle blossoms in bell-like clusters along a stem.

84

FOR GREATER UNDERSTANDING

Questions and Activities

1. How is the elbow/eye mnemonic helpful in understanding gene regulation?


2. What happened when a grammar gene for the development of eyes in
humans was inserted in an embryonic fly?
3. Plan a garden of regulatory mutations that displays as many varieties
as possible of one plant type, with a focus on size, shape, and numbers
of structures.
4. Come up with your own repertoire of examples of regulatory mutations for
either favorite plants or animals. You may not be able to explain exactly
which mutations are involved but consider major changes in morphology to
be due to some difference in the regulation of sets of genes.

Suggested Reading

Schwartz, James. In Pursuit of the Gene: From Darwin to DNA. Cambridge,


MA: Harvard University Press, 2008.

Other Books of Interest

Andrews, Chris. Guide to Fancy Goldfish. Surrey, UK: Interpet Publishing, 2002.
Green-Armytage, Stephen. Extraordinary Pigeons. New York: Harry N.
Abrams, Inc., 2003.

Website of Interest

1. Spruce Hollow Farm in Walton, NY, provides an excellent website that


includes photographs of a variety of animals living at the farm and showing
variations from breeding http://www.sprucehollowfarm.org
2. Aunty Animal website (Small Animal Fostering and Rescue), provides
images of a wide variety of small animals, including many from fancy
breeds http://www.auntyanimal.me.uk/animalpics.htm

85
Lecture 13:
Viruses, Jumping Genes, and Symbionts:
Whose DNA Is It and What Are the New Philosophical Questions?

The Suggested Readings for this lecture are the Microbe Cards avail-
able from the American Society for Microbiology.

Professor Dyer: Introductory biology textbooks will have a section or chapter


on viruses. Note however that the emphasis will be on pathogens. This will
be true also for textbooks devoted entirely to virology. The field of virology is
slowly catching up with the abundance of presumably non-pathogenic viral
DNA in our genomes and eventually may give at least equal treatment to
non-pathogenic viruses.

Brace yourself. We are near the end of the course and you are about to find
out that most of the DNA we are hauling around is not ours. In fact, we may
have to come up with new definitions for we and us and our. An editor of
the journal Nature (May 29, 2008) wrote:
There is a compelling new take on humankinds place in the worlda
realization that Who am I? cannot be fully answered until it is fully
understood who we are.
The editor was referring to the wild profusion of bacteria, fungi, and viruses
that colonize the human body; unseen passengers number in the trillions.
They contribute so much to human biology that it is difficult to say where the
body ends and the microbes begin.
This lecture is an introduction to the complex, advanced topic of what it
means to have so many genes, so much DNA that is not strictly our own.
Our genomes are mostly viral.
This was and continues to be one of the big surprises from the completion of
the human genome project. We are not quite sure what it means. We just
know that it is the case. We have three billion bases of DNA sequences
(actually six billion if you consider that we have double copies of all of it).
Less than 10 percent of six billion comprise genes (of which we have twenty-
five thousand) that we consider to be human genes, although most are very
similar or identical to those of other animals. A certain unknown amount of
the human genome is about gene regulation. However, most of the human
genome, five-sixths of the six billion bases, has been identified as viral or
viral-like, or perhaps derived from viruses. This includes actual identifiable
viruses and pieces of viruses as well as a category of DNA called trans-
LECTURE THIRTEEN

posons or jumping genes, many of which seem to be of viral origin. Viruses


and transposons have (if they are fully functional) independent activities,
sometimes involving replicating on their own or even jumping around from
one section of the genome to another by recombinations. (See recombina-
tions briefly discussed in lecture 10.) However, many or most seem to be not
particularly active and simply wait passively for the entire genome to be repli-
cated, which of course includes themselves. However, there is much we do

86
not understand about these viral entities. Consider this a frontier of genetics,
If you are chatting with a geneticist at a party, ask for his or her opinion as to
what all that viral DNA is about. Meanwhile, here is a tally thus far:
Total DNA per human cell = 6 billion bases
Viral or viral-like DNA per human cell = 5 billion bases
Our genes = 600 million
The rest of itpossibly regulatory DNA = 400 million

Our cells are full of bacterial symbionts with their own genomes.
About 2.5 billion years ago, complex cells such as what make up humans and
other animals, fungi, and plants began to evolve. This greatly predates multi-
cellularity. The first complex cells were single amoeba-like cells. The evolution
of complex cells occurred primarily
through symbioses between bac-
teria, which, up to that point (since
the origin of life four billion years
ago), were the only organisms.
One major change in the transition
from bacterial to complex cells
was the nucleus, a protective inte-
rior compartment in which DNA
may be stored and used. Another
major change was the acquisition

Louisa Howard
(by the ancestral complex cell) of
bacterial symbionts capable of
performing an especially efficient
Transmission electron microscope image of a thin
form of energy metabolism abun-
section cut through an area of mammalian lung tissue.
dantly enough to support them- The high magnification image shows a mitochondria.
selves and their host. These sym-
bionts, called mitochondria, are
roughly analogous to little furnaces burning fuel for energy and transferring
some of that energy to various cell functions. Each of our active cells is full of
mitochondria, without which there would be insufficient energy to maintain all
of the functions we need. Every time our cells divide, our mitochondria each
divide too and thus maintain their numbers. Mitochondria use oxygen as an
essential part of their activity. A lack of oxygen shuts them down and soon
after every single one of our essential, energy-using cells shuts down too.
After six minutes or so without oxygen, it can be lethal. Thats how important
mitochondria areindeed, so important they can be easily taken for granted.
So lets say in each human cell are about one hundred mitochondria and in
each mitochondrion are about ten copies each of a genome about twenty
thousand bases in size. That means each of our cells has twenty million bases
of ancient bacterial (now mitochondrial) DNA. And most of it is composed of
genes. None appears to be viral. Compare that to the DNA representing our
own genes from the calculation above:
Mitochondrial DNA per cell, mostly mitochondrial genes:
20 million bases
DNA for our genes per cell: 600 million bases

87
Maternal inheritance of mitochondria and mitochondrial diseases.
Each of our cells carries about one hundred mitochondria, which provide
most of our energy needs. They are so important that a lack of function is
lethal. Decreased functions can be seriously debilitating. In healthy humans,
mitochondria are easy to overlook because the essential assumption must be
that they are there and working efficiently enough to keep us alive and active.
However, there are plenty of subtle variations in mitochondria (due to varia-
tions in mitochondrial DNA) from person to person. At one extreme, there are
debilitating mitochondrial diseases that affect those tissues that need the
most energy, such as muscles and nerves. Some milder differences in mito-
chondria may be manifested in variations in our abilities to process energy.
These may result in differences in athletic ability or in fat metabolism or even
in rates of aging, involving maintenance and renewal of tissues. Some of
these traits are of such general importance and part of such complex sets of
cell functions that it can be difficult to sort out the mitochondrial contribution.
One of the first things noticed about some mitochondrial diseases (even
before their mechanisms were understood) is that they are inherited mater-
nally. This is due to the size difference between sperm and eggs. An egg
arrives to fertilization loaded with mitochondria. A tiny sperm arrives with just
one, enough to provide energy to propel itself. That single sperm mitochondri-
on is either lost or its effects are greatly diluted at fertilization. The number of
egg mitochondria is overwhelming. They become the mitochondria of the new
embryo. This means that if a woman has a mitochondrial disease but is
healthy enough to have children, she will pass that disease to them.
Meanwhile, a man with a mitochondrial disease will not. How about tenden-
cies to obesity or athletic ability or other complex traits that sometimes are
associated with mitochondria? Are those inherited maternally? Those impor-
tant traits rely on so many different genes, both our own and those of the
mitochondria, that inheritance does not get manifested in patterns that simple.
Meanwhile, the DNA of mitochondria, passed down from mothers to off-
spring (of both sexes) for many generations serves as a good genealogical
record. By analyzing changes in mitochondrial DNA, it can be established that
humans (as a nearly modern species) began with a small population in east
Africa about one hundred thousand years ago. Sometimes the phrase mito-
chondrial Eve has been used as a metaphor. However, it was not just one
woman, but rather a group of closely related women, from whom all of us
(males and females) received our mitochondria.
Our body surfaces and cavities are loaded with hundreds of species of
apparently beneficial (or at least benign) bacteria.
We humans have about 10 trillion cells. However, we are hauling around
within our body cavities and on all of our surfaces about 100 trillion bacteria.
LECTURE THIRTEEN

Bacterial genomes are smaller than those of complex cells, but they also tend
to be more densely packed with genes (albeit fewer of them), compared to
ours. Lets say bacteria have genomes of an average size of one million
bases, and lets give them just one copy, although lots of bacteria have multi-
ple copies. So how much bacterial DNA are we carrying?
10 trillion (our cells) x 6 billion (our genome, shared mostly with viruses)

88
100 trillion (their cells) x 1 million (their genomes)
Total for us = 6 x 1022, of which 6 x 1021 are our genes
Total for them = 1 x 1020, most of which are their genes
Okay, we (humans) win, by an order of magnitude. But that is far from pro-
viding any complacent satisfaction that we are independent genetic entities.
Indeed, since bacteria outnumber us 10:1, we are considered by some bacte-
riocentric people as bacterial communities with human bodies attached.
The genetic significances of our great diversity of DNA (bacterial and viral) is
mostly unknown, but here are some of the more tangible consequences:
1. Our immune systems are kept on alert so that when we encounter
pathogens we are more ready to deal with them, which is better than if we
lacked bacterial symbionts.
2. Our immune systems are somewhat less likely to produce inappropriate
responses such as asthmas and allergies, because a constant pres-
ence of bacteria seems to keep the immune system more finely tuned
to respond specifically to pathogens.
3. Our digestion and use of nutrients may be more efficient.
4. Our myriad symbiotic bacteria may be placeholders of sorts, preventing
pathogens from getting an easy place to establish themselves.
A new understanding of bacterial pathogenicity.
The vast majority of bacteria on Earth (perhaps millions of species) are not
pathogenic and have no particular associations with us. The few that are
notable pathogens (about fifty species) have some particular characteristics.
The most serious ones, such as Yersinia
pestis (black plague), Borrelia burgdorferi
(Lyme disease), and Mycobacterium tuber-
culosis (tuberculosis), have the ability to get
deep inside our tissues, sometimes by
being injected in. Lyme disease bacteria
are injected by ticks; plague bacteria are
Luigi Chiesa/Centers for Disease Control

injected by fleas. Often the serious


pathogens have no particular free-living
existence and have uncanny abilities to
bypass our immune system defenses.
Tuberculosis bacteria actually live inside
our cells. Many serious pathogens are out
of synchrony with the usual rhythms of our
cell divisions. Rather than waiting (as mito- This photomicrograph reveals the pres-
chondria) do to time their divisions with our ence of spirochete, or corkscrew-shaped
bacteria known as Borrelia burgdorferi,
own, and thus maintain steady manageable the pathogen responsible for causing
numbers, pathogens overrun their hosts Lyme disease. These bacteria are trans-
using up resources and releasing toxins. mitted to humans by the bite of an infect-
ed deer tick, and caused more than twen-
Many appear to be new in their relation-
ty-three thousand cases of Lyme disease
ships with us. These characteristics are so in the United States in 2002.
remarkable that they provide clues as to

89
what makes a pathogen and how pathogenicity evolves. Being a pathogen
(especially a lethal one) is actually a tough life (for the pathogen). Every time
they kill a host, they are left (possibly) in a dead end situation, unable to get to
a new host. The tendency in the evolution of bacteria/human relationships
seems to be one of increasing synchrony and subtlety, away from extreme
pathogenicity and closer to benign and even beneficial interactions. The vast
majority of our personal bacteria are in just those sort of benign to beneficial
relationships with us.
A new understanding of viruses.
The lessons of bacterial pathogenicity may be applied to viruses and indeed
must be applied if we are to understand the enormous load of viruses we
carry in our genomes. The history of virology has been a history of patho-
genicity for practical reasons: we cannot easily see viruses, but rather we
detect their presence by looking for their activities. The easiest viral activities
to observe are those that disrupt some normal situation; that is, we see dis-
ease and nothing else of viruses. Therefore, there is a long tradition of equat-
ing viruses with diseases. There simply were no ways to observe viruses in
benign or beneficial relationships. It now appears that the vast majority of
viruses (like the vast majority of bacteria) are not associated in any way with
humans. Of those that are associated with humans, the vast majority are
benign or maybe even beneficial in ways that we do not yet understand.
There are about twenty-five seriously pathogenic viruses. Like the bacterial
pathogens, these unusual (but well-studied and publicized) viruses tend to
get injected deep into tissues, evade immune system defenses, and perhaps
most importantly, have new relationships with us and a lack of synchrony with
our cell processes. Just as with bacteria, this is a tough existence for a lethal
virus. The evolutionary tendencies seem to have been in the direction of
greater synchrony and more subtle manifestations (or none at all). The evi-
dence is that our DNA is mostly viral and yet we are not all suffering from
constant, relentless viral pathogenicities.
By the way, you may need a handy answer to the old question (which actu-
ally is a false dichotomy): Are viruses alive or not? You cheerfully answer:
Neither. They are information encoded in DNA or sometimes RNA. Viruses
are genetic entities just as genes are. And we dont ask about genes, Are
they alive or not?
Plant viruses that you can easily observe.
Plants as usual are amazingly tolerant of some rather exotic (from a human
point of view) genetic conditions. Some viruses and some transposons or
jumping genes (which are virus-like) implant themselves in pigment genes
and other plant genes. They pop in and out, resulting in splashy variegation
of color in leaves and flowers. Look through any comprehensive flower cata-
LECTURE THIRTEEN

logue and watch for the most extravagant colorings: splotches of one pig-
ment against another, streaks of color, irregular spots, the more unstable
and bizarre looking, the better. Whether or not it gets acknowledged in the
print of the catalogue, the showiest variegations are probably due to viruses
or transposons. One hint might be if the colors are described as not breed-
ing true or unstable.

90
At the art museum, look for seventeenth-
century Dutch paintings of tulips. The
wildly variegated Rembrandt type, some
versions of which can still be purchased
today, were a huge sensation in the sev-

Sebastian Barnard/shutterstock.com
enteenth century. Speculation on exotic
tulips (complete with fortunes being made
and bankruptcies being filed) was called
tulipmania. The tulip variegation that
was so desired by speculators was due
to viral or transposon activities.
In the grocery store, you may find deco- A Split tulipa modern variety of the sev-
rative colorful ears of corn at harvest enteenth-century Rembrandt tulip.
time. Look closely, perhaps at hundreds
of colored kernels to find just one kernel with speckles, due to transposons.
That is how transposons were first discovered by Barbara McClintock, as rare
events in corn pigmentation. For many years, transposons were mistakenly
thought to be just that: rare events occurring in plants and nothing much
more. It makes all the more surprising the news of their ubiquity and abun-
dance in all genomes, especially of complex organisms.

Elena Elisseeva/shutterstock.com

91

FOR GREATER UNDERSTANDING

Questions

1. What effect does lack of oxygen have on mitochondria?


2. Why are mitochondrial diseases inherited maternally?
3. What is a virus?
4. Are viruses alive? (Or explain why that is not an appropriate question.)

Suggested Reading

The American Society for Microbiology (http://www.asm.org) distributes


Microbe Cards. These are flash cards of viral, bacterial, and other
pathogens. They are a quick source of information by which one might try to
further understand some of the common parameters of pathogenicity.

Other Books of Interest

Comfort, Nathaniel C. The Tangled Field: Barbara McClintocks Search for


the Patterns of Genetic Control. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press, 2003.

Website of Interest

The National Center for Biotechnology Information (NCBI) provides the Online
Mendelian Inheritance in Man page; for information about mitochondrial muta-
tions, use mitochondria as a search word; note that some hits will involve
mitochondrial conditions that are actually coded for by genes in the nucleus;
to be sure that it is a maternally inherited mitochondrial disease,
also use the search phrase maternal inheritance in quotes
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/omim
LECTURE THIRTEEN

92
Lecture 14:
Conclusions

The Suggested Reading for this lecture can be any current textbook on
genetics, for example, Daniel Hartl and Elizabeth Joness Genetics:
Analysis of Genes and Genomes.

You have just listened to a course in basic genetics. Here are the major top-
ics weve covered, phrased with confidence about your achievements. You
have a repertoire of examples and metaphors by which you understand basic
genetics principles better and you can even explain some aspects to others.
You are also ready for further reading on topics of special interest to you.
You can recognize some Mendelian ratios and simple genetic traits. You
also can explain why there are no simple genetic traits and why that is just
a convenient way to talk about some traits.
You understand some phenotypes as spectrums of possibilities in multi-
gene pathways, where many of the genes may be represented as allele
series. You realize that most traits (perhaps all traits?) are parts of com-
plex pathways.
You try always to think of genes in context of environment. You have a
broad definition of environment that includes other genes, the cellular (or tis-
sue or organismal) environment, and the environmental parameters outside
of the organism (including other organisms).
You have a growing appreciation for the importance of DNA mutations (DNA
variability) of all sorts in evolution. Those mutations (of which there are
many different types and degrees) are an essential part of the simplified
events of natural selection.
You have some interesting examples of chromosome activities and disor-
ders and an idea of how some of those disorders occur.
You can recognize or at least make good educated guesses about regulatory
mutations. You are aware of the importance of gene regulation in the context
of pathways. This is an advanced topic, but you have some accessible
examples in your repertoire.
You can recognize or make good guesses about some patterns of non-Men-
delian inheritance, such as of mitochondria, and can explain how those occur.
You can recognize the possible influences of viruses and/or transposons on
phenotypes. (And you know that those phenotypes are mostly non-pathogen-
ic.) And you are as puzzled (and eager to learn more) as any professional
geneticist is about the enormous load of viruses and transposons we carry.
You have a better idea about that most difficult concept in any science, distin-
guishing between what we know, what we think we know (but could be mis-
taken), what we dont know, and what we dont know yet that we dont know.
And you might agree with me that if you could invite guests from the past to
a dinner party, youd like to invite Gregor Mendel and get into a wonderful
discussion with him about how far we have come with genetics.

93

FOR GREATER UNDERSTANDING

Questions

1. What are the most prevalent misconceptions about genetics?


2. How has your own perception of genetics changed over the course of
these lectures?

Suggested Reading

Hartl, Daniel, and Elizabeth Jones. Genetics: Analysis of Genes and


Genomes. 7th ed. Sudbury, MA: Jones & Bartlett Publishers, 2008.

Other Books of Interest

Wells, Spencer. The Journey of Man: A Genetic Odyssey. New York:


Random House, 2004.
LECTURE FOURTEEN

94
GLOSSARY

Adenine: One of the building blocks of DNA or RNA, typically called A.


Albino: Having little or no pigment (typically melanin pigment).
Allele: A variant form of a gene. Genes have many alleles. New alleles occur through
mutation. Any given animal or plant usually has just two alleles at a time for each
gene. If both alleles are the same, that is a homozygous condition. If they are differ-
ent, that is heterozygous.
Asexual: Reproduction by division and without the extra steps of making gametes
(sperm and eggs or pollen and ova) and getting them fertilized.
Bacteria: Microscopic single-celled organisms with simple cell structure that comprise
the vast majority of organisms on Earth.
Birth defects: Deformities and malfunctions (ranging from major to minor) in organs,
limbs, or systems that occur during development. Many genetic birth defects often are
not well enough understood to have any particular gene associated with them.
However, for those birth defects that have been well analyzed, the associated genes
typically are found to involve the regulation of many other genes at some critical point
in the development of an embryo.
Caenorhabditis: A model organism for the understanding of cells, genes, and develop-
ment. It is a tiny (one millimeter long) nematode worm with just about one thousand
cells, easily viewed because its body is nearly transparent.
Cascade: A metaphor (of a waterfall) used by biologists to describe most pathways of
chemical reactions that occur in organisms. Biological pathways are not linear,
although textbooks sometimes depict them linearly. Rather, they branch in complex
patterns, often resulting in many different products. (See cookie factory)
Chromosome: Colored body, a structure comprising a strand of DNA tightly coiled
with various proteins, often seen under the microscope during cell division when DNA
must be sorted and moved into each of two cells.
Chromosome mutations: A change in the structure or number of chromosomes, often
due to a mistake during cell division (mitosis or meiosis).
Clones: Genetically identical (or nearly identical) organisms; For asexually reproducing
organisms (the vast majority of organisms) this is easy and routine. Just divide a cell
and all of its genes into two cells. For obligately sexual organisms like mammals, in
which cloning never occurs naturally, there have been some laboratory methods
developed that approximate the cloning typical of asexual organisms.
Complement: In genetics, this often refers to the relationships or base pairings
between the building blocks of DNA and RNA. For example, in DNA C complements
(or binds, or base pairs with) G, and A complements T. In RNA, U is used
instead of T to bind A.
Cookie factory: A metaphor used in this course to describe pathways of chemical
reactions controlled by genes. It may be more useful than the metaphor of a cas-
cade (see cascade). In addition, it is best to think of the cookie factory as being con-
voluted and full of extra elaborations such as what Rube Goldberg might design.
Crossover: Also called recombination; a natural tendency of DNA strands to exchange
pieces with each other. This is especially enhanced during some cell processes such
as during first steps of meiosis. It is easy to get crossovers to occur in lab and this is a
fundamental technique of genetic engineering.
Cytosine: One of the building blocks of DNA or RNA, typically called C.
DNA: An elongated molecule comprising building blocks (or bases) As, Gs, Cs, and Ts,
forming two linear chains intertwined and bound together to form a double helix. This

95
GLOSSARY

is the primary information molecule of all organisms. The sequence of As, Cs, Gs, and
Ts encodes the information.
Deletion: Used in two ways by geneticists: it can refer to a mutation to a strand of DNA
in which a base (A or C, or G or T) is accidentally left out. It can also refer to a major
change in a chromosome in which a piece is missing. That piece could represent
thousands of bases.
Determinism (predetermination): Describes a misunderstanding about genetics
that has occasionally caused great controversy. It is the idea that having a particular
gene or set of genes essentially guarantees an organism to have a particular trait
regardless of any of the usual complexities typical of genetic systems, such as
the environment.
Dominant: A relationship between alleles (variants of genes) such that in any pair of
alleles, if one of them is able to manifest its particular trait regardless of whether it is
present in one copy or two, then it is dominant. Sometimes there is no clear-cut domi-
nance of one allele over another. Both alleles of a pair get to manifest their traits.
They are co-dominant. Sometimes it matters whether an allele is present in one or two
copies. If two copies, the trait is manifested more strongly than in one. This is called
either partial dominance or a dosage effect.
Drosophila: A model organisma small fly, easily cultured and displaying many
wonderful, highly visible, and fascinating mutant traits. Much of what we understand
about the workings of fly genetics informs genetics in general for all animals, includ-
ing mammals.
Egg: A gamete of female organisms containing one set of chromosomes. The female
has two sets of chromosomes and must undergo a specialized cell division, meiosis,
in order to produce a gamete. In plants this is an ovum.
Environment: All of the parameters both biological and physical ranging from the imme-
diate environment within a cell or within an organism or within a uterus to the greater
outdoor environment. It also includes interactions of all kinds between organisms.
Epistasis: Used by geneticists to refer to activities of a gene (or allele of a gene) that
produces a product used early in a sequence of chemical reactions. The activities of
that early gene product can have profound effects on all subsequent chemical reac-
tions, including stopping them from occurring. In the cookie factory, epistasis occurs
early on the assembly line (such as with a malfunctioning dough mixer) and the result
is that no subsequent machine on the assembly line can get its job done properly.
Eugenics: An appropriation of some aspects of geneticsincluding some simplifica-
tions and misunderstandings about geneticsto develop political and social policies.
The goals typically are to make improvements in a population by allowing some to
reproduce and others not. The means by which this is accomplished can range widely
from voluntary birth control and abstinence to forced emigrations and genocides.
Breeders of pedigree animals practice a version of eugenics, although it typically is
not given that name.
Eumelanin: Dark melanin pigment that comprises the coat, skin, and eye colors of
many animals as well as parts of some fungi.
Exons: Sections of a gene that contain sequences that eventually may be translated to
protein. But first, all the intervening sequences (introns) must be clipped out and the
exons connected up (spliced) into one continuous string.
GLOSSARY

Expressivity: Geneticists use this word when they realize that a particular gene
(or allele) is not operating in any simple manner, easy to interpret. Probably much
more work will need to be done to figure out where and how this allele is working in
a pathway with other genes. Meanwhile, to describe the range of possible traits for

96
GLOSSARY

the allele, variable expressivity is used. An example is white patterns in dog coat
color, produced by a complex system only partly understood. Some of the alleles
involved yield various numbers and arrangements of white patches and spots for no
easily interpretable reason.
Frame shift: When a base (A, C, G, or T) is either inserted into or deleted from a DNA
sequence for a gene, it can change the reading frame and render the rest of the string
unreadable. Genes are read in words or codons of three. In English (using just
three-letter words), a frameshift of THE-CAT-SAT might result in an unreadable
sequence: HEC-ATS-AT due to the deletion of the first T.
Fungi: A large and diverse group of mostly microscopic organisms, including molds,
yeasts, and mushrooms.
Gamete: A generic term for specialized reproductive cells with one set of chromo-
somessperm, egg, pollen, ova.
Gene: A sequence of DNA that has (or recently had, or at least theoretically has) the
potential to be transcribed and translated into protein.
Gene therapy: An idea still mostly in an experimental phase that some genetic dis-
eases might be cured by inserting (by genetic engineering) a normal functional copy of
a gene to replace the function of one that is causing a disease. The resulting patient
would in essence be genetically modified.
Genetic code: The set of 64 triplet combinations of A, C, G, and T (such as ACT,
GGG, GAT) and the corresponding amino acid (protein building block) or stop (to
indicate end of a gene) for each triplet.
Genetic counselor: A medical professional trained to interpret genetic tests and
to provide information and advice to patients with concerns about particular
genetic conditions.
Genetic cross: Taking two sexual organisms and putting them together such that their
gametes fuse and produce an offspring (or potential offspring). Asexual organisms
(that is, most organisms) dont need to be crossed.
Genetic disease: A condition serious enough to be called a disease and not merely a
trait that is a result of having a particular gene or set of genes operating differently
than what is considered in the range of normal.
Genetic engineering: Laboratory procedures taking advantage of the natural tendency
of DNA to easily recombine (crossover) and to be easily inserted into other organisms.
In the natural world, DNA is constantly recombining and being taken up by other
organisms. In the lab, these functions are facilitated and directed for particular purpos-
es, such as developing gene therapies.
Genetic testing: Examining either gene sequences themselves or the protein products
of gene sequences to determine whether a gene is functioning within some range con-
sidered normal. This can also be done on a larger scale by examining visually entire
sets of chromosomes (also called karyotyping) to see if there are any changes.
Genetically modified: Characterizes organisms that have been genetically engi-
neered to contain foreign genes, usually for some purpose, such as to confer
resistance to disease.
Genome: All the DNA contained within an organism.
Genotype: The genes (often represented by symbols) of an individual that contribute to
a particular trait or phenotype.
Guanine: One of the building blocks of DNA or RNA, typically called G.

97
GLOSSARY

Heirloom: A colloquial name for a strain of domesticated plant or animal implying that it
is true breeding (or at least true breeding for a certain set of traits) and has been for
many generations, having been selected and propagated by humans long ago.
Hybrid: An organism resulting from a cross of two organisms that are true breeding (or
at least true breeding for certain traits). The hybrid offspring may show traits of one or
another parent and, in some cases, traits that result from combinations and interaction
between the two parental sets of genes. The hybrid offspring will not be true breeding,
which is why seeds saved from hybrid plants will not yield reliably the characteristics
of the parents.
Inbreeding: Matings between related organisms.
Insertion: A mutation of DNA by which one or more bases (As, Cs, Gs, and Ts) are
added into a DNA strand.
Intron: An intervening sequence of DNA within a gene that is like a spacer between
sections of DNA that code for proteins. The coding sequences are exons. The introns
eventually are spliced out.
Jumping gene: See transposon.
Karyotype: A methodical examination of the chromosomes of an individual to deter-
mine the number, shapes, and patterns of the chromosomes.
Maternal inheritance: The passing along of genes (including viral genes and mito-
chondrial genes) that reside mostly in the cytoplasm of a cell. Egg cells of many sexu-
ally reproducing organisms often are larger than sperm. Therefore, eggs easily pass
along their various cytoplasmic genes. Thus, those genes are maternally inherited;
they come from the mother.
Meiosis: The process of moving chromosomes during a specialized cell division such
that each newly divided cell potentially receives half the number of chromosomes
compared to the starting cell. This is the division that occurs in ovaries and testes to
produce eggs and sperms. In plants, the gametes are pollen and ova.
Melanin: A dark pigment, typically of animals and fungi. In animals, it is found in the
coat (or hair), skin, and eyes.
Melanistic: Characterizes an organism typically found in the wild with a pale skin or
coat or eye color, but in this case has a mutation that confers darker pigmentation
(albinism is the opposite situation).
Mendelian: (After Gregor Mendel) a simplified genetic investigation in which the
researcher sets up experiments and/or gets results that elucidate the activities of one
or two or just a few genes.
Merle: A coat color of mammals that occurs when melanocytes (melanin-filled cells) fail
to package melanin efficiently and therefore produce patches of pale skin, coat, and
eye color that should have been darker with melanin.
Mitochondria: Membrane-bound structures within complex cells in which occur chemi-
cal reactions relevant to transferring and storing energy. Mitochondria originated as
free-living bacteria and still retain vestiges of their bacterial genomes.
Mitosis: The process of moving chromosomes during cell division such that each newly
divided cell potentially receives a complete set of chromosomes identical to that of the
starting cell.
GLOSSARY

Modifier: Refers to a gene that produces a product that has some moderate, often sub-
tle but enhancing or reducing effect on the product of some other gene.
Mosaic: Refers to genes that are expressed in a sort of patchwork pattern in and on a
female mammal. In particular the genes are on her two X chromosomes. During

98
GLOSSARY

development one or another X chromosome is shut off in each cell of the females
body. Therefore, in some areas one allele of a gene may be expressed and in other
areas a different allele.
Mutation: A change in sequence to a DNA molecule or a change on a larger scale to
the structure of or number of a chromosome.
Nature versus nurture: A false dichotomy suggesting that the characteristics of an
organism might be either due to the genes of an organism or to the environment of an
organism, but not to any combination or interaction of the two. There is no need to
choose one or another. Genes are expressed within the context of environments.
Environments influence the expression of genes.
Neoteny: The retention of juvenile characteristics in the development of an organism.
This is often the characteristic (albeit a complex one) sought by animal and plant
breeders whether they realize it or not.
Nucleus: The membrane-bound compartment of complex cells in which most of the
DNA for the organism is stored. However, other DNA such as for some viruses and in
mitochondria is outside of the nucleus in the cytoplasm.
Outcross: A cross of two organisms that are true breeding (or at least true breeding for
certain traits), resulting in a hybrid organism, displaying traits from a combination of its
parents genes.
Pathogenicity: A relationship between two organisms, a pathogen and its host or
between a virus and its host, marked by a lack of coordination of reproductive cycles.
This may result in the pathogen growing more quickly than the host and affecting the
host by using up resources and producing wastes. Pathogenicity is often connected to
a newness of the relationship and may also include certain functions that allow the
pathogen to avoid detection and defensive responses by the host.
Pathway: As used by biologists, a set of sequential chemical reactions, albeit often
highly redundant, convoluted, and baroque, by which particular functions are accom-
plished by a cell.
Penetrance: A word used, often accompanied by a percent (as in 25 percent), to refer
to the chance of a particular organism with a genotype actually displaying the pheno-
type for those genes. Why arent all organisms (100 percent) displaying a phenotype
that corresponds to a particular genotype? Usually, the answer to that is unknown;
there is likely to be a large, complex pathway with functions hidden (to us) that results
in individuals responding differently to having the same genes.
Phaeomelanin: A lighter version of the mammalian (and fungal) pigment melanin.
Phenotype: The description of a particular trait associated with a genotype.
Pleiotropy: Characterizes a gene that functions early in a large, multibranched pathway
such that its function may result in several different traits at the end of the pathway.
Population: In genetics, it is a group of organisms of the same species, typically in a
geographic area such that they seem to be more or less related to each other. If sexu-
al, it is expected that a population would be interbreeding.
Predisposition: A tendency for an individual with a particular gene or set of genes to
manifest a particular phenotype as long as environmental conditions and other gene
products (perhaps with unknown functions) are manifested as well.
Protein: A linear molecule comprising a string of amino acids (of which there are twen-
ty commonly used ones); think of a string of beads with the beads being of twenty dif-
ferent colors. And then consider that proteins never remain in a linear format but
rather become folded and twisted in particular ways to make distinctive shapes.
Proteins do most of the work of facilitating chemical reactions to occur in organisms.

99
GLOSSARY

They also comprise most of the structures of organisms. Gene sequences code for
the sequences of amino acids in proteins.
Punnett square: Reginald Punnett had the idea of arranging the genes involved in a
genetic cross on a table (called a Punnett square) so that the various combinations
could be readily seen.
Recessive: A relationship of a pair of alleles such that a particular allele is expressed
(produces a phenotype or trait) only if it is present in two copies, that is, with no other
competing allele.
Recombination (also crossover): The common tendency of DNA strands to
exchange pieces with each other, especially when the sequences have some
similarities (are homologous).
Regulation: In genetics, it is the organization and coordination of gene use, such that
complex, multipart, time-sensitive traits result. For example, during the development of
the eyes, hundreds of genes must be turned on and expressed in order to make such
a complex structure.
Replication: Making a complementary copy of a DNA strand. Since DNA is double-
stranded, each of the two strands gets a complement and thus one strand is replicat-
ed to two.
RNA: A single-stranded linear molecule comprising four bases A, C, G, and U, and with
many diverse functions. These include messenger RNA, which delivers sequence
information (on a metaphoric piece of scrap paper) from a gene to be translated to
protein. RNAs also include ribosomal RNA and transfer RNA, both of which are part of
the translational function.
Selection (artificial, natural): If natural selection, it occurs when a set of environmen-
tal conditions allows organisms with a particular set of traits to reproduce more effi-
ciently and abundantly than organisms with a different set of traits. If artificial, the
process is controlled by humans who allow organisms with one set of traits to repro-
duce and prevent reproduction in others. Thus, for example, dog breeds quickly
evolve, especially if the human breeder is determined to make that happen. Darwin
was greatly influenced by the practices of artificial selection when he develop the
concept of natural selection.
Selfing: A mating easily done with many plants such that the male (pollen producing)
parts of a plant fertilize the female (ova producing) parts of the same plant. Selfing
requires that a plant have both male and female parts, which many do. It also requires
that the plant not have mechanisms to prevent self-fertilizations.
Sexual: A type of reproduction requiring the production of gametes (such as sperm and
eggs) with half the number of chromosomes of the parents. The gametes then fuse
and produce offspring.
Species: A surprisingly difficult concept to define: if an organism reproduces sexually,
then others of its species are those with which it presumably could mate and produce
offspring that in turn were fertile. However, few species have been put to the actual test
and many instances are known in which organisms assumed to be of different species
mate. Furthermore, many organisms have both sexual and asexual reproduction or are
exclusively asexual. In those cases, defining a species is more about observing enough
similar characteristics (at least from a human point of view).
GLOSSARY

Spectrum: Used in genetics to refer to a range of traits (or phenotypes) resulting


from a range of variations (alleles) for a gene or set of genes. In the cookie factory
metaphor, we get a spectrum of cookies with various icings and decorations resulting
from a spectrum of subtle differences in the frosting and decorating machines.

100
GLOSSARY

Sperm: A gamete of male organisms containing one set of chromosomes. The male
has two sets of chromosomes and must undergo a specialized cell division, meiosis,
in order to produce a gamete. In plants, the corresponding cell is pollen.
Splicing: An activity that occurs with a newly transcribed messenger RNA, which con-
tains both exons (sequences that code for parts of proteins) and introns (sequences
that interrupt the exons). Splicing occurs to cut out the introns and join together the
exons into one continuous coding sequence.
Substitution: A mutation of change to a sequence of DNA such that one base is
replaced by another.
Thymine: One of the building blocks of DNA , typically called T.
Transcription: The process of making a messenger RNA complementary to a
sequence of DNA that is a gene. Indeed, genes are defined in part by their being
able to be transcribed.
Translation: The process following the transcription of a gene, forming a messenger
RNA. The information in the message is converted to a string of amino acids, forming
a protein.
Transposon: Also called a jumping gene. A sequence of DNA capable of moving
around in the chromosome and sometimes making extra copies of itself. It also
includes sequences that appear to have once had the capability but no longer do.
True breeding: Organisms with a set of genes for a set of traits of interest in which
those genes are all in the homozygous condition. That is, each gene is represented by
a pair of identical alleles. This means that during reproduction, when sorting out of
various alleles occurs, there isnt any sorting out to do. The true breeding parents and
all of their offspring have the same allele pairs.
Twins: In sexual organisms, identical twins result from the splitting of a fertilized egg
and presumably have identical or nearly identical sets of genes. Fraternal twins are
from two different eggs, each with its own fertilization by different sperm. Asexually
reproducing organisms produce identical twins all the time, by simple cell division or
by various plant propagation mechanisms. However, the word twin is not typically
used. Instead the word clone is more typical.
Uracil: One of the building blocks of RNA, typically called U and capable of pairing
with A.
X chromosome: A chromosome of mammals (and some other animals), comprising a
large set of diverse genes, none of which are particularly associated with sex develop-
ment. Females have two X chromosomes and males have one. The other chromo-
some in males is the Y chromosome.
X inactivation: Male and female mammals have different numbers of X chromosomes.
A mechanism to compensate for that (to even up the numbers) is X inactivation. It
occurs early in female development. In each cell one or another X is shut off or inacti-
vated. Therefore, effectively, females have just one functional X per cell.
Y chromosome: A small chromosome of mammals (and some other animals) bearing
just a few genes, some of which are essential to producing male primary and sec-
ondary sex characteristics. Males have one (or in rare cases two) Y chromosomes.
Females have none.
Zygote: An egg newly fertilized with a sperm.

101
ISOLATING DNA

Isolating DNA from Strawberries


1. Cut up one strawberry and place it in a zip-lock bag.
2. Add about 50 ml (1/4 cup) of chilled extraction buffer (ingredients below).
Extraction Buffer (keep in refrigeratormakes enough for three extractions)
1/8 cup (33 ml) of simple shampoo (without conditioner) OR dishwashing liquid
2/3 tsp (5 grams) salt
1-1/8 cup (300 ml) water
The shampoo contains a detergent sodium lauryl sulfate that
breaks apart cell membranes, releasing the DNA and other cell
contents. The salt helps to make a solution that is similar in
saltiness to the inside of the cell.

3. Seal the bag and squish the strawberry and extraction buffer,
being careful not to squirt the mixture from the bag.
4. Pour the strawberry mixture through cheesecloth to remove
the pulp.
5. Pour the red liquid into a tall, narrow vessel.
6. Slowly pour ice-cold ethanol (grain alcohol) down the side of the vessel to form a
layer on top of the red liquid. (Keep the ethanol in the freezer until you are ready to
use it.) If grain alcohol is not available, try using the strongest proof vodka available.
7. Notice the filmy white DNA precipitating where the ethanol and red liquid
are interfaced.
8. Use a glass roda toothpick or skewer will also workto gently spin some DNA
from the interface.
9. Note the thready, mucousy nature of the DNA.
10. When done, rinse all solutions down the sink and wash glassware.

Strawberry procedure described on www.carnegieinstitution.orgDiane Sweeney, Pearson Education

Isolating DNA from Other Materials


Strawberries work well for isolating DNA because they can be macerated by hand in a
plastic bag. Bananas and kiwi fruit work too. For tougher fruits and vegetables or fresh
(or frozen) meat, you need to chop finely and then run through a blender. If you do
choose to try animal tissue (meat), the best choice is organ meat, like liver or kidney or
sweetbread (thymus), frozen, or as fresh as possible.

Isolate Your Own DNA


1. Start with 1 tsp of salt dissolved in a glass of water PLUS another glass in
which you have placed 1 tsp of dishwashing liquid and 3 tsp water. ALSO have
ice-cold alcohol greater than 100 proof. (This could be vodka, gin, whiskey, or
rubbing alcohol.)
2. Wash your mouth vigorously for 30 seconds with the salty water and then spit into
the glass with the diluted dishwashing liquid.
3. Stir this firmly for several minutes.
ISOLATING DNA

4. Then gently layer on the ice-cold ethanol down the side of the glass.
5. Look for spindly white threads forming in the alcohol.

This method is from Mike OHares How to Fossilize Your Hamster: And Other Amazing Experiments for the
Armchair Scientist (New York: Henry Holt & Company, 2008).

102
COURSE MATERIALS

Suggested Readings:

Clutton-Brock, Juliet. A Natural History of Domesticated Mammals. 2nd ed.


Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1999.
Hartl, Daniel, and Elizabeth Jones. Genetics: Analysis of Genes and
Genomes. 7th ed. Sudbury, MA: Jones & Bartlett Publishers, 2008.
Microbe Cards. The American Society of Microbiology. http://www.asm.org/
Miller, Kenneth R., and Joseph S. Levine. Biology. Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Prentice Hall, 2007.
Rutter, Michael. Genes and Behavior: Nature-Nurture Interplay Explained.
Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell, 2006.
Schwartz, James. In Pursuit of the Gene: From Darwin to DNA. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press, 2008.
Other Books of Interest:

Andrews, Chris. Guide to Fancy Goldfish. Surrey, UK: Interpet Publishing, 2002.
Brown, Jack, and Peter Caligari. An Introduction to Plant Breeding. Oxford:
Wiley-Blackwell, 2008.
Coile, D. Caroline. Encyclopedia of Dog Breeds. 2nd ed. Hauppauge, NY:
Barrons, 1999.
Comfort, Nathaniel C. The Tangled Field: Barbara McClintocks Search for
the Patterns of Genetic Control. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press, 2003.
Crick, Francis. What Mad Pursuit: A Personal View of Scientific Discovery.
New York: Basic Books, 1988.
Goldberg, Rube. Rube Goldberg: Inventions! New York: Simon &
Schuster, 2000.
Grandin, Temple, and Catherine Johnson. Animals in Translation: Using the
Mysteries of Autism to Decode Animal Behavior. New York: Harvest
Books, 2006.
Green-Armytage, Stephen. Extraordinary Pigeons. New York: Harry N.
Abrams, Inc., 2003.
Helgren, J. Anne. Encyclopedia of Cat Breeds. 2nd ed. Hauppauge, NY:
Barrons, 1997.
Judson, Horace F. The Eighth Day of Creation: Makers of the Revolution in
Biology. New York: Touchstone, 1979.
Maddox, Brenda. Rosalind Franklin: The Dark Lady of DNA. New York:
Harper Perennial, 2003.
Mawer, Simon. Gregor Mendel: Planting the Seeds of Genetics. New York:
Harry N. Abrams, Inc., 2006.

103
COURSE MATERIALS

Other Books of Interest (continued):

Peterson, Roger Tory, and Margaret McKenny. A Field Guide to Wildflowers.


New York: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt, 1998.
Robinson, Heath. Contraptions. New York: The Overlook Press, 2007.
Sacks, Oliver. The Island of the Colorblind. New York: Vintage, 1998.
Venter, Craig. A Life Decoded. New York: Viking, 2007.
Watson, James. The Double Helix. New York: Signet Books, 1968.
Wells, Spencer. The Journey of Man: A Genetic Odyssey. New York:
Random House, 2004.
Wilson, Edward O. Sociobiology: The New Synthesis. 25th anniversary ed.
Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press/Harvard University Press, 2000.
These books are available online through www.modernscholar.com
or by calling Recorded Books at 1-800-636-3399.
COURSE MATERIALS

104

You might also like