Difficulties in Learning Basic Concepts in Probability and Statistics: Implications For Research
Difficulties in Learning Basic Concepts in Probability and Statistics: Implications For Research
Difficulties in Learning Basic Concepts in Probability and Statistics: Implications For Research
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend
access to Journal for Research in Mathematics Education.
http://www.jstor.org
DIFFICULTIESIN LEARNINGBASICCONCEPTS
IN PROBABILITYAND STATISTICS:
IMPLICATIONSFOR RESEARCH
JOAN GARFIELD, University of Minnesota
ANDREW AHLGREN, University of Minnesota
TEACHINGPROBABILITY
AND STATISTICS
College Level
Stochastics as a scientific discipline is usually first taught at the college
level. (We will use the term stochastics to refer to the study of probability
and statistics, as is common in Europe.) The introductory course is usually
divided into three areas: descriptive statistics, probability theory, and infer-
ential statistics (Borovcnik, 1985). The topics typically included in each area
are listed in Table 1. Over the past 20 years, much of the literature on
teaching stochastics has been at the college level. This literature has been
filled with comments by instructors about students not attaining an ade-
quate understanding of basic statistical concepts and not being able to solve
applied statistical problems (Duchastel, 1974; Jolliffe, 1976; Kalton, 1973;
Urquhart, 1971). The experience of most college faculty members in edu-
cation and the social sciences is that a large proportion of university students
in introductory statistics courses do not understand many of the concepts
they are studying.
Table 1
Typical Content of an Introductory Statistics Course (College Level)
Descriptive statistics
Measures of central tendency (mean, median, mode)
Measures of variability (range, variance, standard deviation)
Measures of position (percentiles, z scores)
Frequency distributions and graphs
Probability theory
Rules (addition, multiplication)
Independent and mutually exclusive events
Random variables
Probability distributions
The binomial distribution
The normal distribution
Samplin
Centrallimit theorem
Inferential statistics
Estimating parameters (mean, variance, proportion, correlation coefficient)
Testing hypotheses
Precollege Level
Most of the recent literature about precollege stochastics instruction,
which is more diverse than the college literature, falls into four categories:
statements concerning the need for instruction at the precollege level
(Pereira-Mendoza & Swift, 1981), suggestions for how to teach at the pre-
college level (Duncan & Litwiller, 1981; Ernest, 1984; Swift, 1983); de-
scriptions of the curricular role stochastics can play (Richbart, 1981), and
descriptions of the difficulties secondary school students have understanding
the concepts (Carpenter, Corbitt, & Kepner, 1981; Hope & Kelly, 1983;
Shaughnessy, 1981). Only the last category is of interest here.
Research on students' understanding of probability is more extensive than
research on statistics and has developed as an area separately from those
studies cited above, which focus on stochastics in general. There have been
two distinct lines of research on probabilistic understanding: One has fo-
cused on school children (e.g., Fischbein, 1975; Green, 1983; Piaget &
Inhelder, 1978), and the other has focused on college students and adults
(e.g., Konold, 1983; Tversky & Kahneman, 1982).
At any level, students appear to have difficulties developing correct intu-
ition about fundamental ideas of probability for at least three reasons. First,
many students have an underlying difficulty with rational number concepts
and proportional reasoning, which are used in calculating, reporting, and
interpreting probabilities (Behr, Lesh, Post, & Silver, 1983). Results re-
ported by the National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP) from
the second and third mathematics assessments indicated that students were
generally weak in rational number concepts and had difficulties with basic
concepts involving fractions, decimals, and percents (Carpenter, Corbitt, &
Kepner, 1981; Carpenter, Lindquist, Matthews, & Silver, 1983). Low per-
centages of students had correct responses to exercises involving complex
concepts and skills requiring understanding of underlying mathematics prin-
ciples. Difficulties in translating verbal problem statements plague stochas-
tics as they do the rest of school mathematics (Hansen, McCann, & Myers,
1985). Second, probability ideas often appear to conflict with students'
experiences and how they view the world (Kapadia, 1985). We discuss this
conflict below in the section on misconceptions in statistical reasoning.
Third, many students have already developed a distaste for probability
through having been exposed to its study in a highly abstract and formal
way. For this reason, Freudenthal (1973) cautioned against teaching any
technique of "mathematical statistics" even to college freshmen.
FOR OVERCOMINGDIFFICULTIES
RECOMMENDATIONS
Several recommendations from teachers for overcoming difficulties in
learning stochastics can be generalized from the literature cited above.
Teachers should-
1. introduce topics through activities and simulations, not abstractions;
2. try to arouse in students the feeling that mathematics relates usefully
to reality and is not just symbols, rules, and conventions;
3. use visual illustration and emphasize exploratory data methods;
4. teach descriptive statistics alone without relating it to probability;
5. point out to students common misuses of statistics (say, in news stories
and advertisements);
6. use strategies to improve students' rational number concepts before
approaching proportional reasoning;
7. recognize and confront common errors in students' probabilistic
thinking;
8. create situations requiring probabilistic reasoning that correspond to
the students' views of the world.
The articles cited that recommend instructional methods contain different
mixes of logical argument, teacher intuition, classroom anecdotes, and ex-
hortation on the importance of stochastics topics in schooling. An excellent
collection of this genre is the 1981 NCTM yearbook (Shulte, 1981). These
recommendations are based almost entirely on experience of what has not
worked and speculation about what might work. However, little empirical
research has focused on the effectiveness of the different instructional
methods or teaching approaches in developing statistical and probabilistic
reasoning.
Recently, some research on problem solving has shown that students
receiving deliberate instruction in how to solve problems do become better
problem solvers and are better able to "think mathematically" (Schoenfeld,
1985). What may be needed is similar research on statistical instruction and
students' ability to "think statistically." In an attempt to help students think
statistically there has been some interesting experimentation with the role
of computers in learning stochastics.
USEOF COMPUTERSIN TEACHINGSTOCHASTICS
The increasing prevalence of computers in schools has already had some
influence on the teaching of stochastics and is producing its own literature.
Computers have been used in several ways to aid in the teaching of intro-
ductory courses in college. Students may access large mainframe computers
and use statistical packages, such as SPSS (Norusis, 1986) or MINITAB
(Ryan, Joiner, & Ryan, 1976), to do the number-crunching operations for
them (Bialaszewski, 1981) or have problem sets assigned by the computer
and then get assistance from the computer in working the problems (Edgar,
1973). Attempts have also been made to use large-system programs to have
students run simulations (Stockburger, 1982). Some mainframe applications
have made use of special graphics terminals to give the students graphic
displays in addition to numerical output (Nygard, 1983). Mainframe com-
puter application is very limited, however, because of the high cost of ter-
minals and a lack of access to special ports.
Microcomputers are more promising than mainframe computers in teach-
ing statistics because they are less expensive and have fairly good graphic
capability. Some recent classroom applications have made use of graphics
to display histograms (Vandermeulen & DeWreede, 1983) or to compute
and display distributions of means of samples drawn from a specific popu-
lation (Thomas, 1984). Graphics-oriented software is now commercially
available to use in supplementing instruction in introductory courses at the
college level (Doane, 1985; Elzey, 1985). The Quantitative Literacy Project
has also made microcomputer activities an important component of its
materials.
The literature contains suggestions as to how microcomputers in schools
provide opportunities to incorporate stochastics into secondary mathemat-
ics courses (Collis, 1983; Fey, 1984). There is, nonetheless, little research
on the efficacy of computers in guiding the design of optimal instruction.
An exceptional research effort to use microcomputer graphics to dem-
onstrate statistical processes to high school students is the design and use of
an Apple II program to illustrate the central limit theorem (Johnson, 1985).
The graphic display of Johnson's program shows samples being taken one
element at a time from a discrete population (one of the several selectable),
indicates the position of the mean for each sample after it is complete, and
moves that mean down to an accumulating distribution of the sample means
at the bottom of the screen. The rationale was that the entire sampling and
distributing story is presented concretely and continuously, with no steps
that the student has to imagine or fill in. Johnson gave microcomputer
presentations to two stratified random samples of students, one group re-
ceiving the dynamic presentation and the other receiving a very similar but
static microcomputer presentation. Delayed retention tests after 2 and 6
weeks showed the dynamic presentation group was significantly better at
identifying likely samples and likely distributions of sample means. Johnson
also noted an interesting side effect: Students watching the dynamic pres-
entation showed particular interest in whether successive sample means
would fill gaps or balance asymmetries that remained in the accumulating
distribution, sometimes even cheering when this happened. There was some
evidence that these students may have (by selective memory) come to believe
that such "missing" means are more likely to occur. Subtleties like this effect
demonstrate the need for integrating research and instructional develop-
ment.
RESEARCHIN COGNITIVEDEVELOPMENT
In general terms, we know what some of the important sources of the
difficulty in learning stochastics are. One already mentioned is students'
level of specific mathematics skills. There is also students' general mental
maturity. A variety of studies of the cognitive development of students in
senior high school indicate that perhaps as many as half cannot think on a
formal operational level (e.g., Herron, 1978; Smith, 1978). For example,
they do not completely grasp proportionality, hypothetical argument, or the
concept of controlling variables (Cantu & Herron, 1978; Green, 1983;
Milakofsky & Patterson, 1979; Stonewater & Stonewater, 1984). Green
(1983) also reports that pupils' verbal ability is often inadequate for accu-
rately describing probabilistic situations.
For many students, a considerable improvement of skills in dealing with
abstractions may be necessary before they are ready for much of the prob-
abilistic reasoning and hypothesis testing that underlie basic statistical in-
ference. For some students, teachers may have to be content to forgo ab-
straction and to convey what statistical ideas they can in simpler, concrete
terms. It is noteworthy that introductory units in the ASA-NCTM Quanti-
tative Literacy Project have avoided virtually all computations, using instead
plotting and counting methods and using medians rather than means, even
for fitting prediction lines in bivariate plots.
Beyond the underlying skills problems, however, there is an even more
serious source of difficulty: the students' intuitive convictions about statis-
tical phenomena. The second NAEP mathematics assessment produced evi-
dence that students' intuitive notions of probability seemed to get stronger
with age but were not necessarily more correct (Carpenter et al., 1981).
Fischbein (1975) also found decrements in probabilistic performance with
increasing age, which he attributed to school experience and to scientific
reductionism. Students' intuitive ideas, presumably formed through their
experience, may be reasonable in many of the contexts in which students
use them but can be distressingly inconsistent with the statistics concepts
that we would like to teach them. Green (1983) found that performance in
recognizing randomness declines with pupils' age. He hypothesized that two
opposing tendencies are involved: maturation on the one hand and a dom-
inance of scientific deductivism on the other, which "stifles the appreciation
of randomness by seeking to codify and explain everything" (p. 774). As
has been found in recent research on students' intuitive ideas about natural
science, students' ideas cannot be ignored or dismissed. Indeed, it is difficult
to drive such ideas out even with concentrated efforts to do so.
IN SCIENCE
MISCONCEPTIONS
There is already a sizable body of research on pedagogical implications
MISCONCEPTIONS
IN STATISTICAL
REASONING
Identifying Misconceptions
Most of the research on inappropriate statistical reasoning has been done
not by educators, but by psychologists. Piaget and Inhelder (1975) are often
cited for initiating the developmental research that has helped reveal diffi-
culties students have with theoretical conceptions of probability. They have
been criticized, however, for using a solely classical approach to probability
and for basing their experiments on theoretical concepts and proportional
reasoning-which often appeared to be games comparing fractions, with
little connection to ideas of chance (Kapadia, 1985).
More recently, an area of inquiry referred to as judgment under uncer-
tainty has emerged. Key studies in this area were brought together in a book
edited by Kahneman, Slovic, and Tversky (1982). Several articles address
the idea of representativeness, which refers to varieties of the idea that an
occurrence is probable to the extent that it is "typical." In the combinational
form of this misconception, for example, the probability of a man's being
black and being unemployed is judged to be higher than the probability of
his being black. In the sampling form of the representativeness misconcep-
tion, small samples are generally believed to be more reliable representatives
of a population than is statistically likely. (For example, a preference for
Brand X over Brand Y in four out of five people would typically be believed
to be clearly indicative of a general preference-although the probability of
getting such an extreme sample of 5 just by chance is 3/8.)
Other types of judgment analyzed include availability (a heuristic that
judges a categorization is probable to the extent that instances of it can be
easily brought to mind), and the inference of causality from correlation.
Another relevant topic is covariation and control, in which a central finding
is that people are not likely to spot a correlation unless they expect it-and
if they do expect it, they tend to see one even if it is not there. The clear
story that runs through the articles in the Kahneman et al. (1982) book is
that inappropriate reasoning is (a) widespread and persistent, (b) similar at
all age levels, (c) found even among experienced researchers, and (d) quite
difficult to change.
There has recently been a good deal of research by educators exploring
the probabilistic intuitions of students from elementary through college
level. Much of this research is being done in Europe, in connection with the
Centre for Statistical Education in Sheffield, England, or with the Institut
fiir Didaktik der Mathematik der Universitit Bielefeld, in the Federal Re-
public of Germany. Results of several studies around the world were pre-
sented at the First International Conference on Teaching Statistics, held in
Sheffield, England. Among these are preliminary attempts to describe stages
in the development of probabilistic thinking (Falk, 1983; Fischbein & Gazit,
1983; Green, 1983).
have, and this may confuse them. Since students' actual heuristic, representativeness, is so
different in form from the appropriate mechanistic belief, it may not be easy to effect any
lasting change in students' beliefs about random samples. (p. 400)
Findings like these among college students imply the need for great cau-
tion in making objectives for instruction in high school. More important,
however, is the opportunity that the interview methods provide for testing
our propositions of what is going on in students' minds. For example,
Konold (1983) inferred from in-depth interviews that students typically have
an "outcome orientation" model of probability that may be more funda-
mental than the representativeness model. The outcome orientation involves
making yes-or-no decisions about single events. Konold compared the rep-
resentativeness and outcome-orientation models in college students' re-
sponses to novel questions. For example, when students were asked what
the outcome would be of selecting six marbles from a population that was
5/6 black and 1/6 white, most students thought it more likely that all six
would be black (i.e., a black marble is the most likely outcome on every
single trial) than that one would be white (the most representative result for
the series).
In the same study, Konold found that when students observed frequencies
in repeated trials, they inferred numerical probabilities by using an indirect,
two-stage process: In the first stage, the observed frequencies led to a feeling
of confidence; if a numerical representation was demanded of them, they
then generated a numerical "probability" from the feeling-not from the
frequencies. If true, this finding would imply considerable circumspectness
in using simulations to change the way that students think about probabil-
ity. Such in-depth exploratory research should be extended to other basic
topics and to students at other levels of education.
IMPLICATIONS
FOR FUTURERESEARCH
The declared importance of introducing statistical concepts into the
school curriculum, together with our limited knowledge about cognitive
development, mathematics learning in general, and misconceptions in prob-
ability and statistics, indicates that the time is ripe for an intensive and
coherent assault on the difficulty of learning basic concepts in stochastics.
Indeed, the creation of the International Study Group on Probability and
Statistics Concepts (ASA-NCTM Joint Committee, 1985) and international
conferences on teaching and learning stochastics (Grey, Holmes, Barnett,
& Constable, 1983) indicate that the learning of stochastics is an area of
major interest and activity. Many researchers in education and psychology
have explored parts of the misconceptions picture. Some researchers have
attempted to design specific remedial treatments, and many insightful and
creative teachers have suggested and tried improved techniques of instruc-
tion. Accounts of these continue to appear in the journal TeachingStatistics
and the newsletter The Statistics TeacherNetwork.
Although strong arguments have been made that students learn best when
instruction is couched in the context of students' "real world" knowledge
(Freudenthal, 1973; Kapadia, 1985; Roth, 1985), there is still only a little
published research on the effectiveness of this approach or any other. This
lack of research is perhaps due, as Bentz and Borovcnik (1985) believe, to
the difficulty of conducting this type of empirical research. Bentz and Bo-
rovcnik provide a catalog of problems that have limited the interpretation
of empirical research on probabilistic concepts.
A related problem is a lack of research on the design and use of instru-
ments to measure statistical understanding (Boveda, 1975; Chervany et al.,
1977; Garfield, 1981). A few instruments have been designed to measure
students' attitudes and anxiety toward statistics (Roberts & Saxe, 1982;
Wise, 1985), and some research has appeared that shows the role of factors
influencing general achievement in a statistics course (Harvey, Planke, &
Wise, 1985). Green (1983) has recently developed a test for children ages
11 to 16 that measures three levels of attainment of probability concepts.
The literature makes it clear that far more research has been done on the
psychology of probability than on other statistical concepts. In spite of this
research, however, teaching a conceptual grasp of probability still appears
to be a very difficult task, fraught with ambiguity and illusion. Accordingly,
we make the pragmatic recommendation for two research efforts that would
proceed in parallel: one that continues to explore the means to induce valid
conceptions of probability, and one that explores how useful ideas of sta-
tistical inference can be taught independently of technically correct proba-
bility. The interpretation of many statistical results must deal in some way
with whether or not we are being fooled by random variation, but it may
be possible to rely on simulation to make semiquantitative arguments. This
view entails accepting students' natural subjectivist view and endeavoring
to get them to draw increasingly on theoretical and frequency information
to set more appropriate belief levels.
As far as goals for instruction go, this view suggests a moratorium on the
typical organization of statistics instruction: (a) descriptive statistics, (b)
probability, and (c) inferential statistics. The intrusion of technical proba-
bility issues that are not likely to be understood will stall the learning
process-and leave a distaste that could compromise subsequent instruction
as well. This view is consistent with at least the beginning of the Quantitative
Literacy Project curriculum, in which qualitative approaches are used as
much as possible, computation is minimized, and probabilities are handled
in large part by simulation. The view may not be consistent, on the other
hand, with some of the ambitious goals for including probability in the K-
12 curriculum, foreshadowed in preliminary drafts of the ASA-NCTM Joint
Committee's (1985) preliminary guidelines. What is needed, however, is not
debate but research.
The difficulty of learning probability and statistics seems not to be a
Bentz, H.-J., & Borovcnik, M. G. (1985). Some remarks on empirical investigations on prob-
ability and statistics concepts. In A. Bell, B. Low, & J. Kilpatrick (Eds.), Theory, research
and practice in mathematical education (pp. 266-273). Nottingham, UK: Shell Centre for
Mathematical Education.
Beyth-Marom, R., & Dekel, S. (1983). A curriculum to improve thinking under uncertainty.
Instructional Science, 12, 67-82.
Bialaszewski, D. (1981). Statistics, computer packages and the real world. Improving College
and University Teaching,29, 162-165.
Bishop, B. A., & Anderson, C. W. (1986). Student conceptions of natural selection and its role
in evolution. (Research Series No. 165). East Lansing: Michigan State University, Institute
for Research on Teaching.
Borgida, E., & DeBono, K. (1985). Hypothesis-testing and the role of expert knowledge. Man-
uscript submitted for publication.
Borovcnik, M. (1985). The role of descriptive statistics. In A. Bell, B. Low, & J. Kilpatrick
(Eds.), Theory, researchand practice in mathematical education (pp. 285-292). Nottingham,
UK: Shell Centre for Mathematical Education.
Boruch, R. F., Nowakowski, J., Zawojewski, J., & Goldberg, E. (1984). Coupling cultures:
Teachingstatistics across disciplines in middle and secondary school. Unpublished manuscript,
Northwestern University, Center for Statistics and Probability, Evanston, IL.
Boveda, A. (1975). Diagnosis of deficiencies in the understanding of concepts of statistics.
Dissertation Abstracts International, 37, 188A. (University Microfilms No. 76-14,724)
Cantu, L. L., & Herron, J. D. (1978). Concrete and formal Piagetian stages and science concept
attainment. Journal of Research in Science Teaching,15, 135-143.
Carpenter, T. P., Corbitt, M. K., & Kepner, H. S., Jr. (1981). What are the chances of your
students knowing probability? Mathematics Teacher,74, 342-345.
Carpenter, T. P., Lindquist, M. M., Matthews, W., & Silver, E. (1983). Results of the third
NAEP mathematics assessment: Secondary school. Mathematics Teacher,76, 652-659.
Champagne, A. B., Gunstone, R. F., & Klopfer, L. E. (1983, April). Effecting changes in
cognitive structures among physics students. Paper presented at the meeting of the American
Educational Research Association, Montreal.
Chervany, N. L., Collier, R. D., Fienberg, S., & Johnson, P. (1977). A framework for the
development of measurement instruments for evaluating the introductory statistics course.
American Statistician, 31(1), 17-23.
Clement, J. (1982). Students' preconceptions in introductory mechanics. American Journal of
Physics, 50, 66-71.
Collis, B. (1983). Simulation and the microcomputer: An approach to teaching probability.
Mathematics Teacher,75, 584-587.
Conference Board of the Mathematical Sciences. (1982). New goals for mathematical sciences
education. Washington, DC: Author.
Conference Board of the Mathematical Sciences. (1983). The mathematicalsciences curriculum,
K-12: What is still fundamental and what is not. Washington, DC: Author.
DiSessa, A.A. (1982). Unlearning Aristotelian physics: A study of knowledge-based learning.
Cognitive Science, 6, 37-75.
Doane, D. (1985). Exploring statistics with the IBM PC. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Duchastel, P. C. (1974). Computer applications and instructional innovation: A case study in
the teaching of statistics. International Journal of Mathematics Education in Science and
Technology,5, 607-616.
Duncan, D. R., & Litwiller, B. H. (1981). Randomness, normality, and hypothesis testing:
Experiences for the statistics class. Mathematics Teacher,74, 368-374.
Edgar, S. M. (1973, June). Teachingstatistics while simultaneously saving time, chalk, etc. Paper
presented at the Conference on Computers in the Undergraduate Curricula, Claremont, CA.
(ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 079 993)
Educational Services. (1968). Elementary science study. St. Louis: Webster/McGraw-Hill.
Jolliffe, F. R. (1976). A continuous assessment scheme for statistics courses for social scientists.
InternationalJournal of Mathematics Education in Science and Technology,7, 97-103.
Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (Eds.). (1982). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics
and biases. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Kalton, G. (1973). Problems and possibilities with teaching introductory statistics to social
scientists. International Journal of Mathematics Education in Science and Technology,4, 7-
16.
Kapadia, R. (1985). A brief survey of research on probabilistic notions. In A. Bell, B. Low, &
J. Kilpatrick (Eds.), Theory, researchand practice in mathematical education (pp. 261-265).
Nottingham, UK: Shell Centre for Mathematical Education.
Kempthorne, 0. (1980). The teaching of statistics: Content versus form. American Statistician.
34(1), 17-21.
Konold, C. E. (1983). Conceptions of probability: Reality between a rock and a hard place.
(Doctoral dissertation, University of Massachusetts, 1983). Dissertation Abstracts Interna-
tional, 43, 4179B.
Landwehr, J., & Watkins, A. E. (1986). Exploring data. Palo Alto, CA: Dale Seymour.
Landwehr, J., Watkins, A. E., & Swift, J. (1987). Exploring surveys: Information from samples.
Palo Alto, CA: Dale Seymour.
McCloskey, M. (1983, April). Intuitive physics. Scientific American, pp. 122-130.
McCloskey, M., Washburn, A., & Felch, L. (1983). Intuitive physics: The straight-down belief
and its origin. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 9, 636-649.
McDermott, L. (1984). Research on conceptual understanding in mechanics. Physics Today,
37, 24-32.
Mevarech, Z. R. (1983). A deep structure model of students' statistical misconceptions. Edu-
cational Studies in Mathematics, 14, 415-429.
Milakofsky, L., & Patterson, H. D. (1979). Chemical education and Piaget. Journalof Chemical
Education, 56(2), 87-90.
Minstrell, J. (1982). Conceptual development research in the natural setting of a secondary
school classroom. In M. B. Rowe (Ed.), Science for the 80's (pp. 129-143). Washington,
DC: National Education Association.
Morier, D., & Borgida, E. (1984). The conjunction fallacy: A task specific phenomenon?
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10, 243-252.
Mosteller, F. (Ed.). (1973). Statistics by example. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Newman, C. M., Obremski, T. E., & Scheaffer, R. L. (1986). Exploring probability. Palo Alto,
CA: Dale Seymour.
Nisbett, R., Krantz, D., Jepson, C., & Kunda, Z. (1983). The use of statistical thinking in
everyday inductive reasoning. Psychological Review, 90, 339-363.
Noddings, N., Gilbert-MacMillan, K., & Lutz, S. (1980, April). What does an individual gain
in small group mathematical problem solving? Paper presented at the meeting of the American
Educational Research Association, Montreal.
Norusis, M. J. (1986). The SPSS guide to data analysis. Chicago: SPSS.
Nygard, K.E. (1983). A system for generating instructional computer graphics. AEDS Journal,
16, 177-187.
Ojemann, R. H., Maxey, E. J., & Snider, B. C. (1965a). Effects of guided learning experiences
in developing probability concepts at the fifth grade level. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 21,
415-427.
Ojemann, R. H., Maxey, E. J., & Snider, B. C. (1965b). The effect of a program of guided
learning experiences in developing probability concepts at the third grade level. Journal of
Experimental Education, 33, 321-330.
Ojemann, R. H., Maxey, E. J., & Snider, B. C. (1966). Further study of guided learning
experience in developing probability concepts in grade five. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 23,
323-336.
Pereira-Mendoza, L., & Swift, J. (1981). Why teach statistics and probability-A rationale. In
A. P. Shulte (Ed.), Teachingstatistics and probability (1981 Yearbook of the National Council
of Teachers of Mathematics, pp. 1-7). Reston, VA: NCTM.
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1975). The origin of the idea of chance in children. London: Routledge
& Kegan Paul.
Pollatsek, A., Konold, C. E., Well, A. D., & Lima, S. D. (1984). Beliefs underlying random
sampling. Memory and Cognition, 12, 394-401.
Pollatsek, A., Lima, S., & Well, A. D. (1981). Concept or computation: Students' understand-
ing of the mean. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 12, 191-204.
Posner, G. J., Hewson, B. W., Gertzog, W. A., & Strike, K. A. (1982). Accommodation of a
scientific conception: Toward a theory of conceptual change. Science Education, 66, 211-
227.
Resnick, L. R. (1983). Mathematics and science learning: A new conception. Science, 220,
477-478.
Richbart, L. A. (1981). Probability and statistics for grades 9-11. Mathematics Teacher,74,
346-348.
Roberts, D. M., & Saxe, J. E. (1982). Validity of a statistics attitude survey: A follow-up study.
Educational and Psychological Measurement, 42, 907-912.
Roth, K. J. (1985, April). Conceptual change learning and student processing of science texts.
Paper presented at the meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Chicago.
Ryan, T., Joiner, B., & Ryan, B. (1976). MINITAB student handbook. North Scituate, MA:
Duxbury Press.
Scheaffer, R. L. (1984). The role of statistics in revitalizing precollege mathematics and science
education. In Proceedings of the Section on Statistical Education (pp. 19-21). Washington,
DC: American Statistical Association.
Schoenfeld, A. H. (1985, March). Understanding and teaching the nature of mathematical
thinking. Paper presented at the University of Chicago School Mathematics Project Inter-
national Conference on Mathematics Education, Chicago.
Shaughnessy, J. M. (1977). Misconceptions of probability: An experiment with a small-group,
activity-based, model building approach to introductory probability at the college level.
Educational Studies in Mathematics, 8, 295-316.
Shaughnessy, J. M. (1981). Misconceptions of probability: From systematic errors to system-
atic experiments and decisions. In A. P. Shulte (Ed.), Teachingstatistics and probability (1981
Yearbook of the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, pp. 90-99). Reston, VA:
NCTM.
Shufelt, G. (Ed.). (1983). The agenda in action (1983 Yearbook of the National Council of
Teachers of Mathematics). Reston, VA: NCTM.
Shulte, A. P. (Ed.). (1981). Teachingstatistics and probability (1981 Yearbook of the National
Council of Teachers of Mathematics). Reston, VA: NCTM.
Smith, P. J. (1978). Piaget in high school instruction. Journal of Chemical Education, 55, 115-
118.
Sneider, C., & Pulos, S. (1983). Children's cosmographics: Understanding the earth's shape
and gravity. Science Education, 67, 205-221.
Stockburger, D. W. (1982). Evaluation of three simulation exercises in an introductory statistics
course. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 7, 365-370.
Stonewater, J. K., & Stonewater, B. B. (1984). Teaching problem solving: Implications from
cognitive development research. AAHE Bulletin, 36(6), 7-10.
Stroup, D. F. (1984). The statistician and the pedagogical monster: Characteristicsof effective
instructors of large statistics classes. In Proceedings of the Section on Statistical Education
(pp. 1-7). Washington, DC: American Statistical Association.
Swift, J. (1982, December). The first international conference on teaching statistics. Statistics
TeacherNetwork, pp. 1-2.
Swift, J. (1983). Challenges for enriching the curriculum: Statistics and probability. Mathe-
matics Teacher,76, 268-269.
Thomas, D. A. (1984). Understanding the central limit theorem. Mathematics Teacher,77, 542-
543.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1982). Causal schemas in judgments under uncertainty. In D.
Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases
(pp. 117-128). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
University of Chicago School Mathematics Project. (1985, Autumn). Chicago: University of
Chicago, UCSMP.
Urquhart, N. S. (1971). Nonverbal instructional approaches for introductory statistics. Amer-
ican Statistician, 25(2), 20-25.
Vandermeulen, H., & DeWreede, R. E. (1983). The amazing histogram. inCider, 1(11), 53-
56.
Wise, S. L. (1985, April). The development of a scale measuring attitudes toward statistics.
Paper presented at the meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Chicago.
AUTHORS
JOAN GARFIELD, Assistant Professor, General College, 106 Nicholson Hall, University of
Minnesota, 216 Pillsbury Drive S.E., Minneapolis, MN 55455
ANDREW AHLGREN, Professor (on leave from College of Education, University of Minne-
sota), American Association for the Advancement of Science, 1333 H Street N.W., Wash-
ington, DC 20005