Intelectual Disability Nas Stigma PDF
Intelectual Disability Nas Stigma PDF
Intelectual Disability Nas Stigma PDF
Edited by
KATRINA SCIOR & SHIRLI WERNER
Intellectual Disability and Stigma
Katrina Scior • Shirli Werner
Editors
Intellectual Disability
and Stigma
Stepping Out from the Margins
Editors
Katrina Scior Shirli Werner
Department of Psychology Paul Baerwald School of Social Work
University College London and Social Welfare
London, United Kingdom Hebrew University of Jerusalem
Jerusalem, Israel
Patrick W. Corrigan
Contents
ix
x Contents
Epilogue 221
Katrina Scior and Shirli Werner
Index 225
About the Editors
xiii
Notes on Contributors
Afia Ali is a senior clinical lecturer at University College London, UK, and
honorary consultant psychiatrist working with adults with intellectual disabili-
ties in North London. Her research interests include the experience of stigma
and health inequalities in people with intellectual disabilities.
Sian Anderson is a research fellow in the Living with Disability Research
Centre and sessional lecturer in the discipline of Social Work at LaTrobe
University in Melbourne, Australia. She recently completed a PhD at LaTrobe
which examined the impact of engagement in self-advocacy groups on the social
identity of adults with intellectual disabilities.
Dorit Barak is the academic coordinator of the network of self-advocacy
groups in Israel led by the organizations Beit Issie Shapiro and Elwyn. Her cur-
rent research interests include empowerment and self-advocacy of people with
disabilities and grassroots organizations for social change. She is active
in the establishment, planning, design, and capacity building of a social
movement of people with disabilities in Israel.
Christine Bigby is Professor of Social Work and Director of the Living with
Disability Research Centre at La Trobe University, Melbourne, Australia. Her
research has focused on the effectiveness of social programs and policies that
aim to support the social inclusion of people with intellectual disabilities in
adulthood and later life. She has published extensively in peer-reviewed journals
xv
xvi Notes on Contributors
and is the founding editor of the Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual and
Developmental Disabilities.
Elizabeth E. Biggs is a doctoral candidate in the Department of Special
Education at Vanderbilt University, USA. Her interests include promoting
meaningful inclusion for students with severe disabilities in school and com-
munity settings. She is particularly interested in social and communication
interventions for students with limited or no verbal speech.
Carly L. Blustein is a doctoral candidate in the Department of Special
Education at Vanderbilt University, USA. She is interested in understanding
how young people with severe disabilities navigate the school-to-work transition
and prepare for postsecondary outcomes.
Erik W. Carter is a professor in the Department of Special Education at
Vanderbilt University, USA, and a Vanderbilt Kennedy Center Investigator. His
research focuses on evidence-based strategies for supporting inclusion
and valued roles in school, work, and community settings for children
and adults with severe disabilities.
Patrick W. Corrigan is Distinguished Professor of Psychology at the Illinois
Institute of Technology, USA, and principal investigator of the US National
Consortium on Stigma and Empowerment. His work focuses on under-
standing ways to replace the stigma of mental illness with affirming atti-
tudes and behaviors.
Jason W. Crabtree is lead clinical psychologist in a community team for adults
with intellectual disabilities in inner London, UK. His work and research inter-
ests include interventions for individuals with complex and challenging
behavior, diagnosis and interventions for individuals with autism spec-
trum conditions, and stigma, self-identity, and self-evaluation in indi-
viduals with intellectual disabilities.
Nicole Ditchman is an assistant professor in the Department of Psychology at
Illinois Institute of Technology, USA. She is a certified rehabilitation counselor
and licensed clinical professional counselor. Her professional and research
work have aimed to advance our understanding of quality of life and suc-
cessful outcomes for young adults with disabilities. Her research focuses
on factors affecting community integration, sense of community, and social reci-
procity for people with disabilities.
Notes on Contributors xvii
Figures
Fig. 7.1 The three-stage process of self-stigma 99
Fig. 9.1 Multilevel model of stigma change interventions 131
Fig. 12.1 Family influences on society and the person
with an intellectual disability 182
Tables
Table 7.1 Summary of studies on stigma in people with intellectual
disabilities (ID) 93
xxi
Part I
Theory and Concepts
1
Toward Understanding Intellectual
Disability Stigma: Introduction
Katrina Scior
K. Scior ()
Division of Psychology & Language Sciences,
University College, London, UK
e-mail: [email protected]
Stigma not only affects the person but may extend to include his or her
whole family as well. Families may be affected in three ways: (1) through
negative attitudes others may hold about the families of someone with
intellectual disability, what has been termed ‘courtesy stigma’ (Ali et al.
2012; Birenbaum 1992); (2) through their fear that others view them
negatively as parents or family members of someone with an intellectual
disability, referred to as ‘anticipated stigma’ (Weiss 2008); and (3) by
internalizing others’ negative attitudes toward them, referred to as ‘affiliate
stigma’ (Mak and Cheung 2008). To date, only limited research has been
conducted on these three aspects and the relationships between them.
Stigma and Identity
One question which crops up repeatedly in discussions of stigma, par-
ticularly its potential internalization and the need to organize in self-
advocacy groups to take collective action against stigma, is whether the
individuals concerned in fact view themselves as having an intellectual
disability. Some have proposed that in order to develop a positive sense
of self, coming to accept one’s intellectual disability and learning to
manage the stigmatized identity are crucial (Szivos and Griffiths 1990).
Others, in contrast, have argued that the label of intellectual disability is
so toxic that individuals given this label have very good reason to reject
it (Gillman et al. 2000). Yet others have questioned the whole notion of
accepting or rejecting this label and have pointed to the fluid, context-
dependent nature of identity (Rapley 2004). A young woman, for exam-
ple, who is of short stature and has Down syndrome, when surrounded
by tall people may view her stature as a prominent and possibly defining
feature. When on a girls’ night out though, being short or tall is likely to
be of much less relevance than being female, someone who shares others’
interest in Karaoke, or perhaps a wearer of trainers of a certain popular
brand. Even in relation to the label of intellectual disability, answers to
the question whether or not someone ascribes this label to themselves are
much less clear cut than often suggested. To illustrate, the young woman
may identify with the label of intellectual disability in some regards, such
as annoyance at everyone taking a much closer interest in her relationship
1 Toward Understanding Intellectual Disability Stigma 9
with her boyfriend than they do for her younger sister, while she may
reject the label when invited to attend segregated activities. Perhaps then
an even fleeting alignment with others similarly labeled, without neces-
sarily assuming an ‘intellectual disabled identity’, is all that is called for as
basis for collective action.
While touching on identity politics, we accept that in drawing atten-
tion to intellectual disability stigma in this book, we inevitably imply the
existence of an essential entity—a group unified by its distinctive features,
rather than focusing on the myriad distinctions between the millions of
children and adults around the world labeled as having intellectual dis-
abilities. As such, we recognize that we are guilty of what Gergen (1999)
termed an essentialist presumption implicit in much identity politics.
This Book
Our aim in producing this edited text is to generate debate around a topic
that has received limited attention but has a major impact on people
with intellectual disabilities, their families, and society at large. We have
arranged the book in three parts that we hope make sense to the reader.
Consideration of broader theoretical issues in Part I is followed with in-
depth analysis of the consequences of intellectual disability stigma in Part
II. In Part III, perhaps the most important part, how to tackle intellectual
disability stigma is addressed.
Looking to the future, in relation to long-term illness it has been sug-
gested that we are perhaps witnessing the end of stigma (Green 2009).
Recent testimonies we gathered from around the globe suggest, sadly,
that this is far from the reality where intellectual disability is concerned
(Scior et al. 2015). While huge progress has been made toward the inclu-
sion and protection of the fundamental rights of persons with intellectual
disabilities, they are still mostly far from being accepted as equal citizens.
In highly industrialized Western countries we are witnessing an inter-
esting paradox—in the midst of frantic activity and the idolization of
autonomy and independence, more and more people are embracing the
slow movement. Where for a long time one’s value in the (Western) world
has been measured in part by one’s capacity for autonomy, and to perform
10 K. Scior
Accessible Summary
• People with intellectual disabilities around the world often face bad
attitudes and actions.
• This often makes life more difficult for them and their families.
1 Toward Understanding Intellectual Disability Stigma 11
References
Ali, A., Hassiotis, A., Strydom, A., & King, M. (2012). Self stigma in people
with intellectual disabilities and courtesy stigma in family carers: A system-
atic review. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33, 2122–2140.
doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2012.06.013.
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders (5th ed.) (DSM-5).Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
Association.
Birenbaum, A. (1992). Courtesy stigma revisited. Mental Retardation, 30,
265–268.
Blascovich, J., Mendes, W. B., Hunter, S. B., & Lickel, B. (2000). Stigma, threat
and social interactions. In T. F. Heatherton, R. E. Kleck, M. R. Hebl, & J. G.
Hull (Eds.), The social psychology of stigma (pp. 307–331). New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
Conner, M., & Armitage, C. J. (2008). Attitudinal ambivalence. In W. D.
Crano & R. Prislin (Eds.), Attitudes and attitude change (pp. 261–286).
New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Dagnan, D., & Waring, M. (2004). Linking stigma to psychological distress:
Testing a social cognitive model of the experience of people with intellectual
disabilities. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 11, 247–254. doi:10.1002/
cpp.413.
Furnham, A., & Procter, E. (1989). Belief in a just world: Review and critique
of the individual difference literature. British Journal of Social Psychology, 28,
365–384. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8309.1989.tb00880.
Gergen, K. (1999). Social construction and the transformation of identity politics.
Retrieved from https://www.swarthmore.edu/sites/default/files/assets/docu-
ments/kenneth-gergen/Social%20Construction_and_the_Transformation.pdf
Gillman, M., Heyman, B., & Swain, J. (2000). What’s in a name? The implica-
tions of diagnosis for people with learning difficulties and their family carers.
Disability and Society, 15, 389–409. doi:10.1080/713661959.
Gilmore, L. A., Campbell, J., & Cuskelly, M. (2003). Developmental expecta-
tions, personality stereotypes, and attitudes towards inclusive education:
12 K. Scior
Paterson, L., McKenzie, K., & Lindsay, B. (2012). Stigma, social comparison
and self-esteem in adults with an intellectual disability. Journal of Applied
Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 25, 166–176. doi:10.1111/j.1468-
3148.2011.00651.x.
Rapley, M. (2004). The social construction of intellectual disability. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Ryan, W. (1971). Blaming the victim. New York, NY: Pantheon Books.
Scior, K., Hamid, A., Hastings, R., Werner, S., Belton, C., Laniyan, A., et al.
(2015). Intellectual disabilities: Raising awareness and combating stigma—A
global review. London: University College London.
Szivos, S. E., & Griffiths, E. (1990). Group processes involved in coming to
terms with a mentally retarded identity. Mental Retardation, 6, 333–341.
Thompson, M. M., Zanna, M. P., & Griffin, D. W. (1995). Let’s not be indif-
ferent about (attitudinal) ambivalence. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.),
Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 361–386). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emo-
tion. Psychological Review, 92, 548–573. doi:10.1007/978-1-4612-4948-1_6.
Weiss, M. G. (2008). Stigma and the social burden of neglected tropical dis-
eases. PLoS Neglected Tropical Diseases, 2(5), e237. doi:10.1371/journal.
pntd.0000237.
Wolfensberger, W. (1981). The extermination of handicapped people in World
War II Germany. Mental Retardation, 19, 1–7.
World Health Organization (1994). International classification of diseases (10th
ed., ICD-10). Geneva: WHO.
World Health Organization & World Bank (2011). World report on disability.
Geneva: WHO.
2
Measurement Methods to Assess
Intellectual Disability Stigma
Shirli Werner
S. Werner ()
Paul Baerwald School of Social Work and Social Welfare,
Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel
e-mail: [email protected]
is closely linked to attitudes and stigma, this scale has been frequently
utilized to measure attitudes. Items were developed in consultation with
self-advocates and users of intellectual disability services.
The CLAS-ID includes 40 items (17 in the short version) scored for
degree of agreement on a 6-point Likert scale. It consists of four subscales:
(1) Empowerment—the view that persons with intellectual disabilities
should be able to make their opinions known in decisions and policies
that affect their lives; (2) Exclusion—the desire to segregate persons with
intellectual disabilities from community life; (3) Sheltering—the extent
to which one believes that individuals with intellectual disabilities need
to have others supervise them in their daily lives or protect them from
the dangers of community life; and (4) Similarity—the extent to which
one perceives persons with intellectual disabilities to be basically like
themselves and others regarding life goals and basic human rights. All
subscales show acceptable internal consistency (α = 0.75 to α = 0.86) and
acceptable test-retest reliability (α = 0.70 to α = 0.75), indicating that
they measure relatively stable attitudes (Henry et al. 1996).
The CLAS-ID has been widely used across populations, including pro-
fessionals working with individuals with intellectual disabilities, college,
university, and medical students, and the general public. It has been used
in Australia, Canada, Hong Kong, Israel, Japan, the Netherlands, the
UK, and the USA.
Attitudes toward Intellectual Disability Questionnaire (ATTID).
This scale, developed by Morin et al. (2013), adopts a multidimensional
perspective by measuring the cognitive, affective, and behavioral dimen-
sions of attitudes. The scale was developed based on previously validated
instruments, items inspired by the Montreal Declaration on Intellectual
Disability (Pan-American Health Organization and World Health
Organization 2004), and literature in the field. This scale takes into con-
sideration that attitudes may differ according to level of intellectual dis-
ability by using two vignettes that illustrate different levels of intellectual
disability.
The ATTID consists of 67 items rated for their degree of agreement
on a 5-point Likert scale. It consists of five subscales: two cognitive, two
affective, and one behavioral. Specifically, Knowledge of the capacity and
rights of persons with intellectual disabilities and Knowledge of the causes
2 Measurement Methods 19
The ID Stigma Scale has only been used, thus far, by the original
author. However, the scale is new and many studies worldwide have used
the Multidimensional Attitudes Scale (Findler et al. 2007) on which it
is based.
Single-Target Implicit Association Test (ST-IAT). An additional
method to overcome the limitations of explicit attitude measures is to use
measures that assess implicit attitudes. Implicit attitudes are automati-
cally activated without effort or intention (Prestwich et al. 2008), provid-
ing a more accurate reflection of attitudes (Wilson and Scior 2015). The
Implicit Association Test (IAT, Greenwald et al. 1998) is a computer-
based task that measures the relative strength of the association between
pairs of concepts/images and words. Participants are asked to categorize
the presented image/word into two groups, each related to a target con-
cept (e.g., White vs. Asian) and to an attribute concept (e.g., pleasant vs.
unpleasant) (Lane et al. 2007). The IAT has been found to be a valid and
reliable measure of implicit attitudes and is fairly robust against social
desirability (Cunningham et al. 2001).
In contrast to the traditional IAT, the ST-IAT (Karpinski and Steinman
2006) allows measurement of attitudes toward only one attitude object.
A ST-IAT version designed to measure implicit attitudes toward individ-
uals with intellectual disabilities has recently been developed by Wilson
and Scior (2015). In the ST-IAT, participants categorize two sets of attri-
bute category words (five ‘pleasant’ words: happiness, laughter, joyful,
rainbow, and sunshine and five ‘unpleasant’ words: sickness, hatred, dis-
ease, terrible, and poison) and five words representing the target category
of ‘intellectual disability’ (dependent, mental handicap, slow learner,
impaired, and special needs) using two keyboard keys. In the different
blocks, the attribute category words are paired with either pleasant or
negative attribute words.
Participants’ implicit attitudes are reflected in the difference in response
time to the different pairings. If participants are quicker in categorizing
words when ‘intellectual disability’ and ‘pleasant’ are paired, this indicates
positive implicit attitudes. Conversely, if they are quicker to categorize
words when ‘intellectual disability’ and ‘unpleasant’ are paired, this indi-
cates negative implicit attitudes. The authors found no significant associa-
tions between implicit and explicit attitudes (Wilson and Scior 2015).
2 Measurement Methods 21
Conclusions and Recommendations
The field of intellectual disability stigma measurement is still in its
infancy. Most available scales have some shortcomings and limitations,
both in terms of theory and methodology. Nevertheless, the few leading
2 Measurement Methods 23
• Scale development should focus on bridging the gap between the theo-
retical and the methodological levels by developing multidimensional
scales intended to measure the stigma construct, rather than by simply
measuring attitudes.
Accessible Summary
• It is important to accurately measure stigma before the professional
staff can offer a program to change it.
• The available measurement tools have some problems and
limitations.
• In this chapter, I describe some measurement scales that have fewer
problems.
• New scales need to measure stigma by examining all of its various
aspects.
References
Antonak, R. F., & Harth, R. (1994). Psychometric analysis and revision of the
mental retardation attitude inventory. Mental Retardation, 32, 272–280.
Bluemke, M., & Friese, M. (2008). Reliability and validity of the Single-Target
IAT (ST-IAT): Assessing automatic affect towards multiple attitude objects.
European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 977–997. doi:10.1002/ejsp.487.
Bogardus, E. S. (1959). Social distance. Los Angeles: University of Southern
California Press.
Bossaert, G., & Petry, K. (2013). Factorial validity of the Chedoke-McMaster
Attitudes towards Children with Handicaps Scale (CATCH). Research in
Developmental Disabilities, 34, 1336–1345. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2013.01.007.
Connolly, T., Williams, J., & Scior, K. (2013). The effects of symptom recognition
and diagnostic labels on public beliefs, emotional reactions and stigma associ-
ated with intellectual disability. American Journal on Intellectual and
Developmental Disabilities, 13, 211–223. doi:10.1352/1944-7558-118.3.211.
Cunningham, W. A., Preacher, K. J., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). Implicit attitude
measure: Consistency, stability and convergent validity. Psychological Science,
12, 163–170. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00328.
2 Measurement Methods 25
Wilson, M. C., & Scior, K. (2015). Implicit attitudes towards individuals with
intellectual disabilities: Their relationship with explicit attitudes, social distance,
emotions and contact (pp. 1–19). September: Plos One. doi:10.1371/journal.
pone.013790.
Woodmansee, J. J., & Cook, S. W. (1967). Dimensions of verbal racial atti-
tudes: Their identification and measurement. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 39, 848–860. doi:10.1037/h0025078.
Part II
The Consequences of Intellectual
Disability Stigma
3
How Stigma Affects the Lives of People
with Intellectual Disabilities:
An Overview
Nicole Ditchman, Kristin Kosyluk, Eun-Jeong Lee,
and Nev Jones
Poverty
Of an estimated 150 to 200 million people with intellectual disabilities
worldwide, 26 million live on less than $1 a day (Inclusion International
2006). The cycle of poverty and disability is caused and maintained in part
by stigma, denial of opportunities for economic and social development,
and reduced political engagement. Exclusion of people with intellectual
disabilities from the workforce, and the financial and social impact of
parents caring for children with intellectual disabilities in social systems
that do not provide adequate, and oftentimes any, support perpetuate
poverty (Emerson 2007). In developing countries, the stigma associated
with the birth of a child with a disability can lead fathers to abandon the
family, leaving mothers with the sole responsibility for care. Thus, the
largest unreimbursed cost associated with intellectual disabilities is that
of caregiving by family members (Inclusion International 2006).
Safety
Poverty and isolation in and of themselves increase risks to safety, and
people with disabilities are at greater risk of harassment, violence, and
abuse compared to individuals without disabilities. Individuals with
intellectual disabilities often experience abuse, ranging from physical
injury, sexual assault, emotional trauma, financial abuse, medication
mismanagement, and/or refusal to provide necessary personal assistance
by others in the community. Although some countries have created or
3 How Stigma Affects the Lives of People 33
Education
Stigma and structural discrimination have led children to be routinely
excluded from education or educated in segregated, and often lower-quality,
facilities. Globally, many children and youth with intellectual disabilities are
not in school due to barriers such as stigma, limited access to transporta-
tion, and prohibitive school fees (Inclusion International 2006; UNESCO
2015). Over 98 % of children with disabilities in developing countries do
not receive any formal education (United Nations 2007). Further, nega-
tive attitudes and lowered expectations by community members, teach-
ers, and peers can lead parents to remove children from school (Inclusion
International 2006). Even in countries where policies and laws mandate
inclusive education, implementation is often lacking. This is particularly
concerning given that studies show that children with intellectual disabili-
ties who are included in regular education are more likely to finish school,
work, and become active community members (Bach and Burke 2002).
Findings from the US National Longitudinal Transition Survey-2
(NLTS2), a large-scale study examining postsecondary outcomes for young
adults 1 month to 8 years post school, reveal that less than one-third of young
adults with intellectual disabilities were engaged in any kind of postsecondary
education post high school—the lowest among all the disability groups stud-
ied (Newman et al. 2011). These trends persist in spite of students and fami-
lies wanting access to higher education (Mock and Love 2012). Moreover,
attempts to fully participate in postsecondary education have been met with
resistance. For example, in the case of Fialka-Feldman v. Oakland University
Board of Trustees (2009), a student with an intellectual disability attending a
special program at Oakland University requested to live in campus housing.
After his request was denied by the university, he sued and the university was
ordered to allow him to move into on-campus housing.
Employment
Stigma has also resulted in the denial of work for people with intellec-
tual disabilities who continue to face very high rates of unemployment
and underemployment, despite the desire of many for employment in an
3 How Stigma Affects the Lives of People 35
open, competitive labor market. In the USA, estimates suggest that only
around 15 % of people with intellectual disabilities are working in paid,
community-based jobs (Anderson et al. 2011). In England, an even lower
proportion, 7 % of people with intellectual disabilities of working age are
in any form of employment (Hatton et al. 2014). Societal beliefs that the
majority of adults with intellectual disabilities are unemployable (Shaw
et al. 2004) or should only work in special workshops (Siperstein et al.
2003) foster this continued segregation. This is concerning given that
research findings suggest that sheltered work is associated with poorer
job satisfaction and well-being compared to competitive and supported
employment for people with intellectual disabilities (Anderson et al.
2011; Jahoda et al. 2008).
Furthermore, recent class action lawsuits in the USA (e.g., Lane v.
Brown) have challenged sheltered workshops that pay subminimum
wages and operate as segregated environments as a civil rights viola-
tion. On the other hand, there is also some evidence of more positive
public views toward the inclusion of people with intellectual disabilities
in employment (Burge et al. 2007). The authors suggested that struc-
tural stigma in the form of inadequate employment training programs
for people with intellectual disabilities represents a greater barrier than
public attitudes. Meanwhile, in the competitive workplace, research
demonstrates that employers appear to prefer people with physical or
sensory disabilities over individuals with intellectual disabilities (Kersh
2011). Discrimination in the workplace occurs as well, with over one-
fourth of the claims filed with the US Equal Employment Opportunity
Commission (EEOC) alleging discrimination at least in part on the basis
of disability—although it is unclear how many of these cases involve
individuals with intellectual disabilities (EEOC 2014).
some research has shown that those who physically live in the commu-
nity, particularly in smaller and more integrated group homes, experience
increased levels of community integration, especially when compared to
those living in large institutions (Kozma et al. 2009). However, mov-
ing persons with intellectual disabilities from institutional settings into
the community without addressing underlying societal and structural
barriers cannot ensure meaningful inclusion; it often results in physical
presence alone while potentially exposing those concerned to negative
community attitudes (Cummins and Lau 2003). Findings from a nation-
ally representative study in the USA of transition outcomes (NLTS2)
indicate that almost half of all young adults with intellectual disabilities
report no participation in organized community activities in the previous
year, and over one-third were not registered to vote, which was statisti-
cally lower than voter registration rates for most other disability groups
(Newman et al. 2011).
Failure to adequately address barriers to full participation in commu-
nity and civic life should be recognized as a reflection of multiple inter-
secting areas of structural stigma. For example, transportation is a major
barrier to community involvement and participation in leisure activi-
ties, as it is often unavailable or residential facilities are located far from
convenient public transportation hubs (Buttimer and Tierney 2005).
Furthermore, it is rare to find public information presented in a cogni-
tively accessible format, which poses a significant barrier to full inclusion
in social and civic life (Yalon-Chamovitz 2009). In addition, nowadays
a major digital divide separates many people with intellectual disabilities
from the rest of the population (McCarron et al. 2011). Inaccessible web-
sites, apps, and software, coupled with financial barriers affecting access
to Internet and technology, likely play a large role in limiting participa-
tion in social media and networking opportunities.
Health
Individuals with intellectual disabilities die at a younger age and experi-
ence poorer health than people without disabilities, mostly for reasons
that are avoidable and unjust (Emerson and Hatton 2014). A recent
3 How Stigma Affects the Lives of People 37
those with intellectual disabilities and the negative attitudes that char-
acterize such care are also well established (Rose et al. 2012; Werner and
Stawski 2012).
At a more structural level, negative attitudes affect health care by
reducing the number of medical professionals willing and/or able to
provide high-quality care to this patient group. Among other things,
improved training can help prevent diagnostic overshadowing, that is,
the attribution of health concerns or unusual behavior to the intellectual
disability diagnosis. Other barriers that at least in part reflect structural
stigma include scarcity of services, physical barriers to access, and fail-
ure to accommodate the needs of individuals with intellectual disabilities
with regard to literacy and communication (Emerson and Baines 2010).
Further, although there has been considerable effort dedicated to health
promotion interventions for individuals without disabilities, this has not
been the case for individuals with intellectual disabilities (Havercamp
and Scott 2015). Instead, they have generally poor knowledge of aspects
of health such as substance use, exercise, and healthy eating (Jobling
and Cuskelly 2006). As in other domains reviewed here, differences in
effort paid to health promotion between those with and without intel-
lectual disabilities can be related to more invisible aspects of stigma. That
is, stigma can be actualized in policy decisions that simply fail to fully
include or equally consider persons with intellectual disabilities.
Beliefs about the innocent and asexual nature of individuals with intellec-
tual disabilities have also put them at risk of sexual exploitation. In some
countries, a mix of a complete disregard for the humanity of persons with
intellectual disabilities and beliefs in their asexual nature can place girls
and women in particular at grave risk; for example, a belief that raping
a girl with a disability because she is presumed to be a virgin will cure
HIV/AIDS has been cited as a major challenge in some countries, such
as Zimbabwe (Inclusion International 2006).
Individuals with intellectual disabilities becoming parents is often
negatively viewed by health professionals, community members, and
families (Aunos and Feldman 2002). Several legal cases in the USA have
been brought against states that have removed children from their homes
for the sole reason of the parents having an intellectual disability. These
cases highlight concerns that child protection staff may lack the necessary
training to perform their duties without discriminating on the basis of
intellectual disability, as well as the need for meaningful assistance to sup-
port families with raising children. These issues are considered in greater
depth in Chap. 6 of this book.
Self-determination
A far reaching and common belief about people with intellectual disabili-
ties is that they lack the ability to make informed choices. This has led to
the limitation of choice and autonomy in decision-making. Paternalistic
attitudes and the infantilization of adults with intellectual disabilities
stop them from being allowed to take risks in their lives and have experi-
ences others take for granted. Reinforcing unwanted dependency on oth-
ers can increase social vulnerability and limit opportunities to engage in
self-determined behaviors and choices. Of note, youths with intellectual
disabilities who report higher perceived self-determination also report
better outcomes and higher quality of life (Wehmeyer and Palmer 2003).
The literature identifies attitudes of service professionals and their
assumptions that people with intellectual disabilities are unable to con-
sent to treatment or to make decisions on their own as a major bar-
rier to self-determination and decision-making (Davison et al. 2015).
40 N. Ditchman et al.
Future Directions
It is clear that people with intellectual disabilities face many inequalities
related to key life areas. Continued research efforts are needed to more
fully understand the impact of stigma across domains. This is not a sim-
ple task given the complexities inherent in the stigma process and the vast
heterogeneity of cultures and countries in which people with intellectual
disabilities live. We close with several suggestions for future research and
continued considerations in this area.
First, documenting the full scope and reach of stigma across differ-
ent settings is needed. Although stigma affects the lives of people with
intellectual disabilities regardless of country or socioeconomic level, it is
clear that individuals in lower income countries face some of the most
difficult living conditions in the world and yet little research is conducted
in these countries. Systemic discrimination and the absence of judicial
protection perpetuate poor living conditions and violations of human
rights. Additionally, reliable mechanisms for monitoring the well-being
of people with intellectual disabilities across the globe are necessary to
inform effective policy making (Fujiura et al. 2010). The role of cultural
contexts in addressing stigma must be taken into consideration as well.
For example, culture shapes how self-determination is understood, and
more family-oriented cultures may not necessarily support independent
decision-making by individuals, including those without disabilities (Lee
et al. 2015). A challenge to continued research in this area will be rec-
ognizing and accounting for the vast differences in policies, practices,
3 How Stigma Affects the Lives of People 41
Accessible Summary
• Negative attitudes can lead to poverty, health problems, threats to
safety, and limited access to education, employment, and community
life.
• People with intellectual disabilities are not usually given the support
necessary to fully participate in the community.
• Many assume that people with intellectual disabilities cannot make
their own decisions. This can lead to low expectations and lack of
opportunities to make their own choices.
• People with intellectual disabilities should be included in research on
stigma.
References
Anderson, L. L., Larson, S. A., & Wuorio, A. (2011). 2010 FINDS National
Survey technical report part 1: Family caregiver survey. Minneapolis, MN:
University of Minnesota, Research and Training Center on Community Living.
Appelbaum, P. S. (2009). Law & psychiatry: Mental retardation and the death
penalty: After Atkins. Psychiatric Services, 60, 1295–1297. doi:10.1176/
ps.32.1.14.
Aunos, M., & Feldman, M. (2002). Attitudes towards sexuality, sterilization and
parenting rights of persons with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Applied Research
in Intellectual Disabilities, 15, 285. doi:10.1046/j.1468-3148.2002.00135.x.
Bach, M., & Burke, M. A. (2002). Toward an inclusive approach to monitoring
investments and outcomes in child development and learning. North York,
Canada: Roeher Institute.
Burge, P., Ouellette-Kuntz, H., Isaacs, B., & Lunsky, Y. (2008). Medical stu-
dents’ views on training in intellectual disabilities. Canadian Family Physician,
54, 568–569.
Burge, P., Ouellette-Kuntz, H., & Lysaght, R. (2007). Public views on employ-
ment of people with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Vocational
Rehabilitation, 26, 29–37.
Buttimer, J., & Tierney, E. (2005). Patterns of leisure participation among ado-
lescents with a mild intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disabilities,
9, 25–42. doi:10.1177/1744629505049728.
44 N. Ditchman et al.
Jahoda, A., Kemp, J., Riddell, S., & Banks, P. (2008). Feelings about work: A
review of the socio-emotional impact of supported employment on people
with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual
Disabilities, 21, 1–18.
Jobling, A., & Cuskelly, M. (2006). Young people with Down syndrome: A
preliminary investigation of health knowledge and associated behaviours.
Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability, 31, 210–218.
doi:10.1080/13668250600999186.
Johnson, I., & Sigler, R. (2000). Forced sexual intercourse among intimates.
Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 15, 95–108.
Jurkowski, J. M. (2008). Photovoice as participatory action research tool for
engaging people with intellectual disabilities in research and program devel-
opment. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 46, 1–11.
doi:10.1352/0047-6765.
Kersh, J. (2011). Attitudes about people with intellectual disabilities: Current
status and new directions. International Review of Research in Developmental
Disabilities, 41, 199–231. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-386495-6.00006-0.
Kozma, A., Mansell, J., & Beadle-Brown, J. (2009). Outcomes in different resi-
dential settings for people with intellectual disability: A systematic review.
American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 114,
193–222. doi:10.1352/1944-7558-114.3.193.
Lee, E.-J., Lam, C., & Ditchman, N. (2015). Self-determination and cultural
considerations: An Asian perspective. In P. W. Corrigan (Ed.), Person-centered
care for mental illness: The evolution of adherence and self-determination
(pp. 211–234). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Lewis, S., & Stenfert Kroese, B. (2010). An investigation of nursing staff atti-
tudes and emotional reactions towards patients with intellectual disability in
a general hospital setting. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities,
23, 355–365. doi:10.1111/j.1468-3148.2009.00542.x.
Mai-Duc, C. (2014). 2 N.C. men wrongly convicted of murder freed after
decades in prison. Retrieved from www.latimes.com/nation/nationnow/la-na-
nn-death-row-inmates-released-mccollum-brown-20140903-story.html
McCarron, M., Swinburne, J., Burke, E., McGlinchey, E., Mulryan, N.,
Andrews, V., et al. (2011). Growing older with an intellectual disability in
Ireland 2011: First results from the intellectual disability supplement of the Irish
longitudinal study on ageing. Dublin, Ireland: School of Nursing & Midwifery,
Trinity College Dublin.
46 N. Ditchman et al.
McDonald, K. E., & Keys, C. B. (2008). How the powerful decide: Access to
research participation by those at the margins. American Journal of Community
Psychology, 42, 79–93. doi:10.1007/s10464-008-9192-x.
Mock, M., & Love, K. (2012). One state’s initiative to increase access to higher
education for people with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Policy and Practice
in Intellectual Disabilities, 9, 289–297. doi:10.1111/jppi.12006.
Newman, L., Wagner, M., Knokey, A.-M., Marder, C., Nagle, K., Shaver, D.,
et al. (2011). The post-high school outcomes of young adults with disabilities up
to 8 Years after high school. A report from the National Longitudinal Transition
Study-2 (NLTS2). Menlo Park, CA: SRI International.
Powers, L. E., & Oschwald, M. (2004). Violence and abuse against people with
disabilities: Experiences, barriers and prevention strategies. Portland, OR:
Oregon Institute on Disability and Development.
Rose, N., Rose, J., & Kent, S. (2012). Staff training in intellectual disability
services: A review of the literature and implications for mental health services
provided to individuals with intellectual disability. International Journal of
Developmental Disabilities, 58, 24–39.
Ryan, T. A., & Scior, K. (2014). Medical students’ attitudes towards people with
intellectual disabilities: A literature review. Research in Developmental
Disabilities, 35, 2316–2328.
Scotti, J. R., Slack, B. S., Bowman, R. A., & Morris, T. L. (1996). College stu-
dent attitudes concerning the sexuality of persons with mental retardation:
Development of the perceptions of sexuality scale. Sexuality and Disability,
14, 249–263. doi:10.1007/BF02590098.
Shaw, L., MacKinnon, J., McWilliam, C., & Sumsion, T. (2004). Consumer
participation in the employment rehabilitation process: Contextual factors
and implications for practice. Work, 23, 181–192.
Siebers, T. (2013). Disability and the theory of complex embodiment—For
identity politics in a new register. In L. Davis (Ed.), The Disability Studies
Reader (pp. 278–297). New York, NY: Routledge.
Siperstein, G. N., Norins, J., Corbin, S., & Shriver, T. (2003). Multinational
study of attitudes toward individuals with intellectual disabilities. Washington,
DC: Special Olympics, Inc..
U.S. Equal Employment Opportunity Commission (2014). Charge statistics FY
1997 through FY 2014. Retrieved from eeoc.gov/eeoc/statistics/enforcement/
charges.cfm
UNESCO (2015). Fixing the broken promise of education for all: Findings from
the global initiative on out-of-school children. Montreal, Canada: UNESCO
Institute for Statistics.
3 How Stigma Affects the Lives of People 47
United Nations (2007). From exclusion to equality: Realizing the rights of persons
with disabilities. Handbook for parliamentarians on the convention on the rights
of persons with disabilities and its optional protocol. Geneva, Switzerland:
United Nations. Retrieved from www.un.org/disabilities/documents/toolac-
tion/ipuhb.pdf
Verdonschot, M. M., de Witte, L. P., Reichrath, E., Buntinx, W. H., & Curfs,
L. M. (2009). Impact of environmental factors on community participation of
persons with an intellectual disability: A systematic review. Journal of Intellectual
Disability Research, 53, 54–64. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2008.01128.x.
Wehmeyer, M. L., & Palmer, S. B. (2003). Adult outcomes for students with
cognitive disabilities three years after high school: The impact of self-
determination. Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities, 38,
131–144.
Werner, S., & Stawski, M. (2012). Mental health: Knowledge, attitudes and
training of professionals on dual diagnosis of intellectual disability and psy-
chiatric disorder. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 56, 291–304.
doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2011.01429.x.
World Health Organization & World Bank (2011). World report on disability.
Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization.
Yalon-Chamovitz, S. (2009). Invisible access needs of people with intellectual
disabilities: A conceptual model of practice. Intellectual and Developmental
Disabilities, 47, 395–400. doi:10.1352/1934-9556-47.5.395.
Legislation
D. Roth (
) • H. Peretz
Beit Issie Shapiro, Ra’anana, Israel
e-mail: [email protected]
D. Barak
Beit Issie Shapiro and Elwyn, Ra’anana, Israel
Markova 2004). Distancing themselves from others who ascribe the label
of intellectual disability has negative implications in terms of sense of
belonging and social isolation (Ali et al. 2012; Cunningham and Glenn
2004; Spassiani and Friedman 2014).
A systematic review on self-stigma among people with intellectual dis-
abilities concluded that research should focus on the process by which
stigma associated with the intellectual disability label is internalized and
on the social and psychological factors associated with stigma (Ali et al.
2012). Accordingly, the study presented in this chapter sets out to inves-
tigate stigma and self-stigma as experienced by people with intellectual
disabilities, to examine their actions in “dealing” with stigma, and the
impact of participating in a self-advocacy group (SAG) on these percep-
tions of stigma and self-stigma.
and bad attitudes affect you?”; “How did self-advocacy change you?” The
questions which included the word “really” were asked by individuals
with intellectual disabilities and may imply that they perceived that some
of the responses provided were perhaps not genuine.
Two groups took part in this study. First, a national Israeli SAG, made up
of self-advocate leaders (or “guides” in Israeli SAG language) of seven dif-
ferent SAGs across Israel who have been meeting monthly for several years.
The group included 12 guides and 5 “enablers” (SAG cofacilitators without
intellectual disabilities). Second, a less well-established Tel-Aviv-based SAG
that meets fortnightly took part. This group included one guide, nine self-
advocates, and one enabler. In both groups there were a similar proportion
of men and women, who ranged in age from 20 to 60 years. The majority
of participants were Israeli Jews of different levels of religiosity and three
Israeli Arab participants. One two-hour meeting was held with each group.
The SAG members were familiar with the researchers, having met them on
several previous occasions and felt comfortable in their presence.
The group discussion was opened by the researchers and one of the
consulting committee members who outlined the purpose of the meeting.
The meetings were audio recorded and transcribed. Further, field notes
were taken by one of the researchers. Of note, rarely did focus group
participants respond to questions or initiate responses spontaneously—
most had to be directly asked and encouraged to participate. Further,
participants who had difficulty expressing themselves were provided with
support and adaptations necessary to help them participate, for example,
rephrasing questions, waiting for a response, and asking questions which
can be answered by yes/no responses.
The study was approved by Beit Issie Shapiro’s Ethics Committee. All
participants provided written consent from their parent or legal guard-
ians as (is still) required by law.
Results
The following five themes represent concerns and responses to stigma
raised most frequently by participants in both groups.
Emotional impact of stigma. Participants talked about being afraid
of stigma, feeling ridicule, anger, shame, rejection, and pain related to
4 How Stigma Affects Us: The Voice of Self-advocates 53
others’ reactions toward them. They described how their life experiences
of negative societal attitudes had “taught” them to expect to be ridiculed
and that people talked about them behind their backs. The statements
that follow exemplify this: “they make a circus [make fun] of me”; “I feel
exploited, they laugh at me”; “they think bad things of me”; “they laughed
at me and said, this one is retarded”; “someone says to you that you are
limited, it hurts, it pinched my heart”; “they think we are retarded.” These
statements clearly provide evidence of the negativity and pain these indi-
viduals experience, but it is unclear whether these stigmatic experiences
were accepted and internalized by them. Some participants appeared not
to internalize stigma but rather perceived it as unjust. They “swallowed”
the insult, but did not accept it.
Not understanding why stigma occurs and is directed toward us.
Many participants indicated that they did not understand why they
were being treated negatively: “I don’t know why they laugh at me …
we should be treated like everybody else…”; “I left my last job, they
said I was too slow, but I worked as fast as possible exactly like the other
worker.” Another participant said: “I do all the work there is to do, I work
harder than all the other workers, but they don’t let me be the coordina-
tor of the children in the kindergarten, despite the fact that I do things
none of the other caregivers do.” Some of the other participants said: “I
am regular like everyone else”; “they think we are retarded and not nor-
mal”; “she said that I am a person with special needs, so I thought, what,
I am retarded? So I asked her, and she said no.” These findings can be
explained in different ways. Firstly, it is possible that participants do not
internalize the stigma associated with intellectual disability. Thus, they
did not understand why they were being ridiculed, seen as unable to do
what “normal” people do, or treated differently from those around them.
This was reflected in answers to the question “why do you think you are
treated like that”, a question many participants were unable to answer.
Instead, they elaborated on how they were treated and not why they were
treated negatively. A second possible explanation is that no one had truly
explained to them what it means to have an intellectual disability and its
potential impact on them and their lives. It is possible that saying out
loud “I have an intellectual disability” is too difficult, as society considers
this term as derogatory. Yet, when listening closely to the participants,
54 D. Roth et al.
it was clear that there was a strong dissonance between their expressed
denial of being different and their unspoken sense of being different and
somehow deficient. This can be seen in statements made by participants
when asked more directly: “we are different in our brain level”; “we can-
not function alone”; “I work slower.”
Confusion in self-concept and identity. Participants referred to
themselves as having many titles and definitions: “special needs”, “handi-
capped”, “retarded”, and “intellectually disabled”. When asked how they
would like their disability to be referred to, they suggested, “limited, this
means that our brain is limited”; “intellectually disabled, this sounds
nicer than limited or disabled”; “special needs because this will cause less
people to laugh at me”; “mild retardation, so they understand that we
have limitations in everything”; “a regular person, a person that does not
have a problem”.
Most of the participants felt very uncomfortable, especially in the
national SAG, to address the issue of their identity related to being a
person with an intellectual disability. They appeared more comfortable
focusing on additional physical or sensory impairments that affected
some of them. For example, in one of the groups there was a lively
discussion about one of the participant’s visual impairment, which
the group was clearly more comfortable to discuss than intellectual
disability.
Ignoring as a default response to stigma. Most participants described
attempting to deal with the insults they faced by trying, in their words,
“to ignore” them or avoid contact with people who had insulted them.
Nevertheless, it seems that their attempts were not always fruitful. The
behavioral response of ignoring carried with it an emotional cost. When
asked how they responded to situations in which they were exposed to
stigma, they said they were silent, looked down, and did not confront
the offending person. Many shared situations in which they had cho-
sen not to confront others: “They laughed at me, said I was limited,
retarded, that I am a retarded one, ‘that one, she has no brain’. I did
not respond, I did not react, and I felt very bad.” “If they laugh at me,
I ignore it”; “if we will respond to them, they will burst at us”; “they
will not listen to me … ignore … ignore …” “They told me ‘you are
from the retarded organization, you are limited’. I don’t answer, I don’t
4 How Stigma Affects Us: The Voice of Self-advocates 55
respond.” (Researcher asked: “How did you feel?”) “I feel sad. I cannot
say anything.”
Participants appeared hesitant to confront the people who hurt them
because they feared they might encounter an even more negative reac-
tion. By ignoring the situation they felt safer at the price of reinforcing
negative reactions. Ignoring was a skill they were taught by their envi-
ronment and those caring for them, who frequently gave them advice
such as “ignore it, it is not true”. While potentially well meaning, such
advice encourages the person neither to stand up for themselves, nor to
deal with their intellectual disability, and instead could be seen as being
complicit in failing to challenge stigma.
The opportunity provided by self-advocacy. Participating in SAGs
clearly provided participants with an alternative, an opportunity to be less
fearful, talk, explain, and stand up for their rights, not ignore. The SAGs
had an optimistic atmosphere where learning about self-advocacy provided
a new and different way of being in the community. They were not focused
on erasing or ignoring the disability but rather on increasing group mem-
bers’ understanding that having a disability and being different from others
does not justify disrespect or hostility. Taking part in the SAG strengthened
their sense of personal control: “I am responsible for myself ”; “I am aware
of my difficulties, but I am equal”; “I can explain what is difficult for me,
but I still deserve fair treatment.” Other statements illustrating this theme
included: “I will talk to people so that they can listen to how people like us
feel”; “SAG does good for me … gives me hope, I can stand on my own
… if I see that things are not right I can say something”; “I can say in a
nice polite way that they should treat us nicely and equally”; “I feel that I
am doing something for myself and not for someone else”; “the group gives
me the power, I am not afraid to tell people to treat me with a little more
respect. Many times I was scared because I was treated like a child and
today I am not afraid to say it”; “to know how to approach normal people
… I also have the right to things.” Other participants provided additional
examples of how membership of a SAG helped them stand up for them-
selves: “If people in the community do not treat you nicely, argue with
them and stand on your own in all cases, because people change their opin-
ions.” Or as another SAG member said: “You must stand on your own, if
you do not stand on your own, you will not get what you want.”
56 D. Roth et al.
Discussion and Conclusions
The findings of this study are consistent with previous studies in indicat-
ing that some individuals with intellectual disabilities do not identify
with having an intellectual disability and prefer to refer to themselves
as having other forms of disability. Further, individuals with intellectual
disabilities who participated in this study described various difficulties
that they faced due to holding a stigmatized status and being treated
badly by others (Ali et al. 2012; Cunningham et al. 2000; Davies 1998;
Davies and Jenkins 1997; Jahoda and Markova 2004). Accordingly, some
individuals with intellectual disabilities feel frustrated about the label of
“retardation” or “intellectual disability” ascribed to them (Dagnan and
Waring 2004). They may respond by distancing themselves from other
individuals with intellectual disabilities and ignoring and avoiding situa-
tions which may elicit stigmatic responses toward them, such as negative
remarks and insults (Gibbons 1985; Jahoda and Markova 2004).
Self-stigma of individuals with intellectual disabilities is a very
complex construct to measure. One question which remained unan-
swered in the current study is in what ways does self-stigma differ
from public stigma? Is self-stigma a product of public stigma, that is,
do all or most individuals who are prone to public stigma internalize
this? On the one hand, the participants’ descriptions of their behavior
reflects that many internalized a sense of themselves as “inferior to
others”, along with a strong fear of society, and a tendency to avoid
contact with those that impose stigma. On the other hand, partici-
pants had great difficulties saying “I have an intellectual disability.”
It seems that many participants had a sense that they “do not deserve
4 How Stigma Affects Us: The Voice of Self-advocates 57
such treatment”, but did not understand why they are treated poorly
or what to do about it.
The group discussions made it clear that ignoring, a strategy frequently
chosen by participants, ultimately served to reinforce stigma. In contrast,
self-advocacy provided a collective sense of strength and injustice. In lis-
tening to their voices, ideas, and experiences, it is clear that there is a
need to support more self-advocacy, convince others of its importance
and benefits, and support the development of different formats of self-
advocacy, as regular discussion-based groups may not suit everyone. It is
necessary to convince families, service providers, and policy makers of the
importance and the necessity of self-advocacy and to encourage diverse
forms of self-advocacy from a younger age as a possible method to miti-
gate the negative impact of stigma. Effort should be directed to examine
how to promote the self-esteem of individuals with intellectual disabili-
ties and provide them with skills to help them cope with their realities,
alongside informing and educating communities at large to reduce and
eliminate stigmatic beliefs.
Accessible Summary
• We asked people with intellectual disabilities what other people with-
out disabilities think about them.
• People with intellectual disabilities felt that others laugh at them. This
made them feel bad about themselves. They didn’t understand why
others treat them this way. Many preferred to ignore situations in
which people made fun of them or hurt them.
• Taking part in SAGs, they learned to speak for themselves and say
what they want and feel. In these groups they are able to learn how to
talk back and not let others treat them badly.
• People with intellectual disabilities should learn how to speak up for
themselves and feel more confident. It is also very important to teach
people without disabilities that people with intellectual disabilities are
people just like them.
References
Ali, A., Hassiotis, A., Strydom, A., & King, M. (2012). Self stigma in people
with intellectual disabilities and courtesy stigma in family carers: A system-
atic review. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33, 2122–2140.
doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2012.06.013.
Beart, S., Hardy, G., & Buchan, L. (2005). How people with intellectual disabilities
view their social identity: A review of the literature. Journal of Applied Research in
Intellectual Disabilities, 18, 47–56. doi:10.1111/j.1468-3148.2004.00218.
Corrigan, P. W., & Watson, A. C. (2002). The paradox of self-stigma and mental
illness. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 9, 35–53. doi:10.1093/
clipsy.9.1.35.
4 How Stigma Affects Us: The Voice of Self-advocates 59
Cunningham, C. C., Glenn, S., & Fitzpatrick, H. (2000). Parents telling their
offspring about Down syndrome and disability. Journal of Applied Research in
Intellectual Disabilities, 13, 47–61. doi:10.1046/j.1468-3148.2000.00012.x.
Cunningham, C., & Glenn, S. (2004). Self-awareness in young adults with
Down syndrome: Awareness of Down syndrome and disability. International
Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 51, 335–361. doi:10.1080
/1034912042000295017.
Dagnan, D., & Waring, M. (2004). Linking stigma to psychological distress:
Testing a social–cognitive model of the experience of people with intellectual
disabilities. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 11, 247–254. doi:10.1002/
cpp.413.
Davies, C. A. (1998). Constructing other selves: Competence and the category
of learning difficulties. In R. Jenkins (Ed.), Questions of competence: Culture,
classification and intellectual disability (pp. 102–124). Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Davies, C. A., & Jenkins, R. (1997). ‘She has different fits to me’: How people
with learning difficulties see themselves. Disability and Society, 12, 95–110.
doi:10.1080/09687599727498.
Finlay, M., & Lyons, E. (1998). Social identity and people with learning diffi-
culties: Implications for self-advocacy groups. Disability and Society, 13,
37–51. doi:10.1080/09687599826902.
Gibbons, F. X. (1985). Stigma perception: Social comparison among mentally
retarded persons. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 90, 98–106.
Jahoda, A., & Markova, I. (2004). Coping with social stigma: People with intellec-
tual disabilities moving from institutions and family home. Journal of Intellectual
Disability Research, 48, 719–729. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2003.00561.x.
Lucksted, A., & Drapalski, A. L. (2015). Self-stigma regarding mental illness:
Definition, impact, and relationship to societal stigma. Psychiatric
Rehabilitation Journal, 38, 99–102. doi:10.1037/prj0000152.
Spassiani, N. A., & Friedman, C. (2014). Stigma: Barriers to culture and iden-
tity for people with intellectual disability. Inclusion, 2, 329–341.
doi:10.1352/2326-6988-2.4.329.
5
Rarely Seen, Seldom Heard: People
with Intellectual Disabilities in the Mass
Media
Rebecca Renwick
R. Renwick (
)
Department of Occupational Science and Occupational Therapy,
University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
e-mail: [email protected]
television, and film, which have the potential to reflect the voices of people
with intellectual disabilities, but rarely do. It discusses the limited literature
about people with intellectual disabilities in mass media to consider how
these media have contributed to reproducing and counteracting stigma.
Because this literature is sparse, relevant literature on other disabilities, such
as physical and developmental disabilities, including autism, is included
to help illuminate the context of media representations. The chapter con-
cludes with a discussion of major gaps in the literature, future directions,
and implications.
Newspapers
Recent research on newspaper representations of intellectual disability
is scarce. One study of UK print media representations of people with
developmental disabilities, including intellectual disabilities, compared
articles published between 1983 and 2001 in The Guardian, a national
newspaper (Wilkinson and McGill 2009). The number of articles more
than doubled over that period, yet they continued to overrepresent chil-
dren with autism. In 2001, articles were mainly about children with
autism and Down syndrome while articles about adults typically referred
to developmental disabilities generally rather than any specific condi-
tion. Articles from 1983 rarely distinguished specific types of develop-
mental disabilities for any age group. In 2001, coverage reflected greater
use of people first language, but continued to link developmental dis-
abilities with other devalued groups, such as people with mental illness.
Increasing differentiation among people with developmental disabilities,
with greater attention to disabilities such as autism and Down syndrome
and much less to individuals with more severe and complex needs, also
featured.
A study of all major Taiwanese newspapers published in 2008 exam-
ined representations of people with intellectual disabilities (Chen et al.
2012). Most of the 355 articles identified appeared in local (77 %) rather
than nationally circulated papers. Three key themes were identified in
depictions of intellectual disabilities through the use of content analysis.
66 R. Renwick
The first theme, dispirited images (45 % of articles), was consistent with
social deviance models of disability (e.g., as victims of exploitation, lack-
ing skills, and experiencing suffering or loss due to their disability). The
second theme, needy images (33 % of articles), was congruent with a char-
ity model of disability (e.g., emphasizing the need for professional sup-
ports, services, and interventions). The final theme, affirmative images
(23 % of articles), was associated with a civil rights model of disability.
These articles focused on ‘normalcy’ describing people with intellectual
disabilities as doing things or having abilities characteristic of the general
public or featured people with intellectual disabilities with supportive,
caring families. The researchers concluded that, collectively, these news-
paper portrayals emphasized the deficiency of people with intellectual
disabilities and thus may support their social exclusion.
Another study examined and compared portrayals of individuals
with intellectual disabilities in several mass media, including articles in
newspapers published in four major American cities between 1995 and
2004 (Special Olympics 2005). Findings indicated that these individu-
als were presented as less competent (in reading, doing math, acting in
a socially appropriate manner) in print than in television programs or
films. Further, articles most frequently presented people with intellectual
disabilities engaged in a narrow range of common activities related to
sports, work, or school.
Television
Research on television portrayals of people with intellectual disabilities
is rare. One study examined footage of first-time domestic coverage on
British television of national Special Olympics games in 2009 (Carter
and Williams 2012). It revealed ‘relentless positivity’ in the language and
tone of the broadcasts, using exaggerated descriptors such as ‘amazing’,
‘fantastic’, ‘inspiring’, and ‘incredible’. Positivity in the tone was evident
in scenes featuring athletes and others smiling and waving very enthusi-
astically and in portraying social aspects of the games rather than their
status as a competitive athletics event. Athletes were routinely and repeat-
edly referred to as ‘them’ and represented as different, with emphasis on
5 People with Intellectual Disabilities in the Mass Media 67
Film
There is no research on films focused only on people with intellectual
disabilities. Two studies examined Hollywood-style films presenting fic-
tional portrayals of people with either developmental or intellectual dis-
abilities. These films were released between 1968 and 2009, distributed
widely to commercial movie theaters, and remain available on DVD or
online.
Renwick et al. (2014) qualitatively analyzed portrayals of engagement
in everyday and meaningful activities by adults with developmental dis-
abilities in eight films (1999–2009). These films depicted mainly leisure
(e.g., socializing, watching movies) and productive (e.g., volunteer or
paid work, taking educational courses) activities which were typically
68 R. Renwick
safe and pleasurable, but not very challenging. Characters had restricted
choices for participation in activities they found meaningful. Activities
usually done by adults were performed in simplified ways and/or in a
manner more characteristic of children and/or the activities were usually
performed by children. These films conveyed many powerful, negative
messages about what constitutes acceptable activities and social roles for
people with intellectual and developmental disabilities and how they usu-
ally perform them. Nevertheless, closer examination revealed some other
more complex portrayals, such as struggles with personal problems (alco-
holism, job loss, death of a loved one) and ongoing challenges of dealing
with a stigmatizing society. However, these were revealed only with care-
ful, repeated viewing. Therefore, they would likely be missed by viewers
watching the films for entertainment.
In an examination of nine films (1968–1997), Devlieger et al. (2000)
found stereotypic portrayals of developmental disabilities, such as some-
thing to be hidden and a tragedy or burden. However, a few films included
more complex representations, for instance, a person with an intellectual
disability who was initially dependent on others but forged more reciprocal
relationships with them and eventually learned to live independently. The
study also found that few films featured voices of persons with intellectual
disabilities as narrators, so that they are more often spoken for or about.
Discussion
Several recurring themes were identified across the literature examined.
One is that these media consistently underrepresent people with intellec-
tual disabilities. Another is their simplistic portrayals in all three media
with frequently repeated stereotypes depicting childlike innocents who
lack capabilities and are vulnerable, needy, passive, a burden to others,
dangerous, and problematic. In addition, disability is the major focus
such that individuality, complexity, nuance, and multiple social roles
are not captured by these portrayals. Finally, negative and stigmatizing
mass media messages are repeatedly emphasized, typically outnumbering
and outweighing more acceptable ones. In general, such portrayals rein-
force and cultivate stigmatizing beliefs, attitudes, and perspectives held
5 People with Intellectual Disabilities in the Mass Media 69
Implications for Practice
Service professionals are exposed to the same media representations of
intellectual disabilities as others in society and may also be influenced
by them. Government policies regulating service delivery, practices, and
5 People with Intellectual Disabilities in the Mass Media 71
Implications for Advocacy
Advocacy strategies are needed to counteract stigmatizing representations
and change the nature and quality of media portrayals. These could take
the form of accessible and novel knowledge translation strategies and for-
mats that could support self-advocates’ efforts, such as short films and
trailers that might be shown as a free community service on television,
feature articles for mainstream and community newspapers, and free
advocacy ads in community newspapers.
Education aimed at children may be especially valuable in counteract-
ing and changing perceptions about people with intellectual disabilities.
Educational vehicles such as short films for classroom viewing or short
videos on YouTube featuring, and co-created by, people with intellectual
disabilities could be used to stimulate discussion among younger stu-
dents. A continuing emphasis on education implemented by advocates
and self-advocates for older students in elementary and high schools
could reinforce and promote media literacy (Englandkennedy 2008),
stigma reduction, and acceptance of people with intellectual disabilities.
Given the power and pervasiveness of mass media and the representa-
tions they disseminate to broad audiences, changing the content of media
portrayals will require considerable persistence and targeted strategizing.
72 R. Renwick
Advocacy could take several forms, for instance, direct efforts to inform
and educate newspaper editors and journalists (Jones and Harwood 2009),
television executives and producers, and film makers (Hartnett 2000). Such
advocacy should focus on potential harm done by stigmatizing portray-
als that reproduce and perpetuate stereotypes, what constitute acceptable
portrayals, and strategies for constructing more and better representations.
Other strategies include employing more actors with intellectual disabili-
ties to play such characters, and featuring characters with intellectual dis-
abilities as narrators, instead of being spoken for and about. Creating more
television programs and films focused on individuals with intellectual dis-
abilities and having these individuals visible in many more programs and
films in supporting roles and in the background could help normalize their
natural presence in media and society. Newspapers could also feature more
interviews that include these individuals themselves (with appropriate sup-
ports) such that their voices are reflected more often. All three mass media
could benefit considerably from ongoing consultation with and feedback
from self-advocates with intellectual disabilities, advocates, and their sup-
porting organizations concerning construction and communication of
more acceptable, nuanced, and inclusive representations.
Accessible Summary
• Stories that newspapers, television, and movies tell about people with
intellectual disabilities often look mostly at the disability.
• These stories often give wrong information about what people with
intellectual disabilities and their lives are really like.
• Stories about people with intellectual disabilities are getting a bit bet-
ter but need to get much better.
• These stories can change what other people think and feel about peo-
ple with intellectual disabilities, and how people with intellectual dis-
abilities feel and think about themselves.
• We can work together to change the bad effects that newspapers, tele-
vision, and movies have on people with intellectual disabilities.
References
Carter, N., & Williams, J. (2012). ‘A genuinely emotional week’: Learning dis-
ability, sport, and television—Notes on the Special Olympics GB National
Summer Games 2009. Media, Culture and Society, 34, 211–227.
doi:10.1177/0163443711430759.
Chen, C. H., Hsu, K. L., Shu, B. C., & Fertzer, S. (2012). The image of people
with intellectual disability in Taiwan newspapers. Journal of Intellectual and
Developmental Disability, 17, 35–41. doi:10.3109/13668250.2011.650159.
Conn, R., & Bhugra, D. (2015). The portrayal of autism in Hollywood films.
International Journal of Culture and Mental Health, 5, 54–62. doi:10.1080/1
7542863.2011.553369.
Devlieger, P. J., Baz, T., & Drazen, C. (2000). Mental retardation in American
film: A semiotic analysis. Semiotica, 129, 1–28. doi:10.1515/semi.
2000.129.1-4.1.
Devotta, K., Wilton, R., & Yiannakoulias, N. (2013). Representations of dis-
ability in the Canadian news media: A decade of change? Disability and
Rehabilitation, 35, 1859–1868. doi:10.3109/09638288.2012.760658.
Dyer, R. (1993). The matter of images: Essays on representation. London, UK:
Routledge.
Englandkennedy, E. (2008). Media representations of attention deficit disorder:
Portrayals of cultural skepticism in popular media. Journal of Popular Culture,
41, 91–117. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5931.2008.00494.x.
Farnall, O., & Smith, K. A. (1999). Reactions to people with disabilities: Personal
contact versus viewing of specific media portrayals. Journalism and Mass
Communication Quarterly, 76, 659–672. doi:10.1177/107769909907600404.
Fudge Schormans, A., Renwick, R., Barker, D., Chasi, E., Smith, B., McWilliam,
L., et al. (2013). Why can’t we be superheroes? Researchers’ with and without
intellectual and developmental disabilities thoughts on Defendor. Media
Review. Journal on Developmental Disabilities, 19, 109–112.
Garland-Thomson, R. (2009). Staring: How we look. New York, NY: Oxford
University Press.
Hall, S. (1980). Encoding/decoding. In S. Hall, D. Hobson, A. Lowe, &
P. Willis (Eds.), Culture, media, language (pp. 128–138). London, UK:
Hutchinson & Co..
Hartnett, A. (2000). Escaping the ‘evil avenger’ and ‘the supercrip’: Images of
disability in popular television. The Irish Communication Review, 8, 21–29.
5 People with Intellectual Disabilities in the Mass Media 75
play a more significant role in these cases at the expense of assessing the
family’s social and economic environment in order to determine their
support needs. We conclude that intellectual disability stigma is very
much alive and evident in contemporary child protection work.
Governing Sexuality
It has been argued that in the early twentieth-century European and
American contexts, the tactics of governing the sexuality of people with
intellectual disabilities, as well as the poor and in some cases immigrants
and racial minorities, were rooted in the trans-Atlantic eugenic project
of preventing the so-called undesirable members of the population from
reproducing (Davis 2010; Mitchell and Snyder 2003; Stubblefield 2007).
Mitchell and Snyder (2003) argued that the Nazi German regime during
the Second World War only represented the most extreme application of
existing eugenic ideologies. These ideas originated during an earlier cross-
Atlantic collaboration between doctors, scholars, practitioners, policy
makers, and eugenic societies and organizations which sought to prevent
the birth of persons with traits perceived negatively (e.g., physical, intel-
lectual, and sensory impairments, along with mental health problems)
and encourage the birth of people with traits perceived positively and as
beneficial to society.
Nazi German physicians and policy makers were influenced by the
existing practices of sterilization and institutional segregation of people
with disabilities that were already in place in Western Europe, the Nordic
countries, and the USA and Canada (Mitchell and Snyder 2003). While
these societies did not engage in murder as the solution to the ‘problem’
of populations deemed deficient, as did the Nazis, their responses, such
as institutionalization (with the sexes firmly segregated) and involuntary
sterilization, were nevertheless still very harsh. All these practices shared
a common scientific and cultural language predicated on stigmatic views
of people with disabilities, and people with intellectual disabilities in par-
ticular (Diekema 2003; Stefánsdóttir 2014).
While practices which seek to govern the sexuality and reproductive
capabilities of people with intellectual disabilities continue in various
6 Intellectual Disability and Parental Custody Rights 79
prima facie evidence of risk of harm to the child and in some states is
legitimized by law. Another is that parents with intellectual disabilities
are unfit parents a priori regardless of whatever interventions are under-
taken (Llewellyn et al. 2010). The conditions under which custody is
removed from parents with intellectual disabilities are illustrated in this
chapter with evidence from the Icelandic context.
with all of the expected caveats; for example, persons with intellectual
disabilities were to live with their own families under circumstances ‘as
close as possible to those of normal life’. The declaration appeared to refer
to the family in a guardianship role and no mention was made of the
right (or expectation) of persons with intellectual disabilities to poten-
tially form their own families and raise children. As such it was consistent
with the notion that people with intellectual disabilities are simultane-
ously asexual and hypersexual, childlike, and in need of constant care and
supervision, but never care providers or parents in their own right, which
is long-standing, persistent, and exists in different national-cultural con-
texts over significant periods of time (Priestley 2003; Simpson 2011;
Stefánsdóttir and Traustadóttir 2015).
A major piece of legislation concerning people with intellectual dis-
abilities in Iceland was the ‘Law on Assistance to the Retarded’ (nr.
47/1979). In some ways this law reflected the ideology of normalization
as seen in the 1971 UN declaration referred to above. The first article
of this law sought to ensure for the ‘mentally retarded’ (Is. þroskaheftir)
equality with other citizens and to create conditions so they could live
as normal a life as possible in the community. While this may seem pro-
gressive for the time, the rest of the text of the law discussed services
that were to be provided in a variety of institutional settings and the
home environment was generally envisioned to be small group homes
(Is. sambýli), an improvement over large institutions but hardly a nor-
mative living arrangement compared with the general public, and one
that often affords little privacy. The idea that people with intellectual
disabilities might form intimate relationships, let alone become parents,
and that such choices might be reflected in their living arrangements did
not appear to be a consideration.
A key piece of modern legislation concerning people with disabili-
ties in Iceland is the 1992 Act on the Affairs of Disabled People (nr.
59/1992, revised in 2010). Within this Act, people with intellectual dis-
abilities are included along with people with other types of disabilities.
The language is less ambivalent (‘create conditions in which they are able
to live a normal life’), and the Act includes a focus on human rights.
However, the Act is silent on issues of sexuality or parenthood, and chil-
dren are referred to in terms of the rights of children with disabilities,
82 H.B. Sigurjónsdóttir and J.G. Rice
was posited to be ‘below average’. It has been suggested that any use of
alcohol or drugs, even mild or recreational, by someone with intellec-
tual disabilities is perceived as problematic and as potentially a sign of
addiction (Simpson 2012). Others have noted that the issue is a lack of
access to treatment for addiction, if such is needed, not the inability to be
treated (Slayter 2010); and if treatment fails, then the treatment methods
need to be revised in conjunction with the service users to be effective
(Taggart et al. 2007).
In another case, one weekend of alcohol misuse by the parents while they
were being investigated by child protection services (CPS) was drawn upon
repeatedly for the duration of the case as indicative of a pattern of alcohol
abuse, when this specific weekend was the only reference we could find in
the court documents to any alcohol use on the part of either parent. This was
despite the fact that at one point the couple was under five and later seven
day-a-week surveillance by CPS. This is typical, Simpson (2012) contends,
of evidence of “an intrinsically pathologized and discrete pattern alcohol
consumption being posited for adults with intellectual disabilities” (p. 186).
In other words, the label of intellectual disability of itself colors and informs
and perhaps distorts the interpretation of such behavior, including as well
the perceived links between intellectual disability and poor parenting.
In the cases we examined, reference to the intellectual disability label
was often made without an explicit connection to allegations of parental
abuse or neglect. This mirrored patterns in other cases where disability
was a factor. It was common to see references to parent(s) spending time
under the care of psychiatric services, in which even voluntary attempts
to seek treatment were consistently used as evidence to present parents
in a negative light. Parents were often referred to as ‘disability pensioners’
(Is. öryrkjar), a heavily stigmatized label in Iceland (Rice 2010), with low
disability pension rates used as evidence of the parents not being able to
adequately support their children. In one case, a mother was referred to as
having a history of epilepsy, without any explicit connection being made
between epilepsy and parenting, as the case mainly focused on children
neglected due to the mother working night shifts. As McConnell and
Llewellyn (2000) note, in this type of situation, “intellectual disability
per se, is treated as prima facie evidence of parental inadequacy” (p. 886).
Tymchuk and Andron (1990) argue that while some intellectual capacity
6 Intellectual Disability and Parental Custody Rights 85
The final justification for permanent custody removal was based primarily
upon the potential risks the parents’ impairments represented to their
children. The main legal justification was Article 29, Section d of the
Child Protection Act (nr. 80/2002), which provides that parents may be
deprived of custody if the CPS believes ‘that it is certain that the child’s
physical or mental health or his/her maturity is at risk because the parents
are clearly unfit to have custody, due, for instance, to drug use, mental
instability, or low intelligence, or that the behavior of the parents is likely
to cause the child serious harm’.
Conclusions
We have argued that child protection is a necessary albeit difficult task
and one fraught with difficulties in the case of parents with intellectual
disabilities. One problem therein is that as our reading of these cases and
the history of Icelandic legislation suggests—enhanced with a reading of
the history of similar developments in Nordic countries and the trans-
Atlantic region in general—that people with intellectual disabilities have
long been viewed through the prism of stigmatic and prejudiced beliefs,
rooted in older eugenic concerns and anxieties about their reproductive
potential, and which in certain ways have been transposed to the area of
child protection and custody. The role of stigma as an influence on the
treatment of people with intellectual disabilities in these processes needs
to be considered and critically reflected upon.
Accessible Summary
• Parents with intellectual disabilities are commonly believed to be unfit
to raise children.
• Such beliefs are based on prejudice about people with intellectual
disabilities.
• Prejudice can lead to children being removed from their parents.
• What others believe does not tell us how things are—it only tells us
how they imagine things to be.
References
Aunos, M., & Feldman, M. (2008). There’s no place like home: The child’s right
to family. In T. O’Neill & D. Zinga (Eds.), Children’s rights: Multidisciplinary
approaches to participation and protection (pp. 137–162). Toronto, Canada:
University of Toronto press.
Davis, L. J. (2010). Constructing normalcy. In L. J. Davis (Ed.), The disability
studies reader (5th ed., pp. 3–19). New York, NY: Routledge.
Diekema, D. S. (2003). Involuntary sterilization of persons with mental retarda-
tion: An ethical analysis. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities,
9(1), 21–26. doi:10.1002/mrdd.10053.
88 H.B. Sigurjónsdóttir and J.G. Rice
Series, L. (2015). Mental capacity and the control of sexuality of people with
intellectual disabilities in England and Wales. In T. Shakespeare (Ed.),
Disability research today: International perspectives (pp. 149–165). London,
UK: Routledge.
Simpson, M. (2012). Alcohol and intellectual disability: Personal problem or
cultural exclusion? Journal of Intellectual Disabilities, 16(3), 183–192.
doi:10.1177/1744629512455595.
Simpson, M. K. (2011). Othering intellectual disability: Two models of classifi-
cation from the 19th century. Theory and Psychology, 22(5), 541–555.
doi:10.1177/0959354310378375.
Slayter, E. M. (2010). Disparities in access to substance abuse treatment among
people with intellectual disabilities and serious mental illness. Health and
Social Work, 35(1), 49–59. doi:10.1093/hsw/35.1.49.
Stefánsdóttir, G. V. (2014). Sterilisation and women with intellectual disability
in Iceland. Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability, 39(2),
188–197. doi:10.3109/13668250.2014.899327.
Stefánsdóttir, G. V., & Traustadóttir, R. (2015). Life histories as counter-
narratives against dominant and negative stereotypes about people with intel-
lectual disabilities. Disability and Society, 30, 368–380. doi:10.1080/096875
99.2015.1024827.
Stubblefield, A. (2007). “Beyond the pale”: Tainted whiteness, cognitive disability,
andeugenicsterilization.Hypatia,22(2),162–181.doi:10.1111/j.15272001.2007.
tb009-87.x.
Taggart, L., McLaughlin, D., Quinn, B., & McFarlane, C. (2007). Listening to
people with intellectual disabilities who misuse alcohol and drugs. Health
and Social Care in the Community, 15, 360–368.
Tøssebro, J., Bonfils, I. S., Teittinen, A., Tideman, M., Traustadóttir, R., &
Vesala, H. T. (2012). Normalization fifty years beyond—Current trends in
the Nordic Countries. Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual Disabilities,
9(2), 134–146. doi:10.1111/j.1741-1130.2012.00340.x.
Tymchuk, A. J., & Andron, L. (1990). Mothers with mental retardation who do
or do not abuse or neglect their children. Child Abuse and Neglect, 14,
313–323.
Wade, C., Llewellyn, G., & Matthews, J. (2008). Review of parent training inter-
ventions for parents with intellectual disability. Journal of Applied Research in
Intellectual Disabilities, 21, 351–366. doi:10.1111/j.1468-3148.2008.00449.x.
Wolfensberger, W. (1980). A brief overview of the principle of normalization. In
R. J. Flynn & K. E. Nitsch (Eds.), Normalization, social integration and com-
munity service (pp. 7–30). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.
90 H.B. Sigurjónsdóttir and J.G. Rice
Legislation
Barnaverndarlög, nr. 80/2002 [Child Protection Act, nr. 80/2002].
Lög um að heimila í viðeigandi tilfellum aðgerðir á fólki, er koma í veg fyrir, að það
auki kyn sitt, nr. 16/1938. [Law setting out when surgery prevent procreation
is appropriate], nr. 16/1938. Retrieved from http://www.althingi.is/
lagas/137/1938016.html
Lög um aðstoð við þroskahefta, nr. 47/1979. [Law on assistance for ‘the mentally
retarded’, nr. 47/1979]. Retrieved from http://www.althingi.is/thingstorf/
thingmalalistareftirthingum/ferill/?ltg=100&mnr=270
Lög um málefni fatlaðs fólks, nr. 59/1992 með síðari breytingum. [Law on the
affairs of people with disabilities, nr. 59/1992 with later amendments].
Retrieved from http://eng.velferdarraduneyti.is/acts-of-Parliament/nr/3704
Lög um ráðgjöf og fræðslu varðandi kynlíf og barneignir og um fóstureyðingar og ófr-
jósemisaðgerðir, nr. 25/1975. [Law on counselling, education on sex and family
planning, abortion and sterilisation (nr. 25/1975 with later amendments].
Retrieved from http://www.althingi.is/lagas/124/1975025.html#G33
7
Self-stigma in People with Intellectual
Disabilities
Rory Sheehan and Afia Ali
R. Sheehan • A. Ali (
)
Division of Psychiatry, University College London, London, UK
e-mail: [email protected]
Evidence for Self-stigmatization
Table 7.1 provides a summary of the research on self-stigma in people
with intellectual disabilities.
Awareness of stigmatized status. Applying public stigma to oneself,
or internalizing negative societal attitudes, lies at the heart of the pro-
cess of self-stigmatization, see Fig. 7.1. This would appear to require an
individual to have knowledge of their intellectual disability identity and
the negative stereotypes associated with it. Indeed, evidence suggests that
many people with intellectual disabilities are aware of the stigma asso-
ciated with the condition. For example, in the early days of deinstitu-
tionalization, Edgerton (1967) observed that individuals resettled from
long-stay institutions into the community often attempted to hide their
disability and distance themselves from peers with intellectual disabilities
for fear of being stigmatized.
In a different study, individuals with mild intellectual disabilities attend-
ing an adult training center were found to be aware of their stigmatized
Table 7.1 Summary of studies on stigma in people with intellectual disabilities (ID)
Author (year) Design Sample Method Main findings
Ali et al. (2015) Quantitative 229 adults with mild Self-report measures of Stigma was associated
or moderate ID stigma (experiences of positively with
recruited from 12 discrimination and psychological distress and
sites/centers in reaction to service use and negatively
7
self-report of stigmatized
treatment and perception
of likelihood of achieving
93
future goals.
(continued)
Table 7.1 (continued)
94
participants to distance
themselves from ID
identity.
Cunningham and Mixed methods 78 parents and 77 Semi-structured Half of people with Down
Glenn (2004) individuals with interviews with syndrome did not
Down syndrome parents; questionnaires recognize their condition.
administered to people One quarter were aware
with ID of the stigma associated
with Down syndrome.
Dagnan and Cross-sectional 43 people with Self-report measures of Social comparison was
Sandhu (1999) mild-moderate ID self-esteem, social related to self-esteem
recruited from adult comparison, and and depression.
training centers depression
Emerson (2010) Cross-sectional 1273 adults with ID Interviews assessed Self-reported exposure to
living in private or self-reported health, bullying and acts of
supported well-being, and disablism were associated
accommodation exposure to bullying with worse self-reported
and disablism health outcomes.
7
Material or social
resources can mitigate
the effect of
discrimination.
Finlay and Lyons Mixed methods 28 people with Interviews assessing Two thirds of the group
(1998) mild-moderate ID representations of ID, endorsed a label of ID,
recruited from ID self-descriptions, although none
services self-esteem, group mentioned this
evaluation, and group spontaneously. Self-
identification esteem was not
correlated with group
evaluation, even when
the group was viewed
negatively.
Finlay and Lyons Qualitative 33 people with ID Semi-structured People with ID tended to
(2000) interviews addressing present themselves in a
descriptions of self and positive light and view
others and social themselves as ‘better’
comparisons than others with ID and
as ‘as good as’ those
Self-stigma in People with Intellectual Disabilities
without ID.
(continued)
95
Table 7.1 (continued)
96
individuals rejected ID
label but were aware
of their limitations and
stigma. Parents
minimized difficulties
and promoted
self-sufficiency.
(3) Vacillation: reluctance
to discuss label; parents
ambivalent or avoidant
and overprotective.
Most had experienced
stigma/discrimination.
(4) Denial: rejection of ID
label and denial of
stigma; parents
ambivalent or avoidant
and overprotective.
7 Self-stigma in People with Intellectual Disabilities 99
2. Endorsement of cultural
stereotypes
For example, I believe that people with
ID are not capable of living
independently
status and reported rejection and bullying from peers. Some individuals
were aware that certain restrictions were imposed on them at home that
did not apply to their siblings and were aware of the stigma attached to
day services (Jahoda et al. 1988). Participants who had recently moved
from an institution to the community described feeling ‘cut off’ from the
outside world and described experiences of rejection and discrimination
(Jahoda and Markova 2004). They were aware of the stigma attached to
the hospital, wanted to distance themselves from it and other patients
who had resided there, and were keen to develop a new identity. Those
who had moved from the family home to residential community settings
100 R. Sheehan and A. Ali
77 individuals with Down syndrome and found that only half of the sam-
ple were aware of having Down syndrome and only a quarter of the sample
recognized the stigma associated with the condition.
Some individuals with intellectual disabilities may downplay their dif-
ficulties or frame their difficulties using phrases that are more socially
acceptable, such as ‘I have difficulties with reading and writing’, or may
emphasize their strengths. Studies of social comparisons suggest that
when individuals with intellectual disabilities compare themselves with
others with intellectual disabilities, they may regard themselves as ‘bet-
ter’ and may even consider themselves to be ‘as good as’ people without
intellectual disabilities (Finlay and Lyons 2000). This lack of identifica-
tion with the stigmatized group may help some individuals avoid stereo-
types associated with intellectual disabilities. However, Finlay and Lyons
(1998) found that group identification was not associated with evaluating
the group negatively. Even if people described themselves as having intel-
lectual disabilities and evaluated people with intellectual disabilities more
negatively compared to those without intellectual disabilities, this did not
lead to a lowering of self-esteem. Some individuals did not believe that
the label of intellectual disabilities was applicable to them. These issues
are considered further in Chap. 14 of this book.
Lack of awareness of intellectual disability or of its stigmatized status
may be influenced by how parents and carers disclose or discuss disability
with their loved ones. For example, Todd and Shearn (1997) found that
most parents avoided discussing their child’s intellectual disability with
them in order to protect them from stigma, and were complicit in agree-
ing with their child’s unrealistic expectations of future jobs or marriage,
even if they themselves did not believe this to be possible. In addition,
carers who appeared overprotective or in denial about their child’s dif-
ficulties were reluctant to disclose to their child that they had an intellec-
tual disability. Zetlin and Turner (1984) found that parents who accepted
their offspring’s condition were more likely to disclose this to the indi-
vidual, and, subsequently, the individual was more comfortable talking
about their disability. In contrast, individuals who were uncomfortable
talking about their disability had parents who were ambivalent about
disclosing such information to their offspring. However, Cunningham
et al. (2000) found that awareness of having an intellectual disability was
7 Self-stigma in People with Intellectual Disabilities 103
Coping with Self-stigma
Resilience and the ability to cope and make sense of stigmatizing and
discriminatory treatment may influence whether individuals internalize
stigma. Reframing experiences in a positive way, or focusing on strengths
rather than limitations, can help individuals maintain self-esteem and protect
against self-stigma. Having a number of meaningful roles such as being an
employee, a mother, or a member of a club can help to act as a buffer against
the emotional consequences of stigma (Dagnan and Sandhu 1999).
The possibility of utilizing experiences of stigma as a force for good has
largely been overlooked in the intellectual disability literature, although
other fields have shown that it is possible for members of a stigmatized
group to be energized by experiences of discrimination and use their
‘righteous anger’ to drive positive change (Corrigan and Watson 2002).
Conclusions
There is a general lack of research on self-stigma as applied to people with
intellectual disabilities. There is some limited evidence to support the three-
stage process of self-stigma in people with intellectual disabilities, but more
research on the validity and utility of applying this model to people with
intellectual disabilities is required. Consideration should also be given to
other models or approaches to understanding how stigma is internalized
(or not) by people with intellectual disabilities, as this may differ from
other stigmatized groups. For example, use of stigma research conducted
with people affected by mental illness may provide a helpful framework to
understand general principles, but several difficulties exist in extrapolating
insights directly to individuals with intellectual disabilities (Ditchman et al.
2013). Stereotypes of people with mental illness and intellectual disabilities
differ substantially, with the former more likely to be described as ‘crazy’
or ‘dangerous’ and the latter as ‘dependent’ or ‘innocent’. In addition, peo-
ple with intellectual disabilities are more likely to experience additional
visible physical disabilities which can alter the public perception of their
106 R. Sheehan and A. Ali
Accessible Summary
• This chapter looks at research on how people with intellectual disabili-
ties view themselves and how they are treated.
• We wanted to find out if people with intellectual disabilities believe
that bad things that are said about them are true and how this affects
them.
• Many people with intellectual disabilities say that they are treated
badly or differently, such as being called names.
• Being treated badly by others can make people feel bad about them-
selves, depressed, and anxious.
• Services need to help people who have experienced bad treatment
from others.
References
Ali, A., King, M., Strydom, S., & Hassiotis, A. (2015). Self-reported stigma and
symptoms of anxiety and depression in people with intellectual disabilities:
Findings from a cross sectional study in England. Journal of Affective Disorders,
187, 224–231. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2015.07.046.
Ali, A., Hassiotis, A., Strydom, A., & King, M. (2012). Self stigma in people
with intellectual disabilities and courtesy stigma in family carers: A system-
atic review. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33, 2122–2140.
doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2012.06.013.
Beart, S., Hardy, G., & Buchan, L. (2005). How people with intellectual disabilities
view their social identity: A review of the literature. Journal of Applied Research in
Intellectual Disabilities, 18, 47–56. doi:10.1111/j.1468-3148.2004.00218.
Chen, C. H., & Shu, B. C. (2012). The process of perceiving stigmatisation:
Perspectives from Taiwanese young people with intellectual disability. Journal
108 R. Sheehan and A. Ali
M. Sherry (
) • A. Neller
University of Toledo, Toledo, OH, USA
e-mail: [email protected]
this criterion was somewhat difficult because disability hate crimes are
not officially recognized as a specific form of criminal activity in many
countries. And third, the crimes had to range in severity; although a large
number of disability hate crimes result in fatalities or serious injury, we
deemed it important to include other examples of hate crimes as well.
Labels and Insults
Over time, terms once associated with the medical diagnosis of intel-
lectual disability have morphed into the language of insult used in hate
speech and in the commission of disability hate crimes. Historically, pub-
lic health institutions often regarded those with intellectual disabilities as
subhuman, relying on eugenic ideas which suggested that some lives are
less worthy than others (Wolfensberger and Nirje 1972). The category
of ‘intellectual disability’ was initially defined through a medical model
replete with negative labels such as ‘feebleminded’, ‘idiot’, ‘mental defec-
tive’, ‘subnormal’, ‘imbecile’, ‘moron’, and ‘retarded’. Such terms over
time have trickled into common usage as generic slurs which take on
extra layers of meaning when aimed at people with intellectual disabili-
ties. For instance, the word ‘retard’ is widely used as an insult as well as
a specific form of hate speech used in the commission of disability hate
crimes.
Disability scholars consistently stress the importance of understanding
intellectual disability in its social context (Gill 2015). This means that
one cannot understand intellectual disability without noting the wider
social context of disablism (prejudice and discrimination against disabled
people) and ableism (processes and practices that privilege nondisabled
minds, senses, or bodies) (Campbell 2009). Attitudes toward intellec-
tual disability are not just characterized by stigma and prejudice; they
may involve hostility and even hatred as well. The results of such atti-
tudes include higher rates of violence, criminal victimization, and social
exclusion. These experiences are framed by disablism and ableism—wider
power systems that devalue and marginalize people with disabilities. The
combined effects of stigma, disablism, ableism, and intolerance are seen
most starkly in the violence of disability hate crimes.
8 Intellectual Disability, Stigma, and Hate Crimes 113
court with disorderly conduct and three were also charged with delin-
quency and assault (Corcoran and Faberov 2014). However, another
element of their actions deserves attention for those interested in the con-
nection between stigma and disability hate crimes: the teenagers assumed
that a crime against someone with an intellectual disability was socially
acceptable, not shameful, and that it was funny or entertaining enough
to share widely online.
Uploading such material online is becoming an alarmingly common
feature of crimes against people with disabilities—four examples (of the
many which are online) should demonstrate their nature. One video
which was uploaded to the Internet by the perpetrators shows two men
tormenting a 42-year-old woman with an intellectual disability outside
a Sacramento donut store. They then push her, spit on her, and punch
her in the face, laughing as they assault her (CBS13 Staff Reporters
2011a, b). Another video shows three teenagers in Winston-Salem, North
Carolina, pushing a man with physical and intellectual disabilities down
an embankment, chasing him, and continuing to attack him until he is
motionless on the ground (Anthony 2015). A third video, uploaded to
Facebook, shows six people attacking a 48-year-old woman with an intel-
lectual disability with their fists and shoes, as well as kicking her (Pow and
Staff Reporter Daily Mail 2013). A fourth video shows a group of girls
beating a man with an intellectual disability in Caruthersville, Missouri,
as he says ‘Baby, leave me alone’ (St. Amand 2012).
This small number of cases from the USA, alongside those discussed
earlier from the UK, only scratch the surface of the global dimension of
this problem. Reports from many other countries suggest that similar
cases of violence, abuse, torture, murder, and kidnapping, as well as sex-
ual assault, occur with alarming frequency—particularly for those people
with disabilities who reside in institutions. Placement in segregated insti-
tutions, located far away from the rest of the population, adds to the
stigma and prejudice experienced by people with intellectual disabilities.
Their social isolation is compounded by the reluctance of other people
to visit them in hospital-like institutions. Without external support and
safeguards, institutionalized people become more vulnerable to victim-
ization, violence, and abuse.
120 M. Sherry and A. Neller
that their experiences may be disability hate crimes, and that they can
get support and seek justice through the legal system if they want
• Stressing to people with intellectual disabilities that such instances are
not ‘just a part of normal life’ but do deserve police involvement
• Ensuring that disability hate crimes are appropriately identified and
not mislabeled as something else
• Acknowledging and addressing disagreements among various parts of
the legal system over the use of disability slurs during the commission
of a crime, especially as to whether they are indicators of hate or sim-
ply generic insults
• Acknowledging the reluctance of victims to report the crimes (particu-
larly their fears of retaliation or of not being believed) and putting in
place meaningful safeguards which ensure that they are safe and pro-
tected and that their complaints are given a fair hearing
• Demonstrating to victims that making a complaint about a disability
hate crime will not just involve reliving some of the worst experiences
in their life without recourse to any real justice
• Community education programs which increase knowledge about the
nature of disability hate crimes and teach people what they can do
about them
• Partnerships between law enforcement agencies and disability groups
to build trusting relationships which might lead to an increase in
reporting of disability hate crimes
• Community education tools which involve personal accounts of dis-
ability hate crimes, discussing their physical and emotional effects
• Removing the stigma attached to intellectual disability so that people
are not afraid of being publicly identified as someone with an intel-
lectual disability
• Innovations that make it easier to report disability hate crimes, such as
a reporting app for mobile phones, and the establishment of third-
party reporting programs
• Enhanced education programs throughout the entire law enforcement
system—from street-level policing to prosecutors, judges, parole offi-
cers, and so on
• Tracking the numbers of reported disability hate crimes which are suc-
cessfully prosecuted
122 M. Sherry and A. Neller
Conclusions
Disability hate crimes are a graphic reminder of the insult, abjection,
and violence directed at people with intellectual disabilities. This chapter
mainly focused on the UK and the USA, but the problem of violence,
abuse, and disability hate crime is not confined to those countries, far
from it. Disability hate crimes are a global problem. In challenging envi-
ronments that produce such crimes, it is necessary to confront both able-
ism and disablism. Both create environments which devalue, segregate,
marginalize, stigmatize, and endanger people with disabilities. But there
are also specific responses to disability hate crimes which are necessary, for
instance, community education, increased liaison between law enforce-
ment and disability groups, improved training for people at all levels of
the criminal justice system, and innovations in reporting processes such
as the advent of third-party reporting systems and the development of
reporting apps on telephones. But most importantly, there needs to be
improved communication with people with intellectual disabilities, and
their families and friends in order to ensure that they know what dis-
ability hate crimes are, and how they can seek justice when such crimes
occur.
Accessible Summary
• Sometimes people will hit you, kick you, or hurt you in some other
way just because you have a disability. This is not okay. It is called a
disability hate crime.
• You are not alone. This type of crime has happened to many other
people and you can get support and help.
• You can report it to the police if you want to, or someone else can help
you report it. They are less likely to hurt you, your family, or your
friends again if you go to the police.
• You have the right to be safe.
• No one has the right to harm you or discriminate against you.
References
Anthony, C. (2015, January 13). Police: W-S teenagers recorded their attack on
disabled man. Retrieved from http://www.wfmynews2.com/story/news/
local/2015/01/12/wfmy-winston-salem-police-teens-record-attack/21621375/
Associated Press (2015, March 16). Teens sentenced after pleading guilty in
videotaped assaults on mentally disabled man. Retrieved from http://www.
foxnews.com/us/2015/03/16/teens-sentenced-after-pleading-guilty-in-
videotaped-assaults-on-mentally/
BBC Staff Reporter (2014, January 7). Rooke family treated vulnerable man as
‘slave’. Retrieved from http://www.bbc.com/news/uk-england-south-yorkshire-
25642431
Beadle-Brown, J., Richardson, L., Guest, C., Malovic, A., Bradshaw, J., &
Himmerich, J. (2014). Living in fear: Better outcomes for people with learning
disabilities and autism. Tizard Centre, Canterbury, UK: University of Kent.
Bottoms, B. L., Nysse-Carris, K. L., & Tyda, K. (2003). Jurors’ perceptions of
adolescent sexual assault victims who have intellectual disabilities. Law and
Human Behavior, 27(2), 205–227.
Campbell, F. K. (2009). Contours of ableism. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave
Macmillan.
Caulfield, P. (2014, September 9). Cops identify bullies who poured urine,
feces on autistic teen in sick Ice Bucket Challenge. Retrieved from http://
www.nydailynews.com/news/national/cops-id-bullies-poured-urine-feces-autistic-
teen-sick-ice-bucket-challenge-prank-article-1.1933509
124 M. Sherry and A. Neller
CBS13 Staff Reporters (2011a). Police arrest second suspect in viral video beat-
ing. Retrieved from http://sacramento.cbslocal.com/2011/11/13/police-
arrest-second-suspect-in-youtube-beating/
CBS13 Staff Reporters (2011b). Video shows brutal attack at Sacramento strip
mall. Retrieved from http://sacramento.cbslocal.com/2011/10/11/video-
shows-brutal-attack-at-sacramento-strip-mall/
Chen, C. H., & Shu, B. C. (2012). The process of perceiving stigmatisation:
Perspectives from Taiwanese young people with intellectual disability. Journal
of Applied Research in Intellectual disability, 25, 240–251. doi:10.1111/j.
1468-3148.2011.00661.
Cockerton, P. (2012, December 5). Bordering upon sadistic torture: Four locked
up for “appalling” attacks on disabled man. Retrieved from http://www.mir-
ror.co.uk/news/uk-news/four-locked-up-for-appalling-attacks-1474522
Coleman, N., Sykes, W., & Walker, A. (2013). Crime and disabled people:
Baseline statistical analysis of measures from the formal legal inquiry into
disability-related harassment. Manchester, UK: Equality and Human Rights
Commission.
Corcoran, K., & Faberov, S. (2014, October 14). Five teens CHARGED over
sickening Ice Bucket Challenge prank in which autistic boy was doused with
urine, feces and cigarette butts. Retrieved from http://www.dailymail.co.uk/
news/article-2793112/five-teens-charged-sickening-ice-bucket-challenge-
prank-autistic-boy-doused-urine-feces-cigarette-butts.html
Cox, C. (2014). Ringleader jailed after gang tortured vulnerable housemate over
rent debt 28 July. Retrieved from http://www.manchestereveningnews.co.uk/
news/greater-manchester-news/lee-carter-jailed-torture-stacey-7522455
Dodenhoff, P. (2014). Challenging ‘hate’ within the UK–The disability hate
crime network. Retrieved from http://www.disabled-world.com/disability/
discrimination/dhcn.php
Dolak, K. (2013, January 23). Hate crime charges for allegedly stealing from
mentally disabled. Retrieved from http://abcnews.go.com/US/hate-crime-
charges-allegedly-stealing-mentally-disabled/story?id=18294194
Federal Bureau of Investigation (2010–2013). Uniform crime reports for the
United States 2010–2013. Washington, DC: US Department of Justice.
Fox 8 News Staff Winterhaven (2015, May 21). Florida man beat woman with
‘stupid stick’. Retrieved from http://myfox8.com/2015/05/21/police-florida-
man-beat-woman-with-stupid-stick
Gill, M. (2015). Already doing it: Intellectual disability and sexual agency.
Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.
8 Intellectual Disability, Stigma, and Hate Crimes 125
Robinson, S. (2013). Preventing the emotional abuse and neglect of people with
intellectual disability: Stopping insult and injury. London, UK: Jessica Kingsley
Publishers.
Scior, K., Hamid, A., Hastings, R., Werner, S., Belton, C., Laniyan, A.,…,Kett,
M. (2015). Intellectual disabilities: Raising awareness and combating stigma—
A global review. London, UK: University College London. Retrieved from
www.ucl.ac.uk/ciddr/documents/Global_ID_Stigma_Report_Final_
July_15.pdf
Sherry, M. (2010). Disability hate crimes: Does anyone really hate disabled people?
Surrey, UK: Ashgate.
Slater, R. (2011, July 31). The disabled woman abandoned to be murdered for
fun by a gang of savages who she thought were her friends. Retrieved from
http://www.dailymail.co.uk/femail/article-2020648/Gemma-Hayter-
Disabled-woman-abandoned-murdered-fun.html
St. Amand, A. (2012, July 12. Teen girls accused of beating disabled Missouri
man. Video posted on Facebook. Retrieved from http://www.stltoday.com/
news/local/crime-and-courts/teen-girls-accused-of-beating-disabled-
missouri- man-posting-video/article_477532c2-cc2b-11e1-bddd-
001a4bcf6878.html#ixzz20bnmaksp
Wagner, M. (2015, May 21). Florida man arrested for allegedly beating disabled
woman, teen with ‘stupid stick’. Retrieved from http://www.nydailynews.com/
news/crime/fla-man-disabled-woman-teen-stupid-stick-cops-article-1.2230860
Wheeler, C. (2015). Hate crimes on disabled up by 213 %. Retrieved from
http://www.express.co.uk/news/uk/551327/EXCLUSIVE-Hate-
crimes-on-disabled-rise-by-213
WTSP10 News Staff (2015). Man beats handicapped victims with “Stupid
stick”. Retrieved from http://www.wtsp.com/story/news/local/2015/05/21/
physical-abuse-handicapped-phillip-simons/27707029/
Wolfensberger, W., & Nirje, B. (1972). The principle of normalization in human
services. Toronto, Canada: National Institute on Mental Retardation.
Part III
Tackling Intellectual Disability
Stigma
9
Interventions Aimed at Tackling
Intellectual Disability Stigma: What
Works and What Still Needs to Be Done
Shirli Werner and Katrina Scior
S. Werner ()
Paul Baerwald School of Social Work and Social Welfare, Hebrew University of
Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel
e-mail: [email protected]
K. Scior ()
Division of Psychology & Language Sciences, University College London,
London, UK
e-mail: [email protected]
Structural
Interpersonal
(beyond family)
Familial
Intrapersonal
Interpersonal-Level Interventions
Interpersonal-level interventions (beyond the familial level) target social
interactions between stigmatized and non-stigmatized individuals (Cook
et al. 2014). Two broad types of interventions have been employed at the
interpersonal level: education and contact. Educational approaches are
those that challenge inaccurate stereotypes by providing factual information
(Seewooruttun and Scior 2014). Many disability organizations and NGOs
in the intellectual disability field, either at national level (such as Mencap in
the UK, AKIM and Keren Shalem in Israel, Community Living Association
in Canada, or Lebenshilfe in Germany and Austria to name but a few) or at
international level (Inclusion International and Special Olympics in particu-
lar), have provided education and messages designed to promote inclusion
and more positive attitudes to children and adults in the general population
via their programs, websites, leaflets, and social media. However, it is ques-
tionable to what extent their efforts reach audiences not already positively
inclined toward people with intellectual disabilities. Further, the impact
of these initiatives on attitude change has rarely been empirically studied,
Special Olympics programs being an exception (Siperstein et al. 2003).
9 Interventions Aimed at Tackling Intellectual Disability Stigma 133
Structural-Level Interventions
Interventions at the structural level focus on social forces and institutions,
through legislative action, mass media, governmental or organizational
policies which aim to reach a large audience (Cook et al. 2014), and ser-
vice delivery. Legislation such as the USA’s Americans with Disabilities
Act (1990), Australia’s Disability Discrimination Act (1992), Israel’s
Disability Equality Act (1998), or the UK Equality Act (2010), to name
just a few of many available acts, place a duty on public sector bodies to
ensure that reasonable adjustments are made to public services to ensure
9 Interventions Aimed at Tackling Intellectual Disability Stigma 139
that all sections of society, including people with disabilities, can access
them.
Universal support for these rights, at least in principle, is reflected
in the 161 nations (as of February 2016), which have ratified the 2006
UN Convention on the Rights of Persons with Disabilities (UN General
Assembly 2007). The convention calls for the prevention of discrimina-
tion through increased awareness raising efforts to combat stereotypes and
prejudice toward individuals with disabilities (Article 8). Other examples
of legislation and policy aimed at decreasing discrimination toward indi-
viduals with intellectual disabilities can be seen in the widespread adop-
tion of inclusive education as well as the naming of ‘disability’ as one of
the categories motivating hate crime under legislation such as the UK’s
Criminal Justice Act (2003) and the USA’s Matthew Shepard and James
Byrd, Jr. Hate Crimes Prevention Act (2009).
The impact of legislation and policy by their very nature are very dif-
ficult to evaluate as their effects do not occur in isolation. Thus, not sur-
prisingly, to the best of our knowledge, there is no evidence on what
impact such structural-level interventions have had in reducing intellec-
tual disability stigma.
2013) suggests that indirect contact (e.g., through films featuring persons
with intellectual disabilities) may also be beneficial and their integration
into more wide-ranging efforts to change attitudes should be considered.
With regard to the need to challenge stereotypes, evidence from other
fields suggests that exposing people to individuals who moderately or
strongly disconfirm common stereotypes, and who vary in terms of their
backgrounds, life roles, and the challenges they face (Clement et al.
2012), is likely to be most effective. These suggestions should be tested
in relation to intellectual disability stigma. There is clear scope for expo-
sure to individuals who challenge common stereotypes of people with
intellectual disabilities as childlike, dependent, and in need of protection.
This will need balancing, carefully though, without denying the needs of
people with severe and profound intellectual disabilities who may be at
risk of being further marginalized.
Contact-based interventions need to be carefully planned to minimize
the risk of unintended, adverse consequences. For example, negative con-
tact experiences, especially in childhood, may in fact increase social dis-
tance (Tachibana 2005), while a moderate amount of contact, as opposed
to no or ample contact, appears to have the strongest association with more
positive attitudes and willingness to interact (Freudenthal et al. 2010).
For the general public, contact is most likely to be facilitated by the
media. Efforts to educate and challenge the formation of prejudice
directed at individuals with intellectual disabilities should start at an early
age. For children and young people, contact can be provided through
inclusive activities and inclusive education. For those more likely to be in
regular contact with people with intellectual disabilities, contact should
be provided as part of training and continuing professional development.
In addition, fighting for the right of people with intellectual disabili-
ties to have increased access to community resources must be an inte-
gral part of efforts to change attitudes. Equal participation in education,
employment, and social and leisure pursuits not only respects the rights
of people with intellectual disabilities but also gives the general public
increased opportunities for, and benefit from, direct contact.
Finally, involving people with intellectual disabilities in delivering atti-
tude change interventions is important, as first person narratives have
been found to have greater impact than narratives by family members or
9 Interventions Aimed at Tackling Intellectual Disability Stigma 143
carers (Walker and Scior 2013). Although the utility of such interventions
needs to be explored further, reliance on first person narratives rightly
privileges the experiences of individuals with intellectual disabilities.
Accessible Summary
• More should be done to fight negative attitudes toward individuals
with intellectual disabilities.
• Research is needed so we learn how to fight negative attitudes.
• People without intellectual disabilities should meet more frequently
with people with intellectual disabilities.
• People should also learn more about intellectual disability.
• People with intellectual disabilities should tell their life stories to help
change negative attitudes.
144 S. Werner and K. Scior
References
Ali, A., Hassiotis, A., Strydom, A., & King, M. (2012). Self-stigma in people
with intellectual disabilities and courtesy stigma in family carers: A system-
atic review. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33, 2122–2140.
doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2012.06.013.
Anti-Bullying Alliance SEN and Disability. n.d. Developing effective anti-bullying
practice. Retrieved from http://www.anti-bullyingalliance.org.uk/send-
programme
Bailey, A., Barr, O., & Bunting, B. (2001). Police attitudes toward people with
intellectual disability: An evaluation of awareness training. Journal of Intellectual
Disability Research, 45, 344–350. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2788.2001.00339.
Beacham, N., & Rouse, M. (2012). Student teachers’ attitudes and beliefs about
inclusion and inclusive practice. Journal of Research in Special Educational
Needs, 12, 3–11. doi:10.1111/j.1471-3802.2010.01194.
Beckett, A. E., Buckner, L., Barrett, S., Ellison, N., & Byrne, D. (2009).
Promoting positive attitudes towards disabled people—The views of schools and
teachers. DEEPS Project Working Paper 2. University of Leeds, UK: School
of Sociology & Social Policy.
Butler, A. C., Chapman, J. E., Forman, E. M., & Beck, A. T. (2006). The empir-
ical status of cognitive-behavioral therapy: A review of meta-analyses. Clinical
Psychology Review, 26, 17–31. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2005.07.003.
Campbell, J., Gilmore, L., & Cuskelly, M. (2003). Changing student teachers’
attitudes towards disability and inclusion. Journal of Intellectual and
Developmental Disability, 28, 369–379. doi:10.1080/136682503100016164
07.
Clement, S., Lassman, F., Barley, E., Evans-Lacko, S., Williams, P., Yamaguchi,
S., et al. (2013). Mass media interventions for reducing mental health-related
stigma (Review). The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 7.
doi:10.1002/14651858.cd009453
Clement, S., van Nieuwenhuizen, A., Kassam, A., Flach, C., Lazarus, A., de
Castro, M., et al. (2012). Filmed versus live social contact interventions to
reduce stigma: Randomised controlled trial. British Journal of Psychiatry, 201,
57–64. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.111.093120.
Cook, J. E., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Meyer, I. H., & Busch, J. T. (2014). Intervening
within and across levels: A multilevel approach to stigma and public health. Social
Science and Medicine, 103, 101–109. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.09.023.
9 Interventions Aimed at Tackling Intellectual Disability Stigma 145
Corrigan, P. W., Morris, S. B., Michaels, P. J., Rafacz, J. D., & Rüsch, N. (2012).
Challenging the public stigma of mental illness: A meta-analysis of outcome
studies. Psychiatric Services, 63, 963–973. doi:10.1176/appi.ps.201100529.
Emerson, E. (2015). The determinants of health inequities experienced by children
with learning disabilities. Durham, UK: Public Health England.
Freudenthal, J. J., Boyd, L. D., & Tivis, R. (2010). Assessing change in health
professions volunteers’ perceptions after participating in Special Olympics
healthy athlete events. Journal of Dental Education, 74, 970–979.
Georgiadi, M., Kalyva, E., Kourkoutas, E., & Tsakiris, V. (2012). Young chil-
dren’s attitudes toward peers with intellectual disabilities: Effect of the type of
school. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 25, 531–541.
doi:10.1111/j.1468-3148.2012.00699.
Hall, H., & Minnes, P. (1999). Attitudes towards persons with Down’s syn-
drome: The impact of television. Journal of Developmental and Physical
Disabilities, 11, 61–76. doi:10.1023/A:1021812702337.
Iacono, T., Lewis, B., Tracy, J., Hicks, S., Morgan, P., Recoche, K., et al. (2011).
DVD-based stories of people with developmental disabilities as resources for
inter-professional education. Disability and Rehabilitation, 33, 1010–1021.
doi:10.3109/09638288.2010.520802.
Lindsay, S., & Edwards, A. (2013). A systematic review of disability awareness
interventions for children and youth. Disability and Rehabilitation, 35,
623–646. doi:10.3109/09638288.2012.702850.
MacDonald, J. D., & MacIntyre, P. D. (1999). A rose is a rose: Effects of label
change, education and sex on attitudes towards mental disabilities. Journal of
Developmental Disabilities, 6, 15–31.
McKenzie, J. A., McConkey, R., & Adnams, C. (2013). Intellectual disability in
Africa: Implications for research and service development. Disability and
Rehabilitation, 35, 1750–1755. doi:10.3109/09638288.2012.751461.
Mencap (2007). Bullying wrecks lives: The experiences of children and young people
with a learning disability. London, UK: Mencap Publications.
Nosse, L. J., & Gavin, K. J. (1991). Influence of direct contact on college stu-
dents’ attitude towards adults with mental handicaps. College Student Journal,
25, 201–206.
Ouellette-Kuntz, H., Burge, P., Brown, H. K., & Arsenault, E. (2010). Public
attitudes towards individuals with intellectual disabilities as measured by the
concept of social distance. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities,
23, 132–142. doi:10.1111/j.1468-3148.2009.00514.
Raczka, R., Theodore, K., & Williams, J. (2014). Can brief training have an
impact on police attitudes towards people with intellectual disabilities?
146 S. Werner and K. Scior
Sharma, U., Forlin, C., & Loreman, T. (2008). Impact of training on pre-service
teachers’ attitudes and concerns about inclusive education and sentiments
about persons with disabilities. Disability and Society, 23, 773–785.
doi:10.1080/09687590802469271.
Staniland, J. J., & Byrne, M. K. (2013). The effects of a multi-component
higher-functioning autism anti-stigma program on adolescent boys. Journal
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 2816–2829. doi:10.1007/
s10803-013-1829-4.
Szivos, S. E., & Griffiths, E. (1990). Group processes involved in coming to
terms with a mentally retarded identity. Mental Retardation, 6, 333–341.
Tachibana, T. (2005). Attitudes of Japanese adults toward persons with intel-
lectual disability: An exploratory analysis of respondents’ experiences and
opinions. Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities, 40, 352–359.
Tracy, J., & Iacono, T. (2008). People with developmental disabilities teaching
medical students: Does it make a difference? Journal of Intellectual and
Developmental Disabilities, 33, 345–348. doi:10.1080/13668250802478633.
UN General Assembly (2007). Convention on the rights of persons with disabili-
ties: Resolution. Adopted by the General Assembly, 24 January 2007.
Walker, J., & Scior, K. (2013). Tackling stigma associated with intellectual dis-
ability among the general public: A study of two indirect contact interven-
tions. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34, 2200–2210. doi:10.1016/j.
ridd.2013.03.024.
Werner, S. (2015). Equal in uniform: Its impact on attitudes of soldiers without
disabilities towards soldiers with intellectual disabilities. Beit Issie Shapiro’s 6th
International Conference on Disabilities: Unity and Diversity in Action. Tel-
Aviv, Israel.
10
Relationships Matter: Addressing
Stigma Among Children and Youth
with Intellectual Disabilities
and Their Peers
Erik W. Carter, Elizabeth E. Biggs, and Carly L. Blustein
Such collective encounters may lead some students with intellectual dis-
abilities to adopt a type of ‘self-stigma’, in which reluctance to participate
in school activities is driven by a belief that one’s own social identity is
devalued (Ditchman et al. 2013).
These various forms of stigma contribute in part to the paucity of peer
relationships and friendships in the lives of many students with intellec-
tual disabilities. According to a large-scale study involving parents of chil-
dren with intellectual disabilities (ages 6–13) in the USA, 17 % of parents
reported their child with an intellectual disability never visited friends during
the previous year and 50 % reported their children never or rarely received
telephone calls from friends (Wagner et al. 2003). The scarcity of social
relationships becomes even more apparent when focusing on high school
students with intellectual disabilities: according to their parents, only 22 %
frequently saw any friends outside of school, 42 % never or rarely received
telephone calls from friends, and only 54 % got together with friends out-
side of school and took part in organized activities at least once each week.
tual disabilities that may replace stigma with positive attitudes and expe-
riences. Additionally, supporting a student with intellectual disabilities to
become a more valued member of the classroom may promote an overall
climate in the class of acceptance, respect, and belonging. Empirical sup-
port for these interventions is most prominent among adolescents with
severe intellectual disabilities; less is known about their efficacy with stu-
dents in earlier grades or who have less extensive support needs.
Peer networks. Peer network interventions focus on increasing social
connections outside of the classroom, such as during lunch, in the play-
ground, in hallways, within extracurricular groups, or before and after
school. Peer networks involve three to six peers and the student with the
disability meeting both formally and informally as a social group (Carter
et al. 2013; Hochman et al. 2015; Koegel et al. 2013). The network meets
weekly or biweekly to participate in an enjoyable shared activity such as
playing a game, eating a meal, or completing a service project together.
School staff (a coach, teacher, or guidance counselor) facilitate the net-
work meetings to ensure all students actively participate. Outside of each
formal meeting, students plan other ways to connect with one another
between classes, such as eating lunch together or meeting for an activity
outside of school. As with peer support interventions, peer networks have
primarily been evaluated in secondary schools. Moreover, much of this
research has involved students with autism.
Peer tutoring. Peer tutoring interventions involve pairs or groups
of students working with one another to practice, review, and master
academic content. These instructional interventions have been widely
evaluated among students with mild intellectual disabilities. They can
involve peers of similar or different ages and can be established for a
single student or carried out on a whole class basis. Although peer tutor-
ing promotes both interactions and academic skill development, static
roles involving peers as ‘helpers’ and students with intellectual disabilities
as the recipients of assistance may inadvertently perpetuate deficit-based
views of students. Although there is an important place for this type of
academic support, promoting opportunities for students with and with-
out disabilities to provide reciprocal support may reduce stigma by high-
lighting the strengths of students with intellectual disabilities.
158 E.W. Carter et al.
Implications for Research
The pervasiveness of segregated service delivery models highlights the need
for high-quality research to push policy and practice in new directions.
We highlight five areas of particular importance. First, little attention
has focused on the broader or longer-term impact of the interventions
described in this chapter, including the ways in which each might shape
the attitudes, expectations, career pathways, and future behaviors of peers
who had the opportunity to get to know fellow pupils with intellectual
disabilities while in school. The few studies that have explored the impact
of interventions that continued beyond a single school term suggest
that some newly formed friendships are maintained and that positive
attitudes may endure (Carter et al. 2015a; Kishi and Meyer 1994).
Recognizing that the peers of present are the civic, corporate, congrega-
tional, and community leaders of tomorrow, well-designed school-based
interventions hold potential to shape broader societal attitudes over time.
Longitudinal studies are needed to explore these possible pathways. The
spread of these interventions should also be considered more closely to
10 Relationships Matter 159
learn whether and how other students who are not directly involved in
these interventions may be affected by what they observe.
Second, much of the existing literature has emphasized student-level
interventions, with only modest attention dedicated to classroom-level
efforts. Scaling up these interventions to be delivered throughout an
entire school in intentional and coordinated ways is a continued need.
What might it take for schools to prioritize addressing stigma amidst
the numerous other priorities school leaders may view as competing or
more pressing? How might schools shift from a reactive to a proactive
posture, in which these interventions are viewed to be an important
investment in creating a safe and inclusive school? Such questions need
strong answers.
Third, the intersection of policy and practice warrants much closer
consideration. Policies addressing educational placement, school staffing
patterns, discipline, and service delivery all have implications for how
students with intellectual disabilities are received and perceived in their
schools. While much attention has focused on how these policies affect
the academic and behavioral outcomes of students, how they shape atti-
tudes and stigma should be explored more fully.
Fourth, the limited extent to which the voices of students have per-
meated this literature is striking. Relatively few studies have focused on
how students with disabilities view the issue of stigma and the recom-
mendations they have for the design and delivery of school-based inter-
ventions. Likewise, the perspectives of participating peers have not been
prominent. The input and ‘buy-in’ of participating students with and
without intellectual disabilities are especially important to understand
when designing interventions aimed at addressing stigma.
Fifth, much of the research described in this chapter has taken place
in Western contexts. Because countries and cultures differ in their
priorities, policies, resources, and prevailing attitudes, additional invest-
ment is needed to replicate and extend available research across diverse
educational and community contexts. The biggest obstacles to ensuring
students with intellectual disabilities are seen as valued members of
schools and communities—as well as the most effective efforts to elimi-
nating these barriers—may be different across various contexts. Moreover,
160 E.W. Carter et al.
Summary
Although substantial changes in the participation and perceptions of
young people with intellectual disabilities have taken place over the past
three decades, far too many students remain on the periphery of every-
day school life and are the focus of considerable stigma. In this chapter,
we highlighted important elements and research-based interventions that
put students in the best position to learn alongside and develop positive
relationships with their peers without disabilities in inclusive school expe-
riences. The importance of this investment to the long-term outcomes of
young people with intellectual disabilities is hard to overestimate.
Accessible Summary
• Some of your classmates at school may think of you differently because
you have a disability.
• You should not allow negative experiences with your peers to change
the way you feel about yourself or make you feel any less of a person.
• Friendships are very important in life. There are many people around
you who want to be your friend or help you make friends.
• Participating in activities inside and outside of school with your class-
mates can give you an opportunity to get to know each other and
become friends.
• Teachers and researchers are always coming up with new ideas to sup-
port you in school and help you make friends easier. There are many
ways adults can help you connect more with your classmates.
References
Brady, M. P., Martin, S., Williams, R. E., & Burta, M. (1991). The effects of
fifth graders’ socially directed behavior on motor and social responses of chil-
dren with multiple handicaps. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 12,
1–16. doi:10.1177/105381519301700301.
Brock, M. E., & Carter, E. W. (2015). Efficacy of teachers training paraprofes-
sionals to implement peer support arrangements. Exceptional Children.
Advance online publication. doi:10.1177/0014402915585564.
Carter, E. W., Asmus, J., Moss, C. K., Cooney, M., Weir, K., Vincent, L., et al.
(2013). Peer network strategies to foster social connections among adoles-
cents with and without severe disabilities. TEACHING Exceptional Children,
46(1), 51–59. doi:10.1352/0895-8017(2005)110[366:FISIAH]2.0.CO.
Carter, E. W., Asmus, J., Moss, C. K., Amirault, K. A., Biggs, E. E., Born, T. L.,
et al. (2015a). Randomized evaluation of peer supports arrangements to
support the inclusion of high school students with severe disabilities. Exceptional
Children. Advance online publication. doi:10.1177/0014402915598780.
Carter, E. W., Bottema-Beutel, K., & Brock, M. E. (2014). Social interactions
and friendships. In M. Agran, F. Brown, C. Hughes, C. Quirk, & D. Ryndak
(Eds.), Equity and full participation for individuals with severe disabilities: A
vision for the future (pp. 197–216). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
162 E.W. Carter et al.
Carter, E. W., Hughes, C., Copeland, S. R., & Breen, C. (2001). Differences
between high school students who do and do not volunteer to participate in
peer interaction programs. Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe
Handicaps, 26, 229–239. doi:10.2511/rpsd.26.4.229.
Carter, E. W., Moss, C. K., Hoffman, A., Chung, Y. C., & Sisco, L. (2011). Efficacy
and social validity of peer support arrangements for adolescents with disabilities.
Exceptional Children, 78, 107–125. doi:10.1352/2008.46:346-363.
Carter, E. W., Moss, C. K., Asmus, J., Fesperman, E., Cooney, M., Brock,
M. E., et al. (2015b). Promoting inclusion, social relationships, and learning
through peer support arrangements. TEACHING Exceptional Children, 48,
9–18. doi:10.1177/0040059915594784.
Carter, E. W., Sisco, L. G., Chung, Y., & Stanton-Chapman, T. (2010). Peer
interactions of students with intellectual disabilities and/or autism: A map of
the intervention literature. Research and Practice for Persons with Severe
Disabilities, 35, 63–79. doi:10.2511/rpsd.35.3-4.63.
Causton-Theoharis, J. N., & Malmgren, K. W. (2005). Increasing peer interac-
tions for students with severe disabilities via paraprofessional training.
Exceptional Children, 71, 431–444. doi:10.1177/001440290507100403.
Christensen, L. L., Fraynt, R. J., Neece, C. L., & Baker, B. L. (2012). Bullying
adolescents with intellectual disability. Journal of Mental Health Research in
Intellectual Disabilities, 5, 49–65. doi:10.1080/19315864.2011.637660.
Chung, Y., Carter, E. W., & Sisco, L. G. (2012). A systematic review of inter-
ventions to increase peer interactions for students with complex communica-
tion challenges. Research and Practice for Persons with Severe Disabilities, 37,
271–287. doi:10.2511/027494813805327304.
Ditchman, N., Werner, S., Kosyluk, K., Jones, N., Elg, B., & Corrigan, P. W.
(2013). Stigma and intellectual disability: Potential application of mental ill-
ness research. Rehabilitation Psychology, 58, 206–216. doi:10.1037/a0032466.
Feldman, R., Carter, E. W., Asmus, J., & Brock, M. E. (2015). Presence, prox-
imity, and peer interactions of adolescents with severe disabilities in general
education classrooms. Exceptional Children. doi:10.1177/0014402915585481.
Hochman, J. M., Carter, E. W., Bottema-Beutel, K., Harvey, M. N., &
Gustafson, J. R. (2015). Efficacy of peer networks to increase social connec-
tions among high school students with and without autism. Exceptional
Children, 82, 96–116. doi:10.1177/0014402915585482.
Hughes, C., & Carter, E. W. (2008). Peer buddy programs for successful secondary
school inclusion. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
10 Relationships Matter 163
Hughes, C., Golas, M., Cosgriff, C., Brigham, N., Edwards, C., & Cashen, K.
(2011). Effects of a social skills intervention among high school students
with intellectual disabilities and autism and their general education peers.
Research and Practice for Persons with Severe Disabilities, 36, 46–61.
doi:10.2511/rpsd.36.1-2.46.
Jackson, L. B., Ryndak, D. L., & Wehmeyer, M. L. (2008). The dynamic rela-
tionship between context, curriculum, and student learning: A case for inclu-
sive education as a research-based practice. Research and Practice for Persons
with Severe Disabilities, 33, 175–195. doi:10.2511/rpsd.33.4.175.
Kasari, C., Rotheram-Fuller, E., Locke, J., & Gulsrud, A. (2011). Making the
connection: Randomized controlled trial of social skills at school for children
with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
53, 431–439. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02493.
Kersh, J., Corona, L., & Siperstein, G. (2013). Social well-being and friendship
of people with intellectual disability. In M. Wehmeyer (Ed.), The Oxford
handbook of positive psychology and disability (pp. 60–81). New York, NY:
Oxford University Press.
Kishi, G. S., & Meyer, L. H. (1994). What children report and remember: A
six-year follow-up of the effects of social contact between peers with and
without severe disabilities. Research and Practice for Persons with Severe
Disabilities, 19, 277–289. doi:10.1177/154079699401900404.
Koegel, R., Kim, S., Koegel, L., & Schwartzman, B. (2013). Improving social-
ization for high school students with ASD by using their preferred interests.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 2121–2134. doi:10.1007/
s10803-013-1765-3.
Leigers, K. L., & Myers, C. T. (2015). Effect of duration of peer awareness edu-
cation on attitudes toward students with disabilities: A systematic review.
Journal of Occupational Therapy, Schools, & Early Intervention, 8, 79–96. doi:
10.1080/19411243.2015.1021067.
Lindsay, S., & Edwards, A. (2013). A systematic review of disability awareness
interventions for children and youth. Disability and Rehabilitation, 35,
623–646. doi:10.3109/09638288.2012.702850.
Rispoli, M., Franco, J., van der Meer, L., Lang, R., & Carmargo, S. (2010). The
use of speech generating devices in communication interventions for individu-
als with developmental disabilities: A review of the literature. Developmental
Neurorehabilitation, 13, 276–293. doi:10.3109/17518421003636794.
Ryndak, D., Jackson, L. B., & White, J. M. (2013). Involvement and progress
in the general curriculum for students with extensive support needs: K-12
164 E.W. Carter et al.
This latter point has been glaringly evident in the new Australian National
Disability Insurance Scheme; the design of the scheme reflected an atten-
dant care model, and during its first 12 months of operation, it failed to
have any formal advisory structures which included people with intel-
lectual disabilities or their representative organizations (Bigby 2015b).
The lack of social model theorizing about people with intellectual dis-
abilities has meant neglect of social processes and structures more specifi-
cally disadvantaging to them, such as societal reliance on complex written
or spoken communication or replacement of staff with technology in
systems such as public transportation. A drawback of diverting attention
away from impairment has been the seemingly unproblematic acceptance
of intellectual disability as a biological given. More recent work in disability
studies on psychosocial and political views of impairment holds promise
for greater attention both to the social construction of impairment and
more nuanced action about the complex interactions between different
types of impairment and social processes (Goodley and Roets 2015).
Inclusion in collective action and the disability rights movement has held
a problematic contradiction for people with intellectual disabilities. The dif-
ficulties posed by the presumption that to be part of the movement one must
adopt a ‘disabled identity’, and thus embrace an ascribed label, have not been
acknowledged (Dowse 2001). The label ‘intellectual disability’ carries with
it a level of stigma and negativity not generally ascribed to those with other
disabilities which may explain a lack of willingness to openly adopt such an
identity. ‘Passing’ rather than identifying with a stigmatized label is the pre-
ferred option for many people with intellectual disabilities (Edgerton 1993;
Rapley 2004). The choice ‘not to identify’ is seen by some disability activists
as a rejection of the social model, an ‘internalized oppression’ (Shakespeare
and Watson 2002). Yet if the common identity to be embraced is highly
stigmatized, it seems unreasonable to pathologize people with intellectual
disabilities by accusing them of ‘internalizing oppression’. As noted in Chap.
14, for many, rejecting the identity of someone with an intellectual disability
is one way to deal with stigma. If an individual is willing to take on the label
and join a group for people with intellectual disabilities advocating change in
social attitudes toward people with stigmatized identities, there remains the
question of the willingness of the broader disability rights movement to be
inclusive enough to support their wearing of the ‘badge’ of an activist.
168 S. Anderson and C. Bigby
Accessible Summary
• In Australia and the UK, governments have tried for many years to
make it easier for people with intellectual disabilities to make their
own choices. But many people are still not living how they would like.
• Sometimes, people with intellectual disabilities have not been included
in groups that fight for better rights for people with disabilities.
• Self-advocacy groups are really important. They challenge some of the
negative ideas people have about people with intellectual disabilities.
They offer different activities which can make people feel more confi-
dent and independent. They are also places where people can enjoy
support from friends.
References
Anderson, S. (2013). ‘We just help them be them really’—Building positive,
included identities: engagement in self-advocacy groups by adults with intellectual
disabilities (Unpublished PhD thesis). LaTrobe University, Melbourne,
Australia.
Anderson, S., & Bigby, C. (2015). Self-advocacy as a means to positive identities
for people with intellectual disability: “We just help them, be them really”.
Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disability. Advance online publica-
tion. doi:10.1111/jar.12223.
Bauman, Z. (1996). From pilgrim to tourist—Or a short history of identity. In
S. Hall & P. du Gay (Eds.), Questions of cultural identity (pp. 18–36). London:
SAGE.
Beart, S., Hardy, G., & Buchan, L. (2005). How people with intellectual disabilities
view their social identity: A review of the literature. Journal of Applied Research in
Intellectual Disabilities, 18, 47–56. doi:10.1111/j.1468-3148.2004.00218.
Beresford, P. (2012). The theory and philosophy behind user involvement. In
P. Beresford & S. Carr (Eds.), Social care, service users and user involvement
(pp. 21–37). London, UK: Jessica Kingsley.
Bigby, C. (2015a). Self-advocacy and inclusion: A summary of the study ‘What can
be learned from speaking up over the years’. La Trobe University, Melbourne,
Australia: Living with Disability Research Centre. Retrieved from https://
engage.dss.gov.au/wp-content/uploads/2015/07/Final-self-advocacy-and-
inclusion- what-can-be-learned-from-speaking-up-over-the-years-
Bigby-21-07-2015.pdf
176 S. Anderson and C. Bigby
Hall, E., & Wilton, R. (2011). Alternative spaces of ‘work ‘and inclusion for
disabled people. Disability and Society, 26, 867–880. doi:10.1080/09687599
.2011.618742.
Hubert, J. (2000). The social, individual and moral consequences of physical
exclusion in long-stay institutions. In J. Hubert (Ed.), Madness, disability and
social exclusion. The archaeology and anthropology of ‘difference’ (pp. 196–207).
London, UK: Routledge.
McNally, S. (2002). A survey of self-advocacy groups for people with learning
disabilities in an English region. Journal of Intellectual Disabilities, 6, 185–199.
doi:10.1177/14690047030073004.
Milner, P., & Kelly, B. (2009). Community participation and inclusion: People
with disabilities defining their place. Disability and Society, 24, 47–62.
doi:10.1080/09687590802535410.
Nind, M., & Seale, J. (2009). Concepts of access for people with learning diffi-
culties: Towards a shared understanding. Disability and Society, 24, 273–287.
doi:10.1080/09687590902789446.
Productivity Commission (2011). Disability support and care. Canberra,
Australia: Productivity Commission.
Rapley, M. (2004). The social construction of intellectual disability. Cambridge,
UK: Cambridge University Press.
Shakespeare, T. (2006). Disability rights and wrongs. London, UK: Taylor and
Francis Routledge.
Shakespeare, T., & Watson, N. (2002). The social model of disability: An out-
dated ideology? Research in Social Science and Disability, 2, 9–28. doi:10.1016/
S1479-3547(01)80018.
Szivos, S., & Griffiths, E. (1990). Group processes involved in coming to terms
with a mentally retarded identity. Mental Retardation, 28, 333–341.
doi:10.1111/j.1468-3156.2008.00507.
12
Tackling Stigma in Developing
Countries: The Key Role of Families
Roy McConkey, Callista Kahonde,
and Judith McKenzie
R. McConkey ()
University of Ulster, Northern Ireland, Coleraine, UK
e-mail: [email protected]
C. Kahonde • J. McKenzie
University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa
across the nations, people with intellectual disabilities and their families
still encounter stigma and discrimination from their fellow citizens. Thus,
the lessons learnt of tackling stigma in the developed world and described
in the earlier chapters of this book may well be applicable to less devel-
oped countries. And the converse is equally true: the methods that have
proved effective in less affluent nations can inform and guide actions that
need to be taken in more affluent countries if full equality of opportunity
is to be obtained for all their citizens. In that sense, this chapter is well
placed to challenge and extend our current understanding of the roots of
stigma within human society and to offer tangible strategies to reduce, if
not remove, the stigmatizing impact of disability in every nation.
An Impossible Dream?
It certainly will be a struggle to attain the aspiration of removing the
stigmatizing impact of disability in every nation because three main fac-
tors may work against us. First, disability never exists in a pure form;
rather, reactions to it are compounded by other societal attitudes, such
as to women, ethnic and religious minorities and by the impact of pov-
erty—the link between disability and poverty is long established globally
(Eide and Ingstad 2011). Thus, the stigma that families and people with
intellectual disabilities experience can be an expression of attitudes that
go beyond those relating to disability and strategies to counter discrimi-
nation must take this into account.
A second consideration is that every nation is an amalgam of differ-
ent cultures and communities with diverse beliefs and values. Thus, we
should expect, as research confirms, variations in public understanding
and perceptions of disability within and between countries (Siperstein
et al. 2003). This suggests that a range of approaches will be needed often
tailored to ‘sub-cultures’ rather than relying on national strategies.
The third challenge is arguably the most daunting: how do we move
beyond changing attitudes to changing people’s behavior? Some would
say we have placed too much reliance on giving people ‘head knowledge’
about disability and insufficient opportunity for them to experience ‘heart
knowledge’ that engages their emotions because that is a more effective
12 Tackling Stigma in Developing Countries 181
Fig. 12.1 Family influences on society and the person with an intellectual
disability
equally the person affects the family. These are dynamic influences that
become ever more entwined over time.
We distinguish between reactive and proactive influences. By reactive
we mean the passive acceptance of prevailing reactions to intellectual dis-
ability that are reinforced within the community and contribute to the
self-stigma of the family and of the person. Four common reactions are
detailed in the following section. In contrast, proactive influences chal-
lenge prevailing attitudes and perceptions of intellectual disability. These
too are mutual so that families can influence their community and also
the person, while also being open to proactive influences coming to them
from the community and from the person. Four examples of proactive
influences will be presented.
It is likely that these two cycles of influence are present in varying
degrees within families, with the balance possibly changing over time
and in relation to certain issues, for example, the availability of sup-
port for the family and varying characteristics of the child/adult with an
intellectual disability. Moreover, intrafamilial differences in reactions to
the disability are a further source of tension between these two cycles that
12 Tackling Stigma in Developing Countries 183
can result in the affected child’s nuclear family ‘splitting away’ from the
reactive influences of the wider family. Sometimes the tension happens
within the nuclear family whereby one of the parents, usually the mother,
gets blamed for the occurrence of a disability (Haihambo and Lightfoot
2010). Such tensions invariably add to maternal stress. However, interna-
tional experience suggests that all families are capable of becoming more
proactive and that this is an essential component in reducing the self-
stigmatization of families and that of their relative with an intellectual
disability, as well as challenging stigmatizing attitudes and behaviors in
their community and wider society.
Reactive Influences
We begin by highlighting the strength of negative reactions in the com-
munity and in families to people with an intellectual disability as uncov-
ered in our ongoing research in South Africa (Kahonde 2015; McKenzie
and McConkey 2015) and in various recent studies undertaken in other
developing countries such as Ghana, Tanzania, Namibia and Pakistan
(Aldersey 2012; Haihambo and Lightfoot 2010). A much more extensive
literature exists in more affluent countries yet it is broadly supportive of
the dominant reactions described here. However, comparisons should be
made cautiously as there have been few well-controlled, cross-cultural
studies. Further research is needed not only to define the issues facing
families in different countries but also to provide a yardstick against
which change can be measured within cultures.
The stigma of shame. One clear message emerges from past research:
intellectual disability brings shame on families. A common response is
then to hide the person from the community and even the wider family
(Essop 2012; Haihambo and Lightfoot 2010). This response further rein-
forces the shamefulness associated with this disability that is still prevalent
in many societies. Intellectual disability is often interpreted as misfortune
befalling the family because of their misdeeds. Other families may prefer
to hide children or adults with intellectual disabilities because of physical
deformities or behavioral challenges (Ingstad 2001). Such reactions nega-
tively affect the growing child. A woman with Down syndrome in Cape
184 R. McConkey et al.
Town described how her mother reacted to the birth of a child with a
disability (quotations are from McKenzie and McConkey 2015):
My mother was in shock when they told her I was Down Syndrome. She
did not know how to handle me and so my mother cried for days. She
locked herself in a dark room and cried because she was in shock because
her child was disabled.
It is not easy to wash them, you have to lift her up, I suffered from back-
ache. I think they need to have separate bathroom that is equipped with
standing frame which will make easier for us to wash them. It is worse for
us staying in the shacks because we do not even have decent toilets.
Proactive Influences
The relationship between parents, especially mothers/grandmothers, and
the child with the disability is the engine that drives a proactive challenge to
existing attitudes. Oftentimes this is expressed when parents proclaim that
they will treat the child just like their other children. This is not to deny
their child’s additional needs, but rather it is recognition that the label of dis-
ability need not alter a ‘normal’ relationship with their child (Essop 2012).
Indeed, this distinction between the person and the label is fundamental to
reshaping societal responses to disability. Four main strategies have proved
effective in challenging negative perceptions of intellectual disability.
Nurturing development. Treating the child as any other sibling
means actively nurturing their development so as to assist them to gain
developmental milestones in mobility, communication and self-care,
albeit at a later age than their peers without disabilities (Einfeld et al.
2012). It also means taking the child outside the home and participat-
ing in family and community events despite the inevitable criticism and
disdain of others. Children who are nurtured in such supportive family
environments will hopefully escape most of the self-stigma that can be
associated with intellectual disability. Equally as they acquire the com-
petencies that others thought impossible, they in turn further convince
186 R. McConkey et al.
their families that disability need not constrain their child’s abilities, tal-
ents and personality.
Nonetheless much resolve and resilience is needed by parents to persist
in their beliefs and not to be defeated. In the African context mothers
frequently draw on their faith in God as their greatest support in coping
with their child’s disability (Essop 2012).
So I go down on my knees still every night and I believe, if you are sincere,
God would provide. Before I used to be a different person but, today with-
out Him I am lost. Every step I take is in His name.
Maintaining Motivation
Naming these four proactive strategies for changing perceptions does not
do justice to the vast amount of physical and emotional energy required
by parents to sustain these actions in the face of rejection and intransi-
gence. Many families are defeated by the task but fortunately in every
country there are parents who have persisted and shown remarkable lead-
ership that has benefited many thousands up to now and will do so for
millions more in the future.
188 R. McConkey et al.
Practical Actions
In this final section, we want to identify certain strategies especially suited
to developing countries that bring together the proactive influences we
have described and that are a counter to the reactive influences that many
families still experience.
Parent and friends associations. The value of parent associations in
changing perceptions within families and communities is well attested
internationally as they can combine a national presence founded on a
network of local groups. As already noted, they can be a source of emo-
tional and practical support to new parents, especially as they come to
terms with having a child with a disability. They can provide information
to counter ignorance and misrepresentations in wider society. They give
parents a sense of solidarity from which they can advocate individually or
corporately locally, nationally and internationally. The UN Convention
on the Rights of Persons with Disabilities was shaped through the involve-
ment of parental advocacy.
In countries such as Lesotho and Zanzibar, the associations are open
to professional ‘friends’, teachers and nurses, for example, as well as fam-
ily friends, such as siblings and grandparents, to create a broad coalition
of mutual support and widen their sphere of influence (McConkey and
Mariga 2010). Most associations exist on a self-help basis, but they can
be grown in localities with support from sympathetic professionals such
as community-based rehabilitation workers or personnel from national
parent associations. Training can be provided to members to equip them
to be community educators or to assist parents with income generation
(McConkey et al. 2000). Many present-day community-based support
services for people with disabilities were started by parent groups.
Parent associations are not without their difficulties and shortcomings.
In rural areas, villages are further apart and the availability and cost of
transport, allied to taking time away from farming activities, can prevent
families from attending meetings. Professionals have an important role
to play in identifying the support that families need to maintain their
resilience and to think of new ways of providing support to the persons
with an intellectual disability and their families, making use of available
12 Tackling Stigma in Developing Countries 189
access to health services and charges levied by minibus and taxi compa-
nies for transporting people in wheelchairs.
The role of traditional healers in combating stigma deserves particular
attention. Many families turn to them, yet they too lack understanding
about the causes of intellectual disability and fall back on supernatural
explanations, such as that disability is a punishment for the breaking of
tribal taboos by family members. These traditional healers could contrib-
ute positively to reducing stigma if they seek to understand and accept
other explanations (Kromberg et al. 2008).
Productive work. In many cultures, one’s human status is judged by
the extent to which people can participate in valued activities within that
society (Aldersey et al. 2014). The active participation of children and
young adults with intellectual disabilities in family and community life
therefore conveys a strong challenge to negative attitudes, especially when
they can be seen to be contributing to family life, such as undertaking
household tasks like fetching water and firewood or taking part in com-
munity activities such as team sports. In rural agrarian settings, most of
the livelihood activities do not require any abstract thinking, so someone
with a mild to moderate intellectual disability, who does not have physical
limitations, can be involved in subsistence farming, household chores and
most activities of daily living. Hence, parents and professionals need to
prepare children to become productive members of their community so
as to counter beliefs around their helplessness. Ultimately being accepted
within communities depends not on labels but on the relationships that
people forge with their peers. Hence, participation in schooling, sports
and religious activities enable others to relate to the person and the person
with them. Indeed, international evidence confirms that personal contact
is a potent force for challenging stigma (Macmillan et al. 2014).
Looking to the Future
In this chapter we have chosen to focus on the family as the channel for
tackling stigma. But theirs is not the only channel for change and indeed
family efforts need to be reinforced by legal rights, national policies that
are enforced and changes in discriminatory practices of professionals and
12 Tackling Stigma in Developing Countries 191
services to name but a few. We accept too that reliance on families and local
communities to tackle stigma will likely result in greater disparities within
countries in their attitudes to intellectual disability as there is unlikely to
be uniform implementation of common actions. We are also conscious of
placing extra burdens on families and implying that they are to blame for
the stigma their relative experiences. So in looking to the future, let’s be
clear of the key messages on tackling stigma. Our aim is to build relation-
ships between people who are perceived to be different with their families
and with their local communities. People’s negative reactions to the label
‘intellectual disability’ can be initially separated from their perceptions of
individuals with whom they have built a relationship. This process can be
proactively facilitated within families and communities, thereby reduc-
ing the stigma associated with the label. In so doing, societal responses to
intellectual disability will change over time and across cultures. In a nut-
shell, that is the journey we have inherited from previous generations and
it is one we must continue to advance in coalitions with all interested and
involved parties. But we continue to assert that central to these endeavors
is the leadership and example that families provide. It is they who have
brought us thus far on the journey and they who will see us home.
Accessible Summary
• People with intellectual disabilities are treated unfairly in every
country of the world and especially in poorer countries.
• Families have led the way in changing attitudes, despite the negative
reactions they experience from others.
• The support from other parents has helped families to promote more
positive attitudes in local communities.
• Village gatherings, radio broadcasts and support from community
leaders have been good ways of doing this.
• Families need support from professionals and politicians nationally to
match their local efforts.
References
Aldersey, H. M. (2012). Family perceptions of intellectual disability:
Understanding of support in Dar es Salaam. African Journal of Disability, 1,
1–12. doi:10.4102/ajod.v1i1.32.
Aldersey, H., Turnbull, A., & Turnbull, R. (2014). Factors contributing to con-
struction of personhood of individuals with intellectual and developmental
disabilities in Kinshasa, Democratic Republic of Congo. Canadian Journal of
Disability Studies, 3, 29–61. doi:10.15353/cjds.v3i2.156
Ali, A., Kock, E., Molteno, C., Mfiki, N., King, M., & Strydom, A. (2015).
Ethnicity and self-reported experiences of stigma in adults with intellectual
disability in Cape Town, South Africa. Journal of Intellectual Disability
Research, 59, 530–540. doi:10.1111/jir.12158.
Eide, A., & Ingstad, B. (2011). Disability and poverty: A global challenge. Bristol,
UK: Policy Press.
Einfeld, S. L., Stancliffe, R. J., Gray, K. M., Sofronoff, K., Rice, L., Emerson, E.,
et al. (2012). Interventions provided by parents for children with intellectual
disabilities in low and middle income countries. Journal of Applied Research in
Intellectual Disabilities, 25, 135–142. doi:10.1111/j.1468-3148.2011.00678.
Essop, F. (2012). Parents’ and special care workers’ understanding of “accep-
tance” and “denial” in relation to a child’s intellectual disability. In Unpublished
Master’s thesis. Disability Studies Division: University of Cape Town, South
Africa.
12 Tackling Stigma in Developing Countries 193
Haihambo, C., & Lightfoot, E. (2010). Cultural beliefs regarding people with
disabilities in Namibia: Implications for inclusion of people with disabilities.
International Journal of Education, 25, 76–87.
Ingstad, B. (2001). Disability in the developing world. In G. L. Albrecht, K. D.
Seelman, & M. Bury (Eds.), Handbook of disability studies (pp. 772–792).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Kahonde, C. (2015). A grounded theory study of family caregivers’ responses to
sexuality and relationship support needs of young adults with intellectual disabili-
ties. PhD thesis in preparation, University of Cape Town, South Africa.
Kelly, A., Ghalaieny, T., & Devitt, C. (2012). A pilot study of early intervention
for families with children with or at risk of an intellectual disability in
Northern Malawi. Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual Disabilities, 9,
195–205. doi:10.1111/j.1741-1130.2012.00354.
Kromberg, J., Zwane, E., Manga, P., Venter, A., Rosen, E., & Christianson, A.
(2008). Intellectual disability in the context of a South African population.
Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual Disabilities, 5, 89–95. doi:10.111
1/j.1741-1130.2008.00153.
Lansdown, G. (2002). Disabled children in South Africa: Progress in implementing
the Convention on the Rights of the Child. Report retrieved from www.daa.org.
uk/uploads/pdf/SA%20Childrens%20report%20.pdf
McConkey, R., Dunne, J., & Blitz, N. (2009). Shared lives: Building relation-
ships and community with people who have intellectual disabilities. Amsterdam,
Netherlands: Sense Publishers.
McConkey, R., & Mariga, L. (2010). Building social capital for inclusive educa-
tion: Insights from Zanzibar. Journal of Research in Special Educational Needs,
11, 12–19. doi:10.1111/j.1471-3802.2010.01174.
McConkey, R., Mariga, L., Braadland, N., & Mphole, P. (2000). Parents as
trainers about disability in low income countries. International Journal of
Disability, Development and Education, 47, 309–317. doi:10.1080/713671113.
McKenzie, J., & McConkey, R. (2015). Caring for adults with intellectual dis-
ability: The perspectives of family carers in South Africa. Journal of Applied
Research in Intellectual Disabilities. Advance online publication. doi:10.1111/
jar.12209.
McKenzie, J. A., McConkey, R., & Adnams, C. (2013). Intellectual disability in
Africa: Implications for research and service development. Disability and
Rehabilitation, 35, 1750–1755. doi:10.3109/09638288.2012.751461.
McKenzie, J., & Muller, B. (2006). Parents and therapists: Dilemmas in part-
nership. In B. Watermeyer, L. Swartz, T. Lorenzo, M. Schneider, &
194 R. McConkey et al.
M. Priestley (Eds.), Disability and social change: A South African agenda
(pp. 311–323). Cape Town, South Africa: Human Sciences Research Council
Press.
Macmillan, M., Tarrant, M., Abraham, C., & Morris, C. (2014). The associa-
tion between children’s contact with people with disabilities and their atti-
tudes towards disability: A systematic review. Developmental Medicine and
Child Neurology, 56, 529–546. doi:10.1111/dmcn.12326.
Mirza, I., Tareen, A., Davidson, L. L., & Rahman, A. (2009). Community man-
agement of intellectual disabilities in Pakistan: A mixed methods study.
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 53, 559–570.
doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2009.01176.
Parmenter, T. R. (2001). Intellectual disabilities—Quo vadis? In G. L. Albrecht,
K. D. Seelman, & M. Bury (Eds.), Handbook of disability studies (pp. 267–296).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Siperstein, G. N., Norins, J., Corbin, S., & Shriver, T. (2003). Multinational
study of attitudes towards individuals with intellectual disabilities. Washington,
DC: Special Olympics Inc.
Tarusarira, W. (2015). Support for Zimbabwean families with a disabled child
seeking asylum in South Africa. MPhil thesis in preparation: University of
Cape Town, South Africa.
World Health Organization & World Bank (2011). World report on disability.
Geneva, Switzerland: WHO.
13
The Law as a Source of Stigma
or Empowerment: Legal Capacity
and Persons with Intellectual Disabilities
Janos Fiala-Butora and Michael Ashley Stein
more difficult to meet as the stigma reflected in and created by the law is
not easy to overcome.
Prejudices against persons with disabilities—and specifically, the widely
held belief that persons with intellectual (and psychosocial) disabilities
are unable to manage their own affairs—are deeply socially entrenched.
For millennia these beliefs have manifested themselves in guardianship
laws which precluded persons with disabilities to make decisions on their
own. In a circular way, these laws also shaped societal conventions regard-
ing the misperceived inability of people with disabilities. Thus, guardian-
ship laws are not solely responsible for the image of a person with an
intellectual disability as incompetent; the prejudice is older and much
more entrenched.
The law is important, however, in the way prejudice against individu-
als with intellectual disabilities currently manifests. The guardianship
system deprives many otherwise capable people of their legal capac-
ity. They are considered incapable of independent life once they were
placed under guardianship, even if they had been able to work and live
alone before that legal procedure. Some persons may not even know
for years that they are legally incapacitated: they continue to take care
of themselves and go about their lives while in the eyes of the law they
are considered incapable of having those very same independent lives
(Sýkora v. the Czech Republic 2012). Once rights are taken away from
them, persons with intellectual disabilities gradually lose these skills,
and guardianship becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy: those deprived
of their legal capacity often become dependent on various forms of
assistance. Guardianship law, often arbitrarily administered, plays a
key role in determining who becomes a totally incapable, severely dis-
abled person in need of society’s help, and in defining the nature of that
dependency.
Nevertheless, and despite the unintended injurious effect, the law can
also have a positive influence on societal attitudes. The task of systemic
reform in this context is to put in place legal institutions that promote the
image of persons with disabilities as equally capable and empowered with
agency. However, abolishing all existing guardianship laws and replac-
ing them with support mechanisms, while helpful, is insufficient. This
is especially true for countries with an established guardianship system
13 The Law as a Source of Stigma or Empowerment 203
where the instantiated culture does not consider persons with disabilities
as autonomous. In those States, social workers, medical professionals,
public administration, guardians, and family members acting under cur-
rent guardianship regimes are all working under the view of persons with
disabilities as objects of care rather than individuals with decision-making
abilities. Simply abolishing guardianship will not immediately change
that attitude. To the contrary, they would likely respond by reinventing
the repressive institutional culture in a new legal framework by renaming
‘guardianship’ as ‘supported decision-making’. Thus, even changing the
criteria of guardianship so that it does not formally restrict legal capacity
is not enough.
Besides establishing a new general framework, legislators must also
identify specific obstacles in which this institutional culture manifests
itself and directly overrule them. Safeguards must be put in place so that
persons with disabilities can indeed make decisions which are currently
most often denied to them. Hence, legal reform has to affect the way
banks engage with their clients and offer loans, doctors talk to patients,
child custody is exercised by parents, public administration handles cus-
tomers, courts hear witnesses, and many other areas which currently con-
stitute a direct obstacle to persons with disabilities exercising their legal
capacity.
The law cannot in itself change embedded societal structures over-
night. That is a much more complex task, requiring a longer period of
time and other factors to be present. The law can and should, however,
establish the structure and create the instruments to allow society to
gradually accept and incorporate the notion that persons with disabili-
ties can make their own decisions. By recognizing persons with disabili-
ties as decision-makers under the law, supported decision-making forces
all other actors to communicate with them instead of their guardians.
Some will no doubt find this burdensome, but the law’s normative goal
should be to expand those areas where the wishes of persons with dis-
abilities will be honored rather than to exclude persons with disabilities
from decision-making on the ground of administrative convenience. In
this process it is crucial that we uncover the hidden assumptions that
continue to undermine the equal place and legal capacity of persons with
intellectual disabilities.
204 J. Fiala-Butora and M.A. Stein
Conclusions
This chapter has argued that the stigmatizing effects of the law are not
always evident. Legal institutions can directly contribute to building a
positive image of persons with intellectual disabilities by, for example,
prohibiting open discrimination against them or expressly securing their
fundamental rights. At the same time, legal institutions are built on incor-
rect assumptions about persons with intellectual disabilities. Through
their daily operation they allow these prejudicial assumptions to influ-
ence the social environment. This negative effect is harder to observe,
but nevertheless very damaging to the public perception of persons with
intellectual disabilities.
Guardianship is an example of a widely used legal mechanism that is
built on the idea of persons with intellectual disabilities as incapable of
managing their own affairs. Due to more than 2000 years of guardianship
laws, unfounded stereotypes regarding the inability of persons with intel-
lectual disabilities to make their own decisions has been deeply embed-
ded across cultures. Family members, medical and legal professionals,
service providers, and all kinds of caregivers have learned to ignore the
wishes of persons with intellectual disabilities, and instead to make deci-
sions on their behalves, to preserve their ‘best interests’.
Supported decision-making, a newly emerged alternative to guardian-
ship, shows that protecting the human rights of persons with intellectual
disabilities does not have to come at the cost of undermining their capac-
ity and social perception. Stigmatization is not a necessary cost of help,
but rather an obstacle to be overcome. Implementing supported decision-
making will be a long process, because it requires changing societal norms
about how to interact with persons with intellectual disabilities.
While this seems to be a daunting task, one must recall that existing
prejudicial attitudes were to a great extent created and are maintained
through the operation of guardianship laws. The law can therefore equally
play a role in dismantling those social constructs. To achieve this goal, the
task of legal reform should be to uncover hidden assumptions behind our
legal institutions that covertly contribute to stigmatizing persons with
intellectual disabilities. Guardianship serves as one example in this chap-
13 The Law as a Source of Stigma or Empowerment 205
Accessible Summary
• The law often treats persons with intellectual disabilities as unable to
make decisions for themselves.
• Guardianship stops persons with intellectual disabilities from making
their own decisions.
• Supported decision-making helps persons with intellectual disabilities
to make their own decisions.
• We need to replace guardianship with supported decision-making. It is
not easy, because many people are used to the old laws and think per-
sons with intellectual disabilities cannot make their own decisions.
• Changing laws can also help change people’s minds.
• The Convention on the Rights of Persons with Disabilities is pushing
countries to change their laws. We should use the Convention to
change laws that are bad for persons with intellectual disabilities.
References
Bach, M., & Kerzner, L. (2010). A new paradigm for protecting autonomy and the
right to legal Capacity. (Paper prepared for the Law Commission of Ontario).
Carney, T. (2012). Guardianship, “social” citizenship, & theorizing substitute
decision-making law. Sydney Law School Research Paper, 12/25.
Dhanda, A. (2007). Legal capacity in the disability rights convention:
Stranglehold of the past or lodestar for the future? Syracuse Journal of
International Law and Commerce, 34, 429–462.
Fiala-Butora, J. (2013). Disabling torture: The obligation to investigate ill-
treatment of persons with disabilities. Columbia Human Rights Law Review,
45, 214–280.
Fiala-Butora, J. (2015). Reconstructing personhood: Legal capacity of persons
with disabilities. SJD Dissertation, Harvard Law School, MA, forthcoming.
Fiala-Butora, J., Stein, M. A., & Lord, J. E. (2014). The democratic life of the
Union: Towards equal voting participation for Europeans with disabilities.
Harvard International Law Journal, 55, 71–104.
Gordon, R. M. (2000). The emergence of assisted (supported) decision-making
in the Canadian law of adult guardianship and substitute decision-making.
International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 23, 61–77.
13 The Law as a Source of Stigma or Empowerment 207
Legislation
Kiss v. Hungary, No. 38832/06 (Eur. Ct. Hum. Rts., May 20, 2010).
Plesó v. Hungary, No. 41242/08 (Eur. Ct. Hum. Rts., October 2, 2012).
Salontaji-Drobnjak v. Serbia, no. 36500/05 (Eur. Ct. Hum. Rts., October 13,
2009).
Sýkora v. the Czech Republic, No. 23419/07 (Eur. Ct. Hum. Rts., 22 November
2012).
X. v. Croatia, no 11223/04, (Eur. Ct. Hum. Rts., July 17, 2008).
14
Intellectual Disability, Group
Identification, and Self-Evaluation
Jason Crabtree, William Mandy, and Hannah Mustard
J. Crabtree (
)
Westminster Learning Disabilities Service, London, UK
e-mail: [email protected]
W. Mandy
Division of Psychology & Language Sciences, University College London,
London, UK
H. Mustard
Central and North West London NHS Foundation Trust, London, UK
Theoretical Background
Early models of self-identity such as Cooley’s (1902) ‘Looking Glass’ self
and Mead’s (1934) Symbolic Interactionist perspective suggested that
the self cannot be separated from the society in which it is located. As
a result, individuals’ self-evaluations are considered a direct consequence
of the views that others hold of them. Similarly, Gergen’s (1977) Social
Constructionist theory proposes that a person’s self-evaluations are formed
through social interactions. Accordingly, individuals with intellectual dis-
abilities will simply interpret stigma held by others toward them as a reflec-
tion of their ‘true’ value and internalize, leading to negative self-evaluations.
Research has found that the self-evaluations made by individuals
belonging to a number of stigmatized groups are frequently comparable
to those of individuals not belonging to stigmatized groups (e.g., ethnic
minority groups, Verkuyten 1994; children with mild intellectual disabil-
ities, Crabtree and Rutland 2001; individuals with mental health prob-
lems, Hayward and Bright 1997). It therefore seems too crude to suggest
that merely belonging to a stigmatized group leads to low self-evaluation
(Camp et al. 2002).
Social Identity Theory (Tajfel and Turner 1979) offers a more complex
understanding of the development of self-identity and evaluation and
accounts for how members of stigmatized groups may maintain positive
self-evaluations. It suggests that individuals’ self-evaluations depend on
the views that society has of their group and how this compares to other
social groups. Therefore, positive self-evaluations can be achieved when
it is possible to make favorable comparisons between one’s in-group and
out-groups. Where this is not possible, as in the case of belonging to a
stigmatized group, individuals’ resultant self-evaluations are likely to be
negative. The theory postulates that individuals strive to maintain posi-
tive self-evaluations, and therefore may seek to distance themselves from
the stigmatized in-group and align themselves with more socially val-
ued groups. In the case of individuals who have no choice in leaving the
group (i.e., the group has no ‘permeability’), Tajfel (1978) suggested that
individuals may promote positive self-evaluation through engaging in
group action to bring about social change. However, a high level of group
identification would appear to be a necessary prerequisite for such action.
14 Group Identification and Self-evaluation 211
come into play, with those who rated themselves as more socially attrac-
tive and able reporting higher self-evaluations. This again suggests that
downward social comparisons have a protective effect on self-evaluations
when an individual identifies with a stigmatized group.
Conclusions
From the research presented in this chapter it appears that the relation-
ship between group identification and self-evaluation in people with
intellectual disabilities is complex. Firstly, some individuals do not explic-
itly appear to acknowledge the label of intellectual disability as applying
to themselves (Finlay and Lyons 1998), potentially as a means of distanc-
ing themselves from belonging to a stigmatized group.
Secondly, for those individuals identifying with the group the evidence
presented suggests that there are a number of mechanisms afforded to
them by group membership that appear to buffer the potentially nega-
tive consequences of belonging to a stigmatized group. These include (1)
comparisons with less able in-group members; (2) mediating the value
placed on attributes based on the performance of the in-group compared
to other groups; and (3) shared group identification to reject stigma.
In terms of clinical implications and future research directions, it seems
important to consider how those with intellectual disabilities compare
to other groups for whom initiatives to reduce stigma and protect self-
evaluations are more developed. There are certainly similarities between
stigma research in the mental health arena and research involving people
with intellectual disabilities in terms of both populations’ awareness of
stigma. However, there appear to be some differences between the two
groups in how self-evaluation is protected. There seems to be more of
an emphasis in the mental health literature on the impact of social sup-
port from other group members, whether from defined support groups
or through a sense of belonging to a wider community, in mediating the
relationship between stigma and self-evaluation (Crabtree et al. 2010;
Watson et al. 2007). This contrasts with the findings from the intellectual
disability literature, which suggests that people assigned to this group are
more likely to try and distance themselves from other members, whether
14 Group Identification and Self-evaluation 217
Accessible Summary
• Having intellectual disabilities does not mean people inevitably feel
bad about themselves.
• People with intellectual disabilities use different ways to feel good
about themselves:
• Comparing themselves to less able people or people who behave in
disruptive ways
• Placing more importance on the things they do well
• Sometimes not seeing themselves as having an intellectual
disability
• More research is needed to look at the benefits peer support has on
how people with intellectual disabilities feel about themselves.
References
Branscombe, N. R., Schmitt, M. T., & Harvey, R. D. (1999). Perceiving perva-
sive discrimination among African-Americans: Implications for group iden-
tification and wellbeing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77,
135–149. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.1.135.
Camp, D. L., Finlay, W. M., & Lyons, E. (2002). Is low self-esteem an inevitable
consequence of stigma? An example of women with chronic mental health
problems. Social Science and Medicine, 55, 823–834. doi:10.1016/
S0277-9536(01)00205-2.
Cooley, C. H. (1902). Two major works: Social organization and human nature
and the social order. Glencoe, UK: Free Press.
Crabtree, J. W., Haslam, S. A., Postmes, T., & Haslam, C. (2010). Mental
health support groups, stigma and self-esteem: Positive and negative implica-
14 Group Identification and Self-evaluation 219
Since the days of the almost complete denial of the personhood and
rights of people with intellectual disabilities and their large-scale confine-
ment in institutions in many countries, much progress has been made.
Many no longer question the human-ness of persons with intellectual
disabilities and the days of their physical segregation from society appear
numbered in many, but by no means all, parts of the world. In the fight
for respect and acceptance, the UN Convention on the Rights of Persons
with Disabilities (CRPD) can be seen as a very important step forward,
though one that needs to be accompanied by action on many levels. The
inclusion of persons with intellectual disabilities under the Convention,
at least theoretically, commits signatory states to enact domestic laws and
measures to improve their rights and to abolish legislation, customs, and
practices that discriminate against them. Nevertheless, the experts from
various fields brought together in this book are unanimous in concluding
that we are a long way from abolishing stigma and accepting children and
adults with intellectual disabilities fully into society. For example, while
in many Western countries there appears to be a growing willingness
among the general public to include people with intellectual disabilities
within their communities, there are many signs that there is a continuing
Prejudice(s), 5, 10, 16, 19, 22, 50, Segregation, 17, 35, 41, 42, 78,
77, 82, 86, 87, 91, 96, 112, 120, 221
113, 115, 119, 139, 141, Self-advocacy, 50, 52, 55, 57, 131,
142, 166, 199, 202, 205, 154, 168–75, 222
211, 213 Self-determination, 32, 39–40, 42, 222
Professionals, 18, 23, 37–9, 50, Self-disclosure/ disclosure,
70–2, 82, 83, 85, 87, 103, 103, 106, 130
136, 153, 155, 169, 171, Self-esteem, 7, 57, 91, 94–7, 101,
181, 187, 188, 190, 192, 102, 104, 106, 185, 212–16
203, 204 Self-stigma, 32, 41, 49, 50, 56, 57,
Public stigma, 37, 56, 91, 92 91–107, 130, 131, 151,
182, 185, 213, 215, 217
Service providers/ service provision,
Q 33, 42, 57, 129, 204, 217
Quality of life, 37, 39, 41, 42, 93, Services, 18, 37, 38, 81, 84, 86, 95,
104–6, 169, 174 99, 103, 105–7, 138, 171,
179, 187, 188, 190, 191,
213, 217, 222, 223
R Sex(es), 38, 78
Research, 5, 16, 32, 50, 64, 79, Sexual relations, 32, 38, 39, 79, 113,
91, 132, 150, 168, 180, 114, 116, 117, 119
209, 223 Social media, 36, 132
Rights/human rights, 4, 5, 7, 9, Social model, 135, 166–8, 174
17, 18, 31–3, 35, Social psychology/ psychologist, 6, 7,
38–42, 55, 66, 77–87, 10, 19, 140
115, 116, 120, 133, Societal views, 62, 129, 209, 211
139, 142, 165–9, 172, Society, 4, 6, 7, 9, 10, 21, 53, 56, 57,
174, 175, 187, 188, 62–4, 68, 70, 72, 78, 79,
190, 195–9, 202, 204–6, 120, 129, 139, 140, 165,
221–3 172, 180–3, 188, 190, 191,
202, 203, 210, 221, 222
Special Olympics, 61, 63, 64, 66, 69,
S 132, 134, 224
Scale(s), 15–24, 97 Stereotype/ stereotypes, 5, 6, 16, 19,
School(s), 17, 19, 34, 66, 71, 72, 93, 23, 61–3, 68, 72, 73, 77,
100, 101, 105, 113, 133, 86, 91, 92, 96, 99–102,
137, 138, 149–61, 171, 105, 132, 139, 142, 150,
186, 189, 190, 214, 222 196, 199, 204, 205
230 Index