Louis G. Castonguay, J. Christopher Muran, Lynne E. Angus, Jeffrey A. Hayes, Nicholas Ladany, Timothy Anderson-Bringing Psychotherapy Research to Life_ Understanding Change Through the Work of Leading.pdf
Louis G. Castonguay, J. Christopher Muran, Lynne E. Angus, Jeffrey A. Hayes, Nicholas Ladany, Timothy Anderson-Bringing Psychotherapy Research to Life_ Understanding Change Through the Work of Leading.pdf
Louis G. Castonguay, J. Christopher Muran, Lynne E. Angus, Jeffrey A. Hayes, Nicholas Ladany, Timothy Anderson-Bringing Psychotherapy Research to Life_ Understanding Change Through the Work of Leading.pdf
Psychotherapy
Research
to Life
Bringing
Psychotherapy
Research
to Life
Understanding Change
Through the Work of Leading
Clinical Researchers
Louis G. Castonguay,
Edited by
J. Christopher Muran, Lynne Angus,
Jeffrey A. Hayes, Nicholas Ladany,
and Timothy Anderson
Published by To order
American Psychological Association APA Order Department
750 First Street, NE P.O. Box 92984
Washington, DC 20002 Washington, DC 20090-2984
www.apa.org Tel: (800) 374-2721; Direct: (202) 336-5510
Fax: (202) 336-5502; TDD/TTY: (202) 336-6123
Online: www.apa.org/books/
E-mail: [email protected]
In the U.K., Europe, Africa, and the Middle East, copies may be ordered from
American Psychological Association
3 Henrietta Street
Covent Garden, London
WC2E 8LU England
The opinions and statements published are the responsibility of the authors, and such
opinions and statements do not necessarily represent the policies of the American
Psychological Association.
Bringing psychotherapy research to life : understanding change through the work of leading
clinical researchers / edited by Louis G. Castonguay . . . [et al.].
p. cm.
ISBN-13: 978-1-4338-0774-9
ISBN-10: 1-4338-0774-2
1. Psychotherapy—Research—History. I. Castonguay, Louis Georges. II. American
Psychological Association.
RC337.B73 2010
616.89'14—dc22
2009046125
vii
Chapter 4. Lester Luborsky: A Trailblazer in Empirical
Research on Psychoanalytic Therapy............................. 39
Paul Crits-Christoph, Jacques P. Barber,
Brin F. S. Grenyer, and Louis Diguer
Chapter 5. Hans Strupp: A Founder’s Contributions to
a Scientific Basis for Psychotherapy Practice ................. 51
Karla Moras, Timothy Anderson,
and William E. Piper
Chapter 6. Aaron T. Beck: The Cognitive Revolution
in Theory and Therapy................................................... 63
Steven D. Hollon
viii CONTENTS
Chapter 12. Michael J. Lambert: Building Confidence
in Psychotherapy .......................................................... 141
Benjamin M. Ogles and Jeffrey A. Hayes
Chapter 13. Clara E. Hill: A Rebel With Several Causes................ 153
Sarah Knox
CONTENTS ix
D. Integration of Multiple Variables..................................................... 245
Chapter 21. Horst Kächele: Bringing Research, Practice,
and People Together .................................................... 247
Bernhard Strauss
Chapter 22. Enrico Jones: Appreciating Complexity ...................... 259
Tai Katzenstein, Peter Fonagy, and J. Stuart Ablon
Chapter 23. David A. Shapiro: Psychotherapeutic
Investigations ............................................................... 271
Michael Barkham, Glenys Parry,
and Gillian E. Hardy
Chapter 24. Robert Elliott: Commitment to Experience................. 283
Rhea Partyka
Chapter 25. William B. Stiles: Empathic Reflections,
Voices, and Theory Building........................................ 295
Meredith Glick Brinegar and Katerine Osatuke
x CONTENTS
V. Conclusion........................................................................................ 351
Chapter 30. Future Directions: Emerging Opportunities
and Challenges in Psychotherapy Research................. 353
Lynne Angus, Jeffrey A. Hayes, Timothy Anderson,
Nicholas Ladany, Louis G. Castonguay,
and J. Christopher Muran
Index ........................................................................................................ 363
About the Editors..................................................................................... 377
CONTENTS xi
CONTRIBUTORS
xiii
Paul Crits-Christoph, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia
Louis Diguer, Laval University, Quebec City, Quebec, Canada
Robert Elliott, University of Strathclyde, Glasglow, United Kingdom
Barry A. Farber, Teachers College, Columbia University, New York, NY
Héctor Fernández-Álvarez, Fundación Aiglé, Buenos Aires, Argentina
Peter Fonagy, University College London, London, United Kingdom
Rhonda N. Goldman, Argosy University, Schaumburg, IL and the Family
Institute at Northwestern University, Evanston, IL
Brin F. S. Grenyer, University of Wollongong, Wollongong, Australia
Alan S. Gurman, University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public
Health, Madison
Gillian E. Hardy, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, United Kingdom
Jeffrey A. Hayes, Penn State University, University Park
Steven D. Hollon, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN
Martin Grosse Holtforth, University of Zürich, Zürich, Switzerland
Tai Katzenstein, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School,
Boston
Sarah Knox, Marquette University, Milwaukee, WI
Merton S. Krause, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL
Nicholas Ladany, Lehigh University, Bethlehem, PA
Michael J. Lambert, Brigham Young University, Provo, UT
Kenneth N. Levy, Penn State, University Park
Robert J. Lueger, Creighton University, Omaha, NE
Wolfgang Lutz, University of Trier, Trier, Germany
Paulo P. P. Machado, University of Minho, Braga, Portugal
Karla Moras, Merion Station, PA
J. Christopher Muran, Adelphi University and Beth Israel Medical Center,
New York, NY
Fredrick L. Newman, Florida International University, Miami
John C. Norcross, University of Scranton, Scranton, PA
Benjamin M. Ogles, Ohio University, Athens
John S. Ogrodniczuk, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British
Columbia, Canada
David E. Orlinsky, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL
Katerine Osatuke, Veterans Health Administration National Center for
Organization Development, Cincinnati, OH
Glenys Parry, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, United Kingdom
Rhea Partyka, Cross Junction, VA
William E. Piper, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British
Columbia, Canada
Alberta E. Pos, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
David L. Rennie, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
xiv CONTRIBUTORS
P. Scott Richards, Brigham Young University, Provo, UT
Michael Helge Rønnestad, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
Jeremy D. Safran, New School for Social Research, New York, NY
Carrie E. Schaffer, Charlottesville, VA
M. Tracie Shea, Veterans Affairs Medical Center and Brown University,
Providence, RI
Anne D. Simons, University of Oregon, Eugene
Bernhard Strauss, Institute of Psychosocial Medicine and Psychotherapy,
Jena, Germany
Margarita Tarragona, Universidad Iberoamericana, Mexico City, Mexico
Christopher C. Wagner, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond
Bruce E. Wampold, University of Wisconsin, Madison
Jeanne C. Watson, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
Joel Weinberger, Adelphi University, Garden City, NY
Ulrike Willutzki, Ruhr-Univerität Bochum, Bochum, Germany
Hadas Wiseman, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
CONTRIBUTORS xv
PREFACE
1Orlinsky, D. E., & Howard, K.I. (1986). The psychological interior of psychotherapy: Explorations with
the therapy session reports. In L. S. Greenberg & W. M. Pinsof (Eds), The psychotherapeutic process:
A research handbook (pp. 477–501). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
xvii
field was deteriorating. In addition to the impact that this had on a personal
level (all of us had interacted with and/or been mentored by some of these leg-
endary figures), we were also saddened by the strong possibility that the contri-
butions of these and other influential researchers, whose work served as the
foundation of our own, may not be known by young scholars or by psychother-
apy researchers primarily active in organizations other than SPR. Particularly
worrisome for the six of us was the thought that many clinicians might not be
aware of clinical implications of the research that giants of our field had con-
ducted for the last 50 years—a painful reminder of the tenuous connection
between practice and science.
There is no doubt that a gap exists between what researchers write
about and what practitioners do in therapy. Clinicians do not find much
guidance for their practice in the empirical literature. This, however, does
not mean that clinicians are not interested in research. What they are not
interested in are studies (including investigations that they may have con-
ducted themselves as part of their master’s and/or dissertation theses) that
fail to be clinically relevant or meaningful. Our experience in working col-
laboratively with clinicians, in the context of training graduate students,
conducting studies in practice research networks, and practicing psychother-
apy ourselves, is that clinicians are thirsty for empirical knowledge that can
help them better understand the complexity of therapy and improve the
impact of their own interventions. Although they may not be avid readers
of original empirical papers, clinicians (both novice and experienced) are
very eager to attend talks or workshops by “well-known” researchers whose
significant contributions to psychotherapy research and practice have been
made via peer-reviewed journals. Yet clinicians are rarely motivated to
seek these findings through methodologically detailed research reports found
in these scientific journals or even dry summaries of these research reports.
They would rather seek out the “big picture” of findings from a full career of
scientific labor. Why?
While clinicians are eager to learn what researchers have to say about
psychotherapy, they tend not to be particularly enthralled with the mechanism
by which most researchers choose to communicate their understanding.
Although the methodological and statistical sections in journal articles are cru-
cial for evaluating the scientific validity of the findings they report, they most
often overshadow the clinical implications of these findings. From a purely clin-
ical standpoint, the papers published in these journals frequently fail to deliver
substantive and detailed practical guidelines. One would be hard-pressed to
learn how to practice psychotherapy by relying primarily on such a prestigious
periodical as the Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology—even though not
publishing in this type of journal can be the kiss of death for the career of many
professional psychologists in academia.
xviii PREFACE
Yet clinicians seek out iconic psychotherapy researchers first and fore-
most because they are interested in research. These researchers often have a
central and clear message about research—one that readily can be applied
to the practice of psychotherapy. Many clinicians want to hear what
researchers have found about what types of patients benefit from therapy,
how relationship problems can best be handled, under which circumstances
emotions can be deepened, how to foster insight, and/or how to facilitate
behavioral activation—just to name a few clinically relevant issues. Thera-
pists want to learn about the major findings of leading researchers and the
lessons one can derive from years of empirical investigations—fully know-
ing that elsewhere they can find the methodological and statistical proce-
dures used in the investigations. They also want to know what led these
researchers to investigate particular aspects of psychotherapy, how their find-
ings have been influenced by (and have impacted) their respective clinical
practice and teaching, and how their research programs reflect some mile-
stones of their careers. When it comes to research, many clinicians want to
hear scientifically rich and clinically relevant messages that are embedded
in meaningful stories.
This book brings together a large number of influential research pro-
grams that have changed the way we think about and practice psychother-
apy. It presents the main findings derived by such scholarly and empirical
endeavors in a way that, we hope, is conceptually meaningful and clinically
relevant. These findings are also described in the context of the personal and
professional journeys within which they originated; they are, in other words,
historicized to some extent. We believe that such conceptualizing of scien-
tific work can bring research to life and, in doing so, may help many clinicians
to get back in touch with the excitement toward science that many of us felt
as we first entered graduate school.
The marriage between research and practice has been long recognized
as a troubled one. There are clinicians who reject research, and there are
researchers who refuse to practice. There are, however, researchers who prac-
tice, and there are certainly practitioners who conduct research. This volume
presents the contributions of those who are the embodiment of the scientist–
practitioner model and depict this marriage at its best.
The volume describes findings that will resonate with many clinicians’
observations and reflections. In addition, it presents empirical results that will
challenge some therapists’ habitual ways of thinking about clients and their
work with them. We hope that, ultimately, it will provide new directions to
improve practice. This attempt of ours to make empirical findings clinically
relevant, and thus to reduce the gap between research and practice, is one of
the two overarching goals of this book. The other goal is to pay tribute to
pioneers in the field of psychotherapy research.
PREFACE xix
A number of outstanding individuals have transformed the modern
field of psychotherapy, and we believe that the time is ripe for a book that
would provide a brief summary of the research contributions of at least some
of them. Clearly, this book could not cover all of the major contributors to
psychotherapy research in the last 50 years. Specifically, it focuses on the
legacies of several leading researchers who have been associated with the
foundation and early growth of SPR—via their direct involvement in the
society, the importance of their research program, and/or the generative
value (in terms of shaping current research trends or methods) of their work.
The editors recognize that, as current and past members of the executive
council of the North American chapter of SPR, the selection of the
researchers celebrated in this book reflects regional bias. We hope, however,
that our consultation with colleagues within and outside of North America
has attenuated this bias.
We are also aware that the research programs highlighted in this book
are predominantly anchored in psychodynamic and humanistic traditions.
While this reflects the roots of SPR, it fails to do justice to the major contri-
butions that leaders of other orientations (especially cognitive–behavioral)
have made to psychotherapy research, let alone to the influence that these
researchers have had on the research of many SPR members. In an attempt
to capture such contributions, as well as to pay tribute to their own theo-
retical allegiance, the first two editors of this book (L. G. Castonguay and
J. C. Muran) are currently working on a companion book to celebrate and
bring to life the research of prominent figures in the cognitive–behavioral
therapy tradition. Dividing psychotherapy researchers across two books may
still be viewed by some as reflecting, if not perpetuating, arbitrary barriers
between theoretical allegiances. This, however, is not our intention, and we
hope that the combination of the two books will, eventually, allow for broad
coverage of clinical researchers who have played an important role in under-
standing and developing psychotherapy, irrespective of the concepts they
have focused on and the research methods they have favored.
The task of selecting the researchers to be featured in this book, even
when restricting ourselves to individuals who have contributed to or substan-
tially influenced the development of SPR, was an arduous one, both intellec-
tually and emotionally. As for any book, we were allowed a limited number of
pages. Within those limits, we also wanted to devote sufficient space for each
chapter to capture the major contributions of recognized scholars, the clinical
implications of their empirical accomplishments, and the developmental and
personal contexts within which their work has evolved. Thus, we had very dif-
ficult choices to make. We spent long conference calls brainstorming and
weighing possible options. We also devoted many hours going over leading
xx PREFACE
textbooks, handbooks, and published reviews of empirical literature to compile
list after list of possible “candidates.” We also consulted with several senior col-
leagues who provided us with suggestions that were very helpful in guiding our
selection process. In the end, though, we had to make painful decisions about
whom not to feature. Several well-known SPR contributors, including some of
our own mentors, did not make the final list despite our profound respect for
the quality and heuristic value of their work. We will have to live with the
choices we made and hope that those not selected, at least for this first edition
of the book, will understand the difficulty of our task.
We also carefully chose the authors for each of the chapters, who in
return provided truly remarkable, eloquent, and inspiring tributes to the work
and legacy of the researchers they were asked to write about. We primarily
chose authors based on their relationships, as mentees or close colleagues,
with the featured researchers (after consulting with each of them, whenever
possible). We also chose authors based on their own contributions to the field.
The majority of these authors are reputed psychotherapy researchers from all
over the world, and many of them will, we hope, have their own work recog-
nized in future editions of this book.
The preparation of this book has been facilitated by the help of many indi-
viduals. In particular, we want to express our gratitude to Susan Reynolds
(senior acquisitions editor at APA Books), not only for her trust and support
but also for her crucial help in creating the outline that guided each of the chap-
ters in this volume. We are also thankful to many friends and colleagues who
provided us with advice and suggestions regarding the general structure of the
book, as well as the selection of featured researchers and selected authors. These
include Jacques Barber, Larry Beutler, Franz Caspar, John Clarkin, Paul Crits-
Christoph, Irene Elkin, Marvin Goldfried, Martin Grosse Holtforth, Paulo
Machado, Erhard Mergenthaler, John Norcross, David Orlinsky, Glenys Parry,
David Rennie, and Bernhard Strauss. We are also grateful to many of the fea-
tured researchers and chapter authors who provided us with much needed
encouragement and support during the completion of our task. Such warmth
has been experienced by us as a reflection of the sense of connection that is
prevalent in SPR.
We hope that this book will further contribute to this spirit of affilia-
tion and collaboration by celebrating the work and impact of some of its
leaders and by encouraging the integration of research and practice. We also
hope that it will foster stronger connections between different generations
of researchers. One of our goals was to raise the awareness of new scholars
about the sources of some of the current themes and methods of research
while highlighting some future research directions they can take in advanc-
ing psychotherapy. In the current stage of our careers, we see ourselves, like
PREFACE xxi
so many of our peers and colleagues in SPR, as part of a cohort that should
facilitate the advancement of our field by creating links between newer and
older generations of psychotherapy researchers. With this role in mind, we
would like to dedicate this book to our mentors, who have shown us the
way, and to our students, who have not only helped us in our quests but
have and will continue to contribute to the growth of SPR and psychother-
apy research.
xxii PREFACE
Bringing
Psychotherapy
Research
to Life
1
A BRIEF INTRODUCTION TO
PSYCHOTHERAPY RESEARCH
J. CHRISTOPHER MURAN, LOUIS G. CASTONGUAY,
AND BERNHARD STRAUSS
3
methods, including operationism and pragmatism, and most notably by logi-
cal positivism with its emphasis on confirmation based on objective data and
quantitative or statistical analysis. In contrast, Continental European psychol-
ogy has been more pluralistic, though largely dominated by the rationalist or
idealist tradition, with its origins in Cartesian principles and developed in
Leibnizian/Kantian thought. From this tradition, methods such as phenome-
nology, hermeneutics, and structuralism emerged that privilege the subjec-
tive and interpretive and promote exploratory and qualitative approaches to
research, such as case studies.
There has been a long-standing tension between these two traditions that
can be boiled down to the fundamental epistemological question How do
we know? From the empirical perspective, our knowledge is a posteriori, depen-
dent on sense experience. The mind is seen as tabula rasa and as essentially pas-
sive in nature. From the rationalist perspective, our knowledge can be intuited
or deduced from intuitions and thus exists a priori to sense experience. In this
regard, the mind is seen as having a potentially active core of its own. The
empirical sees knowledge as socially derived, the rationalist as individually
driven. There have been many challenges to these views in the past few decades
collectively described as postmodern, which suggest a more complex and criti-
cal perspective, a more both/and sensibility, and a more skeptical approach to
knowing (see Muran, 2001a).
Despite this tension, the empirical tradition has mostly dominated scien-
tific method in psychotherapy research, probably because of to the extensive
role that North American and British researchers have played in the field. This
dominance can also be attributed to socioeconomic factors regarding evidence-
based practice and cost containment in Western health care, as well as an over-
all cultural shift toward the values of instrumentality, efficiency, and conformity
(Cushman & Gilford, 2000). There have been challenges to this dominance,
however, especially in the form of attempts to blend the two perspectives (e.g.,
attempts to quantify subjective states and to develop both exploratory and
confirmatory models, as well as challenges to their authority altogether: that
is, attempts to promote methodological plurality and contextual analyses;
see Muran, 2001b, 2002).
REFERENCES
Alexander, F. (1937). Five year report of the Chicago Institute for Psychoanalysis,
1932–1937.
Allport, G. W. (1955). Becoming: Basic considerations for a psychology of personality.
New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Barber, J. P., Gallop, R., Crits-Christoph, P., Frank, A., Thase, M. E., Weiss, R. D., &
Connolly Gibbons, M. B. (2006). The role of therapist adherence, therapist com-
petence, and the alliance in predicting outcome of individual drug counseling:
Results from the NIDA Collaborative Cocaine Treatment Study. Psychotherapy
Research, 16, 229–240. doi:10.1080/10503300500288951
Bergin, A. E. (1971). The evaluation of therapeutic outcomes. In A. E. Bergin &
S. L. Garfield (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (pp. 217–270).
New York, NY: Wiley.
Beutler, L. E., & Crago, M. (Eds.). (1991). Psychotherapy research: An international
review of programmatic research. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association. doi:10.1037/10092-000
Bohart, A. C., Elliott, R., Greenberg, L. S., & Watson, J. C. (2002). Empathy. In J. C.
Norcross (Ed.), Psychotherapy relationships that work: Therapist contributions and
responsiveness to patients (pp. 89–108). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Castonguay, L. G. (1993). “Common factors” and “nonspecific variables”: Clarification
of the two concepts and recommendations for research. Journal of Psychotherapy
Integration, 3, 267–286.
Castonguay, L. G., & Beutler, L. E. (2006). Principles of therapeutic change that work.
New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Chambless, D. L., & Ollendick, T. H. (2001). Empirically supported psychological
interventions: Controversies and evidence. Annual Review of Psychology, 52,
685–716. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.685
17
client-centered (later person-centered) approaches to problems related to
education, marriage, racism, and geopolitical disputes. More generally, his
emphasis on the mutative aspects of the relationship per se has been accepted
by virtually all contemporary schools of psychotherapy, including those aligned
with cognitive behavior therapy and psychodynamically-oriented approaches
(Farber, 2007).
MAJOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Rogers was not the first to conduct psychotherapy research. Crude out-
come studies, most consisting of simple tallies of therapist-perceived improve-
ment of heterogeneous samples of patients, can be traced back to the 1920s
and 1930s (Bergin, 1971). What Rogers and his colleagues did was to change
dramatically the methods and focus of psychotherapy research. In the course
of his career, Rogers pioneered or foreshadowed most of the major genres of
psychotherapy research: process research; systematic, controlled outcome
research; process-outcome research; and finally, surprisingly, qualitative
research. For example, while retaining an interest in outcome, Rogers began
to examine the process of psychotherapy. His awareness that the words and
actions of the therapist as well as the patient contribute substantially to the
effectiveness of therapy was well ahead of its time. He traced client thematic
patterns across sessions, looking for changes in clients’ sense of self (e.g., greater
congruence between their perceptions of real and ideal selves) and also look-
ing for what we would now call “ruptures in the therapeutic alliance.” He also
investigated the ingredients underlying therapists’ effectiveness, famously
EARLY BEGINNINGS
CARL ROGERS 19
family as a reserved and oversensitive child who often retreated to the
comfort of his books.
Motivated by his father’s wish to pursue farming as a side interest (which
soon grew into an early agribusiness venture), and by both parents’ wish to pro-
tect their children from the temptations of living near a big city, the family
moved to a farm outside Chicago when Carl was 12. There, Rogers developed
a great interest in moths, rearing caterpillars and watching their development.
Moreover, encouraged by his father, Rogers studied scientific agriculture,
becoming familiar with experimental methods that emphasized the need for
hypothesis testing, control groups, and random assignment.
Given these influences, it is not surprising that, at the University of
Wisconsin, Rogers first turned his attention to scientific agriculture, and then
to the ministry. Shortly after graduation, he married a young woman he
had known since childhood, Helen, and the two of them moved to New York,
where Carl began his studies at Union Theological Seminary. He soon
became disenchanted with the doctrinaire nature of religious training, and
during his second year of studies he began taking courses in psychology at
Teachers College, Columbia University. Soon, he became a full-time student
there, studying clinical and educational psychology in an academic culture
marked by fairly strict adherence to research, statistics, and measurement. For
his doctoral dissertation he developed a test for measuring adjustment in
young adolescents.
ACCOMPLISHMENTS
CARL ROGERS 21
small samples of clients and no experimental controls. However, they sub-
sequently obtained a series of grants for a larger study, the first large (N = 25)
controlled investigation of psychotherapy outcome. This study used two dif-
ferent nonrandom no-treatment controls (Rogers & Dymond, 1954): One
was a silent control group of nontreatment-seeking individuals matched to
the treatment sample (a design that would be used 20 years later by Strupp
and colleagues for the first Vanderbilt Psychotherapy Project); the other was
a waiting-list control group of clients whose therapy was delayed for 2 months.
While this study was later criticized for failing to randomize clients to treat-
ment (less distressed clients were assigned to the waiting-list condition), it
was an important step forward in the development of what is today consid-
ered the definitive group design for treatment research, the randomized
clinical trial.
Qualitative Inklings
Rogers’s major success during his years at Wisconsin was not in the
research realm but rather with an immensely popular book, On Becoming a
Person (1961). He had never been particularly at home in academia—among
other grievances, he detested academic politics and power struggles—and
had long harbored deep-seated doubts about conventional understandings of
human nature and science. It appears likely that his experiences in Wiscon-
sin permanently soured him on both academia and quantitative psychother-
apy research (Kirschenbaum, 2007). He left full-time university life in 1963
to become a member of a new, nonprofit institution, the Western Behavioral
Sciences Institute (La Jolla, California), dedicated to humanistically oriented
CARL ROGERS 23
research in the field of human relations. His final organizational home was in
another newly created institution, the Center for Studies of the Person, formed
in 1968. During his years in California, he published a number of popular
books, including those that focused on encounter groups, education, politics,
and marriage. His last years were devoted primarily to issues of international
and group relations, and world peace.
Beginning in 1966 at Western Behavioral Sciences Institute, however,
Rogers began a search for alternatives to traditional positivist approaches to
studying people, hosting a series of dialogues among leading philosophers
(Kirschenbaum & Henderson, 1989). By the mid-1980s, shortly before his
death, he had aligned himself instead with more experience-based and inter-
personal approaches to study and understanding, preferring to think in terms
of research partners or coresearchers. He was interested in the kind of infor-
mation that could be gleaned from mutual understanding, an undertaking not
unlike that emphasized by contemporary relationally oriented psychoanalytic
thinkers as well as qualitative methodologists.
These efforts are almost totally unknown today, but they contain within
them the unmistakable first stirrings of the antipositivist revolution in the
social sciences that emerged in the 1980s and eventually gave rise in the
1990s to modern qualitative research methods. As Kirschenbaum (2007)
noted, the most likely explanation for the obscurity of this final research con-
tribution by Rogers is that he was unable to provide a specific alternative to
traditional quantitative research methods. Rogers’s strength had always been
his ability to link ideals to practice via concrete examples. However, in the
mid-1960s there was nothing concrete yet to draw on, and it was not until
shortly before his death in 1987 that the field caught up enough with Carl
Rogers for him to be able to point to specific nonpositivist methods. Never-
theless, it is our view that Rogers’s final contribution to psychotherapy research
lies precisely in his having helped create the initial conditions that, 25 years
later, led to the rapid emergence and dissemination of qualitative psychother-
apy research (e.g., Rennie, Phillips, & Quartaro, 1988), like the legendary
butterfly of chaos theory flapping its wings in Asia.
CONCLUSION
We have tried to make the case for Carl Rogers’s having founded or pio-
neered nearly all the major genres of psychotherapy research, including, indi-
rectly, qualitative research. At first, he did this through his own personal
efforts, but increasingly over the years his influence was mediated through the
students and colleagues he mentored and encouraged. Some of these, like
Gene Gendlin, Laura Rice, and Natalie Rogers, his daughter, went on to
CARL ROGERS 25
never fully embraced Rogers’s iconoclastic views. Fourth, although the person-
centered school of therapy appears to have gone into decline, at least in North
America, it is also quite clear that during Rogers’s last 20 years, his ideas about
the important role of the therapeutic relationship in effective psychotherapy
were absorbed into all the major approaches, suggesting “infusion” as a better
metaphor than “decline” (Farber, 2007).
Today, the person-centered and experiential approaches are enjoying a
lively revival and are expanding their applicability; moreover, research on
these newer client-centered models, especially on humanistic–experiential
therapies, is proliferating (e.g., Elliott et al., 2004). The central contradiction
between idealism and pragmatism that drove Rogers has been assimilated by
his intellectual progeny as a creative tension that continues to inspire con-
temporary theory, research, and practice on humanistic and related therapies.
Rogers’s legacy, as a pioneer in both the clinical and research realms, lives on.
REFERENCES
CARL ROGERS 27
3
JEROME D. FRANK: PSYCHOTHERAPY
RESEARCHER AND HUMANITARIAN
BRUCE E. WAMPOLD AND JOEL WEINBERGER
EARLY BEGINNINGS
Frank was born in 1909 in New York City. He died in Baltimore at age
95, living through a period that spanned two world wars and two post–world
war Asian conflicts (Korea and Vietnam), the industrialization of the West-
ern world, and the spawning of globalization in the context of computers and
technology. He earned his bachelor’s degree at Harvard College in 1930, dur-
ing which time he became acquainted with the work of Kurt Lewin. He
worked with Lewin in Berlin (1930–1931) before earning his PhD at Harvard
in 1934 and while a postdoc at Cornell. The experience in Berlin with Lewin
29
was important intellectually and socially. Throughout his career, Frank sub-
scribed to Lewin’s thesis that cognition, behavior, and emotion are largely
determined by the current social environment. This piqued Frank’s interest
in motivation. Lewin’s emphasis on “action research,” which suggested that
the best way to understand a phenomenon was to attempt to change it, fueled
Frank’s interest in psychotherapy (Frank, 1992). As well, Frank was present
in Germany when the Nazi party won its first significant election; Lewin’s
belief that psychologists should be involved in social issues elicited Frank’s
interest in promoting social justice, which had been nurtured by the Frank
family’s involvement in the ethical culture movement (Julia Frank, personal
communication, Oct. 5, 2007). He was soon involved in political issues in
Berlin (American Psychological Association, 1986).
Having completed his doctorate in psychology, Frank satisfied what was
for some time a latent desire to be a physician by enrolling in the Harvard
Medical School, where he received his MD in 1939. He began psychiatry res-
idency at Johns Hopkins after completing an internship at New York Hospi-
tal. Immediately after residency, he spent several years in the United States
armed forces. He was serving in the Far East when the two atomic bombs were
detonated in Japan.
MAJOR ACCOMPLISHMENTS
Frank contributed to the field of psychology in two major areas: (a) the
psychological issues of nuclear weapons and war and (b) the process and out-
comes of psychotherapy. Although the focus of this volume is on the latter,
it would be remiss to ignore his contributions to the former, which clearly
intertwined with his perspectives on psychotherapy.
Frank began his studies of psychotherapy after his discharge from the
armed services, first with the Veterans Administration, where he investigated
group therapy, and then at Johns Hopkins, where he headed a psychiatric out-
patient department. He was uniquely poised to make a contribution because
of his training and his empirical inclinations. At the time (i.e., the 1950s),
the practice of psychotherapy was mostly restricted to psychiatrists. Psychol-
ogists were involved with research design, data collection, and analysis.
Because Frank was both a psychiatrist and a psychologist, he understood
the practice of psychotherapy, research design and statistics, and psycho-
logical principles related to pathology and change processes. This is a unique
set of skills for any period, but particularly so during the infancy of psychother-
apy research.
JEROME D. FRANK 31
both, but which would not be expected by the theory of therapy being
studied to produce the same effects. (Rosenthal & Frank, 1956, p. 300)
In the early studies at Johns Hopkins, Frank and his colleagues set out
to do exactly this: compare various approaches. The obstacles were immense,
as there were no standard treatments (i.e., treatment manuals did not exist
and psychotherapy was learned from one’s supervisor) or outcome measures.
So, the team set about to standardize treatments and use the best outcome
measures that they could cobble together, and they designed a fair compari-
son between group therapy (1.5 hr per week for 6 months), individual ther-
apy (1 hr per week for 6 months), and minimal therapy (.5 hr every 2 weeks
for 6 months). As noted by Frank (1992), “To our astonishment and chagrin,
despite obvious differences in therapies” only one variable, among many,
showed any difference, and the research team was “forced to conclude that
features shared by all three must have been responsible for much of patients’
improvement” (p. 393). And thus was born Frank’s interest in the com-
mon factors! Although he was not the first to discuss common factors (see
Rosenzweig, 1936), Frank’s thinking and research formed the core of the
common factors school of thought for decades to come, culminating in pub-
lication of two editions of Persuasion and Healing (1961, 1973), and another
edition (Frank & Frank, 1991) with his daughter, Julia B. Frank, MD, a tra-
dition carried forth by such luminaries as Arthur Kleinman (e.g., Kleinman
& Sung, 1979), Judd Marmor (e.g., 1962), Sol Garfield (e.g., 1995), and
E. Fuller Torrey (1972).
We should not gloss over the courage that it took to abandon the med-
ical model and embrace what turned into a model embedded in cultural heal-
ing practices, a journey that in many ways took Frank down a path far from
the mainstream. The decision to pursue this line of research and thinking
appears to have emanated from Frank’s commitment to evidence foremost
and his dedicated desire to improve lives, personal values that saturated his
work. Over the years he attributed these values to his training and associa-
tion with colleagues. For example,
Psychiatric training at the Johns Hopkins Hospital under Adolf Meyer
and John C. Whiteborn instilled a nondogmatic outlook, a respect for
facts, and a conviction that systematic observation of individuals in their
social context could yield worthwhile insights into human functioning.
(Frank & Frank, 1991, p. 296)
We suspect that although colleagues may have nurtured these characteristics,
Frank’s fierce belief in evidence and his gentle respect for his fellow humans
were intrinsic characteristics. Parloff and Shapiro (2005), in their American
Psychologist obituary for Frank, quoted Paul McHugh, director of the Depart-
ment of Psychiatry at the Johns Hopkins School of Medicine:
JEROME D. FRANK 33
more recent common factors approaches as well as rejoinders to critiques of
such approaches, see Imel & Wampold, 2008; Wampold, 2007; Weinberger,
1995; Weinberger & Rasco, 2007.)
The common factors model Frank developed diverged radically from his
initial belief that specific factors unique to specific treatments underlay the
effectiveness of psychotherapy. He ended up arguing that radically diverse
forms of psychotherapy have underlying similarities and that their effective-
ness is due to these similarities (Frank, 1973). But his model was richer and
more innovative than merely asserting common factors. He detailed what he
thought those factors were. According to Frank’s model (1973, 1978, 1982;
Frank & Frank, 1991), patients have concluded that they are unable to deal
with issues of importance to them. As a result, they feel incompetent and suf-
fer for it. In Frank’s terminology (e.g., Frank, 1974), they become demoralized.
They hope and expect that psychotherapy can help them overcome these
heretofore insurmountable issues. Once these hopes and positive expectations
are in place, the demoralized state improves.
Although the mere intention to seek help results in some improvement,
the elements of the treatment itself may enhance and solidify positive change.
Additionally, the improvement based on positive expectation and hope needs
to be maintained or it will dissipate over the course of treatment. Through
certain practices, psychotherapy can improve morale beyond the effects of
initial expectations and hopes. All effective psychotherapies, according to
Frank (1973, 1978, 1982), employ four factors that function to mobilize hope
and restore morale. The most important of these is an emotionally charged, con-
fiding relationship with a helping person. Frank averred that such a relationship
is absolutely essential to therapeutic improvement. Recent evidence has sup-
ported Frank’s views on this matter (see, e.g., Weinberger & Rasco, 2007). A
recent task force commissioned by Division 29 (Psychotherapy) of the Amer-
ican Psychological Association conducted a comprehensive review of the
extant data and concluded that the relationship was a (in fact, the) critical
factor in psychotherapy (Norcross, Beutler, & Levant, 2006).
Frank’s second factor was a healing setting. Therapy has to take place some-
where. The therapeutic setting can and usually does symbolize the therapist’s
role as a healer. The setting can be a prestigious hospital or an impressive
office, although even the most humble of settings will usually contain diplo-
mas and impressive-looking books. Whatever the specifics, the setting pro-
vides a cultural context that communicates authority, expertise, and healing
to the patient. All of this helps to enhance the patient’s expectations and
hope that the therapist knows what he or she is doing and can help. It sets
the stage for the therapy and actively affects it. The setting also is a haven. The
patient can feel safe and therefore more open in his or her communications
to the therapist and to the therapist’s interventions.
JEROME D. FRANK 35
research (Wampold, 2001; Weinberger & Rasco, 2007), and when it does not
appear, brand loyalty seems to account for the differences (Luborsky et al.,
1999; Wampold, 2001).
CONCLUSION
REFERENCES
JEROME D. FRANK 37
Norcross, J. C., Beutler, L. E., & Levant, R. F. (2006). Evidence-based practices in
mental health. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Parloff, M. B., & Shapiro, D. L. (2005). Jerome D. Frank (1910–2005). American Psy-
chologist, 60, 727. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.60.7.727
Peters, L. (1978). Psychotherapy in Tamang shamanism. Ethos, 6, 63–91.
Piper, W. E., & Wogan, M. (1970). Placebo effect in psychotherapy: An extension of
earlier findings. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 34, 447. doi:10.1037/
h0029345
Rosenthal, D., & Frank, J. D. (1956). Psychotherapy and the placebo effect. Psycho-
logical Bulletin, 53, 294–302. doi:10.1037/h0044068
Rosenzweig, S. (1936). Some implicit common factors in diverse methods of psycho-
therapy: “At last the Dodo said, ‘Everybody has won and all must have prizes.’”
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 6, 412–415.
Shapiro, A. K., Struening, E., & Shapiro, E. (1980). The reliability and validity of
a placebo test. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 15, 253–290. doi:10.1016/0022-
3956(79)90016-5
Stampfl, T. G., & Levis, D. J. (1967). The essentials of implosive therapy: A learning-
theory-based psychodynamic behavioral therapy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
72, 496–503. doi:10.1037/h0025238
Thong, D. (1976). Psychiatry in Bali. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry,
10, 95–97.
Torrey, E. F. (1972). What Western psychotherapists can learn from witch doctors.
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 42, 69–76.
Wampold, B. E. (2001). The great psychotherapy debate: Model, methods, and findings.
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Wampold, B. E. (2007). Psychotherapy: The humanistic (and effective) treatment.
American Psychologist, 62, 857–873. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.62.8.857
Weinberger, J. (1995). Common factors aren’t so common: The common factors
dilemma. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 2, 45–69.
Weinberger, J., & Rasco, C. (2007). Empirically supported common factors. In
S. G. Hofmann & J. Weinberger (Eds.), The art and science of psychotherapy
(pp. 103–129). New York, NY: Routledge.
Lester Luborsky, who died in October 2009 at the age of 89, was one of
the pioneers of psychotherapy research. His career began in the 1950s and
only ended with his retirement in 2006. We are lucky to be intimately famil-
iar with Lester’s ideas and contributions throughout his career through our
long-standing collaborations with him. We have benefited enormously from
Lester’s productivity, creativity, and perseverance in completing studies and
projects. In this chapter we hope to provide a sense of his contributions to the
field of psychotherapy research, not only in terms of his publications but also
his personal influences on others.
MAJOR CONTRIBUTIONS
39
Articulation of a Theory of Symptom Formation
40 CRITS-CHRISTOPH ET AL.
Studies of the Efficacy of Supportive–Expressive Psychotherapy
EARLY BEGINNINGS
Lester Luborsky was born in 1920 and grew up during the Great Depres-
sion in Philadelphia, where he played in fields that disappeared as what was a
town became a sprawling city. His family originated from Eastern Europe.
After high school graduation, Luborsky took a research assistant job at Penn
State to pursue his passion, botany. But while at Penn State he came across
some of the works of Freud in his landlady’s bookcase, and these books—
together with a desire to work with people rather than plants—set the course
of his future career. Luborsky then attended college at Temple University and
graduated at the age of 22 with a bachelor of arts degree. He then proceeded
to Duke University in North Carolina for his master’s and PhD in psychology.
Even after turning to psychology and psychotherapy as his vocation,
the theme of botany continued to be apparent throughout Luborsky’s life
and career. His passion for botany stemmed from a delight in what he termed
“watching things grow”—which he later used as a natural basis for studying
patients’ growth and change over the course of psychotherapy. This theme
was also placed by Luborsky on the last page (1984, p. 180) of his psycho-
dynamic psychotherapy manual through an illustration showing the watering
of a plant alongside a magnifying glass. These two symbols convey how the
LESTER LUBORSKY 41
therapist provides the conditions and nutrients for growth and also provides
a close examination of the thoughts, feelings, desires, and behaviors of the
patient. One relevant memorable anecdote regarding botany was when, dur-
ing Luborsky’s training, he was discussing an idea with Eric Erikson. There
was some contention between Luborsky and Erikson, but Erikson diplomat-
ically replied that Luborsky’s ideas “needed some watering.” Also linked with
Luborsky’s interest in botany and gardening was that of rearranging stones
and rocks within a natural setting (in the style of Japanese Zen rock gardens),
an activity he pursued both in terms of the features in his own garden (which
he tended religiously) and in miniature at his work desk. Luborsky’s portrait,
which hangs in the University of Pennsylvania Medical School, shows him
with some rocks, an intentional reference to this aspect of his life and its
influence on his other thinking.
Luborsky’s PhD thesis, completed in 1945 and developed in the context
of World War II, was on the topic of visual perception of aircraft recognition.
Around this time his work for Raymond Cattell spurred his interest in psycho-
logical measurement. Following several positions at Duke and then the Uni-
versity of Illinois, Luborsky cemented his interest in Freud and psychoanalysis
when he obtained a research post in 1946 at the world-renowned Menninger
Foundation. The Menninger Foundation over the course of Luborsky’s years
there was a hotbed of influential psychoanalytic thinkers and researchers,
including David Rapaport, Karl Menninger, George Klein, Roy Schafer, Philip
Holzman, Herbert Schlesinger, Howard Shevrin, Merton Gill, Otto Kernberg,
Robert Holt, and Robert Wallerstein. These individuals had an enormous influ-
ence on Luborsky’s clinical training and research ideas. While working within
the Menninger clinic research program, he was offered and accepted full psy-
choanalytic training with the affiliated Topeka Psychoanalytic Institute. This
was at the time a rare opportunity, as psychologists generally were unable to
obtain psychoanalytic training, and he was able to integrate his analysis with
his paid work, making his analytic training affordable and possible.
His 13 years (1946–959) in Kansas at the Menninger Foundation were
critical to shaping much of the direction of Luborsky’s future research, for it
was here that he worked on one of the first empirical studies of psychoana-
lytic psychotherapy, the Menninger Foundation Psychotherapy Research
Project. In those years, research on psychotherapy and psychoanalytic was in
its infancy. Important researchers at the time who were influences included
Henry Murray and Christina Morgan at Harvard. Components of their work
with the Thematic Apperception Test were probably influential in the later
development of the tripartite structure of Luborsky’s CCRT method.
Luborsky was offered, and accepted, a post at the University of Pennsyl-
vania in 1959. Aaron Beck was on the panel that supported his professorship.
The move “back home” to Philadelphia was his last career move, and he was
42 CRITS-CHRISTOPH ET AL.
highly productive at Penn until his retirement in 2006. The move to Penn
allowed him to take leadership of projects and ideas that had been germinat-
ing in Kansas, and major projects began to emerge, including the Penn Psy-
chotherapy Research project. Throughout most of his years at Penn, he was
not only a researcher but actively involved as a clinical psychotherapist and
a teacher of psychotherapy. He was highly sought after as a clinical super-
visor and won an award within the Department of Psychiatry for his teach-
ing of psychiatric residents and postdoctoral fellows.
ACCOMPLISHMENTS
LESTER LUBORSKY 43
minor changes, the HRSR became the basis for the widely used Global Assess-
ment of Functioning scale in the DSM.
Like any fledgling field of study, an early step in the development of the
evolving field of psychotherapy research was to hold a consensus conference
to map out the primary issues and problems that needed to be addressed.
An initial consensus conference was held in 1958. The second, and more sig-
nificant, consensus conference was held in 1961. Luborsky was prominently
involved in the planning and implementation of this consensus conference,
and participated as an editor of a book that summarized the conference find-
ings (Strupp & Luborsky, 1962). His collaboration with Hans Strupp on this
volume marked the beginnings of a lifelong friendship and friendly competi-
tion with Strupp. As Strupp remarked to a Vanderbilt graduate student who
had worked in Luborsky’s research program, he and Luborsky were the “Macy’s
and Gimbels” (an allusion to the department stores’ legendary rivalry) of psy-
chodynamic psychotherapy research—often pursuing similar research agen-
das and each keeping a close eye on what the other was doing.
After coming to the University of Pennsylvania, Luborsky obtained a
major grant, in 1968, from the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH)
to conduct a 5-year comprehensive study of the factors that influence the out-
come of psychotherapy. This study, later named by Luborsky the “Penn Psy-
chotherapy Project,” was the primary source of data and transcripts for a large
number of his subsequent papers, chapters, and books for the rest of his career.
At the same time he received the NIMH grant to conduct the Penn Psy-
chotherapy Project, Luborsky applied for and received a Research Scientist
grant (K-award) from NIMH (1968–1973). He would obtain renewals of
this Research Scientist grant from NIMH from 1973 to 1992, and he then
received similar awards from 1992 to 2000 from the National Institute on
Drug Abuse.
Luborsky’s interest in examining predictors of therapy outcome origi-
nated from his experiences working at the Menninger Foundation before
coming to the University of Pennsylvania. The Menninger Foundation Psy-
chotherapy Research Project also had the goal, among other goals, of exam-
ining predictors of treatment outcome. However, he left the Menninger
Foundation before the study came to fruition. This seemed to motivate him,
as he described in the acknowledgment section of the 1988 book, Who Will
Benefit From Psychotherapy? Predicting Therapeutic Outcomes, to conduct his
own study that would make use of the strengths but rectify the weaknesses of
the Menninger study. Another input to his interest in examining predictors
of therapy outcome was his previous work at Menninger’s with Robert Holt
on predictors of which physicians would make good psychiatrists (Holt &
Luborsky, 1958). The difficult issues in the work with Holt regarding the
assessment of outcome and how to manage a large number of predictors helped
44 CRITS-CHRISTOPH ET AL.
Luborsky address similar methodological concerns when working on a com-
prehensive study of predictors of therapy outcome.
In preparation for conducting the Penn Psychotherapy Project, Luborsky
and colleagues published in the Psychological Bulletin a review of all previous
studies examining predictors of therapy outcome. This review (Luborsky et al.,
1971) became highly influential, eventually receiving recognition as a “citation
classic” by the Science Citation Index in 1981.
In 1975, Luborsky and colleagues published another highly cited and
influential paper reviewing the literature to date on comparative studies of
psychotherapies. The subtitle of the paper, “Everyone Has Won and All Must
Have Prizes,” was taken from the verdict of the dodo bird commenting on a
race in Lewis Carroll’s famous Alice’s Adventures in Wonderland. Originally
applied by Rosenzweig (1936) in a clinical analysis to convey the sense that
diverse psychotherapies achieve comparable success owing to common fac-
tors, Luborsky’s borrowing of the dodo’s verdict to summarize his seminal
review of the comparative effects of different psychotherapies has had a last-
ing impact on psychotherapy research. Since the publication of his paper, it
is actually rare to see a paper that addresses comparative effects of psychother-
apies that does not make reference to the “dodo bird verdict” and Luborsky’s
paper, either in support of his conclusion or attempting to refute it.
The primary results of the Penn Psychotherapy Project took several
years to appear in print, with the full set of results published in book form later
(Luborsky et al., 1988). The delay in publishing the results of the Penn Psy-
chotherapy Project (data collection was finished in 1973, but the first primary
paper giving results appeared in 1979) was probably due to a number of fac-
tors. One was that the funding for the study had ended and Luborsky’s pri-
mary collaborator, Jim Mintz, who had provided the methodological and
statistical expertise for the study, had moved on to another job. A second
factor is that the results of the study were largely negative: The outcomes of
psychotherapy were not very predictable from pretreatment information on
patients or therapists. Luborsky always preferred to tell a positive message
about his research studies, commenting once (to Paul Crits-Christoph) that
“there are two types of researchers, those that say there is nothing in the data
and those that say there is something in the data.” The lack of findings left
Luborsky searching for something to say. This then eventually spurred him to
attack the question anew: If pretreatment factors mattered little, what deter-
mined the outcome of psychotherapy?
The answer, of course, was that aspects of the process of psychotherapy
were particularly important to treatment outcome. The immediate stimulus
for Luborsky’s turning to creating new measures for studying the process of
psychotherapy was his participation in a panel at the Society for Psychother-
apy Research meetings in 1975. It was at this panel that Ed Bordin introduced
LESTER LUBORSKY 45
his influential ideas on the concept of the working alliance. In preparation
for the meeting, Luborsky began looking closely at transcripts of sessions from
the Penn Psychotherapy Study to see if he could devise a way of measuring the
alliance. The result was the creation of observer-rated scales to measure
the helping alliance (Luborsky’s term for what others referred to as the thera-
peutic alliance or working alliance), which he first published in a book chapter
(Luborsky, 1976). He subsequently published on the development of an
alliance questionnaire and conducted a variety of studies looking at the role
of the alliance in psychotherapy.
In many ways it appears as though Luborsky’s success at inventing a
scale to measure the alliance unleashed his creativity and energies that set the
stage for the rest of his career. Much of his research on psychotherapy up to
this point (1975) was atheoretical in nature. The breakthrough of seeing that
a central clinical notion like the alliance could be measured allowed him and
others to ask further theoretically and clinically important questions about
the process and outcome of psychotherapy, particularly psychodynamic psy-
chotherapy. This shift in creative energies is evident in Luborsky’s productiv-
ity: From 1945 to 1975 he published 87 publications and from 1975 to 2005
he published over 300 more.
Immediately after developing a measure of the alliance, Luborsky turned
his attention to another key aspect of clinical psychodynamic psychotherapy:
the assessment of the patient’s central relationship theme. The concept of the
CCRT was initially formulated in 1977 (Luborsky, 1977). With the CCRT,
Luborsky wished to reach with empirical tools the deepest roots of clinical
psychodynamic practice, that is, the transference, which is regarded as the
keystone and the most specific characteristic of psychoanalytic and psycho-
dynamic treatments. His efforts at measuring this element of psychotherapy
were very productive. Numerous studies used the CCRT in a variety of con-
texts (summarized in Luborsky & Crits-Christoph, 1998). Further, the work
on the CCRT fostered the emergence of other measures and methodologies
that aim at exploring psychotherapy processes that were thought before to be
too difficult to approach empirically.
The CCRT was also an important component of one of Luborsky’s other
most influential contributions: the codification of supportive–expressive psy-
chodynamic psychotherapy in a treatment manual (Luborsky, 1984). After
emerging from a period of “generic” or atheoretical psychotherapy research,
Luborsky had now turned back completely to his psychoanalytic roots that
were nurtured at the Menninger Foundation. The concept of a treatment
manual—a guide to assist in the training of therapists and the standardization
of treatment—was new to psychotherapy research. As part of his clinical
teaching of therapy, Luborsky had developed in 1976 a rough unpublished
guide to training psychodynamic psychotherapists called The Task of the Psy-
46 CRITS-CHRISTOPH ET AL.
chotherapist. Sensing that a more formal treatment manual would help move
the scientific study of psychodynamic therapy forward, he embarked on a
systematic description of how to do what he felt was typical psychodynamic
therapy. His model for the treatment was the supportive–expressive therapy
taught at the Menninger Foundation. A further impetus for Luborsky’s mov-
ing quickly on publishing his manual was the knowledge that Hans Strupp
was also working on a guide to psychodynamic therapy. Both treatment man-
uals were published in 1984 with the same publisher (Strupp & Binder, 1984;
Luborsky, 1984).
Luborsky’s supportive–expressive treatment manual became a major suc-
cess both clinically and in research circles. The manual was frequently used as
a training device in graduate programs and psychiatric residencies. The man-
ual, together with more specific addendums tailoring the treatment to specific
patient population, served as the basis for studies of supportive–expressive ther-
apy for generalized anxiety disorder, chronic depression, cocaine dependence,
personality disorders, opiate dependence, and cannabis dependence.
Beyond his publications, Luborsky influenced psychotherapy research
in many other ways. He was the fourth president (1973–1974) of the Society
for Psychotherapy Research (SPR), the only professional conference that he
regularly attended. Across a span of 32 years (from the first meeting in 1970
to 2002), we are aware of only one annual SPR meeting that he did not
attend, presenting a paper at each and every meeting he attended.
Not being in a psychology department, Lester did not work directly with
graduate students as an advisor. However, within the Department of Psychia-
try at the University of Pennsylvania, his influence as a clinical teacher of psy-
chiatric residents and postdoctoral fellows was enormous. His model of clinical
supervision was a group one. This group supervision was a sought-after aspect
of the psychiatric residency—with four to five residents or postdoctoral fellows
participating every year for the more than 25 years that he offered this group.
While other psychodynamic supervisors relied on the overwhelmingly large
and diverse psychoanalytic literature as the base for their teaching, Luborsky’s
approach to psychodynamic therapy, anchored in his supportive–expressive
therapy manual and the CCRT formulation, provided an understandable entry
into the complex psychoanalytic arena. Thus, Luborsky was typically the first
supervisor who made a generation of clinical trainees at the University of
Pennsylvania comfortable with the ambiguous task of being a therapist.
Within the research domain, Luborsky had a major influence on a num-
ber of young PhDs or MDs who were junior faculty at the University of Penn-
sylvania and went on to have careers in psychotherapy research. Art Auerbach
joined Luborsky at the beginning of the Penn Psychotherapy Project and con-
tinued working closely with him for about a decade. Jim Mintz was the proj-
ect director of the Penn Psychotherapy Project. Paul Crits-Christoph and
LESTER LUBORSKY 47
Jacques Barber were also hired by Luborsky as junior faculty in the Department
of Psychiatry. All four of these investigators went on to become presidents of
SPR. By this metric, Luborsky mentored significantly more future presi-
dents of SPR than did any other senior psychotherapy researcher in history.
Other significant collaborators at the University of Pennsylvania were Tom
McLellan, George Woody, and Charles O’Brien. Luborsky also had an influ-
ence internationally, developing strong collaborations with investigators and
clinicians in Germany (Horst Kächele), Canada (Louis Diguer, Howard Book),
and Australia (Brín Grenyer), among others.
CONCLUSION
REFERENCES
48 CRITS-CHRISTOPH ET AL.
Luborsky, L., & Crits-Christoph, P. (Eds.). (1998). Understanding transference: The
core conflictual relationship theme method (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Luborsky, L. B. (1954). A note on Eysenck’s article: The effects of psychotherapy: An
evaluation. British Journal of Psychology, 45, 129–131.
Luborsky, L. (1962). Clinician’s judgments of mental health: A proposed scale.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 7, 407–417.
Luborsky, L. (1972). Another reply to Eysenck. Psychological Bulletin, 78, 406–408.
doi:10.1037/h0020022
Luborsky, L. (1976). Helping alliances in psychotherapy: The groundwork for a study
of their relationship to its outcome. In J. L. Claghorn (Ed.), Successful psychother-
apy (pp. 92–116). New York, NY: Brunner/Mazel.
Luborsky, L. (1977). Measuring a pervasive psychic structure in psychotherapy:
The core conflictual relationship theme. In N. Freedman & S. Grand (Eds.),
Communicative structures and psychic structures (pp. 367–395). New York, NY:
Plenum Press.
Luborsky, L. (1984). Principles of psychoanalytic therapy. A manual for supportive-
expressive treatment. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Luborsky, L., Chandler, M., Auerbach, A. H., Cohen, J., & Bachrach, H. M. (1971).
Factors influencing the outcome of psychotherapy: A review of quantitative
research. Psychological Bulletin, 75, 145–185. doi:10.1037/h0030480
Luborsky, L. (1996). (Ed). Symptom-Context Method—Symptoms as opportunities in
psychotherapy. Washington: American Psychological Association.
Luborsky, L., Crits-Christoph, P., Mintz, J., & Auerbach, A. (1988). Who will bene-
fit from psychotherapy? Predicting therapeutic outcomes. New York, NY: Basic
Books.
Luborsky, L., Singer, B., & Luborsky, Lise. (1975). Comparative studies of psychother-
apies: Is it true that “Everyone has won and all must have prizes”? Archives of
General Psychiatry, 32, 995–1008.
Rosenzweig, S. (1936). Some implicit common factors in diverse methods of psy-
chotherapy. The American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 6, 412–415.
Strupp, H. H., & Binder, J. L. (1984). Psychotherapy in a new key: A guide to time-
limited dynamic psychotherapy. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Strupp, H. H., & Luborsky, L. B. (Eds.). (1962). Research in psychotherapy. [Proceed-
ings of a conference held in Chapel Hill, NC, May 1961.] Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
LESTER LUBORSKY 49
5
HANS STRUPP: A FOUNDER’S
CONTRIBUTIONS TO
A SCIENTIFIC BASIS FOR
PSYCHOTHERAPY PRACTICE
KARLA MORAS, TIMOTHY ANDERSON, AND WILLIAM E. PIPER
51
4th decades of his career while he was a Distinguished Professor at Vanderbilt
University (Bein et al., 2000; Henry, Strupp, Butler, Schacht, & Binder, 1993;
Strupp, 1993; Strupp & Hadley, 1979).
Hans Strupp remained an active, leading figure until about the year
2000, when his eloquent and incisive clinical–theoretical and research writ-
ing (e.g., Strupp, 1977) was increasingly challenged by Parkinson’s disease.
Hans’s sonorous voice and charming, subtle wit stayed with us longer. His
impact as mentor, colleague, and model for conducting one’s life in ways that
honor being human are as legendary as they are enduring.
MAJOR CONTRIBUTIONS
EARLY BEGINNINGS
HANS STRUPP 53
psychology was primarily due to pragmatic postdegree employability consid-
erations, rather than to the field’s intrinsic appeal to him, at least as it was
taught at the time. The same was not true of Freud’s writings. In Strupp’s
(n.d.) words:
Freud’s writings captivated me and opened horizons of unimaginable pro-
portions. Untutored as I was in psychoanalysis, much of what I read
escaped me but what I did comprehend left a deep impression. No other
writer had a comparable effect on my life. Freud has remained one of my
great heroes, the other being Mozart.
A fellow student, also a German refugee, serendipitously introduced
Strupp to Freud’s works in about 1940. By 1946, Strupp pursued clinically ori-
ented experiences, including a personal psychoanalysis, at the Washington
School of Psychiatry (WSP). The WSP was renowned for the interpersonal
model of one of its founders, Harry Stack Sullivan (Sullivan, 1953). Hans’s
decision to apply to the WSP was likely bolstered in 1945 by a chance meet-
ing with Frieda Fromm-Reichman, also a German refugee, who taught at
WSP and was a prominent psychiatrist at the famous Chestnut Lodge in
Maryland. Strupp never forgot “her indelible injunction: The therapist listens”
(Strupp, n.d., italics in original). He seemed to agree that “it is the crux of
all psychotherapy.”
The completion of his doctorate in 1954, when Strupp was 32 years
old, launched his psychotherapy research career. Although his doctoral sub-
specialty was social psychology, his dissertation project was psychotherapy
research. Strupp received instant and strong reinforcement for the effort. His
dissertation yielded three articles, all published in 1955 and in the most pres-
tigious, then and now, clinical research journal of the American Psycholog-
ical Association (e.g., Strupp, 1955), the Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology.1
Strupp’s “heart’s desire” became “to become a clinical psychologist,” but
he encountered difficulty because his educational background was not the
accepted route to the profession (Strupp, n.d.). A hurdle he could not clear
even with doctorate in hand was the lack of a predoctoral internship. Unsuc-
cessful applications for clinical positions and his signature persistence culmi-
nated in 1955 with a success—a grant from the National Institute of Mental
Health (NIMH) for a therapy research project. Strupp always believed that
Jerome Frank, a reviewer of the grant application (Strupp’s first) with whom
he met during the review process, was integrally responsible. To Strupp, the
grant was “one of the greatest strokes of luck in [his] professional career”
(Strupp, n.d.).
In Strupp’s view, his most constant goal was to elucidate how therapists
can purposefully and consistently potentiate beneficial effects—and also avoid
the opposite. The latter came to be a primary focus as his career progressed.
His keen interest in the therapist’s contribution is traceable to his first therapy
study, his dissertation (e.g., Strupp, 1955). It and other of his early studies led
HANS STRUPP 55
to what Strupp called a “serendipitous finding” that became a central, guid-
ing thread (Henry & Strupp, 1992):
I adduced evidence that negative attitudes toward a patient tended to
be associated with unempathic therapist communications and unfavor-
able clinical judgments, whereas the opposite was true of respondents
who felt more positively toward the patient. I considered this a serendip-
itous finding whose implications for research, training, and practice I
have been trying to explore ever since. (p. 437)
Any review of Strupp’s major accomplishments requires noting that
even his early research was characterized by rigorous scientific methods and
thinking—and at a time when few therapy research studies existed and when
most of them exhibited weak designs. Further, Strupp knew that it was crucial
to determine if psychotherapy can work: Does it potentiate more rapid and/or
more extensive benefits than the simple passage of time or informal, naturally
occurring sources of help, such as caring, wise friends? He also viewed evidence
that therapy can work as preliminary knowledge for both practice and science.
If therapies work, then the next main practice-relevant challenge is to dis-
cover why and how they work: Do they work for theoretically posited reasons?
Knowledge of mechanisms will enable practitioners to conduct treatments
systematically and efficiently. Vanderbilt I was among Strupp’s contributions
to a fundamental mechanism question of his, and contemporary, times.
Jerome Frank (1961, 1971) proffered his now classic common factors
model of how psychotherapies work in the early 1960s. The model partially
explained surprising but recurrent, then and now, findings of few if any statis-
tically significant differences between the outcomes of theoretically different
forms of psychotherapy. (More recent evidence indicates that the same often is
true for psychotherapy vs. medication.) The common factors model logically
attributes equivalent outcomes to variables that must be common to, that is,
shared by, different therapy approaches rather than to their unique, theory-
driven specific techniques. (Frank was aware of an alternative explanation:
Outcome measurement methods could be a key cause of no difference findings.)
Frank’s model, also cast as “nonspecific vs. specific factors,” became a
preoccupation for Strupp during groundbreaking NIMH-funded projects on
which he and Allen Bergin collaborated. One fruit of their effort was a dili-
gently crafted set of research questions and proposed experiments to answer
them. The questions and experiments appear in Changing Frontiers in the Sci-
ence of Psychotherapy (Bergin & Strupp, 1972), along with working papers in
which Strupp began to sketch out what became a classic experiment on non-
The period after Vanderbilt I’s launch was filled with intensive process–
outcome studies (e.g., Gomes-Schwartz, 1978) and innovative research. For
2Strupp (1993; Strupp & Hadley, 1979) knew that the finding could be challenged due to the semiana-
logue nature of the sample, which consisted of randomly selected male undergraduate students recruited
by mail, many of whom were not independently seeking treatment. The limitation contributed to the
Vanderbilt II design in which treated adults were recruited by advertisement for low-cost therapy.
HANS STRUPP 57
example, Strupp, in the scientific tradition of systematically exploring data to
better understand surprising monothetic (aggregated group level) findings
and develop new hypotheses, conducted a series of post hoc, within-therapist,
case study comparisons of four Vanderbilt I therapists’ good- and poor-outcome
patients (e.g., Strupp, 1980). He concluded that patient characteristics and
also weaknesses in some therapists’ responses to interpersonally challenging
patient features contributed to poorer outcomes. Additionally, Strupp and his
research team surveyed relevant experts’ views on the critical but neglected,
then and now, topic of potential negative effects of psychotherapy (Strupp,
Hadley, & Gomes-Schwartz, 1977). The team also developed an observer-
rated process measure to identify predictors and potential causes of negative
outcomes (Suh, Strupp & O’Malley, 1986).
Research of the foregoing type helped solidify Hans’s conclusion that
the common factors model, which supported the ongoing nonspecific vs. spe-
cific factors debate, was fatally flawed. The work also paved the way to both
Vanderbilt II and to what ultimately became his aforementioned second
major conclusion.
CONCLUSION
HANS STRUPP 59
because most psychotherapists’ best intentions and natural relationship skills
are inadequate to optimally serve individuals who stand most to benefit from
what psychotherapy, as a unique treatment modality, might offer.
REFERENCES
Bein, E., Anderson, T., Strupp, H. H., Henry, W. P., Schacht, T. E., Binder, J. L.,
& Butler, S. (2000). The effects of training in time-limited dynamic psycho-
therapy: Changes in therapeutic outcome. Psychotherapy Research, 10, 119–132.
doi:10.1080/713663669
Bergin, A. E. (2007). Hans H. Strupp (1921–2006). American Psychologist, 62, 249.
doi:10.1037/0003-066X.62.3.249
Bergin, A. E., & Strupp, H. H. (1972). Changing frontiers in the science of psycho-
therapy. Chicago, IL: Aldine-Atherton.
Butler, S. F., & Strupp, H. H. (1986). “Specific” and “nonspecific” factors in psycho-
therapy: A problematic paradigm for psychotherapy research. Psychotherapy, 23,
30–40. doi:10.1037/h0085590
Frank, J. D. (1961). Persuasion and healing: A comparative study of psychotherapy.
Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins Press.
Frank, J. D. (1971). Therapeutic factors in psychotherapy. American Journal of Psycho-
therapy, 25, 350–361.
Goldfried, M. R. (Ed.). (1982). Converging themes in psychotherapy: Trends in psycho-
dynamic, humanistic, and behavioral practice. New York, NY: Springer.
Gomes-Schwartz, B. (1978). Effective ingredients in psychotherapy: Prediction of
outcome from process variables. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46,
1023–1035. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.46.5.1023
Henry, W. P., Schacht, T. E., & Strupp, H. H. (1986). Structural analysis of social
behavior: Application to a study of interpersonal process in differential psycho-
therapeutic outcome. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, 27–31.
doi:10.1037/0022-006X.54.1.27
Henry, W. P., & Strupp, H. H. (1992). The Vanderbilt Center for Psychotherapy
Research. In D. K. Freedheim (Ed.), History of psychotherapy: A century of change
(pp. 436–442). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Henry, W. P., Strupp, H. H., Butler, S. F., Schacht, T. E., & Binder, J. L. (1993).
Effects of training in Time-Limited Dynamic Psychotherapy: Changes in
therapist behavior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 434–440.
doi:10.1037/0022-006X.61.3.434
Strupp, H. H. (1955). An objective comparison of Rogerian and psychoanalytic tech-
niques. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 19, 1–7. doi:10.1037/h0045910
Strupp, H. H. (1973). On the basic ingredients of psychotherapy. Journal of Consult-
ing and Clinical Psychology, 41, 1–8. doi:10.1037/h0035619
HANS STRUPP 61
6
AARON T. BECK:
THE COGNITIVE REVOLUTION
IN THEORY AND THERAPY
STEVEN D. HOLLON
Aaron T. Beck is one of the leading clinical theorists of the last half
century. Coming of age at a time when dynamic theory was monolithic and
psychoanalysis the dominant method of treatment, he began his career by
seeking to confirm the primacy of unconscious motivations and ended up for-
mulating a theory of disorder that emphasized the role of inaccurate beliefs
and errors in thinking that were largely accessible to conscious introspection.
This novel cognitive theory led him to formulate principles of change that he
codified into a cognitive therapy that has become one of the most widely
practiced and best empirically supported interventions in the field today
(DeRubeis & Crits-Christoph, 1998). Beck has been the recipient of numerous
honors and is the only psychiatrist to have received research awards from the
American Psychological Association, the American Psychological Society, the
American Psychiatric Association, and the Institute of Medicine. He has
lectured throughout the world and was named one of the most influential psy-
chotherapists of all time by the American Psychologist (July 1989). Perhaps the
capstone of his career came when he received the Lasker Award, the nation’s
most prestigious medical prize (New York Times, September 17, 2006). In
announcing the award, Dr. Joseph L. Goldstein, the chairman of the Lasker
63
jury, called cognitive therapy “one of the most important advances—if not
the most important advance—in the treatment of mental diseases in the last
50 years.”
MAJOR CONTRIBUTIONS
64 STEVEN D. HOLLON
processing that serve to warp the way that information is processed in the direc-
tion of existing beliefs. He also introduced the rudiments of an approach to
treatment in which he laid out basic strategies for teaching patients how to
explore the accuracy of their own beliefs and how to protect themselves from
the biasing effects of schema-driven processing. In so doing, he drew heavily on
recent advances in cognitive psychology that emphasized the way in which
existing beliefs could bias information processing and developed a sophisticated
set of clinical procedures to offset those proclivities.
By the early 1970s he had developed a coherent approach to treatment
based on the principles that he called cognitive therapy. At this time, there was
no evidence that any psychosocial intervention was as efficacious as medications
in the treatment of depression or even superior to pill-placebo controls. Before
the end of the decade, he and colleagues at the University of Pennsylvania pub-
lished a randomized controlled trial in which cognitive therapy outperformed
medications, the current standard of treatment (Rush, Beck, Kovacs, & Hollon,
1977). Patients in that trial were not only as likely to respond to cognitive
therapy as to medications, but they also were considerably more likely to stay
well after treatment termination. This was not only the first time that any psy-
chosocial treatment had held its own with medication in the treatment of
depression but also the first clear evidence of an enduring effect for psychother-
apy, something that had long been claimed but never before demonstrated.
Thirty years of subsequent research have fully supported these early
claims. Cognitive therapy is now widely recognized as an empirically sup-
ported psychosocial treatment for depression, and the proposition that it has
enduring effects not found for medications is well supported in the literature
(Hollon, Stewart, & Strunk, 2006). Moreover, evidence of its efficacy is not
limited to depression; he has taken the lead in extending the approach to
other disorders and many others have followed. There now is clear evidence
for its efficacy and enduring effects for nearly all of the nonpsychotic dis-
orders (including panic and the anxiety disorders, somatic disorders such as
hypochondriasis, eating disorders such as anorexia and bulimia, substance
abuse and addiction, marital distress, and a variety of both internalizing and
externalizing childhood disorders), as well as emerging work in the personal-
ity disorders (including borderline personality disorder and antisocial person-
ality) and the psychoses (including bipolar disorder and the schizophrenias;
Butler, Chapman, Forman, & Beck, 2006).
Beck has been committed to the empirical evaluation of his theories
and the therapy that developed from them. He viewed his theories as provi-
sional only and sought to subject them to empirical disconfirmation in as
timely a manner as possible. His empirical studies have consistently pitted his
preferred intervention against the best existing treatments in the field, and
he has shown a keen awareness of the need to balance investigator allegiance
AARON T. BECK 65
to be sure that each modality tested has a fair chance at success. The quality
and impartiality of these investigations have contributed greatly to his impact
on the field, and the ease with which they have been replicated speaks to the
generalizability of the approach. Cognitive theory has evolved over the years,
and cognitive therapy has been revised on the basis of both experimental
findings and clinical insights, allowing it to be generalized to numerous other
disorders across a variety of clinical situations. There is even evidence that
cognitive therapy can be taught to persons at risk in the service of preventing
the emergence of subsequent distress. His commitment to the principles of sci-
ence and his willingness to subject his beliefs to potential disconfirmation have
contributed both to shaping the approach and to the success it has enjoyed.
EARLY BEGINNINGS
Aaron T. Beck was the youngest of five children of parents who both emi-
grated from Russia (see Weishaar, 1993, for a detailed description of his early
life). His father was a printer by trade and an intellectual by nature who was a
strong supporter of socialistic principles. His mother was a strong-willed woman
who gave up her dream of going to medical school to care for her younger sib-
lings after the untimely death of her own mother. There are indications that
his mother became depressed herself following the loss of a daughter during the
great influenza epidemic of 1919 and that her distress remitted only after the
birth of her youngest son, Aaron (perhaps his first successful cure).
Beck himself nearly died at age seven after a broken bone in his arm
became infected and he developed septicemia, an infection of the blood that
was nearly always fatal at that time. The surgery itself was traumatic. He was
separated from his mother without warning and put under the knife before
the anesthetic had taken effect. This experience led to fears of abandonment
and health-related phobias that he only mastered later in life by thinking
through their cognitive antecedents and testing their accuracy by exposing
himself to the situations that he feared (one of his reasons for later going into
medicine). Moreover, he missed so much time from school that he was held
back a grade, leading him to think of himself as “dumb and stupid,” but he
sought help from his older brothers and came to excel in school, leading him
to believe that he could overcome misfortune through hard work and use a
“never say die” attitude to turn “a disadvantage adversity into an advantage”
(Weishaar, 1993, p. 10). In many respects, In many respects, the seeds of his
later theoretical innovations were sown by his own early life experiences;
his initial response to these traumatic life events was to develop exagger-
ated beliefs (reasonable under the circumstances) that overestimated the risk
inherent in health-related or educational situations and that underestimated
66 STEVEN D. HOLLON
his own capacity to cope—beliefs he overcame by thinking them through and
forcing himself to engage in what he feared, to test their accuracy.
He graduated first in his high school class and followed his older broth-
ers to Brown University. Although he majored in English and political sci-
ence, he decided to pursue a career in medicine, only to be discouraged by a
professor from applying to medical school because of the anti-Semitism of the
time. He pursued extra premed course work nonetheless and graduated magna
cum laude in 1942, having been elected to Phi Beta Kappa. He applied to only
three or four medical schools and was admitted to Yale, in part because he
shared an interest in the works of Aldous Huxley with the professor of pedi-
atrics who conducted his admissions interview.
He was not originally interested in psychiatry or psychotherapy and
embarked on a residency in neurology after completing medical school. He
was attracted to neurology by its disciplined diagnostic procedures and its
capacity to pinpoint the precise location of lesions in the nervous system on
the basis of careful clinical observation. It was during the course of a manda-
tory 6-month rotation in psychiatry that he became fascinated with its sub-
ject matter and with psychoanalysis in particular, which he thought would
reveal the inner workings of the human mind. He completed a 2-year fellow-
ship at Austin Riggs Center in Stockbridge Massachusetts (supervised by Erik
Erikson) before volunteering to serve at the Valley Forge General Hospital,
an army hospital near Philadelphia. He was board certified in psychiatry in
1953 and became an instructor in psychiatry at the University of Pennsylva-
nia the following year. He graduated from the Philadelphia Psychoanalytic
Institute in 1958 and became an assistant professor at Penn the following
year, receiving his first research grant (to study dreams) in the process.
It was during his internship that he met his wife, Phyllis. She was a jour-
nalist by training who wrote for Time magazine and the Berkshire Eagle (Pitts-
field, MA) before taking a master’s degree in social work and ultimately going
on to law school while raising four children. She enjoyed a long and success-
ful career in the law and became the first woman to be elected a superior court
judge in Pennsylvania. Retired from the bench, she is now the chief counsel
for the Barnes Foundation.
ACCOMPLISHMENTS
AARON T. BECK 67
made psychoanalytic principles seem counterintuitive. Nonetheless, he decided
to suspend his initial disbelief and threw himself into his analysis. He remained
troubled by its lack of a scientific basis and came to believe that empirical evi-
dence was necessary to convince the hard-headed skeptic. He came to view psy-
chological research as a way to validate psychoanalytic concepts and make them
acceptable to the scientific community.
He decided to focus on depression, the most frequent disorder in his
practice, and began a series of studies designed to show that depression was a
consequence of unconscious rage against others that became repressed and
turned against the self. Adhering to the notion that dreams represented the
“royal road to the unconscious,” he began to study the dreams of his depressed
patients and to compare them with the dreams of patients who were not
depressed. What he found, to his dismay, was that the dreams of his depressed
patients actually contained less hostility than those of his nondepressed con-
trols. What they did contain were the same themes of rejection and failure
that patients expressed in their waking conscious verbalizations. He consid-
ered other more complex interpretations that preserved the primacy of the
unconscious (none supported by the data) before coming to what he termed
the “simple-minded hypothesis” that the negative way in which patients see
themselves is actually the basic process, rather than the derivative of uncon-
scious forces (Beck, 2006, p. 1139). The essence of this formulation was that
there was no need to go deeper; a model based on his patients’ internal rep-
resentations of themselves, their experiences, and their future could account
for both their dreams and their symptoms.
Experimental work was crucial to this paradigmatic shift. In an effort to
test between the competing interpretations, Beck and colleagues put depressed
and nondepressed patients in controlled performance situations and manipu-
lated their success or failure. In opposition to psychodynamic theory, which
would have predicted a masochistic worsening of mood in reaction to posi-
tive feedback, what he found is that both the mood and performance of his
depressed patients improved when they experienced instances of success
(Loeb, Beck, & Diggory, 1971; Loeb, Feshbach, Beck, & Wolf, 1964). These
studies not only contradicted predictions generated from psychodynamic
theory but also pointed to the kind of clear and pragmatic strategies that could
be used clinically to disconfirm the patients’ negative beliefs.
Over the next several years, he began to experiment with helping
patients recognize their own internal dialogue (often in the form of fleeting
negative “automatic thoughts” that consisted of demeaning self-evaluations
and distorted misinterpretations of innocuous events) and found that he could
guide them to examine the validity of their own beliefs through a process of
Socratic questioning and the use of behavioral experiments. He described his
approach, which he called cognitive therapy, in a series of case reports that
68 STEVEN D. HOLLON
he presented at the Association for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy
(Beck, 1970). John Rush, one of his residents at the time, encouraged him to
conduct a randomized controlled trial that found that cognitive therapy was
both superior to and longer lasting than medication (Rush et al., 1977).
While Beck was developing his cognitive theory and therapy of depres-
sion, he also began to ask whether cognitive processes played a role in other
disorders and whether he could identify a specific profile of distortions and
beliefs that was associated with each. In order to carry out this work, he orga-
nized a clinic, called first the Mood Clinic and later the Center for Cognitive
Therapy, that enabled him to study individuals with a variety of disorders and
to use their clinical materials to probe the nature of their distress and to
explore methods of intervention with his colleagues and his students. His
treatise on the Cognitive Therapy of the Emotional Disorders provided an early
road map to this approach (Beck, 1976). The basic strategy that he followed
was to collect a large number of clinical observations for a specific disorder
(focusing on the automatic thoughts and underlying beliefs), derive a formu-
lation for the particular disorder, and devise inventories and rating scales to
measure the specific clinical variables. Then, on the basis of the cognitive
profile, he would adapt the generic cognitive model to fit the specific charac-
teristics of the disorder.
He and his colleagues and students would then generate clinical inter-
ventions based on these principles and observations that were collated into
treatment manuals that could be used in randomized controlled intervention
trials. This basic research strategy, first developed in his work on depression
(Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979), was subsequently applied to the study
and treatment of panic and the anxiety disorders (Beck, Emery, & Greenberg,
1985), personality disorders (Beck, Freeman, & Associates, 1990), and sub-
stance abuse (Beck, Wright, Newman, & Liese, 1993).
Beck has had a long-standing interest in suicide and its prevention.
Early in his career he and his colleagues constructed a new classification sys-
tem and developed instruments to validate it (Beck, Resnik, & Lettieri,
1974). He found, for example, that persons with elevated levels of hopeless-
ness were at significant risk of ultimate suicide and that predictors of sub-
sequent risk among suicide attempters included expressions of regret over the
failure of their attempt(s) and increasing intensity of ideation across attempts
(Beck, Morris, & Beck, 1974). This work culminated in the development of
a brief cognitive therapy for suicide that has been shown to cut the frequency
of subsequent attempts in half among high-risk patients with a recent history
of attempts (Brown et al., 2005).
AARON T. BECK 69
During the 1970s and 1980s, Beck made a series of extended visits to
Britain and particularly Oxford University, where the chairman of psychiatry,
Michael Gelder, was strongly supportive of the cognitive therapy approach.
There he met with John Teasdale and Mark Williams (joined by Zindel Segal
of Toronto) who added meditation to develop a mindfulness-based approach
to cognitive therapy. He had a strong influence on David M. Clark and Paul
Salkovskis (later joined by Anke Ehlers), who used the systematic approach
he applied to depression to adapt cognitive therapy to the treatment of a vari-
ety of anxiety disorders, including panic, social phobia, hypochondrias, post-
traumatic stress disorder, and obsessive–compulsive disorder, among others.
This latter group (who subsequently moved to the Institute of Psychiatry in
London) has done some of the most elegant translational work in the field
today. Dominic Lam, another former trainee, adapted the approach to the pre-
vention of recurrence in bipolar disorder, and Christopher Fairburn at Oxford
and Kelly Bemis Vitousek in Hawaii have been strongly influenced by cogni-
tive therapy in their respective work with eating disorders. David Kingdon and
Douglas Turkington in England applied the basic framework to successfully
adapt cognitive therapy to the treatment of residual symptoms in schizophre-
nia (Kingdon & Turkington, 1994). Other research groups in the United
Kingdom have found similarly promising results in the treatment of both acute
and chronic patients. This work has been slow to be adopted in the United
States (where clinical lore has long presumed that you cannot reason with
someone who is psychotic) but is beginning to make its way across the Atlantic
(Beck & Rector, 2005). In addition to his work on mindfulness, Zindel Segal
in Toronto has investigated the neural processes underlying change in treat-
ment in cognitive therapy versus medications, and David A. Clark in Nova
Scotia has explored the cognitive process underpinning depression and related
anxiety disorders. Much of the work in the United States has focused on
depression (including that by Robert J. DeRubeis and Steven D. Hollon),
and Martin Seligman at the University of Pennsylvania and Judy Garber at
Vanderbilt have each investigated the role of cognitive interventions in the
prevention of depression in at-risk children and adolescents.
Other Contributions
70 STEVEN D. HOLLON
the Academy of Cognitive Therapy, an organization that certifies compe-
tence in cognitive therapy based on actual tape ratings, and has founded both
the Center for Cognitive Therapy at the University of Pennsylvania and the
Beck Institute in nearby Bala Cynwyd.
Dr. Beck has authored over 450 articles in peer-reviewed journals and
17 books and treatment manuals. Many of these publications (on depression,
anxiety, personality disorders, and other clinical problems) have become clas-
sics in the field (e.g., Beck, 1967, 1976; Beck et al., 1979, 1985; Beck, Free-
man, & Associates, 1990). In addition, Beck and his colleagues and students
have written a number of treaties and self-help manuals for the public (Beck,
1988; Burns, 1980; Greenberger & Padesky, 1995). Beck has developed a num-
ber of major self-report and clinical rating instruments. The Beck Depression
Inventory (Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961) is the most
widely used self-report instrument in the field and the Hopelessness Scale
(Beck, Weissman, Lester, & Trexler, 1974) has been shown to be a better pre-
dictor of risk of suicide than depression (Beck, Brown, Berchick, Stewart, &
Steer, 1990). The Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale is widely used as a measure of
beliefs and attitudes conferring risk for depression (Weissman & Beck, 1978)
and the Cognitive Therapy Scale is widely used as a measure of competence
with cognitive therapy (Young & Beck, 1980). He also developed the Suicide
Intent Scale (Beck, Morris, & Beck, 1974) and the Scale for Suicide Ideation
for work in the assessment of suicide and the prediction of risk (Beck, Brown,
& Steer, 1997).
INFLUENCES
AARON T. BECK 71
today; Marika Kovacs, a noted developmental psychopathologist who does
longitudinal research on the development of risk for depression in children;
and David M. Clark, one of the most innovative and highly regarded anx-
iety researchers of his generation. Others greatly influenced by Beck include
his daughter, Judith Beck (a major theorist in her own right), David Burns,
Robert J. DeRubeis, Arthur Freeman, Steven D. Hollon, Christine Padesky,
Jackie Persons, Brian F. Shaw, Kelly Bemis Vitousek, and Jeffrey Young, among
others, three of whom have gone on themselves to become presidents of ABCT.
CONCLUSION
Aaron T. Beck has had a major impact on what the field thinks about
psychopathology and the nature of treatment for the mental disorders. He is
the architect of one of the most widely used and efficacious psychotherapies
in the field today. His work has been prodigious and his influence profound,
in part because of his insistence on subjecting his ideas to the stiffest possible
empirical tests. His theoretical notions about the role of cognition in the eti-
ology and maintenance of psychopathology have revolutionized the field, and
the clinical innovations he developed have coalesced into one of the most
widely practiced and best empirically supported interventions of the day. He
is truly a giant in the field.
REFERENCES
72 STEVEN D. HOLLON
Beck, A. T., Emery, G., & Greenberg, R. (1985). Anxiety disorders and phobias: A cog-
nitive perspective. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Beck, A. T., Freeman, A., & Associates. (1990). Cognitive therapy of personality dis-
orders. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Beck, A. T., & Rector, N. (2005). Cognitive approaches to schizophrenia: Theory
and therapy. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 577–606. doi:10.1146/
annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.144205
Beck, A. T., Resnik, H. L. P., & Lettieri, D. (1974). The prediction of suicide. Bowie,
MD: Charles Press.
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). The cognitive therapy of
depression. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J. E., & Erbaugh, J. K. (1961). An
inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561–571.
Beck, A. T., Weissman, A., Lester, D., & Trexler, L. (1974). The measurement of
pessimism: The Hopelessness Scale. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
42, 861–865. doi:10.1037/h0037562
Beck, A. T., Wright, F. D., Newman, C. F., & Liese, B. S. (1993). Cognitive therapy
of substance abuse. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Beck, R. W., Morris, J. B., & Beck, A. T. (1974). Cross-validation of the Suicidal
Intent Scale. Psychological Reports, 34, 445–446.
Brown, G. K., Have, T. T., Henriques, G. R., Xie, S. X., Hollander, J. E., & Beck,
A. T. (2005). Cognitive therapy for the prevention of suicide attempts: A
randomized controlled trial. JAMA, 294, 563–570. doi:10.1001/jama.294.5.563
Burns, D. D. (1980). Feeling good: The new mood therapy. New York, NY: William
Morrow.
Butler, A. C., Chapman, J. E., Forman, E. M., & Beck, A. T. (2006). The empirical
status of cognitive-behavioral therapy: A review of meta-analyses. Clinical Psy-
chology Review, 26, 17–31. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2005.07.003
DeRubeis, R. J., & Crits-Christoph, P. (1998). Empirically supported individual and
group psychological treatments for adult mental disorders. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 66, 37– 52. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.66.1.37
Greenberger, D., & Padesky, C. A. (1995). Mind over mood. New York, NY: Guilford
Press.
Hollon, S. D., Stewart, M. O., & Strunk, D. (2006). Cognitive behavior therapy has
enduring effects in the treatment of depression and anxiety. Annual Review of
Psychology, 57, 285–315. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190044
Kingdon, D. G., & Turkington, D. (1994). Cognitive-behavioral therapy of schizophrenia.
New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Loeb, A., Beck, A. T., & Diggory, J. (1971). Differential effects of success and failure
on depressed and nondepressed patients. The Journal of Nervous and Mental
Disease, 152, 106–114. doi:10.1097/00005053-197102000-00003
AARON T. BECK 73
Loeb, A., Feshbach, S., Beck, A. T., & Wolf, A. (1964). Some effects of reward upon
the social perception and motivation of psychiatric patients varying in depression.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 68, 609–616. doi:10.1037/h0044260
Rush, A. J., Beck, A. T., Kovacs, M., & Hollon, S. (1977). Comparative efficacy of cog-
nitive therapy and pharmacotherapy in the treatment of depressed outpatients.
Cognitive Therapy and Research, 1(1), 17–37. doi:10.1007/BF01173502
Weishaar, M. E. (1993). Aaron T. Beck. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Weissman, A. N., & Beck, A. T. (1978, November). Development and validation of the
dysfunctional attitude scale: A preliminary investigation. Paper presented at the
meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Toronto, Canada.
Young, J., & Beck, A. T. (1980). Cognitive Therapy Scale: Rating manual. Unpublished
manuscript, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia.
74 STEVEN D. HOLLON
II
DOES PSYCHOTHERAPY
WORK?
7
IRENE ELKIN: “THE DATA ARE
ALWAYS FRIENDLY”
KARLA MORAS AND M. TRACIE SHEA
Clinical psychologist Irene Elkin1 has had unique and profound influ-
ence on psychotherapy research since the early 1970s. Her career has shown
her to be a consummate scientist, possessing a keen intellect and fund of
knowledge, unassailable scientific integrity, and commitment to bringing the
best science possible to research on psychotherapy. Elkin’s achievements
also document her commitment to balancing the tension between conduct-
ing rigorous experimental science and preserving the essentially human
qualities of psychotherapy. Among her many contributions, Elkin is perhaps
best known for spearheading a study that had a paradigm-shifting impact
on the field during the last decades of the 20th century, the Treatment of
Depression Collaborative Research Program (TDCRP; e.g., Elkin, 1994;
Elkin et al., 1989, 1995; Parloff & Elkin, 1992) for the U.S. National Institute
of Mental Health (NIMH). The TDCRP demonstrated the feasibility of
several powerful scientific methods for use in psychotherapy outcome research.
It thereby helped set new methodological standards for the field—standards
that strengthened conclusions that could be drawn about a therapy’s potential
1Elkin published some of her work as Irene Waskow, using her former husband’s surname.
77
effects, its effects compared with those of other forms of therapy and also of
medication, and related practice-relevant questions. More broadly, Elkin has
brought conceptual and methodological clarity to many of the field’s key
questions.
MAJOR CONTRIBUTIONS
IRENE ELKIN 79
published in the Journal of Consulting Psychology in 1962, she examined the
effects of the therapist’s behavior on the process of therapy, an enduring
theme in her research (e.g., Elkin, 1999; Elkin et al., 2006). While complet-
ing her PhD at UW, Elkin worked closely with Carl Rogers and Gendlin on
Rogers’s famed study of psychotherapy with patients with schizophrenia. She
identifies Rogers, Harry Stack Sullivan, and Miller and Dollard as major influ-
ences on her research career. Elkin’s career-long research mantra, “the data
are always friendly,” likely was adapted from a similar statement that has been
attributed to Rogers.
After graduate school, Elkin held a postdoctoral research fellowship at
the NIMH intramural Psychology Laboratory, under the sponsorship of David
Shakow. There she was exposed to a variety of psychotherapy approaches and
researchers, learning about various therapeutic strategies from Paul Bergman
and receiving training from Allen Dittmann in verbal and nonverbal emo-
tional expression. A central, enriching, and lifelong colleagueship also began
at the time: Elkin met Morris Parloff, then chief of the laboratory’s section on
personality, with whom she later worked closely on the conceptualization and
initiation of the TDCRP, and other shared contributions to therapy research.
In 1969, Elkin joined the Psychotherapy and Behavioral Intervention Sec-
tion, marking the start of her many years of influence on the field of therapy
research from NIMH.
ACCOMPLISHMENTS
IRENE ELKIN 81
for outpatient depression: cognitive therapy (Beck, Rush, Shaw & Emery,
1979) and interpersonal psychotherapy (Klerman, Weissman, Rounsaville,
& Chevron, 1984). A pharmacological treatment (imipramine) served as a
standard reference condition, and a pill placebo was included as a control for
imipramine, to determine if imipramine was a valid standard reference con-
dition in the study’s sample of depressed patients. The placebo also served as
control for the psychotherapies.
Elkin invested an enormous amount of time, energy, and passion in
every aspect of the TDCRP throughout the many years covering its planning,
implementation, and publication of findings. Her rigorous scientific standards
and meticulous approach were crucial for such a large-scale, highly visible,
and potentially influential—both on research and practice—study. At the
inception of the TDCRP, the multisite, randomized controlled comparative
clinical trial design had been widely used in psychopharmacology research but
not psychotherapy research. Applying the design to test the efficacy of psycho-
therapies introduced new challenges. For example, in contrast to psychophar-
macology studies, standardized delivery of psychotherapy could not be assumed.
Ensuring adequate delivery of the putative “active ingredient” is far more
complex in psychotherapy, requiring detailed articulation and assessment of
specific therapist interventions. Development of manualized therapies and
the assessment of therapist adherence to, and competence with, a specified
form of therapy had begun, but they were in an early stage when the TDCRP
was initiated. Elkin recognized the critical importance, for a comparative out-
come study’s internal validity, of being able to demonstrate that therapists
administered the treatments as defined by their developers (adherence) and
with an acceptable level of competency, and also to show that the treat-
ments compared were distinct in their delivery. Through contracts, Elkin
provided for the development and application of a reliable and comprehen-
sive observer-based measure of therapist adherence to the TDCRP treatments,
the Collaborative Study Psychotherapy Rating Scale (CSPRS; Hollon,
Waskow, Evans, & Lowry, 1984). The CSPRS quickly became regarded as
the “Cadillac” of such instruments. It was used to assess adherence to the
theoretically key interventions of each therapy tested in the TDCRP and
also to the clinical management component of the pharmacotherapy condi-
tions (Hill, O’Grady, & Elkin, 1992). The use of such scales is now standard
practice in the field.
One key aim of the TDCRP, to determine if the multisite clinical trial
design was feasible for psychotherapy research, yielded a clear answer: Yes.
Many such studies have been conducted since (e.g., Barlow, Gorman, Shear,
& Woods, 2000). Further, the main TDCRP outcome paper (Elkin et al.,
1989) alone has been cited more than 1,000 times to date, a sign of the study’s
huge impact. Although some controversy arose about interpretation of the
For several years while at NIMH, Elkin headed the Treatment Devel-
opment and Process Research section of the Psychosocial Treatments Research
Branch. She first began to master process research concepts and methods dur-
ing her dissertation study and early work with Carl Rogers and others. Elkin’s
foundation in therapy process research no doubt highlighted, for her, the
TDCRP’s potential value beyond its implications for assessing the efficacy of
psychotherapies. She knew that it also could serve the field as a psychother-
apy process-outcome study, enabling many key theoretical and practice-
relevant mechanisms of action questions to be investigated. Elkin worked
to ensure that the TDCRP’s potential in this regard was realized.
The TDCRP study design, methods, and other planning allowed for
both the collection and archiving of data needed to conduct process-outcome
analyses. For example, standardized instruments as well as measures developed
specifically for the TDCRP were included to examine questions relating to
(a) the effects of patient and therapist characteristics, (b) the therapeutic
relationship, and (c) differential effects of different therapies. Data from such
IRENE ELKIN 83
instruments have been used by a number of researchers as well as by Elkin to
address a variety of questions. For example, based on data from the Attitudes
and Expectations form developed in the TDCRP, Elkin and others found
that congruence between a patient’s assigned treatment and his or her pre-
treatment predilections (patient–treatment fit) predicts early engagement
in therapy (Elkin et al., 1999). Also, in one illustrative TDCRP process-
outcome study, Krupnick et al. (1996) examined the relationship between
the therapeutic alliance and overall treatment outcomes in both the psychother-
apy and pharmacotherapy treatment conditions. The results were a valuable
addition to existing findings that the strength of the patient–therapist alliance
relates to therapy outcomes across treatments.
Generativity
Elkin’s foresight about the TDCRP’s potential to enable both the exam-
ination and heuristic exploration of fundamental mechanisms (process and
process-outcome) questions is among her most generative accomplishments.
Realization of this foresight required her perseverant effort to ensure that
TDCRP data were archived and thereby made widely available to the field.
Elkin oversaw the transfer of the main data to a TDCRP Public Access Data
Tape that many non-TDCRP investigators have used. She also arranged, in
collaboration with two of the TDCRP research sites and Paul Crits-Christoph
at the University of Pennsylvania (UP), to have TDCRP assessment and
treatment session audio- and videotapes archived for use by investigators at
UP. The archives generated influential therapy process and other research
(e.g., Tang & DeRubeis, 1999).
Also, as previously noted, Elkin worked to support and advance psy-
chotherapy research for many years and in a variety of ways via resources
available at NIMH. Her “Fantasied Dialogue” chapter (Waskow, 1975) is an
early illustration of her mentoring via consultation to the field. Prominent
and diverse contemporary investigators who have made major, practice-
relevant contributions, such as Marsha Linehan and Leslie Greenberg, credit
Elkin for nurturing them toward successful therapy research careers. While at
NIMH, Elkin also helped guide the field by taking an active role in the annual
conference of the Society for Psychotherapy Research (SPR). Aside from her
own numerous panels and presentations, her informal, encouraging consul-
tations to researchers remain among her trademarks at SPR conferences. In
her last year at NIMH, Elkin and Ken Howard organized one of the first pre-
SPR institutes on statistical methods useful for psychotherapy research. Elkin
further served the field as an early president of SPR, 1978–1979. Most recently,
Elkin’s mentoring activities as a professor in the School of Social Service
Administration at the University of Chicago helped to expand the role of
CONCLUSION
IRENE ELKIN 85
study findings, because they provide models of clear and precise inductive and
deductive scientific logic, as well as superb methodological and statistical
sophistication (e.g., Elkin et al., 1989; Elkin et al., 2006); (c) rejoinders to
challenges of TDCRP findings because the responses illustrate the strength
of broad and deep scientific expertise, immutable scientific integrity, and the
ability to maintain focus on the key scientific issues at hand (Elkin et al.,
1990, 1996; Elkin, Falconnier & Martinovich, 2007).
We end on a personal note. In addition to her remarkable professional
contributions, Irene has always remained a warm and engaging friend. She is
honest and true and loyal. Her great sense of fun and energy for life can be
heard in her laugh, which is one way to find Irene in a very crowded room.
We are honored and proud to have been among her mentees.
REFERENCES
Barlow, D. H., Gorman, J. M., Shear, M. K., & Woods, S. W. (2000). Cognitive-
behavioral therapy, imipramine, or their combination for panic disorder: A ran-
domized controlled trial. JAMA, 283, 2529–2536. doi:10.1001/jama.283.19.2529
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depres-
sion. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Elkin, I. (1994). The NIMH Treatment of Depression Collaborative Research Pro-
gram: Where we began and where we are. In A. E. Bergin & S. L. Garfield (Eds.),
Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (4th ed., pp. 114–139). New York,
NY: Wiley.
Elkin, I. (1999). A major dilemma in psychotherapy outcome research: Disentan-
gling therapists from therapies. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 6,
10–32. doi:10.1093/clipsy/6.1.10
Elkin, I., Falconnier, L., & Martinovich, Z. (2007). Misrepresentations in Wampold
and Bolt’s critique of Elkin, Falconnier, Martinovich, and Mahoney’s study of
therapist effects. Psychotherapy Research, 17, 253–256.
Elkin, I., Falconnier, L., Martinovich, Z., & Mahoney, C. (2006). Therapist effects
in the NIMH Treatment of Depression Collaborative Research Program. Psycho-
therapy Research, 16, 144–160. doi:10.1080/10503300500268540
Elkin, I., Gibbons, R. D., Shea, M. T., & Shaw, B. F. (1996). Science is not a trial (but
it sometimes can be a tribulation). Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
64, 92–103. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.64.1.92
Elkin, I., Gibbons, R. D., Shea, M. T., Sotsky, S. M., Watkins, J. T., Pilkonis, P. A.,
. . . Hedeker, D. (1995). Initial severity and differential treatment outcome
in the NIMH Treatment of Depression Collaborative Research Program.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 841–847. doi:10.1037/0022-
006X.63.5.841
IRENE ELKIN 87
Tang, T. Z., & DeRubeis, R. J. (1999). Sudden gains and critical sessions in cognitive-
behavioral therapy for depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
67, 894–904. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.67.6.894
Waskow, I. E. (1975). Fantasied dialogue with a researcher. In I. E. Waskow & M. B.
Parloff (Eds.), Psychotherapy change measures: Report of the Clinical Research
Branch Outcome Measures Project (pp. 273–327). Washington, DC: U.S. Gov-
ernment Printing Office.
Waskow, I. E., & Parloff, M. B. (Eds.). (1975). Psychotherapy change measures: Report
of the Clinical Research Branch Outcome Measures Project. Washington, DC: U.S.
Government Printing Office.
Kenneth Irwin Howard was born in 1932 and died in 2000. He was a
professor of psychology at Northwestern University in Evanston, Illinois,
for 32 years, and before that was deputy director of research and chief of
the program in measurement and evaluation at the Illinois Department of
Mental Health’s Institute for Juvenile Research. In these positions, he
made major contributions to the field of psychotherapy research and also
to research on adolescence and delinquency, psychometrics, and clinical
research methodology. Howard coauthored six books and more than 175 book
chapters, articles, and research instruments. He served as coeditor of
the Journal of Clinical Psychology (1996–2000) and as consulting or associ-
ate editor for six other journals. He held a National Institute of Mental
Health (NIMH) Senior Research Scientist Award and was a fellow of the
American Psychological Association’s (APA’s) Divisions 5 (Evaluation,
Measurement, and Statistics), 12 (Society of Clinical Psychology), and
29 (Psychotherapy), the American Psychological Society, the Association
The first four sections were prepared by David E. Orlinsky; the fifth section was prepared by Frederick L.
Newman, Robert J. Lueger, and David E. Orlinsky; the sixth section was prepared by Robert J. Lueger;
the seventh section was prepared by Wofgang Lutz; and the eighth and ninth sections were prepared by
Fredrick L. Newman. All coathors shared in the final version.
89
for Clinical Psychosocial Research, and the American Association for the
Advancement of Science. He cofounded and led the Society for Psy-
chotherapy Research. His scientific and professional work has been recog-
nized by many major groups, including APA.
EARLY INFLUENCES
90 ORLINSKY ET AL.
late 1970s (e.g., Howard, Orlinsky, & Perilstein, 1976), culminating in a book
on the Varieties of Psychotherapeutic Experience (Orlinsky & Howard, 1975).
These studies demonstrated the feasibility and the importance of methodolog-
ically objective research on subjective experience for understanding the process
of therapy, going well beyond the clinical case history in rigor yet also correct-
ing the overreliance of process researchers on audio recordings.
Ken Howard’s most distinctive contribution to this project was his talent
for psychological measurement and quantitative analysis and his singular per-
spective on clinical work. Through a deft strategy of multilevel factor and clus-
ter analyses, Howard empirically defined the dimensions and types of patients’
in-session experience, therapists’ in-session experience, and their often oblique
but clinically significant interdependence. The latter include several patterns
of conjoint experience, such as therapeutic alliance vs. defensive impasse,
healing magic vs. uncomfortable involvement, and sympathetic warmth
vs. conflictual erotization, where the experiences of patient and therapist were
statistically linked despite apparent differences in their manifest content
(Orlinsky & Howard, 1975). These patterns provide clues for therapists about
the nature of a patient’s experience, not through observing the patient directly
but rather by focusing on their own experiences during sessions.
KENNETH I. HOWARD 91
growth, burnout), for other persons in patients’ and therapists’ lives, and for the
social and cultural environments in which therapy takes place. Research on
both input and output variables were examined in relation to the varied facets
of therapeutic process that researchers had studied. The latter included the ther-
apeutic contract (e.g., goals, methods, schedule, fees), therapeutic operations
(intervention procedures or techniques), the therapeutic bond (interpersonal
rapport and compatibility), patient’s and therapist’s internal self-relatedness
(openness, defensiveness), and therapeutic realizations (in-session impacts).
The predictive value of the Generic Model was subsequently tested in several
studies (e.g., Kolden & Howard, 1992; Saunders, Howard & Orlinsky, 1989).
NORTHWESTERN–CHICAGO STUDY
92 ORLINSKY ET AL.
ment despite the diverse theories and processes of treatment. The solution to
this problem was essential to solving the second problem—identifying a
mathematical model that would describe the relationship between amount of
treatment and amount of effect. The potential value of a mathematical model
lay in its ability to predict the amount of benefit for selected durations of treat-
ment and thus to identify those applicants for psychotherapy who are in need
of treatment and to set rational limits on the duration of therapy.
Analyzing data on 2,431 patients from 15 studies conducted by them-
selves or others over a period of 30 years, Howard and his colleagues identi-
fied a log-linear relationship between the amount of therapy (dose) and the
amount of improvement (effect) in which the outcome of interest was the
proportion of patients achieving a dichotomously expressed result (improved
or not improved). This has become known as the dose–effect model (Howard,
Kopta, Krause, & Orlinsky, 1986). Improvement could be mathematically
modeled by a negatively accelerating curve with a higher frequency of improve-
ment earlier compared with later in treatment. The extrapolated curve did not
reach zero at its origin but suggested that 10% to 18% (expressed in confidence
intervals) of patients showed some improvement even before the first session
of psychotherapy, possibly as a result on the patient’s sense of efficacy in hav-
ing made the appointment. With eight sessions of psychotherapy, 48% to
58% of the patients showed a measurable degree of improvement, and after a
year of treatment, the improvement curve approached asymptote, with about
85% of patients having shown some improvement. By the common criterion
of effective exposure used in pharmacological dosage studies, the amount of
psychotherapy at which 50% of patients respond to treatment is about six to
eight sessions. Analyses taking account of diagnosis at intake revealed that
dosage estimates vary in relation to patient characteristics. Finally, the upper
bound of responsiveness to psychotherapy, approximately 85% improvement,
matched the results of earlier large meta-analyses of psychotherapy outcomes.
In the 20 years after its publication, the dose–effect paper was cited
over 350 times in the psychological or medical literature, providing a mea-
sure of its influence on other researchers. The dose–effect relationship iden-
tified by Howard has been replicated by colleagues in his own group using
larger samples and different self-report measures of improvement, and by
other researchers with even larger sample sizes, different self-report measures,
and alternative statistical analyses such as survival analysis.
Introduction of the phase model of outcome extended the dose–effect
model to explore the differential response rates of diverse outcome domains
(Howard, Lueger, Maling, & Martinovich, 1993). The phase model showed
that three problem areas—general demoralization, symptoms, and deficits
in life functioning—tend to change at different rates, and perhaps at differ-
ent times, in the improvement trajectory. These changes define outcome
KENNETH I. HOWARD 93
phases defined as remoralization, remediation of symptoms, and rehabilita-
tion of deficits.
The phase model proposes that these phases occur in a sequence sug-
gesting a probable causal order. In the remoralization phase, clients begin to
recover from feelings of powerlessness and hopelessness. This may start with
the client taking steps to seek help, and it may continue in early sessions of
therapy as symptoms and problems are clarified, some initial success in mas-
tery of problems is realized, and clients gain hope that therapy will help in
addressing their problems. In the remediation phase, therapy mobilizes the
client’s coping skills and continues to develop a sense of mastery or control,
resulting in the reduction of symptomatic distress. In the rehabilitation
phase, clients attempt to modify or control long-standing maladaptive pat-
terns of thought and behavior, to develop more adaptive interpersonal and
self-management skills, and to consolidate their ability to achieve positive
change in their lives.
The value of the phase model for investigators is indicated by the
many times it has been cited in the research literature. The basic precepts
of the model—domains and probabilistic causal sequence—have been repli-
cated in a large sample of patients using somewhat different measures of the
same constructs. The value of the model for those who formulate service
delivery policy is that it informs the stakeholders how much treatment will
likely be required to produce improvement in various outcome domains.
PATIENT-FOCUSED RESEARCH
AND EXPECTED TREATMENT RESPONSE
94 ORLINSKY ET AL.
the generalizability of results to uncontrolled real-world settings where inter-
nal validity often is compromised by treatment delivery conditions (Howard,
Krause, & Orlinsky, 1986; Howard, Orlinsky, & Lueger, 1995).
Effectiveness research addresses the question of how well a treatment works
as it is administered in actual clinical settings. These studies emphasize the gener-
alizability of findings (external validity) and deal with the application of validated
treatments to the circumstances of clinical practice. However, the quasi-exper-
imental and systematic naturalistic designs of effectiveness research tend to
sacrifice certainty of internal validity. Moreover, like efficacy research, effec-
tiveness research says nothing about the progress of an individual patient.
Patient-focused research, by contrast, asks how well a particular treatment
works for individual patients (i.e., whether the patient’s condition is respond-
ing to the treatment). This model recognizes that patients differ in their
expected outcomes or expected courses of treatment. To understand and eval-
uate the observed course of an individual’s response to therapy, one needs to
know the reasonable expected course of treatment for that patient, and a
method for calculating an expected treatment response was developed using
hierarchical linear modeling. This approach provides an individualized pro-
file of each patient’s progress in relation to the expected course of treatment
response for that patient by modeling a patient’s change over treatment as
a log-linear function of session number and pretreatment clinical charac-
teristics (Lutz, Martinovich, & Howard, 1999). The prediction weights of
a patient’s initial clinical status and the individual differences in growth
curve characteristics allow the prediction of the course of treatment for
individual patients once their intake information is available. Ongoing
therapeutic effectiveness can be assessed for a single patient by tracking the
patient’s actual progress in comparison with expected progress based on pre-
treatment clinical characteristics.
In addition to many different research and outcomes management
applications, the value of the model for clinical practice has been illus-
trated with many examples of successful and unsuccessful treatment cases.
Further studies using this model have found (a) that predictions for change
in later sessions are enhanced by incorporating information about the
change that patients experienced during early sessions, (b) that initial pre-
dictions based on patient characteristics are reliable, (c) that predictions
are useful for clinical case management, (d) that the model can be applied
to different diagnostic groups and various symptom patterns, and (e) that
a three-level hierarchical linear model can identify differential therapist
influence on patient change.
Ken Howard recognized that the validity of growth curve prediction
weights for any particular patient depends on the extent to which the study
sample (reference group) is representative of the population of which that
KENNETH I. HOWARD 95
patient was a member and that most predictors work only for specific sub-
sets of patients (Krause, Howard, & Lutz, 1998). To address this problem,
an extended expected response model using nearest neighbor techniques
was introduced. The nearest neighbor approach identifies those previously
treated patients in the reference group who most closely match the target
patient (hence “nearest neighbors”) on intake variables. It then uses this
homogeneous subgroup to generate predictions of treatment progress for the
target patient.
The patient-focused model has been applied to large databases in the
United Kingdom and the United States, and it has been used for continuous
patient feedback into clinical practice to identify potential treatment failures
early in the course of therapy. In this respect, patient-focused research has the
potential to inform the clinical decisions of therapists in the course of treat-
ment and thus to reduce the scientist–practitioner gap (Howard et al., 1996).
For many years Howard and his colleagues argued that the random-
ized clinical trial had only limited use in setting the standards and policies
for what is now called evidence-based practice (Howard, Krause, & Lyons,
1993; Krause & Howard, 2003). They clarified issues, such as attrition and
selection bias, that severely limited the inferences that can be drawn from
randomized clinical trials, and they stressed the inadequacy of relying only
on mean group differences and effect sizes to assess the value of treatments
(Howard, Krause, & Vessey, 1994). They promoted the use of “systematic
naturalistic” designs in studies, following a research protocol implemented
in actual treatment settings with a minimum of intrusion on the clinical
practice (Howard et al., 1996). In place of no-treatment and/or alternative-
treatment control conditions, Howard argued for comparison of clinical
samples with representative samples of “normal” persons in order to assess
whether, and to what extent, patients progressed in a clinically significant
direction, that is, toward behaviors statistically indistinguishable from
those in a nonclinical population similar to those who entered treatment.
Over a series of studies, they argued that one could provide an empirical
basis for assessing the clinical significance of specific interventions as a
function of a variety of moderator variables and thereby estimate what
works best for whom (Howard, Krause, Caburnay, Noel, & Saunders, 2001;
Howard, Krause, & Lyons, 1993; Lutz, Martinovich, & Howard, 1999;
Lyons & Howard, 1991; Saunders, Howard, & Newman, 1988). Howard
and his colleagues must be recognized as pioneers in the use of what is now
96 ORLINSKY ET AL.
called practice-based evidence, which became possible in large part through
his example in the application of sound statistical methods and his leader-
ship in encouraging others to depart from exclusive reliance on data from
randomized clinical trials.
KENNETH I. HOWARD 97
they developed methods capable of providing data from naturalistic settings
that could impact treatment guidelines and standards and influence evidence-
based practice and policy (e.g., in special issues of the Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology that he instigated). The influence of Ken Howard’s later
research may be seen in the adaptation by several research groups of the clin-
ical significance concept and community norms in their use of practice-based
evidence and clinical progress feedback to the therapists as means of improv-
ing service outcomes in the United States, United Kingdom, Switzerland, and
Germany; the development of new instrumentation for assessing outcomes
of mental health services; and the development of practice-based research
networks.
It is probably no accident that the research groups that have done most to
develop and apply these concepts, methods, and statistical procedures had their
intellectual home in the Society for Psychotherapy Research—a society that
Howard cofounded and served as its first president and later as its first executive
officer. In all his work, Ken Howard was known as a man of remarkable intelli-
gence, unusual wit, and great personal charm. He is remembered by us and many
others as a devoted friend and generous mentor.
REFERENCES
Howard, K. I., Kopta, S. M., Krause, M. S., & Orlinsky, D. E. (1986). The dose-effect
relationship in psychotherapy. American Psychologist, 41, 159–164.doi:10.1037/
0003-066X.41.2.159
Howard, K. I., Krause, M. S., Caburnay, C. A., Noel, S. B., & Saunders, S. M. (2001).
Syzygy, science, and psychotherapy: The Consumer Reports study. Journal of Clin-
ical Psychology, 57, 865–874. doi:10.1002/jclp.1055
Howard, K. I., Krause, M. S., & Lyons, J. S. (1993). When clinical trials fail: A guide
to disaggregation. In L. S. Onken, J. D. Blaine, & J. J. Boren (Eds.), Behavioral
treatments for drug abuse and dependence (NIDA Research Monograph No. 137,
pp. 291–302). Washington, DC: National Institute for Drug Abuse.
Howard, K. I., Krause, M. S., & Orlinsky, D. E. (1969). Direction of affective influence
in psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33,614–620.
doi:10.1037/h0028299
Howard, K. I., Krause, M. S., & Orlinsky, D. E. (1986). The attrition dilemma:
Toward a new strategy for psychotherapy research. Journal of Consulting and Clin-
ical Psychology, 54, 106–110. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.54.1.106
Howard, K. I., Krause, M. S., & Vessey, J. (1994). Analyzing clinical trial data: The
problem of outcome overlap. Psychotherapy, 31, 302–307. doi:10.1037/h0090213
Howard, K. I., Lueger, R. J., & Kolden, G. G. (1997). Measuring progress and out-
come in the treatment of affective disorders. In L. M. Horowitz, M. J. Lambert,
98 ORLINSKY ET AL.
& H. H. Strupp (Eds.), Measuring patient change after treatment for mood, anxiety,
and personality disorders: Toward a core battery (pp. 263–281). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Howard, K. I., Lueger, R. J., Maling, M. S., & Martinovich, Z. (1993). A Phase Model
of psychotherapy: Causal mediation of change. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 61, 678–685. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.61.4.678
Howard, K. I., Moras, K., Brill, P. L., Martinovich, Z., & Lutz, W. (1996). Evaluation
of psychotherapy: Efficacy, effectiveness, and patient progress. American Psychol-
ogist, 51, 1059–1064. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.51.10.1059
Howard, K. I., & Orlinsky, D. E. (1972). Psychotherapeutic processes. In P. H. Mussen
& M. R. Rosenzweig (Eds.), Annual review of psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 615–668).
Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.
Howard, K. I., Orlinsky, D. E., & Lueger, R. J. (1995). The design of clinically rele-
vant outcome research: Some considerations and an example. In M. Aveline &
D. A. Shapiro (Eds.), Research foundations for psychotherapy practice (pp. 3–47).
Chichester, England: Wiley.
Howard, K. I., Orlinsky, D. E., & Perilstein, J. (1976). Contributions of therapists
to patients’ experiences in psychotherapy: A components of variance model
for analyzing process data. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 44,
520–526. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.44.4.520
Howard, K. I., Orlinsky, D. E., Saunders, S. M., Bankoff, E., Davidson, C., &
O’Mahoney, M. (1991). Northwestern University–University of Chicago psy-
chotherapy research program. In L. E. Beutler & M. Crago (Eds.), Psychotherapy
research: An international review of programmatic studies (pp. 65–74). Washing-
ton, DC: American Psychological Association.
Kolden, G. G., & Howard, K. I. (1992). An empirical test of the Generic Model of
Psychotherapy. Journal of Psychotherapy Practice and Research, 1, 225–236.
Krause, M. S., & Howard, K. I. (2003). What random assignment does and does not
do. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 59, 751–766. doi:10.1002/jclp.10170
Krause, M. S., Howard, K. I., & Lutz, W. (1998). Exploring individual change.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 838–845. doi:10.1037/0022-
006X.66.5.838
Lutz, W., Martinovich, Z., & Howard, K. I. (1999). Patient profiling: An application
of random coefficient regression models to depicting the response of a patient to
outpatient psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67,
571–577. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.67.4.571
Lyons, J. S., & Howard, K. I. (1991). Main effects analysis in clinical research: Sta-
tistical guidelines for disaggregating treatment groups. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 59, 745–748. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.59.5.745
Orlinsky, D. E., & Howard, K. I. (1975). Varieties of psychotherapeutic experience:
Multivariate analyses of patients’ and therapists’ reports. New York, NY: Teachers
College Press.
KENNETH I. HOWARD 99
Orlinsky, D. E., & Howard, K. I. (1978). The relation of process to outcome in psycho-
therapy. In S. Garfield & A. Bergin (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behav-
ior change (2nd ed., pp. 283–329). New York, NY: Wiley.
Orlinsky, D. E., & Howard, K. I. (1986). Process and outcome in psychotherapy. In
S. L. Garfield & A. E. Bergin (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior
change (3rd ed., 311–384). New York, NY: Wiley.
Saunders, S. M., Howard, K. I., & Newman, F. L. (1988). Evaluating the clinical
significance of treatment effects: Norms and normality. Behavioral Assessment,
10, 207–218.
Saunders, S. M., Howard, K. I., & Orlinsky, D. E. (1989). The Therapeutic Bond
Scale: Psychometric characteristics and relationship to treatment effectiveness.
Psychological Assessment, 1, 323–330. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.1.4.323
Allen E. Bergin has been a guiding light for the development and
growth of psychotherapy by demonstrating that psychotherapy works for the
majority of clients, pointing out the harm that psychotherapy can do, encour-
aging the use of a broad range of research methods that inform clinical practice,
and integrating new dimensions of human functioning in our understand-
ing and facilitation of change. These impressive contributions are noteworthy
in scientific, clinical, and practice communities over a span of 40 years. At the
center of Bergin’s influence on the field is the publication of the seminal Hand-
book of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change, his outstanding scholarship on the
general effects of psychotherapy, his inspirational effort to stimulate collabo-
ration in psychotherapy research, his timely analysis of deterioration effects,
his examination of the therapeutic effectiveness of paraprofessionals, and his
opening up new horizons of scholarship and research on religious values and
spirituality.
101
EARLY BEGINNINGS
MAJOR ACCOMPLISHMENTS
The Handbook
For most of its early history (extending into the 1930s) psychotherapy
relied almost exclusively on clinical reports, case studies, and theoretical
accounts of the effects of psychotherapy. Practice was based on theoretical alle-
giance with reference to authority figures or recourse to personal clinical expe-
rience. By 1970, a sufficient body of psychotherapy research existed to enable
Allen Bergin and Columbia University (TC) colleague Sol Garfield to edit
the first edition of the Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change (Bergin
& Garfield, 1971). Their anticipation was that the exponential increase in
empirical research would
guide practice so that harmful and useless methods will be discarded and
the best techniques (whether they be drugs, different methods of psycho-
therapy, or social intervention) will be used in the most efficient manner
to help individuals overcome their problems. (Bergin & Garfield, 1971,
p. xii)
Over its history, the Handbook became the primary reference on the
critical review and integration of empirical investigations of all the major
methods of psychotherapy. It rapidly ascended to the status of required read-
ing for a majority of graduate programs in clinical and counseling psychology,
and thereafter became a Science Citation Classic. It is not difficult to judge
the importance of the first and subsequent editions of the Handbook. Gener-
ations of practitioners have been guided by the carefully integrated evidence
for positive clinical practice and equally as important, it had a significant
influence on the nature and types of questions that were being asked and the
methodologies that guided psychotherapy researchers for decades.
Perhaps it is not surprising that during the latter half of his career,
Bergin increasingly began to turn his professional attention to broader social
issues that were pertinent to the science and practice of psychotherapy,
including the topics of values and psychotherapy, religion and mental health,
and spirituality and psychotherapy. Bergin had long been interested in such
issues. As a young adult at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology and
then at Reed College, in Portland, Oregon, Bergin considered himself agnos-
tic regarding matters of faith. Although he respected the scientific enterprise
and thrived on the rigor of empirical inquiry, Bergin found himself person-
ally unsatisfied by the deterministic and reductionistic assumptions of the
natural and behavioral sciences. After a couple of years of intense question-
ing and searching, Bergin’s spiritual quest led him to enroll at Brigham Young
University and to convert to the Church of Jesus Christ of Latter-day Saints
ADDITIONAL ACHIEVEMENTS
Bergin was among those who founded the Society for Psychotherapy
Research (SPR), serving as its fifth president, 1974–1975, and he organized its
first international meeting (London, 1975). SPR, like the Handbook, was ded-
icated to following the evidence wherever it led. Bergin’s work always embod-
ied and exemplified the attitude of his early mentor Rogers, who emphasized
that “the facts are always friendly, every bit of evidence one can acquire . . .
leads one that much closer to what is true” (Rogers, 1961, p. 26). Always
eager to spread the word about the importance and excitement of investigat-
ing psychotherapy, Bergin attracted many of his 1971 clinical students to the
Second International Annual Meeting of SPR in Saddle Brook, New Jersey,
across the Hudson River from Manhattan. During the latter years of his
career, despite significant health challenges, Bergin remained productive in
scholarship, editing the fourth edition of the Handbook of Psychotherapy and
Behavior Change and coauthoring and coediting two other volumes about spir-
ituality and psychotherapy (Richards & Bergin, 1997, 2000) in which a vari-
ety of authors made suggestions for more effective, culturally sensitive practices
for religious clients from a diversity of Western and Eastern spiritual tradi-
tions. Bergin remained open to diverse views.
113
MAJOR CONTRIBUTIONS
EARLY BEGINNINGS
ACCOMPLISHMENTS
In Bern, a new era in his activities began, as he now had better resources
for the realization of many of his ideas. A major postulate was to allow students
to integrate practice with the theoretical knowledge acquired at the univer-
sity, very much in the sense of the Boulder scientist–practitioner model. With
his colleagues, Grawe installed an outpatient clinic in which qualified stu-
dents had the opportunity to practice psychotherapy with patients. At this
Praxisstelle he and his coworkers ran a comparative randomized study, in
OTHER CONTRIBUTIONS
CONCLUSION
Klaus Grawe was a prolific academic writer who published more than
150 articles and book chapters, as well as five books. Although he elaborated
his view of mental problems and psychotherapy in many publications, his
emphasis was on the continuous development of knowledge, rather than on
the elucidation of particular concepts. For example, the approach of general
psychotherapy that he developed remained for him a vehicle or template for
research, rather than a specific theory in competition with the traditional
schools of psychotherapy.
REFERENCES
Beutler, L. E., & Clarkin, J. (1990). Systematic treatment selection: Toward targeted
therapeutic interventions. New York, NY: Brunner/Mazel.
Beutler, L. E., Moleiro, C., Malik, M., Harwood, T. M., Romanelli, R., Gallagher-
Thompson, D., & Thompson, L. (2003). A comparison of the Dodo, EST, and
ATI indicators among co-morbid stimulant dependent, depressed patients. Clin-
ical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 10, 69–85. doi:10.1002/cpp.354
Caspar, F. (2007). Plan Analysis. In T. Eells (Ed.), Handbook of psychotherapeutic case
formulations (2nd ed., pp. 251–289). New York, NY: Guilford.
Gassmann, D., & Grawe, D. (2006). General change mechanisms: The relation
between problem activation and resource activation in successful and unsuccess-
ful therapeutic interactions. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 13(1), 1–11.
doi:10.1002/cpp.442
Grawe, K. (1995). Grundriss einer Allgemeinen Psychotherapie [Blueprint of a gen-
eral psychotherapy]. Psychotherapeut, 40, 130–145.
Grawe, K. (1997). Research informed psychotherapy. Psychotherapy Research, 1, 1–19.
doi:10.1080/10503309112331334001
Grawe, K. (2004). Psychological therapy. Toronto, ON: Hogrefe & Huber.
Grawe, K. (2006). Neuropsychotherapy. How the neurosciences can inform effective
psychotherapy. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Grawe, K., Donati, R., & Bernauer, F. (1994). Psychotherapie im Wandel. Von der
Konfession zur Profession [Psychotherapy in transition. From confession to
profession]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.
129
EARLY BEGINNINGS
Marvin Goldfried was born in Brooklyn in 1936 to parents who had fled
Eastern Europe in the early 1900s. As the first person in his family to go beyond
high school, he attended Brooklyn College and majored in psychology. A num-
ber of faculty members in the Psychology Department were active, empirical
researchers, and as an undergraduate he was encouraged by several professors to
attend graduate school. Maintaining an active, empirically based research pro-
gram would become a sustaining theme in his future academic career.
Goldfried received his graduate education in clinical psychology at the
University of Buffalo, now known as the State University of New York at
Buffalo. While projective testing and psychoanalytic theory were all the
rage at the time, he found that much of what he read was speculative, with
little or no research backing (Goldfried, 2001). By contrast, he also received
a firm grounding in principles of learning and perception that were based
on empirical research evidencebut which, at the time, seemed to have lim-
ited application for clinical practice. It was at an early point in his clinical
graduate training that Goldfried experienced firsthand the gap between
clinical practice and research.
While still in graduate school he also had the good fortune to meet and
have dinner with Paul Meehl. This was an especially rare treat for Goldfried,
as he had read virtually everything Meehl had written and had enormous
respect for his insights on research, practice, and the philosophy of science.
At one point during the evening, however, someone asked Meehl about the
extent to which his clinical work was informed by research. Without any hes-
itation, he replied, “Not at all.” As a clinical psychology graduate student,
struggling to adopt the identity of a scientist–practitioner, Goldfriend found
Meehl’s response deeply disheartening and, as it turned out, impactful.
Reflecting on this event in later years, he stated, “I don’t think I ever fully
recovered” (M. R. Goldfried, personal communication, January 2007). It is
noteworthy that since his graduate school days—over 50 years ago—bridging
the gap between research and clinical practice has become a defining theme
that has shaped his many contributions to clinical psychology training,
research, and practice.
A high point in his graduate career involved the completion of an intern-
ship at the Veterans Administration hospital in Palo Alto, California, in the
late 1950s. It was truly a golden era, with Ullmann and Krasner laying the
groundwork for behavior therapy at one end of the Psychology Department
while, at the other end, Bateson, Haley, and Weakland developed systemic
interventions for families. The exposure to a diverse range of theoretical,
research, and therapy approaches during his internship placement may have
planted the seeds for his later interest in psychotherapy integration.
MAJOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Cognitive–Behavior Therapy
Psychotherapy Integration
Psychotherapy Process
Across the decades and throughout his career, Goldfried has contributed
significantly to the study of the psychotherapy process. His work on psycho-
therapy integration, in particular, took the form of comparative process and
process–outcome research. Many practitioners are committed to going beyond
the boundaries of a single theoretical orientation, but until recently there was
little in the research literature to support doing so.
In a seminal 1980 article in American Psychologist, Goldfried persua-
sively argued that treatment methods prescribed by particular approaches
are largely (though not entirely) idiosyncratic manifestations of a smaller
number of change principles that cut across disparate psychotherapies. He
described such principles of change as heuristics or guidelines at a middle-
level of abstraction that exists somewhere between the specific treatment
methods (e.g., interpretation, cognitive restructuring, two-chair) and the
global theoretical systems (e.g., psychoanalytic, cognitive–behavioral, expe-
riential). The delineation and identification of key principles of change (e.g.,
establishment of productive relationship, provision of a new view of self) in
differing therapy approaches has offered the field a new integrative concep-
tualization of how client change happens in psychotherapy.
Although not the first to explicate common factors or to promote prin-
ciples of change, Goldfried spurred a renewed interest in these matters and
offered an explanation for a perplexing paradox in psychotherapy research,
specifically, that diverse forms of psychotherapy tend to produce equivalent
outcomes. The principles of change have guided Goldfried and his colleagues
in their investigating of unique and common mechanisms of change across
different forms of psychotherapy.
As part of their research program, they developed an instrument to mea-
sure the therapist’s focus in attempting to offer the patient a new view of self
(Goldfried, Newman, & Hayes, 1989). In one of the earliest studies, Goldfried
and colleagues found that cognitive–behavioral (CB) and psychodynamic–
interpersonal (PI) therapists placed a comparable focus on intrapersonal and
OTHER ACCOMPLISHMENTS
CONCLUSION
REFERENCES
Castonguay, L. G., Goldfried, M. R., Wiser, S., Raue, P. J., & Hayes, A. H. (1996).
Predicting outcome in cognitive therapy for depression: A comparison of unique
and common factors. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 497–504.
doi:10.1037/0022-006X.64.3.497
141
MAJOR CONTRIBUTIONS
EARLY BEGINNINGS
Mike Lambert was one of five children born and raised in Salt Lake City,
Utah. His father was a small business owner who finished his career operat-
ing a successful travel agency. His mother stayed at home to raise the children.
Lambert was part of a large and engaged extended family that worked, played,
worshipped, and traveled together. He was an energetic youth who enjoyed
outdoor activities. As a teenager, he was an avid sportsman who excelled in
wrestling—he was the state champion in his weight class as a junior in high
school. Later in life, he took up tennis and biking, but he also enjoyed
basketball and other sports. When a young faculty member, he occasionally
ended up in a wrestling match with a student or two when traveling to
research conferences. Although the students were often bigger, Lambert was
a formidable challenge who could never be pinned. Lambert has shared fond
memories of his childhood with his students and displayed the interpersonal
ease that is often typical of a child raised in a large family.
Lambert married his high school sweetheart soon after he graduated
from high school. His responsibilities were increased when they had their first
child before Lambert was 19 years old. As a result, he moved quickly into
adulthood with family responsibilities while also working to gain an under-
graduate and then graduate degrees. These were financially challenging years
for the Lamberts, filled with many hours of work, class, study, and caring for
family. By the time Lambert graduated with his PhD in counseling psychol-
ogy from the University of Utah in 1971, he and his wife, Linda, had four chil-
dren of their eventual five.
Lambert was first introduced to the field when he enrolled in a high
school psychology class. While still in high school, he also stumbled on and
read Karl Menninger’s The Human Mind (1945). Its numerous case studies
and in-depth analyses fascinated him, and he began to consider the possibil-
ity of becoming a practicing psychologist. He later enrolled in an educational
psychology course taught by Ted Packard, at the same time he was working
as an undergraduate research assistant for Ernst Beier. Both of these faculty
ACCOMPLISHMENTS
Research Studies
OTHER CONTRIBUTIONS
REFERENCES
Chiles, J. A., Lambert, M. J., & Hatch, A. L. (1999). The impact of psychological
interventions on medical cost offset: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychol-
ogy: Science and Practice, 6, 204–220. doi:10.1093/clipsy/6.2.204
Derogatis, L. R. (1994). SCL-90-R: Administration, scoring, and procedures manual.
Minneapolis, MN: National Computer Systems.
Hamilton, M. (1967). Development of a rating scale for primary depressive illness.
British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 6, 278-296.
Harmon, S. C., Lambert, M. J., Smart, D. M., Hawkins, E., Nielsen, S. L., Slade, K.,
& Lutz, W. (2007). Enhancing outcome for potential treatment failures: Ther-
apist-client feedback and clinical support tools. Psychotherapy Research, 17,
379–392. doi:10.1080/10503300600702331
Horowitz, L. M., Rosenberg, S. E., Baer, B. A., Ureno, G., & Villesenor, V. S. (1988).
Inventory of Interpersonal Problems: Psychometric properties and clinical appli-
cations. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 885–892.
Jacobson, N. S., & Truax, P. (1991). Clinical significance: A statistical approach to
defining meaningful change in psychotherapy research. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 59, 12–19. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.59.1.12
Lambert, M. J. (1976). Spontaneous remission in adult neurotic disorders: A revision
and summary. Psychological Bulletin, 83, 107–119. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.83.
1.107
Lambert, M. J. (1989). The individual therapist’s contribution to psychotherapy
process and outcome. Clinical Psychology Review, 9, 469–485. doi:10.1016/0272-
7358(89)90004-4
Rare is the individual who has not only contributed to her or his profes-
sion’s accumulated knowledge but has also created new research methods
through which that very knowledge is gained. Perhaps rarer still is the researcher
who has selflessly shared her accumulated wisdom with colleagues and stu-
dents and, in so doing, has both enriched the experiences of all in her profes-
sion and planted the seeds for its continued growth. Clara E. Hill is that rare
individual. This chapter, after briefly summarizing her major contributions
and describing her early beginnings, will then more closely examine her
accomplishments in a variety of areas.
MAJOR CONTRIBUTIONS
153
an empirically and theoretically grounded, integrated approach to teaching
helping skills, one that is clearly articulated and accessible to helpers of all
developmental stages, and one that has become a central resource in many
training programs. In addition, she applied the same level of theoretical and
empirical thoroughness to her work with dreams and here, too, developed an
extremely useful model for working with clients’ dreams in therapy, a model
that has shaped research in this area. An additional, and no less remarkable,
contribution was her development of an intensely rigorous qualitative
method that has enabled researchers to examine new questions, in new ways,
and thereby further our understanding of therapy phenomena. Finally, she has
contributed powerfully to her profession through several leadership positions.
In each of these areas, Clara Hill’s passion and enthusiasm for psychotherapy,
whether in learning more about how psychotherapy works or in helping to
train psychotherapists, have been extraordinary.
EARLY BEGINNINGS
Clara Edith Hill was born on September 13, 1948, in Shivers, Mississippi,
the fourth of her parents’ five children (a younger sister died in infancy). Her
father, trained as a Baptist minister, had moved his family to Mississippi to be
a preacher in a small church. When he could not make enough money to sup-
port his family, he tried being a teacher; the income afforded by that profes-
sion was also insufficient, so he moved the family to Rockford, Illinois, where
he worked in a factory and Clara’s mother became a social worker. Given her
father’s training, religion was a constant presence in Clara’s early life, with
nightly Bible meetings and church all day Saturday and Sunday, as well as
Wednesday evenings. The Hills were also avid readers, through which Clara
learned to think for herself and discover a world beyond the religiously con-
fining environment of her home. Clara also played the flute from fifth grade
through her first year in college, and it was from band that she learned her
work ethic—“results, not excuses.” An eagerness to examine fundamental
questions and not necessarily conform to established dicta found its roots
early in Clara Hill’s life, and also carried through to her professional career.
As an adolescent, for example, Clara questioned her father regarding
Christian tenets, remaining unsatisfied with his response. “You just have to
believe.” Since that time, she has remained skeptical of and resistant to any
force (whether religious, psychotherapeutic, or political) that tries to use per-
suasion or coercion to convert others or impose its views.
Hill’s first year as an undergraduate at Southern Illinois University
(SIU) was difficult—both her religious upbringing and her uncertainty
regarding what she wanted to study left her on the outside. Once she decided
ACCOMPLISHMENTS
Therapy Process
Hill’s most abiding interest over the years has been in the investigation
of therapist techniques, following her conviction that what therapists do
in sessions indeed makes a difference. In examining therapist intentions,
for example, she found that clients were not very accurate in identifying such
intentions. Intriguingly, though, she also found that they need not be, for
therapy to be successful. Through her exploration of therapist response
The bulk of Helping Skills is devoted to the three stages of Hill’s integrated
helping skills model. Exploration, the first stage, is rooted in client-centered
theory; insight, the second stage, is based on psychoanalytic and interpersonal
theories; action, stage three, is built upon behavioral and cognitive theories.
For each stage, Hill presents helping skills appropriate for that stage (e.g., open
questions and restatements in exploration, challenges and interpretation in
insight, process advisement in action) and not only defines and provides exam-
ples of each skill but also addresses therapist intentions for the skill, possible
client reactions and behaviors evoked by the skill, and potential difficulties
encountered when using the skill. Each skill-based chapter closes with help-
ful hints for using the skill, written exercises, and a practice activity through
which students’ mastery of the skills may be enhanced. Thus, Helping Skills
not only provides excellent theoretical and empirical grounding for Hill’s
conceptualization of the helping process but also presents specific interven-
tions appropriate for each stage, thereby enabling students to learn both the
what and the how of counseling. Students find this text to be engaging and
user-friendly, and they feel comfortable using it as a good primer for learning
the basic skills and becoming empathic helpers.
Of course it was not enough just to propose a model of training. Hill next
devoted considerable effort to understanding the effects of training (see Hill
& Lent, 2006; Hill, Stahl, & Roffman, 2007). To facilitate this endeavor, she
developed measures of helping skills usage (Hill & Kellems, 2002) and coun-
selor self-efficacy (Lent, Hill, & Hoffman, 2003; Lent et al., 2006). Recent
Dreams
Leadership Contributions
Clara Hill’s mark on her profession has extended beyond the empirical,
training, and methodological realms. She has served as editor of leading jour-
nals in her field (Journal of Counseling Psychology; Psychotherapy Research) and
views this role as one in which she is afforded an opportunity to guide and
shape her profession. Hill clearly enjoys her role as editor and firmly believes
that she has an obligation to provide educational reviews that mentor authors
in improving their manuscripts. Hill was also elected to serve as president of
both the North American Chapter of the Society for Psychotherapy Research
INFLUENCES
CONCLUSION
REFERENCES
Carkhuff, R. R. (1969). Human and helping relations (2 vol.). New York, NY: Holt,
Rinehart & Winston.
Castonguay, L., & Hill, C. E. (Eds.). (2007). Insight in psychotherapy. Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/11532-000
165
EARLY BEGINNINGS
Gendlin was born in Vienna, Austria, in 1926. Being Jewish, the family
fled the Nazis to the United States in 1939. Gendlin enrolled in the Depart-
ment of Philosophy at the University of Chicago, where he developed a method
of accepting a given system of thought in its entirety and then trying to for-
mulate any point in terms of the ideas and symbols from within that system
(see Gendlin, 1988). What interested him was that even though alternative
systems entailed differing formulations and implications, there was never-
theless a sameness that cut across the systems. Meanwhile, he did his master’s
thesis on the philosophy of Wilhelm Dilthey (see, e.g., Makkreel, 1992), who
challenged Enlightenment rationalist thought through his emphasis of the
importance of Erlebnis, generally translated as “lived experience.” This
emphasis led Gendlin to the role of the body in felt experiencing of some-
thing, whence came the notion of an underlying sameness conceptualized
as embodied experiencing, which became the linchpin of his thought.
Gendlin received his PhD in philosophy from the University of
Chicago in 1958, under the mentorship of Richard McKeon. Around that
time he became interested in psychotherapy because he saw it as involving
the process of symbolizing experiencing freshly. He joined Rogers’s team
at the Counseling Center there, later moving with the team to the Uni-
versity of Wisconsin, where Gendlin directed the team’s schizophrenia
research project. Later he joined the Psychology Department at the Uni-
versity of Chicago, during which time he also founded the Focusing Institutes
in Chicago and Spring Valley, New York, offering training in focusing and
focusing-oriented psychotherapy.
ACCOMPLISHMENTS
Psychotherapy Research
CONCLUSION
REFERENCES
175
MAJOR CONTRIBUTIONS
EARLY BEGINNINGS
ACCOMPLISHMENTS
Process–Experiential Psychotherapy
CONCLUSION
Butler, J. M., & Rice, L. N. (1963). Audience, self-actualization, and drive theory. In
J. M. Wepman, & R. W. Heine, R. W. (Eds.), Concepts of personality (pp. 79–110).
Hawthorne, NY: Aldine.
Butler, J. M., Rice, L. N., & Wagstaff, A. K. (1963). Quantitative naturalistic research.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Diamond, G. M., Diamond, G. S., & Hogue, A. (2007). Attachment-based family
therapy: Adherence and differentiation. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy,
33, 177–191. doi:10.1111/j.1752–0606.2007.00015.x
Duncan, S., Rice, L. N., & Butler, J. M. (1968). Therapists’ paralanguage in peak
and poor psychotherapy hours. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 73, 566–570.
doi:10.1037/h0026597
Elliott, R., Watson, J. C., Goldman, R., & Greenberg, L. (2004). Learning emotion
focused psychotherapy: The process-experiential approach to change. Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/10725–000
Greenberg, L. S., Rice, L. N., & Elliott, R. (1993). Facilitating emotional change: The
moment-by-moment process. New York, NY: Guilford.
Greenberg, L. S., & Watson, J. C. (2005). Emotion-focused therapy for depression.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/11286–000
Rice, L. N. (1965). Therapist’s style of participation and case outcome. Journal of
Consulting Psychology, 29, 155–160. doi:10.1037/h0021926
Rice, L. N. (1973). Client behavior as a function of therapist style and client resources.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 20, 306–311. doi:10.1037/h0034805
Rice, L. N. (1974). The evocative function of the therapist. In D. A. Wexler & L. N.
Rice (Eds.), Innovations in client-centered therapy (pp. 289–311). New York, NY:
Wiley.
Rice, L. N. (1983). The relationship in client-centered therapy. In M. J. Lambert (Ed.),
Psychotherapy and patient relationships (pp. 36–60). Homewood, IL: Dow Jones Irwin.
Rice, L. N. (1984). Client tasks in client-centered therapy. In R. Levant & J. Shlien
(Eds.), Client-centered therapy and the person-centered approach: New directions in
theory, research and practice (pp.182–202). New York, NY: Praeger.
Rice, L. N. (1988). Integration and the client-centered relationship. Journal of Integra-
tive and Eclectic Psychotherapy, 7, 291–302.
Rice, L. N. (1992). From naturalistic observation of psychotherapy process to micro the-
ories of change. In S. Toukmanian & D. Rennie (Eds.), Psychotherapy process
research: Paradigmatic and narrative approaches (pp. 1–21). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Rice, L. N., & Gaylin, N. (1973). Personality processes reflected in client vocal style
and Rorschach performance. Journal of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 40,
133–138. doi:10.1037/h0034052
Rice, L. N., & Greenberg, L. S. (Eds.). (1984). Patterns of change: Intensive analysis of
psychotherapy process (pp. 29–66). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
MAJOR CONTRIBUTIONS
185
validate a highly innovative events-based research paradigm that was designed
to precisely answer the question he had posed. The events-based approach
introduced a new, rational–empirical method for the intensive analysis of
psychotherapy events—task analysis—that held the promise of providing
greater understanding and specification of productive client performances
and the interventions that facilitate them. Over the next 20 years, events-
based task analyses of specific emotion markers and therapy tasks would
become an important line of research for Greenberg and his collaborators
(Elliott, Watson, Goldman, & Greenberg, 2004; Greenberg & Pinsof, 1986;
Greenberg, Rice, and Elliott, 1993), resulting in the development and refine-
ment of a unique, integrative psychotherapy model.
In a series of publications, Greenberg and graduate student Jeremy
Safran argued that emotional responses, not cognitions or beliefs, prompted
primary evaluations of goal attainment success and signaled the personal sig-
nificance of events for clients, and as such should be the focus of therapeutic
intervention. In Emotion in Psychotherapy (1987), they began to articulate the
principles of working with emotion. With another graduate student, Susan
Johnson, Greenberg suggested that emotion as a primary communication sys-
tem was of major importance for couple therapy (Greenberg & Johnson,
1988). Ultimately, this gave way to the development of two innovative,
research-supported psychotherapy approaches—emotion-focused therapy
(EFT) for individuals and couples. Emotion-focused psychotherapy integrates
gestalt methods into a client-centered relationship and synthesizes emo-
tional, systemic, and constructivist perspectives. Together with collaborators
and authors Laura Rice and Robert Elliott, Greenberg first laid down the guid-
ing principles of the individual approach in the book Facilitating Emotional
Change.
Having articulated a theoretically grounded, integrative model of huma-
nistic psychotherapy, Greenberg and his coauthors now found themselves
challenged by fellow psychotherapy researchers and funding agencies to
empirically demonstrate the efficacy of their new treatment approach to
working with clients. Although psychotherapy process researchers at heart,
he and his colleagues received a major grant in 1992 to compare the effects
of client-centered and process–experiential therapy for depression and ulti-
mately to relate process to both session and final outcome. Over the years,
strong empirical support has also been established for the efficacy of the indi-
vidual EFT approach for depression (Goldman, Greenberg, & Angus, 2006;
Greenberg & Watson, 1998) for resolving unfinished business and promot-
ing forgiveness to help resolve emotional injuries in individuals (Paivio &
Greenberg, 1995) and for the emotion-focused couples approach (Johnson,
Hunsley, Greenberg, &, Schindler, 1999; Goldman & Greenberg, 1992).
ACCOMPLISHMENTS
Emotion-Focused Psychotherapy
REFERENCES
Ellison, J. A., Greenberg, L. S., Goldman, R., & Angus, L. (2009). Maintenance of
gains following experiential therapies for depression. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 77(1), 103–112. doi:10.1037/a0014653
Elliott, R., Watson, J., Goldman, R. N., & Greenberg, L. S. (2004). Learning emotion-
focused therapy: The process-experiential approach to change. Washington, DC:
American Psychological Assocation. doi:10.1037/10725-000
Goldman, A., & Greenberg, L. (1992). Comparison of an integrated systemic and
emotionally focused approach to couples therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clin-
ical Psychology, 60, 962–969.
Goldman, R. N., Greenberg, L. S., & Angus, L. A. (2006). The effects of adding
emotion-focused interventions to the client-centered relationship conditions in
the treatment of depression. Psychotherapy Research, 16, 536–546. doi:10.1080/
10503300600589456
Goldman, R., Greenberg, L., & Pos, A. (2005). Depth of emotional experience and out-
come. Psychotherapy Research, 15, 248–260. doi:10.1080/10503300512331385188
Greenberg, L. S. (2002). Emotion-focused therapy: Coaching clients to work through their
feelings. Washington, DC: American Psychological Assocation. doi:10.1037/
10447-000
Greenberg, L. S., Auszra, L., & Hermann, I. R. (2007). The relationship among emo-
tional productivity, emotional arousal and outcome in experiential therapy of
depression. Psychotherapy Research, 17, 482–493. doi:10.1080/10503300600977800
Greenberg, L. S., & Goldman, R. N. (2008). Emotion-focused couples therapy: the
dynamics of emotion, love, and power. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Assocation. doi:10.1037/11750-000
Greenberg, L. S., & Johnson, S. M. (1988). Emotionally focused therapy for couples.
New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Greenberg, L. S., & Paivio, S. C. (1997). Working with emotions in psychotherapy. New
York, NY: Guilford Press.
Greenberg, L. S., & Pinsof, W. M. (1986). The psychotherapeutic process: A research
handbook. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Greenberg, L. S., Rice, L. N., & Elliott, R. (1993). Facilitating emotional change: The
moment by moment process. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
199
relational factors); (b) psychotherapy integration; (c) psychometrics (as
related to the measurement of such constructs as ambiguity, depth of inter-
pretation, resistance, and client compliance with free association); (d) psycho-
therapy research and design (including laboratory and effectiveness studies);
(e) statistical issues; (f) diagnosis; and (g) supervision. As a psychotherapy
researcher, Bordin argued for situating the science of psychotherapy within
a larger context of human behavior, thus championing the integration of
basic and applied knowledge. And as an exemplary scientist–practitioner,
Bordin also understood the inherent tensions in clinical and research moti-
vations and agendas, as he eloquently discussed in his article “Curiosity,
Compassion, and Doubt: The Dilemma of the Psychologist” (Bordin, 1966).
With his keen understanding of this dilemma, devotion to both sides of
this false dichotomy, and his genuine synthesizing character, Bordin’s
career personified the marriage of science and practice. In the remainder
of this essay, we elaborate on Bordin’s personal life and professional con-
tributions, with particular attention to his pioneering working alliance
theory.
EARLY BEGINNINGS
MAJOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Bordin (1987), during his 1986 Leona Tyler Award Address (an honor
bestowed on him by the Division of Counseling Psychology of the American
Psychological Association [APA]), humbly described having two enduring
preoccupations. However, one could view his early career focus on vocational
decision-making and his later career focus on his working alliance theory as
the end points on a continuum of influence tied together by related and recip-
rocal contributions in counseling and psychotherapy integration, process, and
research. We use these categories as organizational heuristics in recounting
Bordin’s major contributions.
Vocational Decision-Making
I was saved from a chameleonlike eclecticism and led toward true inte-
gration. I assumed that each of the proponents was seeing and expressing
an important point of view. I thought that each was concentrating on dif-
ferent aspects of persons as well as on different kinds of persons with dif-
ferent pathological states. Rather than choosing one approach, I selected
aspects of how they helped another and developed understandings of how
the situation and the disposition of the person being helped influenced
response. (Bordin, 1987, p. 362)
It was perhaps at this stage of his career that Bordin’s name started to
become, from our perspective, synonymous with the notions of synthesis and
integration within the counseling and psychotherapy fields. In fact, it was his
integrationist thinking that led him to collaborate at Michigan with such col-
leagues as Harold Raush and Allen Dittman in examining the active ingredi-
ents of psychotherapeutic change. This quest for uncovering what makes
psychotherapy work began with the careful development of psychometrically
sound measures of ambiguity, depth of interpretation, client resistance, and
client compliance with free association (all constructs that underscore his
psychoanalytic background).
I do not find it credible that change goals are interchangeable and that
all of the therapeutic tasks that are embedded in various treatment pack-
ages are alternate paths to the same goal. It seems more likely to me that
each of many sensitive and creative therapists arrived at methods that
were appropriate to the kinds of persons he or she was trying to help who
were different from the persons who were being helped by another
equally sensitive therapist. (Bordin, 1987, p. 363)
OTHER ACCOMPLISHMENTS
CONCLUSION
As should be clear, we agree with Elkin that Bordin was one of the
most important and influential figures in the fields of counseling and psycho-
therapy. He was an innovative thinker who used his creative, integrative,
and pioneering character and spirit to both define (when the field of voca-
tional counseling required new definitions) and to rejuvenate (when the
field of psychotherapy required rejuvenation of the relationship construct).
All the while, Bordin was a masterful scientist, inspirational mentor, and
strong leader. As a testament to his influence, we offer the threefold lesson
that Ed Bordin taught T. D. Borkovec during a dinner one evening—a les-
son that we believe can prove inspirational to anyone who aspires to be a
psychotherapy researcher:
REFERENCES
211
on biopsychosocial models of mental disorders and studying the role of health
care interactions in medical patient outcomes.
EARLY BEGINNINGS
MAJOR ACCOMPLISHMENTS
Thus, to begin the second therapeutic stage, the disengaged stage, the therapist’s
first task is to identify how the client is constraining him or her. The therapist
does this by noticing the experienced impact messages—the feelings, action
tendencies, attributions, and fantasies that the therapist experiences while
interacting with or imagining the client.
The therapist’s second task is to interrupt his or her automatic responses
to the client. If pulled to advise or comfort the client, the therapist notices
this and begins to withhold advice or comfort to interrupt the pattern of rein-
forcement of the client’s pathological style. The therapist then goes beyond
withholding automatic complementary responses to the client’s bids for rein-
forcement and begins engaging in asocial ways, intended to influence the
client to act differently because old behaviors no longer elicit predictable and
comforting reinforcement. Kiesler precisely defined these actions in relation
to the interpersonal circle model, identifying acomplementary and anti-
complementary responses that reject the client’s bid for reinforcement in
terms of either degree of affiliation or control in the relationship (or both).
As one particular type of asocial response, Kiesler focused extensively
on the process of metacommunication, in which the therapist draws explicit
attention to the verbal and nonverbal influencing behaviors of the client and
the role that they play in shaping in-session interactions as well as the
extratherapeutic social interactions that ultimately sustain the client’s prob-
lematic self-definition and relationship difficulties. The therapist prioritizes
client communications that directly refer to the therapist for feedback. The
therapist carefully focuses on both positives and negatives, being careful to
convey a supportive attitude and helpful intent. The therapist discloses the
impacts the client makes upon him or her and ties these impacts to specific
client actions to help the client understand how he or she influences others.
Then therapy explores the ways in which the client uses communication to
influence others besides the therapist, helping the client also explore how he
or she is in turn influenced by significant others’ communications.
Kiesler continued to develop this approach to psychotherapy over the
remainder of his career and featured it in his encyclopedic book Contempo-
rary Interpersonal Theory and Research (Kiesler, 1996). In this book, Kiesler
made a compelling case for interpersonal theory as one of psychology’s hall-
CONCLUSION
I love to work with seriously troubled people who are driving everyone else
crazy and have been unresponsive to many previous treatments. . . . I want
to help relieve suffering. I want to see change in the patient’s personal life.
I want to see the patient rediscover delight and to thrive. I want patients
to become fully engaged in their lives to the best of their abilities. The best
reward for me is to see people who were once remarkable for causing trou-
ble, for being almost nonexistent, or for other unhappy reasons, learn to fly.
— (Benjamin, 2001, p. 27).
MAJOR CONTRIBUTIONS
221
approach to PD diagnosis (Benjamin, 1993/1996) and contributing substan-
tially to other assessment methods, including the Structured Clinical Interview
for the DSM–IV Personality Disorders (SCID-II; First, Gibbon, Spitzer,
Williams, & Benjamin, 1996), and the Wisconsin Personality Inventory
(WISPI; Klein et al., 1993).
Across her career, Benjamin has played many important roles, includ-
ing as an advisor to the DSM–IV work group on Axis II. She also contributed
to Fetzer Institute and National Institute of Mental Health–sponsored meet-
ings to explore inclusion of relational disorder in future Diagnostic and Statis-
tical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) systems (Benjamin, Wamboldt, &
Critchfield, 2006). She is a past president of the Society for Psychotherapy
Research (SPR) and was granted an honorary doctoral degree from the Uni-
versity of Umea, Sweden, for her work with SASB. She has received many
awards, including the Society for Personality Assessment’s Klopfer Award, for
outstanding long-term professional contributions to personality assessment, and
the Distinguished Research Career Award from SPR.
As a clinician, Benjamin is well-known for her work in treating patients
for whom standard interventions have not been sufficient to prevent repeated
hospitalizations and suicide attempts. IRT clinic trainees and hospital staff
observe her consultations directly as she develops a case formulation that makes
sense of patient pathology in light of the unique interpersonal history.
Benjamin’s approach uses SASB to identify repeating patterns in patient nar-
ratives and quickly narrow the focus to the most important themes. She views
psychopathology as reflecting failed attempts at adaptation using previously
internalized values and learning. In her view, maladaptive internalized patterns
often persist because they are driven by love and loyalty to the attachment fig-
ures with whom they were first learned. In the introduction to the 2006 edi-
tion of Interpersonal Reconstructive Therapy, she called this “attachment gone
awry” (Benjamin, 2003/2006, p. v) and had boldly asserted elsewhere that
“every psychopathology is a gift of love” (Benjamin, 1993). Her treatment
approach, IRT, directly addresses the relationship with these internalizations.
BEGINNINGS
Benjamin was born and raised in upstate New York, near Rochester. Her
father was a research chemist who later became an executive for the Eastman
Kodak Company. He was a major source of her lifelong fascination and respect
for the sciences. Early socialization on the family farm instilled values of hard
work, competence, and performance. It also brought early insight and attune-
ment to how relationships work. She described how in childhood she trained
and befriended an aggressive quarter horse that had been severely abused:
Professional Training
Across her career Benjamin has repeatedly emphasized the need for
well-articulated theory that can be tested directly with observable data. Like
Harlow, her preference is for direct demonstrations of principle in well-
designed experiments with few subjects. Reflecting her concern with the vital
ACHIEVEMENTS
CONCLUSION
Benjamin’s work brings together all of the early themes in her life, refined
and enhanced through roughly 50 years of clinical experience and careful
research. These themes most crucially involve attunement to relationship pat-
terns or what she has referred to as “the harmonics of therapy” (Benjamin,
1993/1996), the importance of attachment as fundamental to our evolutionary
heritage, and a sharp focus on empirical data organized by her periodic table of
interpersonal relating. For Benjamin, psychopathology is about love, or more
precisely about love gone wrong. Her theory offers a profoundly humanizing
view of psychiatric disorder and is backed up with compelling data. She sum-
marized her view succinctly: “Psychopathology is best attributed to broken
hearts (attachment gone awry) rather than to broken brains” (Benjamin, 2008,
p. 414). According to Benjamin, when the past is reckoned with and impossi-
ble wishes successfully grieved, patients can revise impacts of abuse, neglect,
and other toxic patterns. They then have the opportunity to learn new, more
adaptive skills, and in this respect “learn to fly.”
REFERENCES
Benjamin, L. S. (1965). A special Latin Square for the use of each subject “as his own
control.” Psychometrika, 30, 499–513.
Benjamin, L. S. (1967). Facts and artifacts in using Analysis of Covariance to “undo”
the Law of Initial Values. Psychophysiology, 4, 187–206.
Benjamin, L. S. (1973). A biological model for understanding the behavior of indi-
viduals. In J. Westman (Ed.), Individual differences in children (pp. 215–241). New
York, NY: Wiley.
Benjamin, L. S. (1979). Structural analysis of differentiation failure. Psychiatry, 42, 1–23.
Benjamin, L. S. (1986). Operational definition and measurement of dynamics shown
in the stream of free associations. Psychiatry, 49, 104–129.
233
integrative, research-based generic model of psychotherapy; and (e) cofound-
ing and organizing the Society for Psychotherapy Research (SPR), which he
continues to sustain as a vital community of researchers at the national, inter-
national, and local levels.
David Orlinsky has received formal recognition for distinguished scientific
and professional contributions through awards from the American Psychologi-
cal Association (APA) Division of Psychotherapy, the Illinois Psychological
Association, and SPR, and he received an award for teaching excellence from
the University of Chicago. For nearly 5 decades he has influenced generations
of undergraduate and graduate students and has supervised, mentored, and
inspired numerous doctoral students and younger colleagues nationally and
internationally within the field of psychotherapy research.
EARLY BEGINNINGS
David Orlinsky was born in 1936 in New York City, the first child of his
parents and the first grandchild and nephew in a large, close-knit, upwardly
mobile working-class family of East European Jewish background. Looking
back at his origins (Orlinsky, 2005), he noted that having parents who were
also first-born meant that his youngest uncles and aunts were as close as or
closer in age to him than to his parents, which effectively put him between
generations. In relating “up” to his elders, he received much attention and
affection but was clearly not one of them. In relating “down” to cousins and
brother, he was naturally a leader, a “first among equals.” In this early family
environment, he learned to value close personal relationships and close-knit
groups, and he gained an awareness of complexity and context, an ability to
view questions from multiple perspectives, and a heightened sensitivity to
issues of inclusion and exclusion—traits that we think have influenced his
work in the field of psychotherapy.
Orlinsky received his elementary and secondary education at public
schools in New York, where his interest and talent in science and poetry were
nurtured. After high school, he attended the College of the University of
Chicago (1953–1954) where a coherently organized curriculum and com-
mitted teachers introduced him to classical works in the fields of humani-
ties, social science, and the physical and biological sciences. Next, he studied
in the University of Chicago’s master’s program on the history of culture
(1954–1955) and won an award for excellence in humanities and first prize
in the university’s poetry contest. Thereafter, he transferred to the Univer-
sity of Chicago’s doctoral program in clinical psychology (1955–1962), where
he met his classmate Kenneth Howard, who was to become his lifelong friend
and research partner.
ACCOMPLISHMENTS
CONCLUSION
We can summarize what has been said about David Orlinsky by highlight-
ing some dominant characteristics of his work. One is a consistent focus on the
scientific study of subjective experience, rooted in his early interests in science
and poetry and brought to fruition by his undergraduate and graduate studies at
the University of Chicago. Another is his attention to the contextual embed-
dedness of experience—including the experience of psychotherapy—reflecting
his broad background in the social sciences and humanities. His interest in
REFERENCES
Elliott, R., Orlinsky, D., Klein, M., Amer, M., & Partyka, R. (2003). Professional
characteristics of humanistic therapists: Analyses of the Collaborative Research
Network Sample. Person-Centered and Experiential Psychotherapies, 2, 188–203.
Geller, J. D., Norcross, J. C., & Orlinsky, D. E. (Eds.). (2005). The psychotherapist’s
own psychotherapy: Patient and clinician perspectives. New York, NY: Oxford
University Press.
Howard, K. I., & Orlinsky, D. E. (1972). Psychotherapeutic processes. Annual Review
of Psychology, 23, 615–668. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.23.020172.003151
Kolden, G. G. (1991). The Generic Model of Psychotherapy: An empirical investi-
gation of process and outcome relationships. Psychotherapy Research, 1, 62–73.
doi:10.1080/10503309112331334071
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society from the perspective of a social behaviorist.
Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Orlinsky, D. E. (1994). Ansaetze zu einer wissenschaftlichen Integration psycho-
therapeutischer Behandlungsmethoden [Learning from many masters]. Psycho-
therapeut, 1, 2–9.
Orlinsky, D. E. (1995). The greying and greening of SPR: A personal memoir on
forming the Society for Psychotherapy Research. Psychotherapy Research, 5,
343–350.
247
Subsequently, the research group at Ulm, where Horst Kächele has spent
most of his professional life, holds specific importance among the German
research groups in at least two respects. One is the intensive and ongoing
effort to contribute to the development of psychoanalytic treatment based
upon theory, clinical work, and empirical research, and the other is the early
attempt of Horst Kächele and Helmut Thomä, the former head of the Depart-
ment of Psychotherapy, to bring researchers and research approaches from
around the world together through international collaboration and organiza-
tion, especially within the Society for Psychotherapy Research (SPR).
This chapter is primarily aimed at describing how Horst Kächele achieved
his two major contributions, namely, (a) advocating empirical process and out-
come research within psychoanalysis and (b) developing international collab-
orations in psychotherapy research and professional practice.
MAJOR CONTRIBUTIONS
EARLY BEGINNINGS
ACCOMPLISHMENTS
Research Studies
OTHER CONTRIBUTIONS
INFLUENCES
Horst Kächele’s primary mentor was Helmut Thomä, but there were
several others who continuously worked with him and largely influenced his
theoretical thinking and the way he conceptualized psychotherapy research.
North American colleagues have already been mentioned (Luborsky, Strupp,
Orlinsky, Howard, Dahl, among others). Within the German-speaking world,
it was especially Adolf-Ernst Meyer, chair of the Department of Psycho-
somatic Medicine in Hamburg, with whom Horst was closely connected and
who supported his scientific ideas. Meyer’s conception of psychoanalytic
research was very similar to Horst Kächele’s, especially with respect to his
request that psychoanalytical concepts (and treatments) always should to be
empirically supported.
Conversely, Horst Kächele has mentored a variety of researchers in his
departments in Ulm and Stuttgart—Hans Kordy, Erhard Mergenthaler,
Michael Hölzer, Anna Buchheim, Reiner Dahlbender or Dan Pokorny, to
mention just a few—and he has significantly contributed to the development
of a well-functioning network of researchers and institutions active in psycho-
dynamic psychotherapy research inside and outside of Germany.
Kächele and Thomä attended several early SPR meetings in the United
States. After presenting at the first international conference on psychoanalytic
CONCLUSION
REFERENCES
Brisch, K., Bechinger, D., Betzler, S., Heinemann, H., Kächele, H., Pohlandt, F.,
. . . Buchheim, A. (2005). Attachment quality in very low birthweight premature
infants in relation to maternal representations and neurological development.
Parenting: Science and Practice, 5, 311–331. doi:10.1207/s15327922par0504_1
Buchheim, A., Brisch, K. H., & Kächele, H. (1999). Clinical significance of attachment
research for the premature infant group: An overview of most recent research.
Zeitschrift für Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 27, 125–138.
Buchheim, A., & Kächele, H. (2003). Adult attachment interview and psychoanalytic
perspective: A single case study. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 23, 81–101. doi:10.1080/
07351692309349027
Dahl, H., Kächele, H., & Thomä, H. (Eds.). (1988). Psychoanalytic process research
strategies. Berlin, Germany: Springer.
Dührssen, A. M., & Jorswieck, E. (1965). Eine empirisch-statistische Untersuchung zur
Leistungsfähigkeit psychoanalytischer Behandlung [An empirical statistical study
on the effectiveness of psychoanalytic long-term treatment]. Der Nervenarzt, 36,
166–169.
Fenichel, O. (1930). Statistischer Bericht über die therapeutische Tätigkeit 1920–1930.
[Statistical report about therapeutic work 1920-1930] In S. Radó, O. Fenichel,
& C. Müller-Braunschweig (Eds.), Zehn Jahre Berliner Psychoanalytisches Institut
(pp. 13–19). Vienna, Austria: Int. Psychoanalytischer Verlag.
Grulke, N., Bailer, H., Hartenstein, B., Kächele, H., Arnold, B., Tschuschke, V., &
Heimpel, H. (2005). Coping and survival in patients with leukaemia undergoing
allogeneic bone marrow transplantation: Long-term follow-up of a prospective
study. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 59, 337–346.
Grünbaum, A. (1984). The foundations of psychoanalysis. Berkeley: University of
California Press.
Kächele. H. (1970). Der Begriff “psychogener Tod” in der medizinischen Literatur
[The term “psychogenic death” in the medical literature]. Zeitschrift Psychosoma-
tische Medizin und Psychoanalyse, 16, 105–129/ 202–223.
Kächele, H. (1992a). Une nouvelle perspective de recherche en psychotherapie :
Le projet PEP. Psychothérapies 2, 73–77.
MAJOR CONTRIBUTIONS
259
In the late 1970s, Jones’s research focused on the psychological aspects
of race. He actively challenged racial stereotypes present in the literature. He
insisted that psychologists not accept simple answers. He warned against the
dangers inherent in regarding African Americans as a homogeneous group.
In keeping with the complex and subtle thinking for which he was so widely
respected, he voiced his reservations about the construction of ethnically spe-
cific norms of measurement. On the one hand, culture-specific measures
acknowledged a pluralism that had previously been disregarded. On the other
hand, Jones believed that these measures could themselves strike an exclu-
sionary note (by upholding a static view of society).
Enrico Jones was one of the earliest researchers to examine race and
psychotherapy systematically. In a brief and wonderfully cogent critique of
the field in the late 1980s, he argued that race was the wrong level of concep-
tualization for the individual differences among therapists (Jones, 1985). Pre-
saging some of the most salient themes to emerge in discussions about cultural
competence, Jones wrote succinctly: “The question is not how to treat the
black client but how to treat this black client” (p. 175). Jones’s emphasis on
the individuality of the patient and of each therapy relationship became cen-
tral to the second part of his career.
During the 1980s, Jones developed the PQS. His programmatic line of
research using this measure established him as a leading psychotherapy process
researcher. Whereas many researchers were pursuing the question of whether
therapy worked, Jones found the question of how therapy worked more com-
pelling. To this day, many regard the PQS as one of the most comprehensive
and clinically relevant empirical measures of psychotherapy process.
Jones’s research on psychotherapy process culminated in the publica-
tion of his book, Therapeutic Action: A Guide to Psychoanalytic Theory (Jones,
2000). This book details a conceptualization of therapeutic action that Jones
termed repetitive interaction structures. His interaction structure theory pro-
vides an empirical method with which to deconstruct not only the essence of
the therapy relationship but also the directions of influence that operate
between patient and clinician. Jones’s model of therapeutic action uses both
a methodological paradigm and a theoretical frame to answer one of the field’s
most important questions: What is it about the nature of the therapy relation-
ship that either facilitates or hinders change?
BEGINNINGS
ACCOMPLISHMENTS
Enrico Jones dedicated his research career to the following four domains
(listed chronologically in the order in which they emerged): (a) social aspects
of individual differences; (b) social factors in psychotherapy; (c) the study of
the psychotherapy process using the PQS; and (d) interaction structures and
therapeutic action.
During the 1980s, Enrico Jones responded to the field’s need for quan-
titative methods that preserved the depth and complexity of clinical material
while conforming to the requirements of empirical science. In an effort to
address this need, Jones spent more than a decade pioneering and developing
the PQS; Jones, 2000). The Q-set instructs coders to sort its 100 items accord-
ing to a normal distribution in terms of how well they describe a therapy session.
The items concern (a) patients’ attitudes, behavior, and experience; (b) thera-
pists’ actions and attitudes; and (c) the nature of the interaction of the dyad. The
Q-set is designed to provide a basic language for the description and classifica-
tion of treatment processes in a form suitable for quantitative analysis. This
instrument captures the uniqueness of each treatment hour while also per-
mitting the assessment of the similarities or dissimilarities between hours and
patients. (See Jones, 2000, for a more detailed discussion of the instrument
and its psychometric properties.)
The PQS has been used reliably for both group comparison and inten-
sive single-case designs. It has been applied to study treatments as diverse as
psychoanalysis, long- and short-term psychodynamic psychotherapy, cogni-
tive behavior therapy (CBT), interpersonal therapy, rational emotive ther-
apy, and gestalt therapy. The Q-set has also been used by panels of experts
from different theoretical orientations to construct prototypes of ideal treat-
ment hours instantiating their respective orientations. Use of the Q-set in
this way (referred to as prototype methodology) facilitates examining differences
Interaction Structures
OTHER CONTRIBUTIONS
Enrico Jones was the first African American professor in the psychology
department at Berkeley. Over the course of his career, he assumed a major
leadership role in the education and training of ethnic minority students. He
received the Kenneth and Mamie Clark Award for Outstanding Contribu-
tions to the Professional Development of Ethnic Minority Students. Jones
also served on the editorial boards of several organizations, including the Jour-
nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, the Journal of the American Psycho-
analytic Association, the International Journal of Psychoanalysis, and Psychological
Issues. He was a member of the Society for Psychotherapy Research, the Inter-
national Psychoanalytic Association, and the board for the Scientific Affairs
of the American Psychoanalytic Association. He enjoyed a number of fruitful
intellectual collaborations with both mentors and students (Robert Wallerstein,
Sheldon Korchin, Jack Block, and Stuart Ablon).
CONCLUSION
A central theme throughout this chapter has been Enrico Jones’s com-
mitment to avoiding oversimplification. Yet, ultimately, his achievement was
that of simplicity. He made the complexities of psychotherapeutic process,
including psychoanalytic thought and technique, understandable and acces-
sible. He mastered the dialectic of not succumbing to illusions generated by
reductionism and simplification, while avoiding the trap of creating mystique
and religion where the innocent questions can no longer be asked and the
truth is buried under multiple layers of false sophistication.
Enrico Jones’s legacies are considerable and multifaceted. His creation
and application of the Psychotherapy Process Q-set originated in his strong
belief that both process and outcome needed to be investigated in order to
understand why certain treatments worked and certain others failed.
Jones’s commitment to identifying and examining the processes that were
actually (as opposed to theoretically) unfolding and facilitating change in
treatment has laid important groundwork for the field’s newfound interest
REFERENCES
Ablon, J. S., & Jones, E. E. (1998). How expert clinicians’ prototypes of an ideal
treatment correlate with outcome in psychodynamic and cognitive-behavior
therapy. Psychotherapy Research, 8, 71–83. doi:10.1093/ptr/8.1.71
Ablon, J. S., & Jones, E. E. (1999). Psychotherapy process in the National Institute
of Mental Health Treatment of Depression Collaborative Research Program.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 64–75. doi:10.1037/0022-
006X.67.1.64
Ablon, J. S., & Jones, E. E. (2002). Validity of controlled clinical trials of psychother-
apy: findings from the NIMH Treatment of Depression Collaborative Research
Program. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 775–783. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.
159.5.775
Jones, E. E. (1978a). Black–White personality differences: Another look. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 42, 244–252.
Jones, E. E. (1978b). Effects of race on psychotherapy process and outcome: An
exploratory investigation. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 15,
226–236.
Jones, E. E. (1982). Psychotherapists’ impressions of treatment outcome as a function
of race. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38, 722–731.
Jones, E. E. (1985). Psychotherapy and counseling with Black clients. In P. Pedersen
(Ed.), Handbook of cross- cultural counseling and therapy (pp. 173–179). Westport,
CT: Greenwood Press.
Jones, E. E. (2000). Therapeutic action: A guide to psychoanalytic therapy. Northvale,
NJ: Jason Aronson, Inc.
271
MAJOR CONTRIBUTIONS
EARLY BEGINNINGS
David Shapiro was born in 1945 and grew up on the borders of South
London and Kent, attending Eltham College and winning a Sacher Open
Scholarship in Modern Studies to New College Oxford. There he gained first
ACCOMPLISHMENTS
Through the early and mid 1970s, Shapiro published a series of articles
focusing on the theme, “What are the ingredients of change?” Shapiro carried
out studies testing key Rogerian concepts, in particular that of empathy. In one
study he evaluated the rating scales devised by Truax and Carkhuff for rating
empathy, genuineness, and nonpossessive warmth and found supporting evi-
dence for the use of the empathy scale but not for the other two conditions
(Shapiro, 1973). He also employed innovative experimental designs using non-
clinical participants with the aim of bringing precision to what had previously
been considered somewhat vague concepts. An exemplar of such work was a
study in which participants attempted to reconstruct the serial order of dia-
logues according to examples defining successive points on the Truax Accurate
Empathy (AE) Scale (Shapiro, 1976). Contrary to expectations, findings
showed a decrease in accuracy with higher AE ratings, suggesting that dialogue
representing higher levels of AE indicated tacit understanding rather than an
explicit structure identifiable to external raters.
Meta-Analytic Work
On his joining the MRC unit in 1977, Shapiro’s focus moved to the ques-
tion of comparative outcomes across therapeutic approaches, working with
Diana Shapiro. Informed by Smith and Glass’s (1977) classic meta-analytic
study on the effects of psychotherapy—a watershed in outcomes research that
had always impressed Shapiro—his work with Diana Shapiro provided him
with a question to be addressed in terms of replicating and refining the Smith
and Glass study. They refined Smith and Glass’s study by implementing a num-
ber of amendments, primarily in response to criticisms made of it by Stanley
Rachman and G. Terence Wilson. The resulting study comprised 143 outcome
studies published between 1975 and 1979 in which two or more psychological
treatments were compared with a control group (Shapiro & Shapiro, 1982).
The meta-analytic research provided the basis for two parallel streams of
work, each of which captures the clear strengths of Shapiro’s research reper-
toire. One activity focused on a conceptual treatment of issues around evidence
of equivalence of outcomes and its implications for research, in work carried
out collaboratively with Bill Stiles and Robert Elliott. This arose from the
meeting of like-minded scientist–practitioners during the SPR meetings of the
early 1980s and yielded a watershed article titled “Are All Psychotherapies
Equivalent?” in the American Psychologist (Stiles, Shapiro, & Elliott, 1986). The
other activity focused on designing a comparative study of contrasting thera-
pies that investigated the processes as well as the outcomes of therapies. Shapiro
referred to this approach as a “comparative, content-impact-outcome research
strategy.”
David Shapiro was subsequently joined by Jenny Firth (later Firth-
Cozens), Glenys Parry, and Chris Brewin. With Jenny Firth, Shapiro set about
designing a trial to test whether delivering contrasting therapies in differing
sequences might yield enhanced outcomes (Shapiro & Firth, 1987). Two
therapies were selected in terms of their ability to represent broadly differing
approaches: prescriptive (later to be renamed cognitive behavioral [CB]) and
exploratory (later to be renamed psychodynamic interpersonal [PI]) therapy.
A driving concern behind this study, which became known as the (first)
Sheffield Psychotherapy Project, was to generate an even playing field between
contrasting therapies and, in particular, to deliver quality research on non-
CBT approaches. Shapiro, informed by his father’s priority of strategy over pro-
cedure, realized that the evidence base will always be incomplete; for example,
he was always clear that there was no such thing as a “definitive” study—and
its application to many clinical situations would be uncertain. Hence, the most
compelling need for scientist–practitioner skills arises when the evidence is
Process Studies
Over 1992 and 1993, a review by the MRC determined that the Social
and Applied Psychology Unit would be closed, and Shapiro was appointed pro-
OTHER CONTRIBUTIONS
In the 1970s in the UK, the only journal focusing on clinical psychology
was the British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, published by the British
Psychological Society. In 1981, the journal was split into separate journals, and
Shapiro was appointed the first editor of the British Journal of Clinical Psychology,
a position he held for 6 years.
Shapiro’s discovery in 1979 of the SPR meeting at St. Catherine’s
College, Oxford, made him a devoted conference attendee. He was also an
astute organizer and was responsible for bringing the 1983 international meet-
ing to Sheffield at very short notice. From this experience Shapiro saw the
need and potential for establishing a UK chapter, and the decision was taken
INFLUENCES
Many of the themes in Shapiro’s work have been influenced by his par-
ents, Monte and Jean Shapiro, both of whom were highly committed people
with powerfully independent minds. Monte Shapiro (M. B. Shapiro) came to
the UK from South Africa and became one of a powerful group of clinical
psychologists (including Gwynne Jones, Victor Meyer, Jack Rachman, and
James Inglis) in Hans Eysenck’s department at the Maudsley Hospital in
London in the late 1950s. Monte Shapiro’s central axiom was his espousal of a
stringent methodological approach to building a scientific basis for the assess-
ment and treatment of psychological problems. The whole research culture
intrinsic to the Maudsley and Institute of Psychiatry had a pervasive influence
on David Shapiro’s approach, informing but also showing the potential limita-
tions of single approaches to practice and research. Beyond M. B. Shapiro, the
influence of Bob Hobson is foremost as a researcher and practitioner, with his
commitment to the supervision process and his encouragement to junior staff
pervading Shapiro’s own practice.
Shapiro developed lasting collaborations with Robert Elliott, William
B. Stiles, and Marv Goldfried, and each had a considerable influence
on Shapiro’s work. Their differing theoretical orientations but common inter-
est in researching client change yielded a highly productive program of
CONCLUSION
REFERENCES
Aveline, M., and Shapiro, D. A. (1995). Research foundations for psychotherapy practice.
Chichester, England: Wiley in associations with the Mental Health Foundation.
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inven-
tory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561–571.
Shapiro, D. A. (1973). Naïve British judgements of therapeutic conditions. British
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 12, 289–294.
Shapiro, D. A. (1976). Conversational structure and accurate empathy: An exploratory
study. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 15, 213–215.
Shapiro, D. A. (1985). Recent applications of meta-analysis in clinical research.
Clinical Psychology Review, 5, 13–34. doi:10.1016/0272-7358(85)90027-3
Shapiro, D. A. (1995). Finding out how psychotherapies help people change. Psycho-
therapy Research, 5, 1–21.
Shapiro, D. A., & Firth, J. (1987). Prescriptive vs. exploratory psychotherapy:
Outcomes of the Sheffield Psychotherapy Project. British Journal of Psychiatry, 151,
790–799. doi:10.1192/bjp.151.6.790
283
introduced a new psychometric method—Rasch analysis—for the analysis of
common therapy outcome measures.
In addition to his contributions in research methods, Elliott has also
played a vital role in the development of process–experiential (PE) therapy
(also known as emotion-focused therapy, or EFT) and its application to various
types of clients and presenting issues. He has worked with colleagues to develop
a model of how clients progress through experiential tasks, which has led to the
publication of influential PE treatment and training manuals. In fact, he has
become an internationally recognized expert in PE training and supervision.
Furthermore, Elliott’s research on PE therapy and depression has made a sig-
nificant contribution to the recent recognition of PE therapy (EFT) as an
empirically supported treatment of depression by APA Division 12 (Society of
Clinical Psychology).
EARLY BEGINNINGS
The oldest of six children, Elliott was raised in California and describes
his parents as intellectuals. Strongly influenced by his father, he attributes the
use of humor in his work, as well as his desire to understand and help others, to
experiences with his dad. Moreover, Elliott also shared a passionate interest in
science fiction with his father and, as a young man, thought he would become
a science fiction writer when he grew up. A turning point in his career aspira-
tions occurred after he read Karen Horney’s Our Inner Conflicts (1945) as part
of a high school creative writing course. His mother and grandmother shared
an interest in Jungian psychology, and his grandmother, a writer, was also
quite interested in the field of parapsychology, hoping one day her grandson
would become a parapsychologist. Although parapsychological concepts
seemed too ephemeral and abstract to Elliott, he realized observable psycho-
logical processes, such as empathy and the therapeutic interaction, discovered
in Horney’s writing, could be equally powerful and intellectually challenging.
He came to view empathy and therapy as “magical” in their power to transform
human experiences, about the time he realized that he wanted to become a
therapist—which he naively imagined as a kind of magician/healer—an insight
he described as similar to a religious conversion.
Elliott’s undergraduate training at the University of California–Santa
Cruz (UCSC) was primarily humanistic in nature, especially as UCSC was
home to several influential humanistic psychologists, including Ted Sarbin,
Brewster Smith, Frank Barron, and Bert Kaplan. During college and graduate
school, Elliott’s goal was to become a therapist; however, his professors recog-
nized his talent as a “natural academic” and suggested that he also pursue
research. He began his clinical training in graduate school at the University of
ACCOMPLISHMENTS
Robert Elliott has held many leadership positions within the field of
psychotherapy research. He served as president of the North American Chapter
of the Society for Psychotherapy Research (SPR) in 1991, and he served as
president of the international SPR in 2000–2001. He has served as North
American editor of Psychotherapy Research (1994–1998) and as coeditor of
Person-Centered and Experiential Psychotherapies (2002–2007). When describ-
ing his leadership positions and his role as a journal editor, Elliott stated, “It’s
a way for me to give back what has been given to me.” Most recently, Elliott
REFERENCES
BEGINNINGS
William B. Stiles was born in Seattle, the first of four children, and grew
up in the Washington, DC, area. He did his undergraduate studies at Oberlin,
the historically liberal college in Ohio that his wife, parents, siblings, and
many other relatives have attended. He had planned on majoring in econom-
ics but decided to pursue psychology because, he admits, his grades were good
in that field and mediocre in everything else. In hindsight, Stiles speculated
295
that his higher grades reflected interest in the subject, but at the time he inter-
preted it as differential aptitude. Stiles first became aware of his interest in
psychology and personality when discussing his student paper on psychopaths
with his clinical psychology instructor, John Thompson. A decision to pur-
sue graduate studies seemed like a natural fit for him; he came from an academ-
ically minded family. His father was a specialized librarian, his mother a
primary school teacher, his brother a biology professor, and his maternal grand-
father (a junior high school principal) had always wanted to be a professor.
Stiles was accepted into the clinical psychology doctoral program at the
University of California–Los Angeles (UCLA), where his interest in theory
building gradually came to focus on psychotherapy research. He saw psychol-
ogy as a science “not all thought through.” It offered the challenge of explain-
ing complex phenomena in a rich way, without oversimplifying and without
losing methodological rigor. Stiles’s UCLA training was half clinical and half
neuroscience. His dissertation focused on a theory of human experience (e.g.,
grief, anger, sexual excitement) and was neurologically based. This early study
foreshadowed his approach to explaining something as broad as human expe-
rience, by articulating theoretical concepts and grounding them in observa-
tions. Stiles’s specific interest in psychotherapy research emerged out of
doing, supervising, and teaching therapy. It reflected his attempt to under-
stand more thoroughly what happens in therapy. He was strongly influenced
by a clinical supervisor at UCLA, Jerry Goodman, who studied response
modes: a classification of possible responses in an interview setting. Studying
therapy offered Stiles a chance to explain real-world psychological processes,
with both realism and precision.
By the time of his graduation, Bill Stiles had decided to look for an aca-
demic position in clinical psychology, finding the openness of the field
appealing. After completing his internship at the Neuropsychiatric Institute
at UCLA Center for Health Sciences, he became an instructor and then an
assistant professor at the University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill. After
reading Carl Rogers’s work, he adopted a student-centered, nondirective form
of teaching. He developed a career-long reputation for teaching classes in a
radically nondirective way. His students had a hand in developing the syl-
labus, choosing their own readings, structuring group discussions, and evalu-
ating themselves. Although this nontraditional classroom environment often
elicited strong reactions (both positive and negative), Stiles’s students had
opportunities to learn and grow in ways previously unimagined. Stiles is well
known for his use of empathic reflections: Usually accurate, they can be both
irritating and therapeutic. A typical sequence reported by students who have
been exposed to his manner of teaching is, first, feeling left to their own
devices, then resenting it, deciding to fend for themselves, and then eventu-
ally discovering a new level of professional independence or developing a new
ACCOMPLISHMENTS
Theory Building
The finding that VRMs did not directly predict outcome was consistent
with the enigmatic yet common finding of null correlations between types
of interventions and outcomes. These results influenced Stiles’s thinking
about one of the long-standing puzzles in psychotherapy research: the
equivalence paradox. Also known as the Dodo verdict, it refers to the estab-
lished finding of equivalent outcomes for therapies that use vastly different
methods (Luborsky, Singer, & Luborsky, 1975; Stiles, Shapiro, & Elliott,
1986). To help understand this paradox, Stiles articulated the concept of
responsiveness (Stiles, 1988; Stiles, Honos-Webb, & Surko, 1998). Respon-
siveness suggests that therapeutic interventions tend to be delivered to
clients in ways that match their emerging needs on a moment-to-moment
basis. That is, interventions are delivered responsively rather than rigidly
as advised by therapy manuals. Stiles suggested that responsiveness to
clients’ needs rather than the amount of any type of intervention per se may
be driving therapeutic success. For example, the fact that empathic reflec-
tion may generally be helpful does not always mean that more of it will yield
a better outcome (the client may not need more). Explaining outcomes as
driven by the nonlinear, complex patterns in which therapists responsively
meet clients’ emerging needs required qualitative approaches to capture
interactions, timing, and context of interventions. This approach provided
Assimilation Model
Qualitative Research
Stiles and his research group have applied qualitative methods to study-
ing therapy, particularly with the assimilation model. This work, along with
Stiles’s elaboration of the philosophy and rationale of qualitative approaches,
helped establish qualitative inquiry as a scientific method in its own right
rather than a lesser strategy used for lack of alternatives. Stiles addressed the
issue of methodological rigor by articulating good practice standards (criteria
of reliability and validity) in qualitative research (Stiles, 1993, 2003). Much
of his recent thinking has centered on comparing and contrasting the episte-
OTHER CONTRIBUTIONS
Stiles has been intimately connected with the Society for Psychother-
apy Research (SPR) since attending his first conference in 1981 in Aspen,
Colorado. In 1980, Al Mahrer invited him to participate in an APA sympo-
sium, “Psychotherapy Process Research: A Preview of the Next Decade.” The
other participants, Clara Hill and Robert Elliott, encouraged him to attend
the next SPR conference. He has collaborated with numerous individuals
from this organization, who have served as a valuable reference group. Stiles
served as program chair for the 28th international annual meeting in 1997 in
Geilo, Norway, and was president for the 1997–1998 year. In 2009, he
received the prestigious Distinguished Research Career Award from SPR.
Stiles helped establish a local area group, Ohio SPR, uniting three centers
of psychotherapy research (Miami University, Ohio University, and Univer-
sity of Toledo) into a consortium. This small, intimate conference—geared
toward grad students—met annually between 2001 and 2007.
Stiles’s affiliation with SPR led to his service as associate editor and then
editor for the organization’s journal, Psychotherapy Research. He has engaged
in editorial duties with his usual sense of quiet commitment and conscientious-
ness, hoping to ensure the integrity of the peer review process he has long val-
ued. At this writing, Stiles is the coeditor of Person-Centered and Experiential
Psychotherapies and associate editor of the British Journal of Clinical Psychology.
REFERENCES
Agnew-Davies, R., Stiles, W. B., Hardy, G. E., Barkham, M., & Shapiro, D. A.
(1998). Alliance structure assessed by the Agnew Relationship Measure (ARM).
British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 37, 155–172.
Honos-Webb, L., & Stiles, W. B. (1998). Reformulation of assimilation analysis in
terms of voices. Psychotherapy, 35, 23–33. doi:10.1037/h0087682
Horvath, A. O., & Greenberg, L. S. (1986). The development of the Working
Alliance Inventory. In L. S. Greenberg & W. M. Pinsof (Eds.), The psycho-
therapeutic process: A research handbook (pp. 529–56). New York, NY: Guil-
ford Press.
Horvath, A. O., & Greenberg, L. S. (1989). Development and validation of the
Working Alliance Inventory. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 36, 223–233.
Luborsky, L., Singer, B., & Luborsky, L. (1975). Comparative studies of psycho-
therapies: Is it true that “Everyone has won and all must have prizes”? Archives
of General Psychiatry, 32, 995–1008.
Rogers, C. R. (1951). Client-centered therapy. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
Stiles, W. B. (1979a). Psychotherapy recapitulates ontogeny: The epigenesis of inten-
sive interpersonal relationships. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, and Practice,
16, 391–404. doi:10.1037/h0088365
Stiles, W. B. (1979b). Verbal response modes and psychotherapeutic technique. Psy-
chiatry, 42, 49–62.
Stiles, W. B. (1981). Science, experience, and truth: A conversation with myself.
Teaching of Psychology, 8, 227–230. doi:10.1207/s15328023top0804_11
Stiles, W. B. (1988). Psychotherapy process-outcome correlations may be mislead-
ing. Psychotherapy, 25, 27–35. doi:10.1037/h0085320
The death of Sol Garfield in August of 2004 portended the end of an era
in psychotherapy and clinical psychology. Sol Garfield was one of a handful of
psychologists, largely educated in the years before and during World War II,
who established clinical psychology as both a health profession and a science.
With professional colleagues like Allen Bergin, Hans Strupp, Lester Luborsky,
Kenneth I. Howard, Morris Parloff, and David Orlinsky, Sol worked to estab-
lish recognition for psychotherapy research as a legitimate scientific discipline
and thereby firmly established the scientist–professional model of training and
practice as an ideal for clinical psychology.
Facing the daunting task of challenging the giants of psychodynamic the-
ories in the middle and late 20th century, Sol Garfield became a controversial
man. Although often seen as a “kindly curmudgeon” (Beutler, 1998), he was
vocally and notably in disagreement with many of the developments in clini-
cal psychology and psychotherapy research that dominated the discipline in
the 20th and early 21st centuries. He was a vocal critic of randomized clinical
trials as the gold standard for determining psychotherapy’s effects, of process
research without first obtaining strong evidence of beneficial outcome, of the
PsyD degree and the accompanying professionalization of psychology, of
prescriptive authority for psychologists, and of the empirically unsupported
309
practice of psychoanalysis. These issues troubled Garfield immensely, and he
used the spoken and written word to try to hold clinical psychology on a course
that would be true to its scientific foundations, as well as to keep the Society
for Psychotherapy Research (SPR), a group that he cofounded, to its commit-
ment to students and scholarship. Garfield viewed clinical psychology as “sci-
entifically oriented” with a mission for “providing services to all segments of
the public without a dominant interest in our own economic aggrandizement”
(Garfield, 1991a; p. 119). His career was devoted to establishing this ideal, and
it served as a model, for those of us who followed, of what we might become
if we adopted his view of a worthy and accountable clinical psychology.
MAJOR CONTRIBUTIONS
During his career, which spanned more than 60 years, Sol Garfield pub-
lished nearly 200 articles, chapters, and books. These contributions had an
immense impact on the field, as noted by the extensive list of honors and
awards that Garfield received during this time. However, none of his contri-
butions match or exceed that of editing and publishing four volumes of The
Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change, with his good friend and col-
league Allen Bergin. The first of these volumes appeared in 1971 (Bergin &
Garfield, 1971) and rapidly became the psychotherapy researcher’s and clin-
ical psychologist’s bible. Among the most widely cited books in American
psychology, the Handbook set the bar of evidence for effective practice. It left
an indelible mark on two generations of clinical psychologists and researchers;
and it became the definitive reference for the information and relationships
“factually embedded” in scientific methods. Indeed, the Handbook ushered
in the age of accountability in clinical psychology, expressed best by Garfield’s
(1994) words: “Let scientific evaluation make determinations about what
treatments, provided by whom, are best applied to what types of client
problems.”
Sol Garfield provided some of the earliest work on the types and numbers
of psychotherapies being practiced. The context for this work was the very rapid
surge in different variants of psychotherapy dating from the 1960s. Surveying
so many different therapies, Garfield and his longtime colleague at Washington
University, Richard Kurtz, reported very interesting data on how therapists
described their work, finding a large number who espoused eclecticism, reject-
ing any one particular theoretical orientation in favor of picking and choosing
from a number of models (Garfield & Kurtz, 1977). Garfield’s Psychotherapy: An
Eclectic Approach (1980/1995) provided a way forward, advocating a form of
eclectic treatment built on optimizing the common client, therapist, and inter-
vention characteristics of all effective treatments (Garfield, 1987, 2000).
EARLY BEGINNINGS
Sol Garfield’s acceptance of the mantles conferred with the various roles
that became clinical psychology came in stages, and not always by his election
and choice. He was virtually driven into psychology by the anti-Semitic atti-
tudes of the time. However, Garfield developed his concern for people long
before he obtained his formal education. While still in high school and college,
he learned by experiencing the trials of helping to support his family—his par-
ents and a younger sister—during the Great Depression. He gained an appreci-
ation for struggle and pain by observing his father’s efforts to overcome the
financial failure of his grocery business and rise above it; he learned patience
and empathy as one of few non-Black residents in his Chicago neighborhood;
ACCOMPLISHMENTS
Sol Garfield’s life reflects much of the American dream in the postwar
years. He came from humble beginnings, as the child of immigrant Polish Jews
who sought in America an opportunity for freedom from oppression and big-
otry. The fact that this dream was not realized, given that Garfield experienced
some of the anti-Semitism in Middle America that his parents had sought to
escape in Poland, makes his achievements all the more impressive.
In Sol’s self-reflections, he noted progressive dissatisfaction with his col-
leagues’ psychoanalytic traditions and their authoritative, but meaningless,
formulations of patients’ problems. He also reported some satisfaction with the
OTHER CONTRIBUTIONS
INFLUENCES
There were many who influenced Garfield throughout the years. Cer-
tainly, he was close to his parents, and after his marriage sought wisdom and
support from his wife, Amy. His early mentor, Paul Witty, helped set him in a
direction of research interest and education, a direction further encouraged by
Professor A. R. Gilliland, who was then chair of the Department of Psychology
at Northwestern University. In his postgraduate years, others came to influence
him strongly. The list is endless, but certainly Ralph Heine, Craig Affleck,
Allen Bergin, Jerome Frank, Leonard Eron, and his senior colleagues and coor-
ganizers of SPR were among them. He spoke of these people often, both in his
personal communications with colleagues (present authors among them) and
in his personal reflective writings (Garfield, 1991a; 2000).
Sol Garfield was a mentor to many. He published with a large cadre of stu-
dents, the last of whom is a coauthor of this chapter (Simons). Many of his pre-
vious students are now involved in clinical psychology training in various
settings; all remember his insistence on empirical support for clinical work and
CONCLUSION
REFERENCES
Bergin, A., Garfield, S., & Thompson, A. (1967). The Chicago Conference on Clinical
Training and Clinical Psychology at Teachers College. American Psychologist, 22,
307–316.
Beutler, L. E. (May, 1998). A tribute to Sol Garfield. Unpublished paper presented to
the faculty and students at Washington University, St. Louis, MO.
Garfield, S. L. (1963). A note on patients’ reasons for terminating therapy. Psychological
Reports, 13(1), 38–42.
Garfield, S. L. (1966). Clinical psychology and the search for identity. American
Psychologist, 21, 353–362. doi:10.1037/h0023529
Garfield, S. L. (1971). Research on client variables in psychotherapy. In A.E. Bergin
& S.L. Garfield (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change: An empir-
ical analysis (1st ed., pp. 271–298). New York, NY: Wiley.
Garfield, S. L. (1978). Research on client variables in psychotherapy. In A. E. Bergin
& S. L. Garfield (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (2nd ed.,
pp. 191–232). New York, NY: Wiley.
Garfield, S. L. (1986). Research on client variables in psychotherapy. In S. L. Garfield
& A. E. Bergin (Eds.) Handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (3rd ed.,
pp. 213–256). New York, NY: Wiley.
Garfield, S. L. (1987). Towards a scientifically oriented eclecticism. Scandinavian
Journal of Behavior Therapy, 16(3), 95–109.
MAJOR CONTRIBUTIONS
319
nonetheless, and these include psychotherapy research and systematic treat-
ment selection. These two areas of concentration reflect his early career focus
on both the art of helping others and a scientific approach to that endeavor.
The search for specific factors that would maximize treatment matching
to a patient’s individual characteristics was present in most of his scientific
production and research projects, and it will probably become his most sig-
nificant contribution to the field of psychotherapy research. Illustrative
examples of this long-term commitment are the seminal book published with
John Clarkin (Beutler & Clarkin, 1990) and later with John Clarkin and
Bruce Bongar (Beutler, Clarkin, & Bongar, 2000) on systematic treatment
selection and his research projects on the treatment of depression (Beutler
et al., 1991), alcoholism (Beutler et al., 1993), and treatment matching
(Beutler, Moleiro, & Talebi, 2002), and several papers in which he challenged
the field to find and test principles that cut across theoretical orientation
and perspectives and helped develop optimal interventions adapted to the
individual characteristics of each patient.
EARLY BEGINNINGS
Larry E. Beutler was born on February 14, 1941, in a small private clinic
in Logan, Utah. He was the younger of two children. After the bombing of
Pearl Harbor, the family moved to San Francisco, where his father briefly
worked in the shipyards. From there, they moved to Arizona and then back
to Logan. His father continued to work in construction, taking the jobs he
could find and farming with his brothers, as well. The period 1943–1944, as
the war continued, was particularly hard for the family, with several severe
losses over those 12 months. His step-grandmother died, his uncle was killed
in the Battle of Okinawa, and then finally came the death of Beutler’s mother
from rheumatic heart disease. After that last loss, Larry and his sister were
shuffled among various relatives, coming to rest with his aunt. They stayed with
her for about a year, until his father remarried. The new family then moved
back into one of the family dwellings, a basement house, a type of dwelling
common at the time. They then moved to Ucon, Idaho, about 150 miles
away, and lived in a one-room cabin while his father built a house for them.
By then Larry was 5 years old, and his career as a cowboy was well developed in
his head. All told, during the first 6 years of his life, he lived in eight different
houses, four different states, and with four different families. Remembering
this, Beutler commented, “No wonder I could never stand being in one place
for a prolonged period of time.”
During his early teenage years, which were marked by more family
moves, illness, and separation, Beutler pursued his interests as a cowboy and
ACCOMPLISHMENTS
OTHER CONTRIBUTIONS
REFERENCES
Beutler, L.E. (1979). Toward specific psychological therapies for specific conditions.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 47, 882–897.
Beutler, L. E. (1983). Eclectic psychotherapy: A systematic approach. New York, NY:
Pergamon.
Beutler, L. E. (1991). Have all won and must all have prizes? Revisiting Luborsky
et al.’s verdict. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 226–232.
doi:10.1037/0022-006X.59.2.226
Beutler, L. E. (2000). David and Goliath: When psychotherapy research meets
health care delivery systems. American Psychologist, 55, 997–1007. doi:10.1037/
0003-066X.55.9.997
Beutler, L. E., Bongar, B., & Shurkin, J. L. (2000). A consumer’s guide to psychotherapy:
A complete guide to choosing the therapist and treatment that’s right for you. New York,
NY: Oxford University Press. (Originally published in 1998 as Am I crazy or is it
my shrink?)
Within the field of clinical psychology, contributors who are both psycho-
analysts and leading empirical researchers are increasingly rare. Yet one figure
who has made extensive contributions as an analytic clinician, as a researcher,
and as a theoretician is Sidney J. Blatt. In addition to being trained as a psycho-
analyst, he has conducted extensive research on personality development,
psychological assessment, psychopathology, and psychotherapeutic outcomes.
Along with his many students and colleagues, he has developed several widely
used measures, both self-report and projective, for assessing depressive style,
self- and object representations, and boundary disturbances in thought disorder.
In short, Sid has been a wide-ranging and productive scholar in a career of more
than 40 years’ duration, and throughout this career he has been committed
to the proposition that it is not only possible but also essential to investigate
psychoanalytically derived hypotheses through rigorous empirical science.
329
(or self-definitional) forms of depression (e.g., Blatt, 1974; Blatt & Shichman,
1983) and his cognitive morphology of personality development and psycho-
pathology (e.g., Blatt, 1991, 1995b). Regarding the two-configurations model,
Blatt early in his career began to differentiate between relational forms of
depression, which derive from interpersonal dependence and experiences
of loneliness and loss, and self-definitional forms, which involve experiences
of guilt, self-criticism, and failure. He has applied this distinction to psycho-
pathology in general, personality development, and psychotherapy research.
Regarding his cognitive morphology, Blatt proposed a psychoanalytically
informed cognitive developmental model of personality, according to which
psychological growth involves the maturation of underlying representational
structures of interpersonal or object relations. Initially, these representational
structures focus primarily on need gratification, and they progress toward
representations that integrate this early focus on needs with abstract concep-
tual properties involving complex psychological states and intersubjectivity
(e.g., Blatt, Auerbach, & Levy, 1997; Diamond, Kaslow, Coonerty, & Blatt,
1990). Blatt’s broad theoretical ideas serve as the foundation for his psycho-
therapy research, with its focus on how personality style and level of develop-
ment influence clinical outcome. In those studies, Blatt and his collaborators
demonstrated how anaclitic and introjective patients have very different
responses to psychotherapy, whether short-term or long-term, whether behav-
ioral or psychoanalytic, and how the personality of the patient may indeed
matter as much as or more than the type of the psychotherapy he or she
receives.
EARLY BEGINNINGS
The oldest of three children, Sidney Blatt was born on October 15, 1928,
to Harry and Fannie Blatt. Raised in modest circumstances, he grew up in a
Jewish family in South Philadelphia, where his father owned a sweets shop;
his family lived in the apartment upstairs. Sid recalls that every year he would
accompany his father to the cemetery where his grandmother was buried, and
there Sid would hold his father’s hand and attempt to console him as his
father wept over the grave. Sid also recalls that, at age 13, he accompanied
his mother on a painful 2-hour bus trip to New Jersey as she responded to an
urgent phone call informing her that her father had just suffered a heart
attack. He tried to comfort his mother during the trip while she, correctly
anticipating her father’s death, grieved his loss. Regarding these childhood
memories, Sid says that it is no surprise that he eventually was to become
interested in studying depressive experiences that focus on separation and loss.
He further recalls that, at age 9, he became disillusioned with his father for
ACCOMPLISHMENTS
Two-Configurations Model
Although Blatt is perhaps best known for his work on the two-
configurations model, he has always developed his cognitive representational
understanding of personality and psychopathology in conjunction with his
understanding of relatedness and self-definition. In 1974, he delineated a
Piaget-influenced cognitive affective model of personality development.
He proposed (e.g., Blatt, Chevron, Quinlan, Schaffer, & Wein, 1988; Blatt,
Wein, Chevron, & Quinlan, 1979) that personality development proceeds
from a sensorimotor-enactive stage, in which a person’s object relations are
dominated by concerns with gratification and frustration, through a concrete
perceptual stage, in which object relations are based on what the other looks
like, an external iconic phase, in which object relations involve mainly what
others do, an internal iconic phase, in which object relations involve mainly
what others think and feel, and finally, a conceptual stage, in which all previous
levels are integrated into a complex, coherent understanding of significant
others. Blatt used this model in developing the Conceptual Level Scale for
rating open-ended descriptions of parents and other significant figures. Later,
he integrated ideas from the two-configurations model with concepts from his
representational model of cognitive development and from intersubjectivity
theory in constructing the Differentiation–Relatedness Scale (Diamond
et al., 1990; Diamond, Blatt, Stayner, & Kaslow, 1991), a measure that rates
significant-figure descriptions from a more relational perspective. The theo-
retical assumptions underlying these scales are that cognitive development and
the development of object relations occur in parallel and that the emergence
of psychopathology is closely linked to disturbances in the development
of object relations and cognitive organization (Behrends & Blatt, 1985). For
example, low levels of differentiation–relatedness are usually found in psychosis,
intermediate levels in borderline states, and higher levels in neurotic conditions
and normality. Gradually, therefore, Blatt articulated his cognitive morphology,
a comprehensive, integrated model of personality development, psycho-
pathology, and therapeutic change that connects psychological maturation to
the level of an individual’s representation of significant interpersonal relation-
ships (Blatt, 1991, 1995b; Blatt & Blass, 1990; Blatt & Shichman, 1983).
CONCLUSION
Sidney Blatt has integrated two main ideas into psychotherapy research:
(a) the role of differences in patient personality characteristics and (b) the
importance of changes in cognitive representational aspects of personality.
Perhaps his most important contribution to psychotherapy is to have shown
that anaclitic and introjective patients have differential responses to treat-
ment that may have more influence on therapeutic outcome than the specific
therapy or therapies to which they are assigned. Thus, the distinction between
relatedness and self-definition that Sid Blatt, inspired by psychoanalytic theory,
began exploring some 35 years ago has had relevance not only for psycho-
pathology, personality theory, and psychoanalysis, as Sid originally theorized,
but for short-term, nonpsychoanalytic approaches to therapy as well. In a field
that remains divided by theoretical approach and that lacks the unified body
of knowledge that characterizes physical sciences, it is no small accomplish-
ment to have ideas that are relevant across theoretical boundaries. But this
broad relevance is precisely the case in Sidney Blatt’s work, perhaps because
he has always worked to translate complex psychoanalytic ideas into concepts
useful to clinicians and researchers of all theoretical persuasions—in essence,
because he has lived with the tension of simultaneously asking deep questions
about what it means to be human and submitting his ideas to empirical test.
REFERENCES
Allison, J., Blatt, S. J., & Zimet, C. N. (1968). The interpretation of psychological tests.
New York, NY: Harper & Row.
Auerbach, J. S., & Blatt, S. J. (2001). Self-reflexivity, intersubjectivity, and therapeutic
change. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 18, 427–450. doi:10.1037/0736-9735.18.3.427
Auerbach, J. S., Levy, K. N., & Schaffer, C. E. (2005). Relatedness, self-definition, and
mental representation: Essays in honor of Sidney J. Blatt. London, England: Routledge.
Behrends, R. S., & Blatt, S. J. (1985). Internalization and psychological development
throughout the life cycle. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 40, 11–39.
Blatt, S. J. (1959). Recall and recognition vocabulary: Implications for intellectual
deterioration. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1, 473–476.
Blatt, S. J. (1974). Levels of object representation in anaclitic and introjective
depression. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 29, 107–157.
Blatt, S. J. (1991). A cognitive morphology of psychopathology. Journal of Nervous
and Mental Disease, 179, 449–458. doi:10.1097/00005053-199108000-00001
MAJOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Much of Bill Piper’s work has been progressive in nature, involving serial
investigations that built upon each other in a systematic way. Thus, it is diffi-
cult and inappropriate to isolate particular studies as Piper’s major contribu-
tions. Instead, it is more apt to highlight the major themes of Piper’s research.
Certainly, the most prominent theme of Piper’s research program concerns
his focus on psychodynamically oriented psychotherapies. Piper found the
339
complexity of psychodynamic theories of normal and abnormal behavior to be
attractive. Their emphasis on unconscious processes was intriguing and fit with
his personal experiences and proclivity to think about underlying reasons for
these experiences. Although Piper was not among the first to study psycho-
dynamic psychotherapy, he was one of the early figures who consistently
applied rigorous methodological practices to the scientific study of psycho-
dynamic therapy.
William Piper’s career began at a time when a shift was occurring in the
field—a shift away from psychoanalytically based, long-term therapies to more
structured and short-term therapies. Although it would have been logical for
Piper to adjust his research program to this shift to survive as an academic, he
did not relent (commitment is, indeed, one of Bill’s endearing traits). His per-
sonal conviction that psychodynamic theory and therapy were appropriate and
necessary for understanding and working with the richness and complexities of
human mental development and psychopathology allowed him to stay the
course he had set for his research. Furthermore, Piper saw opportunity. With
the increasing emphasis on short-term and time-limited forms of therapy, Piper
was intrigued by the opportunity to study psychodynamic forms of therapy that
attempted to adopt such parameters. He went on to develop one of the most
comprehensive research programs on short-term, time-limited dynamic
psychotherapy in the field. Now, with the pendulum swinging back to the
middle, there is an increasing appreciation and renewed interest in psycho-
dynamic psychotherapy (Gabbard, Gunderson, & Fonagy, 2002). Piper’s work
in this area stands out as exemplary and is often referred to as a source for empir-
ical support of psychodynamic therapy.
Another theme that runs through much of Piper’s research, which has had
a significant impact on the field, concerns his focus on group psychotherapy.
The complexity of group dynamics and phenomena was intriguing and repre-
sented a challenge for Piper in terms of understanding how social processes can
contribute to both the development and amelioration of mental illness. Group
therapy research is also complex and not easy to conduct. Few have attempted
it in a consistent fashion. From his early days as a graduate student, Piper has
had an interest in groups. He was intrigued by the potentially powerful and
unique change agents in groups. His work has examined sensitivity training
groups for hospital corpsmen (Piper, 1972), long-term groups with mixed
patient samples (Piper, Debbane, & Garant, 1977), comprehensive group-
oriented partial hospitalization for patients with comorbid mood and personal-
ity disorders (Piper, Rosie, Azim, & Joyce, 1993), and short-term groups for
people who have not adapted well to death losses (Piper, McCallum, Joyce,
Rosie, & Ogrodniczuk, 2001), the latter of which is most dear to Piper’s heart.
Piper’s commitment to group therapy research has been praised as an
example of how to approach work in this area. For example, Burlingame,
EARLY BEGINNINGS
Bill Piper is the oldest of three boys from a middle-class family from Ohio.
His father was a city fireman with a 10th-grade education who did not attribute
much value to academics. His mother was a housewife until his parents
divorced when he was 14 years old. She then went to work as a grocery store
clerk in order to support the family. Bill did well academically and was also
active in a number of sports, his favorites being basketball, tennis, and track.
He was also active in church, where he met his future wife, Martha. After grad-
uating from high school, Bill won a scholarship to attend a small liberal arts col-
lege in Wooster, Ohio. He initially chose to major in chemistry, at which he
excelled. However, after having taken an introductory psychology course in the
second semester of his 1st year, he decided that he found people more interest-
ing than molecules and declared psychology as his major. During his under-
graduate work, Bill completed an honors thesis that included a study of
feedback mechanisms in small groups. Bill contends that he became curious
about groups because he was shy. He recognized that he found group situations
to be intimidating, and he challenged himself to master and overcome his fears
by understanding group phenomena. After completing his undergraduate
degree, Bill applied to graduate school in clinical psychology and was accepted
at the University of Connecticut with a U.S. Public Health scholarship.
He was a graduate student in clinical psychology in the late 1960s, a time
when sensitivity training was very popular. Sensitivity training groups (or
T-groups, as they were called) were regarded as a relatively easy way to learn
ACCOMPLISHMENTS
Between 1977 and 2008, Bill Piper completed a series of eight large-scale,
randomized clinical trials of psychotherapy. The studies’ objectives, design, and
methodology shared a number of common features. For example, each investi-
gated the efficacy of one or more forms of dynamically oriented psychotherapy,
most of which were short-term treatments. Also, in addition to including a large
battery of outcome assessments, his studies always monitored one or more
process variables during therapy, for example, therapeutic alliance and thera-
pist technique. Finally, all involved the assessment of patient characteristics to
help determine appropriate patient–treatment matches.
Each of these trials was significant in its own right, but a few stand out as
particularly meaningful to Piper and, likely, the field in general. The first of
these was conducted when Piper was at McGill in Montreal (Piper, Debbane,
Bienvenu, & Garant, 1984). The trial compared four forms of psychoanalyti-
cally oriented psychotherapy: individual or group therapy that lasted either
6 or 24 months. What emerged as important from this trial was the particular
form of therapy received, not the general type of therapy or the general dura-
tion of therapy. The results favored long-term group therapy and short-term
individual therapy. This stands as a landmark study because it remains one of
the few factorial designs in comparative therapy research.
A second significant trial occurred when Piper moved to the University
of Alberta in Edmonton. This study compared two different forms of dynami-
cally oriented, short-term individual therapy (Piper, Azim, McCallum, & Joyce,
1990). This was one of the first studies to use a patient–treatment matching par-
adigm. Specifically, the trial was designed to test the question of whether QOR
(quality of object relations; see Azim, Piper, Segal, Nixon, & Duncan, 1991;
Blatt, Wiseman, Prince-Gibson, & Gatt, 1991) had a differential effect on the
outcome of two very different types of therapy. The hypothesis was that
higher QOR patients were better suited for interpretive therapy and lower
OTHER CONTRIBUTIONS
INFLUENCES
CONCLUSION
REFERENCES
Azim, H. F. A., Piper, W. E., Segal, P. M., Nixon, G. W. H., & Duncan, S. (1991). The
quality of object relations scale. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 55, 323–343.
Blatt, S. J., Wiseman, H., Prince-Gibson, E., & Gatt, C. (1991). Object representa-
tions and change in clinical functioning. Psychotherapy, 28, 273–283.
Burlingame, G. M., Fuhriman, A. J., & Johnson, J. (2004). Process and outcome in
group counseling and psychotherapy. In J. L. DeLucia-Waack, D. A. Gerrity,
C. R. Kalodner, & M. T. Riva (Eds.), Handbook of group counseling and psycho-
therapy (pp. 49–61). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Gabbard, G. O., Gunderson, J. G., & Fonagy, P. (2002). The place of psychoanalytic
treatments within psychiatry. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 505–510. doi:10.
1001/archpsyc.59.6.505
Ogrodniczuk, J. S., & Piper, W. E. (2004). The evidence: Transference interpretations
and patient outcomes. A comparison of “types” of patients. In D. Charman (Ed.),
Core processes in brief psychodynamic psychotherapy (pp. 165–184). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
353
Garfield, Howard, and Orlinsky, undertook the important task of collating,
critically evaluating, and disseminating outcome research findings through
the publication of landmark texts such as the Handbook of Psychotherapy and
Behavior Change (Bergin & Garfield, 1978).
Irene Elkin’s innovative effort to refine randomized controlled research
designs for application in multisite collaboration psychotherapy research
trials, the Treatment of Depression Collaborative Research Program, set the
stage for the development of brief therapy approaches designed to address
specific clinical disorders such as depression and anxiety. In addition to
Elkin, numerous researchers featured in this book have conducted random-
ized clinical trial (RCT)-based therapy outcome studies that have con-
tributed to the establishment of empirical support for psychodynamic
(Blatt, Luborsky, Piper, Shapiro, Strupp), interpersonal (Strupp), client-
centered/emotion-focused (Greenberg), gestalt/emotion-focused (Beutler),
and cognitive behavioral (Beck, Beutler, Goldfried, Grawe, Shapiro) treat-
ments for depression.
The number and variety of comparative treatment trials that have been
completed over the past 30 years clearly attest to the impact that RCT
designs and the evaluation of treatment outcomes have had on the field of
psychotherapy research and practice as a whole. However, the equivalency
of positive outcome findings achieved across different therapy approaches
for the treatment of depression has led a number of researchers (Strupp,
Goldfried, Elliott, Howard, Piper) to question whether future research efforts
and funding should be focused on RCT designs that test differential treat-
ment approaches for specific clinical disorders. Several researchers have
encouraged the field to go one step beyond the question of whether one ther-
apy is superior to another and have emphasized the importance of conduct-
ing studies to identify what forms of treatment might be more effective for
particular types of clients or clinical problems (Beulter, Blatt, Grawe, Elkin,
Jones, Kiesler, Piper, Stiles).
Influential contributors such as Frank, Bordin, Luborsky, Strupp, and
Orlinsky have long advocated taking a new direction in psychotherapy
research and funding that would entail the identification and empirical vali-
dation of key mechanisms of change—across therapy approaches—that are
causally linked to efficacious treatment outcomes. Frank and Goldfried, along
with Castonguay and Beutler (2006), have also suggested that if we are to
understand how therapists can achieve more effective clinical outcomes with
their patients, future research efforts should focus on the identification and
empirical evaluation of a shared corpus of key principles of change that are
evidenced in a diverse range of evidence-based practices. Understanding
specifically how, when, and where key principles of change are most effec-
CONCLUSION
Barkham, M., Stiles, W., Connell, J., Twigg, E., Leach, C., Lucock, M., . . . Angus, L.
(2008). Effects of psychological therapies in randomised trials and practice-based
studies. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 47, 397–415. doi:10.1348/0144665
08X311713
Bergin, A. E., & Garfield, S. L. (Eds.). (1978.) Handbook of psychotherapy and behav-
ior change: An empirical (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Wiley.
Borkovec, T. D., & Castonguay, L. G. (1998). What is the scientific meaning of
“Empirically Supported Therapy”? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
66, 136–142. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.66.1.136
Borkovec, T., Echemendia, R. J., Ragusea, S. A., & Suiz, M. (2001). The Pennsylvania
Practice Research Network and future possibilities for clinically meaningful and
scientifically rigorous psychotherapy effectiveness research. Clinical Psychology:
Science and Practice, 8, 155–167. doi:10.1093/clipsy/8.2.155
Castonguay, L. G., & Beutler, L. E. (Eds.). (2006). Principles of therapeutic change that
work. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Eysenck, H. J. (1952). The effects of psychotherapy: An evaluation. Journal of
Consulting Psychology, 16, 319–324. doi:10.1037/h0063633
Horvath, A. O., & Greenberg, L. S. (1989). Development and validation of the Work-
ing Alliance Inventory. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 36, 223–233. doi:10.
1037/0022-0167.36.2.223
Kazdin, A. E. (2008). Evidence-based treatment and practice: New opportunities to
bridge clinical research and practice, enhance the knowledge base, and improve
patient care. American Psychologist, 63, 146–159. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.63.3.146
Lambert, M .J., Hansen, N. B., & Finch, A. E. (2001). Patient-focused research: Using
patient outcome to enhance treatment effects. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 9, 159–172.
Rice, L., & Wagstaff, A. K. (1967). Client vocal quality and expressive styles as indexes
of productive psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 31, 557–563.
Wampold, B. E. (2001). The great psychotherapy debate: Models, methods and findings.
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
AABT. See Association for the Advance- Assessment instruments, 71, 83–84,
ment of Behavior Therapy 119. See also specific instruments
ABCT. See Association for Behavioral Assimilation of Problematic Experiences
and Cognitive Therapies Sequence (APES), 301–302
Academy of Cognitive Therapy, 71 Association for Behavioral and Cognitive
Accurate Empathy (AE) Scale, 274 Therapies (ABCT), 6, 70
Action research, 30 Association for the Advancement of
Action stage (helping skills model), 157 Behavior Therapy (AABT), 6–7,
Adjudication model (for HSCED), 288 69
AE (Accurate Empathy) Scale, 274 Attachment, 254
AFFIRM: Psychologists Affirming Their Attitudes and Expectations form
Gay, Lesbian, and Bisexual Family, (of TDCRP), 84
135–136 Audiotaping, of therapy sessions, 21,
African Americans, 260, 262–263 272
Agnew Relationship Measure (ARM), Auditory hallucinations, 226
303 Austen Riggs Center, 334
Aiglé Foundation, 326
Alliance, measurement of, 303. See also Bandura, Albert, 102
Therapeutic alliance; Working Barrett-Lennard Relationship Inventory,
alliance 22
American Association of Applied and Beck, Aaron T., 42, 63–72
Preventive Psychology, 78 accomplishments, 67–71
American Psychiatric Association, 63, early beginnings, 66–67
324 legacy of, 72
American Psychoanalytic Association, 55 major contributions, 64–66
American Psychological Association Beck Depression Inventory, 71
(APA), 6, 34, 192, 240 Beck Institute, 71
American Psychological Foundation, 325 Begin and Garfield’s Handbook of Psycho-
American Psychological Society, 63 therapy and Behavior Change
Anaclitic form of depression, 329, 332, (Michael J. Lambert), 147
333 Behavioral analyses, 116, 117
Antipositivist revolution, 24 Behaviors
Anxiety disorders, 70, 132 common interactional, 225–226
APA. See American Psychological of therapists, 80, 81, 106
Association Behavior therapy, 4–5, 64, 102, 131. See
APA Presidential Task Force on also Cognitive–behavioral therapy
Empirically Supported Therapy Beliefs, 63, 64, 67–70
Relationships, 136 Benjamin, Lorna Smith, 221–229
APA Research Committee, 136 achievements, 224–229
APES (Assimilation of Problematic and Edward S. Bordin, 204–205
Experiences Sequence), 301–302 early beginnings, 222–224
Approval, need for, 334 legacy of, 229
ARM (Agnew Relationship Measure), major contributions, 221–222
303 Bergin, Allen E., 7, 101–109
Army, 312 early beginnings, 102–103
363
Bergin, Allen E., continued CBT. See Cognitive–behavioral therapy
and Sol L. Garfield, 310, 311, 313, CCRT. See Core conflictual relationship
315 theme
and Kenneth I. Howard, 91 Center for Cognitive Therapy, 69, 71
involvement in Society for Psycho- Center for Studies of the Person, 24
therapy, 109 Center for the Study of Collegiate
and Michael J. Lambert, 142–145, 148 Mental Health (CSCMH), 356
major accomplishments, 103–107 Central relationship patterns, 40
research on religion, psychotherapy, Centre National de la Recherche
and mental health, 107–109 Scientifique, 261
and Hans Strupp, 56 Change
Berkeley Psychotherapy Research Project, clinically significant, 146–147
262–263 cognitive information-processing
Beutler, Larry E., 7, 319–327 paradigm of, 177
accomplishments, 322–325 and emotional processes, 189
early beginnings, 320–322 and empathy, 274
legacy of, 326 Change mechanisms
major contributions, 319–320 Irene Elkin, 83–84
other contributions, 325–326 in evidence-based therapy, 357–360
Bipolar disorder, 70 William E. Piper, 341–342
Bisexual issues. See Lesbian, gay, and Laura Rice, 176–177
bisexual issues and treatment outcomes, 354–356
Blatt, Sidney J., 329–336 Change process
accomplishments, 332–335 Larry E. Beutler, 319
early beginnings, 330–332 Edward S. Bordin, 199
influences on, 335–336 Robert Elliot, 283, 287–288
legacy of, 336 Marvin R. Goldfried, 134
major contributions, 329–330 Klaus Grawe, 120–121
Bond, therapeutic, 92 Leslie Greenberg, 189
Bordin, Edward S., 199–207 Enrico Jones, 266
early beginnings, 200–201 and language, 357
and Leslie Greenberg, 190 David A.Shapiro, 274
legacy of, 206–207 Changing Frontiers in the Science of
and Lester Luborsky, 46–47 Psychotherapy (Allen E. Bergin
major contributions, 201–206 and Hans Strupp), 105
other accomplishments, 206 Checklist of Interpersonal Transactions
Bowlby, John, 223 (CLOIT), 215
Brief structured recall (BSR), 283, 287 Chestnut Lodge, 54
British Psychological Society, 271 Chicago Conference on Training in
BSR. See Brief structured recall Clinical Psychology, 314
Butler, J. M., 175, 176 Chicago Counseling Center. See Coun-
Butler-Haig Q-Sort measure of self- seling Center of University of
concept congruence/ Chicago
incongruence, 22 Child Study Department of Rochester
Society for the Prevention of
Canadian Group Psychotherapy Cruelty to Children, 20
Association, 346 Classical functional behavioral analysis,
Case formulation, comparative studies 116, 117
of, 115–117 Client-centered therapy. See also Person-
CB (cognitive behavioral) insight events, centered therapy
286 Allen E. Bergin, 102
364 INDEX
Klaus Grawe, 116 Cognitive developmental model
Leslie Greenberg, 188, 191–192 (of personality), 330
Michael J. Lambert, 142 Cognitive information-processing
Laura Rice, 176–177 paradigm (of change), 177
David A. Shapiro, 274 Cognitive morphology research, 330, 333
Client-Centered Therapy (Carl Rogers), Cognitive theory, 63, 64
21, 178 Cognitive therapy
Client Experiencing Scale, 357, 359 Aaron T. Beck, 65–66, 69
Clients. See also Patients Irene Elkin, 82, 83
engagement of, 179 Jerome Frank, 33
experiencing by, 167–170, 189 Cognitive Therapy Scale, 71
insight/insight events of, 156, 286, Collaborations (collaborative therapy
353 research)
intersession experiences of, 237 Allen E. Bergin, 104–105
perceptions of therapy process by, 285 Klaus Grawe, 118
reactions of, 156 Collaborative Research Network
session evaluations of, 303 (CRN), 237–240, 360, 361
vocal quality of, 179, 357 Collaborative Study Psychotherapy
Client–therapist mutuality, 205 Rating Scale (CSPRS), 82
Client variables, 311 Color blindness, 263
Client Vocal Quality Measure, 179 Columbia University (Teachers College),
Clinical constructs, measurement of, 342
102, 106, 108
Clinically significant change, 146–147
Common factors model (nonspecific vs.
Clinical outcome (in Generic Model of
specific factors)
Psychotherapy), 239
Jerome D. Frank, 32–36
Clinical psychology
Sol L. Garfield, 311, 314
Sol L. Garfield, 309, 310
Marvin R. Goldfried, 134
Marvin R. Goldfried, 130
Klaus Grawe, 107
scientific basis for, 52
Michael J. Lambert, 149
David A. Shapiro, 278–279
Clinical psychotherapy Lester Luborsky, 41
Larry E. Beutler, 323–324 Hans Strupp, 52–53, 56–57, 59
and research, xviii–xix Common interactional behaviors,
Clinical support tools, 146 225–226
Clinical trials, 81–82, 85. See also Communication
Randomized clinical trials interpersonal, 211, 217–219
CLOIT (Checklist of Interpersonal meta-, 218
Transactions), 215 nonverbal, 211
Coding, 214, 359 Communications analysis, 214–217
Cognitive–affective behavior therapy, Comparative studies of psychotherapy
133 Irene Elkin, 81
Cognitive–affective functioning, 176, Klaus Grawe, 118–119
177 Lester Luborsky, 41
Cognitive affective model David A. Shapiro, 275–277
(of personality), 333 Comprehensive process analysis (CPA),
Cognitive behavioral (CB) insight 283, 286–288
events, 286 Computer-aided text analyses, 252
Cognitive–behavioral therapy (CBT), 8 Computer-assisted technology, in
Marvin R. Goldfried, 129, 131–133 process research, 359–360
Enrico Jones, 265–266 Conceptual Level Scale, 333
David A. Shapiro, 272, 275–276 Conceptual stage (personality), 333
INDEX 365
Concrete perceptual stage (personality), RCT-based therapy for, 354
333 David A. Shapiro, 277, 279
Conflict resolution, 193 Depressive Experiences Questionnaire
Conjoint experience dimensions (in (DEQ), 333
Psychotherapy Session Project), Deterioration effect
237 Allen E. Bergin, 105–106
Consensual qualitative research (CQR), Michael J. Lambert, 146–147, 149
159, 287 Development, of psychotherapists,
Consistency theory, 120–121 237–239, 360
Contertransference, 9 Development of Psychotherapists
Context (in comprehensive process Common Core Questionnaire
analysis), 286 (DPCCQ), 238, 360
Contract, therapeutic, 91 DGPs (German Society for Psychology),
Core conflictual relationship theme 121
(CCRT), 40, 46 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Counseling, psychotherapy and, 202–203. Mental Disorders (DSM), 43, 44
See also Clinical psychology Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Counseling Center at University of Mental Disorders, 4th Ed.
Michigan, 206 (DSM–IV), 222
Counseling Center of University of Differentiation–Relatedness Scale, 333
Chicago, 21, 22, 165, 166, 178 Distress, 193
Couples therapy, 186, 192–194 Dittmann, Allen, 80, 202
CPA. See Comprehensive process Dodo bird verdict, 45, 300
analysis Dose–effect (dose–response model), 9
CQR. See Consensual qualitative Kenneth I. Howard, 92–94
research David A. Shapiro, 278
CRN. See Collaborative Research William B. Stiles, 301
Network DPCCQ. See Development of Psycho-
Cross-theoretical collaborations, 118 therapists Common Core
CSCMH (Center for the Study of Questionnaire
Collegiate Mental Health), 356 DRAW model (of dream interpretation),
CSPRS (Collaborative Study Psycho- 158
therapy Rating Scale), 82 Dreams
Cultural–intellectual view, of psycho- Aaron T. Beck, 68
therapy research, 3–4 Allen E. Bergin, 102
Culture-specific measures, 260 Clara E. Hill, 156, 158
Dropout rates, 145
Depression. See also Treatment of DSM. See Diagnostic and Statistical
Depression Collaborative Manual of Mental Disorders
Research Program (TDCRP) DSM–IV (Diagnostic and Statistical
Aaron T. Beck, 64, 65, 68, 70 Manual of Mental Disorders,
Larry E. Beutler, 320, 323–324 4th ed.), 222
Sidney Blatt, 329–330, 332–333 Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale, 71
Robert Elliot, 284
Robert Elliott, 289, 290 Eating disorders, 70, 253–254
Jerome Frank, 33 Eclectic–integrative therapy, 285
Eugene Gendlin, 169 Eclectic therapy, 134
Leslie Greenberg, 186, 191–192 Editorial (journal) contributions
Donald Kiesler, 216–217 Aaron T. Beck, 70–71
Leeds Depression Project, 279 Larry E. Beutler, 325–326
postnatal, 254 Eugene Gendlin, 171
366 INDEX
Marvin R. Goldfried, 136 Engagement, of clients, 179
Clara Hill, 159 Equivalence paradox, 300–301
Enrico Jones, 267 Erikson, Eric, 42, 67
Michael J. Lambert, 147 Errors, Type I, 97
David A. Shapiro, 279–280 EST (empirically-supported treatment)
William B. Stiles, 303 movement, 8, 9
Effectiveness research. See also Outcome Ethnic matching, of therapist and
research; Process–outcome patient, 262–263
research and studies Events-based research paradigm, 186
patient-focused research vs., 95 Evidence-based therapy
personality in, 334, 335 Allen E. Bergin, 107
EFT. See Emotion-focused therapy Kenneth I. Howard, 96
EFT-C (emotion-focused therapy for mechanisms of change in, 357–360
couples), 192–194 Evocative unfolding technique, 180
Elkin, Irene, 25, 77–86, 354 Evoking messages, 215
accomplishments, 80–85 Expected treatment response, 95
and Edward S. Bordin, 199, 203 Experiences, of clients and therapists,
early beginnings, 79–80 90–91, 235–237
legacy of, 85–86 Experiencing
major contributions, 78 Eugene Gendlin, 167–170
and TDCRP, 8 Marvin R. Goldfried, 132–133
Elliott, Robert, 175, 283–291, 303 Leslie Greenberg, 189
accomplishments, 285–290 Experiencing Scale, 168–170
early beginnings, 284–285 Experiential phenomenology theory,
and Leslie Greenberg, 186, 191 166–167
legacy of, 291 Experiential therapy, 22, 25, 26
professional accomplishments, Experimental case study approach, 106
290–291 Exploration stage (of helping skills
and David A. Shapiro, 275, 277, 280 model), 157
and William B. Stiles, 301 Expressive Stance Measure, 179
Ellis, Albert, 71, 108, 109 Externalizing vocal quality, 179
Emotional experiencing, 132–133 Eysenck, Hans
Emotional injuries, 192 on effectiveness of psychotherapy,
Emotional processes, 186, 189–191 104, 353
Emotional vocal quality, 179 in history of psychotherapy, 5–6
Emotion-focused therapy (EFT), 186, and Donald Kiesler, 213
189–194. See also Process– and Lester Luborsky, 43
experiential psychotherapy
(PE-EFT) Families, working alliance in, 206
Emotion-focused therapy for couples Feedback, 355–356
(EFT-C), 192–194 Klaus Grawe, 119–120
Empathy, 9, 274, 278 Michael J. Lambert, 146–147, 149
Empirically-based research (empiricism) Fetzer Institute, 222
Aaron T. Beck, 65–66 Figuration analysis, 119
Larry E. Beutler, 325 Focused vocal quality, 179
Irene Elkin, 79 Focusing Institutes, 166
Kenneth I. Howard, 95–96 Focusing-oriented psychotherapy, 170
in psychology, 3–4 Forgiveness, 186
Empirically-supported treatment (EST) Frank, Jerome D., 29–36, 354
movement, 8, 9 accomplishments, 30–36
Encompassing frameworks, 278 early beginnings, 29–30
INDEX 367
Frank, Jerome D., continued Grawe, Klaus, 113–122
in history of psychotherapy, 5 accomplishments, 115–121
and Michael J. Lambert, 148 early beginnings, 114–115
legacy of, 36 and Eugene Gendlin, 170
and Hans Strupp, 54, 56 involvement in professional
Frank, Julia B., 32 organizations, 121
Freud, Sigmund, 3, 19, 42, 48, 52, 54, and Horst Kächele, 253, 255
64, 71, 331 legacy of, 121–122
Fromm-Reichman, Frieda, 54 major contributions, 114
Functional behavioral analysis, classical, Greenberg, Leslie, 7, 9, 185–194
116, 117 accomplishments, 188–194
early beginnings, 187–188
Garfield, Sol L., 309–316 and Robert Elliot, 285, 289
accomplishments, 313–315 and Eugene Gendlin, 169
and Allen E. Bergin, 101–103 major contributions, 185–186
and Larry E. Beutler, 321, 322 and Laura Rice, 175, 176, 180
early beginnings, 311–313 task analysis of, 358
and Kenneth I. Howard, 91 Group psychotherapy, 340–341, 343, 345
influences of, 315–316 Growth, personal, 299
and Michael J. Lambert, 145, 148 Growth collaboration and Growth
legacy of, 316 Collaborators, 205, 228
major contributions, 310–311
Gay issues. See Lesbian, gay, and bisexual Hallucinations, auditory, 226
Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior
issues
Change (Allen E. Bergin and Sol
Gendlin, Eugene, 24, 165–171
L. Garfield), 7, 354
accomplishments, 166–167
Allen E. Bergin and, 101, 103, 109
early beginnings, 166
Sol L. Garfield and, 310, 311, 313,
and Irene Elkin, 79, 80
315
and Leslie Greenberg, 188
Kenneth I. Howard and, 91
legacy of, 171
Michael J. Lambert and, 142–143, 145
other contributions of, 171 David E. Orlinsky and, 233, 239
psychotherapy research of, 167–170 Health–Sickness Rating Scale (HRSR),
Generativity, 84–85 43–44
Generic Model of Psychotherapy Helping alliance, 46
Kenneth I. Howard, 91–92 Helping skills, 154, 157–158
David E. Orlinsky, 239–240 Hermeneutic single case efficacy design
German Society for Psychology (DGPs), (HSCED), 283, 287
121 Hill, Clara E., 153–160, 358
Germany, 247–248 accomplishments, 155–160
Gestalt therapy, 190 early beginnings, 154–155
Gift of love hypothesis, 228 and Robert Elliot, 287
Global Assessment of Functioning, 44 and Eugene Gendlin, 168
Goal attainment, 186 influences, 160
Goldfried, Marvin R., 129–137, 354, 361 legacy of, 160
early beginnings, 130–131 major contributions, 153–154
legacy of, 137 and William B. Stiles, 303
major contributions, 131–136 Horney, Karen, 284
other accomplishments, 136–137 Howard, Kenneth I., xvii, 9, 89–98
and David A. Shapiro, 277, 280 analyses of research data, 97–98
and Hans Strupp, 51 and Lorna Smith Benjamin, 223
368 INDEX
and Sidney Blatt, 326, 361 Interaction structure theory, 266–267
dose-effect and outcome phase Internal dialogue, of patients, 68
models, 92–94 Internalized love, 228
early influences, 90 International Focusing Society, 171
empirical basis for mental health International Psychoanalytic
service policy, 95–96 Association, 267
Generic Model of Psychotherapy, International research networks,
91–92 360–361
and Michael J. Lambert, 148 Interpersonal Circle taxonomy, 215
Northwestern–Chicago Psycho- Interpersonal communication model,
therapy Research Program, 92 211, 217–219
and David E. Orlinsky, 234–237, 239 Interpersonal process recall procedure,
patient-focused research model and 285
expected treatment response Interpersonal reconstructive therapy
work, 94–95 (IRT), 221, 227–229
research on patients’ and therapists’ Interpersonal relating and relationships,
experiences in therapy, 90–91 225–227, 299
Society for Psychotherapy Research Interpersonal theory and therapy, 8
and, 6, 84 Irene Elkin, 82, 83
HRSR (Health–Sickness Rating Scale), Marvin R. Goldfried, 134–135
43–44 Donald Kiesler, 215–217
HSCED. See Hermeneutic single case Hans Strupp, 53
efficacy design Intersession Experience Questionnaires
Humanistic psychotherapy (humanism), (IEQs), 237
5 Interventions, matching patients and
Leslie Greenberg, 186, 188 Larry E. Beutler, 320, 322–323
Michael J. Lambert, 144 William E. Piper, 341, 344–345
Carl Rogers, 25 Interview types, of therapists, 178–179
Intrapersonal focus, of therapists, 135
IEQs (Intersession Experience Introjective form of depression,
Questionnaires), 237 329–330, 332–335
Illinois Psychological Association, 234, IRT. See Interpersonal reconstructive
315 therapy
IMI (Impact Message Inventory), 215
Impact Message Inventory (IMI), 215 Jones, Enrico, 259–268
Impact messages, 215 accomplishments, 262–267
Individual differences, social aspects of, beginnings, 260–262
259, 262 legacy of, 267–268
Input variables (in Generic Model of major contributions, 259–260
Psychotherapy), 91, 239 Journals, xviii, 7. See also Editorial
Insight (insight events), 156, 286, 353 (journal) contributions
Insight stage (helping skills model), 157 Justice, social, 30
Institute of Medicine, 63
Institute of Psychiatry, London, 273 Kächele, Horst, 9, 247–256, 358, 359
Integration, of counseling and psycho- accomplishments, 250–253
therapy, 202–203 early beginnings, 248–250
Intellectual–cultural view, of psycho- and Klaus Grawe, 118
therapy research, 3–4 influences of, 254–255
Interaction patterns legacy of, 255–256
Enrico Jones, 264–265 major contributions, 248
David E. Orlinsky, 225–226 other contributions, 253–254
INDEX 369
Kiesler, Donald, 7, 8, 211–219 Meaning bridges, 286, 301
and Edward S. Bordin, 204 Medical Research Council (MRC), 273,
early beginnings, 212 274, 278
and Eugene Gendlin, 169 Meehl, Paul, 130
legacy of, 219 Meichenbaum, Donald, 71
major accomplishments, 212–219 Menninger, Karl, 5, 143
Menninger Foundation, 46–47
Lambert, Michael J., 9, 141–150, Menninger Foundation Psychotherapy
355–356 Research Project, 42–44, 334
accomplishments, 144–148 Mental Health Foundation Psycho-
early beginnings, 142–143 therapy Initiative, 280
and Eugene Gendlin, 168 Mental health service policy, 95–96
influences of, 148 Mentoring
legacy of, 149–150 William E. Piper, 347
major contributions, 142–143 Irene Elkin, 84–85
Language, in change process, 357 Sol L. Garfield, 315–316
Leadership contributions Marvin R. Goldfried, 137
Aaron T. Beck, 70–71 Horst Kächele, 254
Robert Elliott, 290 Donald Kiesler, 219
Sol L. Garfield, 314–315 David E. Orlinsky, 234
Eugene Gendlin, 171 Laura Rice, 181
Marvin R. Goldfried, 136 William B. Stiles, 298
Clara E. Hill, 154, 159–160 Meta-analyses, 274–275
Enrico Jones, 267 Metacommunication, 218
William E. Piper, 346 Mindfulness-based approach to
David A. Shapiro, 279–280 cognitive therapy, 70
William B. Stiles, 303 Mood disorders, 345
Learning theory, 79, 102 MRC. See Medical Research Council
Leary, Timothy, 214, 215, 217, 224 Multidimensional analyses, 8–9
Leeds Depression Project, 279 Multisite clinical trials, 81–82, 85
Lesbian, gay, and bisexual issues, 129, Murray, Henry, 42, 331
135–136, 361 Myth (in common factors model), 35
Lewin, Kurt, 29–30
Limited vocal quality, 179 Narratives, 9, 250–253
Logical positivism, 4 National Committee for a Sane Nuclear
Love, internalized, 228 Policy (SANE), 30
Luborsky, Lester, 39–48 National Institute of Drug Abuse, 44
accomplishments, 43–48 National Institute of Mental Health
early beginnings, 41–43 (NIMH)
in history of psychotherapy, 6 awards from, 89
legacy of, 48 Irene Elkin and, 77, 78, 80–81,
major contributions, 39–41 83–84
Marvin R. Goldfried and, 136
Maladaptive transaction cycles, 216–217 grants from, 44, 54, 178, 191, 240
Managed care, 92, 147 in history of psychotherapy research,
Matching 7–8
of patients and treatments, 322–323, National Institute of Mental Health
341, 344–345 Clinical Research Branch, 8
of therapists and patients, 262–263 National Institute of Mental Health
Maternal deprivation, 223 Collaborative Study of Treatments
McGlenn, M. L., 287 of Depression, 25. See also Treat-
370 INDEX
ment of Depression Collaborative Outcome Questionnaire-45 (OQ-45),
Research Program (TDCRP) 142, 145, 147, 149
National Institute of Mental Health Outcome research and studies. See also
Outcome Measures Project, 206 Process–outcome research and
National Institute of Mental Health studies
Psychosocial Treatments Allen E. Bergin, 103–104
Research Branch, 8 in future of psychotherapy, 353–356
National Institute of Mental Health Sol L. Garfield, 313–314
Treatment Development and Eugene Gendlin, 168
Process Research section, 83 Leslie Greenberg, 190–191
Naturalistic clinical settings, 203 and history of psychotherapy, 4–5
Naturalistic observations, 176 Lester Luborsky, 40, 43–45
Need for approval, 334 David E. Orlinsky, 239
Negative cognitive triad, 64 Laura Rice, 179
Neuropsychotherapy research, 121 Carl Rogers, 21–22
Neuroses, 169 David A. Shapiro, 275–277
NIMH. See National Institute of Mental William B. Stiles, 300–301
Health Outpatient care, for mood and personal-
Nonspecific vs. specific factors, 52–53. ity disorders, 345
See also Common factors model Output variables (in Generic Model of
Nonverbal communication, 211 Psychotherapy), 91, 239
Normed self-monitoring scales of work
involvement, 238 Panic disorder, 288
Northwestern–Chicago Psychotherapy Paraprofessionals, 107
Research Program, 92 Patient-focused research model, 94–95
Nuclear weapons, psychological aspects Patient outcome measures, session level,
of, 30 355–356
Patients. See also Clients
Obsessive–compulsive disorder, 227 ethnic matching of therapists and,
Ontario Mental Health Foundation, 262–263
192 experiences of therapy by, 90–91,
Operationalized psychodynamic diagnosis 235–237
system, 252 interactions of therapists and, 9, 40,
Operations, therapeutic, 91 264–265
OQ-45. See Outcome Questionnaire-45 internal dialogue of, 68
Organizational structure, of psycho- intersession experiences of, 237
therapy research, 6–8 Patient–treatment matching
Organizations, involvement with. See Larry E. Beutler, 320, 322–323
Leadership contributions William E. Piper, 341, 344–345
Orlinsky, David E., xvii, 233–241, 360, PD. See Personality disorder
361 PE-EFT. See Process–experiential
accomplishments, 235–240 psychotherapy
early beginnings, 234–235 Penn Helping Alliance Scales, 359
and Eugene Gendlin, 170 Penn Psychotherapy Research Project,
and Kenneth I. Howard, 90, 92 6, 40, 43–47
and Michael J. Lambert, 148 Pennsylvania Psychological Association’s
legacy of, 240–241 Practice Research Network, 356
Society for Psychotherapy Research PEP. see Psychotherapeutiche Einzelfall
and, 6 Prozess-forschung
Outcome Measures Project, 80 Perfectionism, 334
Outcome phase model, 9, 93–94 Perls, Fritz, 188
INDEX 371
Personal growth, 299 Process research and studies
Personality, 330, 333–335 Sol L. Garfield, 309
Personality disorder (PD) Marvin R. Goldfried, 134–135
Lorna Smith Benjamin, 221–222, Clara Hill, 153, 155–156
226–227 William E. Piper, 341–342, 345–346
William B. Piper, 345 in psychotherapy field, 357–360
Person-centered therapy, 22, 25–26. Carl Rogers, 18, 21
See also Client-centered therapy David A. Shapiro, 277–278
Pharmacological treatment, 82, 83 Process Scale, 168, 170
Piaget, Jean, 117 Programmatic linguistic analyses, 9
Piper, William E., 339–348 Prototype methodology, 263–264
accomplishments, 344–346 Psychoanalytic theory, 251–253
early beginnings, 342–344 Psychodynamic interpersonal (PI)
influences of, 346–347 therapy
legacy of, 347–348 Robert Elliott, 286
major contributions, 339–342 Marvin R. Goldfried, 134–135
other contributions, 346 David A. Shapiro, 272, 275–276
PI therapy. See Psychodynamic inter- Psychodynamic theory and therapy
personal therapy Aaron Beck, 64, 68
Placebo effects, 31, 33, 142 Lorna Smith Benjamin, 221
Plan Analysis, 115, 116 William E. Piper, 339–340
Positive regard, 9 Enrico Jones, 265–266
Positivism, 4, 24, 167 Lester Luborsky, 40
Postnatal depression research, 254 Psychologists for Social Responsibility,
PQS. See Psychotherapy Process Q-set 171
Practice-based evidence, 96–97 Psychooncology research, 254
Practice-research networks, 356 Psychopathology, 225, 229
Primates, 223, 224 Psychotherapeutiche Einzelfall Prozess-
Problematic reaction points, 180, 286 forschung (PEP), 118, 253
Problem-solving training, 132 Psychotherapeutic investigations, 271
Process coding systems, 214 Psychotherapists. See Therapists
Process–experiential psychotherapy Psychotherapy, 3–10, 353–361. See also
(PE-EFT). See also Emotion- Comparative studies of psycho-
focused therapy (EFT) therapy
Robert Elliott, 284, 288–290 clinical, xviii–xix, 323–324
Leslie Greenberg, 191–192 effects of, 144, 145
Laura Rice, 175, 180–181 history of, 4–9
Process–outcome analyses, in TDCRP, 83 integration of counseling and,
Process–outcome research and studies. 202–203
See also Outcome research and intellectual–cultural view of, 3–4
studies outcome research in, 353–356
Larry E. Beutler, 322–323 process and process–outcome studies
Robert Elliott, 289 in, 357–360
Jerome D. Frank, 31–36 social factors in, 262–263
in future of psychotherapy, 357–360 therapist contributions in, 360–361
Marvin R. Goldfried, 129 Psychotherapy integration movement,
Leslie Greenberg, 185, 188–189 133–134
Enrico Jones, 265–266 Psychotherapy Process Q-set (PQS),
David E. Orlinsky, 239–240 259, 260, 263–267
Carl Rogers, 5 “Psychotherapy Recapitulates Ontogeny”
Hans Strupp, 57–58 (William B. Stiles), 299
372 INDEX
Psychotherapy Session Project, 90–91, Remoralization phase (of outcome phase
235–237 model), 93–94
Repetitive interaction structures, 260
QOR. See Quality of object relations Representational theory of personality,
Qualitative research 333, 334
Robert Elliot, 286–288 Responsiveness, 300–301
Clara E. Hill, 159, 358 Rhesus monkeys, 223
Carl Rogers, 23–24 Rice, Laura, 9, 24, 175–181
William B. Stiles, 302–303, 358 accomplishments, 178–181
Quality of object relations (QOR), Client Vocal Quality Scale of, 357
344–345 early beginnings, 177–178
Quantitative naturalistic research and Robert Elliot, 285, 286, 289
Kenneth I. Howard, 96–98 and Leslie Greenberg, 185–189, 191
Laura Rice, 178–179 legacy of, 181
Quantitative outcome measures major contributions, 176–177
(in HSCED), 287 Ritual (in common factors model), 35
Rogers, Carl, 17–26, 331
Race, 260, 262–263 accomplishments, 20–22
Randomized clinical trials, 22, 31 and Lorna Smith Benjamin, 223
William E. Piper, 344–346 and Allen E. Bergin, 102, 109
Irene Elkin, 354 and Edward S. Bordin, 200–202, 204
Sol L. Garfield, 309 early beginnings, 19–20
outcome studies based on, 354–355 and Irene Elkin, 80
Randomized Controlled Trials Commit- and Eugene Gendlin, 165, 166, 168
tee for the Canadian Institutes of and Leslie Greenberg, 188
Health Research, 346 in history of psychotherapy, 5
Rank, Otto, 20 and Kenneth I. Howard, 90
Rapaport-Klein Study Group, 335 and Donald Kiesler, 211–213
Rasch analysis, 284, 288 and Michael J. Lambert, 144, 148
Rationale (in common factors model), legacy of, 24–26
35 major contributions, 18–19
Rationalism, 4 process–outcome research, 23–24
Realism, 3–4 and Laura Rice, 175, 176, 178
Realizations, therapeutic, 92 on valuing clients, 302
Recording, of psychotherapy sessions, Rorschach test, 21, 179, 311
21, 272 Rosenzweig, Saul, 45, 48
Reductionism, 106
Regressive loyalty and Regressive SANE (National Committee for a Sane
Loyalists, 205, 228 Nuclear Policy), 30
Rehabilitation phase (of outcome phase SASB-based Intrex questionnaire, 225
model), 94 SASB model. See Structural analysis of
Relational disorders, 225 social behavior model
Relational form of depression, 329, 330, Scale for Suicide Ideation, 71
332, 333 Schemas, 64–65, 301
Relationship factors, 177 Schema Theory, 117
Relationship patterns, central, 40 Schizophrenia, 70, 80, 169. See also
Remediation phase (of outcome phase Wisconsin Schizophrenia Project
model), 94 SCID-II (Structures Clinical Interview
Remission, spontaneous, 141, 142, 144, for DSM–IV Personality
145 Disorders), 222
INDEX 373
Scientific Affairs of the American Single-case research, 357
Psychoanalytic Association, 267 Robert Elliott, 287–288
Scientific methods and standards Horst Kächele, 250–253
of Irene Elkin, 81–83 Social aspects, of individual differences,
of Hans Strupp, 55–56 262
Scientist–practitioner model, xix Social justice, 30
Self-actualization, 176 Society for Personality Assessment, 222
Self-definitional form of depression, Society for Psychopathology Research, 70
330, 332–335 Society for Psychotherapy Research
Self-disclosure, 156 (SPR)
Lorna Smith Benjamin and, 222
Self-relatedness (in Generic Model of
Allen E. Bergin and, 109
Psychotherapy), 92
Larry E. Beutler and, 321, 325
Sensitivity training groups (T-groups),
Edward S. Bordin and, 199, 204, 206
342–343
Irene Elkin and, 84
Sensorimotor-enactive stage
Robert Elliott and, 285, 290
(personality), 333 in future of psychotherapy, 360
SEPI (Society for the Exploration of Sol L. Garfield and, 310, 315
Psychotherapy Integration), Marvin R. Goldfried and, 136
133–134 Klaus Grawe and, 121
SEQ (Session Evaluation Questionnaire), Leslie Greenberg and, 190, 191
303 Clara E. Hill and, 159–160
Sequential process research paradigm Kenneth I. Howard and, 89, 98, 240
studies, 21 Enrico Jones and, 267
Serendipitous findings, 56 Horst Kächele and, 250, 254–255
Service delivery research, 278–279 Michael J. Lambert and, 148
Session Evaluation Questionnaire Lester Luborsky and, 47, 48
(SEQ), 303 origin of, 6
Session evaluations, of clients, 303 David E. Orlinsky and, 234, 240
Session level patient outcome measures, Laura Rice and, 175, 181
355–356 David A. Shapiro and, 271, 279–280
Sexual orientation. See Lesbian, gay, William B. Stiles and, 303
and bisexual issues Hans Strupp and, 55
Shapiro, David A., 271–281 Society for Psychotherapy Research
accomplishments, 274–279 Collaborative Research Network
early beginnings, 272–274 (CRN), 237–240, 360, 361
and Robert Elliott, 288 Society for the Exploration of Psycho-
therapy Integration (SEPI),
influences of, 280–281
133–134
legacy of, 281
Specificity, 31–33
major contributions, 272
Spontaneous remission, 141, 142, 144,
other contributions, 279–280 145
and William B. Stiles, 301 SPR. See Society for Psychotherapy
Sheffield Psychotherapy Projects, Research
275–278, 281, 288–289 SPRI project. See Single-case Process
Short-term forms of therapy Research project
Sidney J. Blatt, 334–335 State University of New York at Stony
William E. Piper, 340, 344–346 Brook, 131, 137
Significant events (in HSCED), 288 Statistical analyses, 97–98
Single-case Process Research (SPRI) Stiles, William B., 295–304
project, 118, 253 accomplishments, 298–303
374 INDEX
beginnings, 295–298 Theory building, 295, 298–299
and Klaus Grawe, 117 Theory of personality, 176
legacy of, 304 Therapeutic alliance, 359
qualitative strategies of, 358 Irene Elkin, 83–84
and David A. Shapiro, 275, 277, Marvin R. Goldfried, 135
278, 280 Lester Luborsky, 46
Stimulus hunger, 176 Carl Rogers, 18–19
Strong Vocational Interest Inventory, Therapeutic bond (in Generic Model of
312 Psychotherapy), 92
Structural Analysis of Social Behavior, Therapeutic change, 120–121
359 Therapeutic contract (in Generic
Structural analysis of social behavior Model of Psychotherapy), 91
(SASB) model, 221, 224–227 Therapeutic cycle model, 252
Structures Clinical Interview for Therapeutic operations (in Generic
DSM–IV Personality Disorders Model of Psychotherapy), 91
(SCID-II), 222 Therapeutic realizations (in Generic
Strupp, Hans, 6, 51–61, 358 Model of Psychotherapy), 92
accomplishments and honors, 55–59 Therapeutic relationship, 34, 59, 240
and Allen E. Bergin, 104, 105 Therapeutic setting (in common factors
early beginnings, 53–54 model), 34
and Michael J. Lambert, 148 Therapist contributions
legacy of, 59–60
in future of psychotherapy, 360–361
and Lester Luborsky, 44, 47
Hans Strupp on, 55, 57
major contributions, 52–53
Therapist-offered conditions, 144, 145
and David E. Orlinsky, 236
Therapist response modes, 155–156
Stuttgart Center for Psychotherapy
Therapists
Research, 248, 253
accounts and perceptions of, 285, 288
Subjectivity, quantification of, 9
behavior of, 80, 81, 106
Suicide, 69, 71
congruence of, 214
Suicide Intent Scale, 71
Sullivan, Henry Stack, 80, 148, 216 development of, 237–239, 360
Supervision, 150, 206 ethnic matching of patients and,
Supportive–expressive psychotherapy, 262–263
41, 46–47 experiences of therapy by, 90–91,
Symptom Checklist-90-R, 288 235–237
Symptom formation, 40 focus of, 134–135
interactions of patients and, 9, 40,
Task analysis, 9, 358 264–265
Robert Elliott, 289 intersession experiences of, 237
Leslie Greenberg, 186, 188–189 interview types of, 178–179
Laura Rice, 180 response modes of, 286
TAT. See Thematic Apperception Test responsiveness of, 117
TDCRP. See Treatment of Depression Therapist Session Reports, 359
Collaborative Research Program Therapy
Teaching Patients’ and therapists’ experiences
of focusing, 170 of, 90–91, 235–237
of helping skills, 157–158 Therapists’ perceptions and accounts
by William B. Stiles, 296–297 of, 285, 288
Temple Psychotherapy Project, 6 Therapy Session Reports (TSR), 235–237
Thematic Apperception Test (TAT), Therapy Spectrum Analysis (TSA),
21, 42, 311 114, 119–120
INDEX 375
Thompson, John, 296 University of Michigan, 200, 202, 206
Time-limited dynamic psychotherapy University of North Carolina, Chapel
(TLDP), 58–59, 145 Hill, 296–297
Topeka Project, 251 University of Pennsylvania, 42–44, 47
Topeka Psychoanalytic Institute, 42 University of Sheffield, 273–247
Training University of Utah Neuropsychiatric
by Larry E. Beutler, 326 Institute, 228–229
by Robert Elliott, 290 University of Wisconsin, 23, 25. See also
by Sol L. Garfield, 313 Wisconsin Schizophrenia Project
by Klaus Grawe, 118
Clara Hill’s study of, 157–158 Vanderbilt I and II experiments, 6,
by Lester Luborsky, 47–48 51–52, 56–59, 358
in psychotherapy field, 360–361 Vanderbilt Psychotherapy Process Scale
research methods in, 19 (VPPS), 236, 359
by David A. Shapiro, 279–281 Verbal Response Modes (VRMs),
Hans Strupp on, 59 299–300, 357
Transference interpretations, 345–346 Vertical behavioral analysis, 116, 117
Treatment of Depression Collaborative Veterans Administration, 31
Research Program (TDCRP), 8 Victor Frankl Foundation, 171
Sidney J. Blatt, 334, 335 Videotaping, of therapy sessions, 272
Visual perception, 42
Irene Elkin, 77–78, 80–86
Vocal quality, of clients, 179, 357
Enrico Jones, 265, 354
Vocal Quality Scale, 357
Carl Rogers, 25
Vocational decision-making, 199,
Treatment–patient matching. See
201–202
Patient–treatment matching
Von Uexküll, Thure, 249
Treatment response, in psychotherapy
VPPS. See Vanderbilt Psychotherapy
research, 4–6. See also Process–
Process Scale
outcome research and studies
VRMs. See Verbal Response Modes
TR-EAT project, 253
TSA. See Therapy Spectrum Analysis Wachtel, Paul, 133, 335
TSR (Therapy Session Reports), 235–237 WAI. See Working Alliance Inventory
Two-chair dialogue events, 189, 285, Washington School of Psychiatry
358 (WSP), 54
Two-configurations model, 329–334 Western Behavioral Sciences Institute,
Type I errors, 97 23–24
Wisconsin Schizophrenia Project, 22
Ulm Textbank, 249, 252 Eugene Gendlin, 165, 169
Ulm University, 9, 248–250, 252 Donald Kiesler, 211–214
Unconscious, 68 Carl Rogers, 23
Uniformity myth, 213 Working alliance, 9
University of Chicago Edward S. Bordin, 199, 201–202,
Irene Elkin, 78, 84–85 204–206
Eugene Gendlin, 165, 166 Leslie Greenberg, 190
Kenneth I. Howard, 90 Lester Luborsky, 46
David E. Orlinsky, 234–235, 240–241 Working Alliance Inventory (WAI),
Laura Rice, 175, 178 190, 303, 359
Carl Rogers, 21–22 “WOW!-effects,” 106
376 INDEX
ABOUT THE EDITORS
377
the Narrative Processes Coding System (with Heidi Levitt and Karen
Hardtke) for application in differing treatment approaches.