(Journal of Property Investment Finance.) Behavi (B-Ok - CC)
(Journal of Property Investment Finance.) Behavi (B-Ok - CC)
(Journal of Property Investment Finance.) Behavi (B-Ok - CC)
Journal of
Property Investment
& Finance
Issue 4 is a special issue on Behavioural real estate
Guest Editor: Deborah Levy
www.emeraldinsight.com
Journal of ISSN 1463-578X
Volume 22
Property Investment & Number 4
2004
Finance
Behavioural real estate
Guest Editor
Deborah S. Levy
As a subscriber to this journal, you can benefit from instant, Additional complementary services available
electronic access to this title via Emerald Fulltext. Your Your access includes a variety of features that add to
access includes a variety of features that increase the value the functionality and value of your journal subscription:
of your journal subscription.
E-mail alert services
How to access this journal electronically These services allow you to be kept up to date with the latest
To benefit from electronic access to this journal you first need additions to the journal via e-mail, as soon as new material
to register via the Internet. Registration is simple and full enters the database. Further information about the services
instructions are available online at www.emeraldinsight.com/ available can be found at
rpsv/librariantoolkit/emeraldadmin Once registration is www.emeraldinsight.com/usertoolkit/ emailalerts
completed, your institution will have instant access to all
articles through the journal’s Table of Contents page at Emerald WIRE (World Independent Reviews)
www.emeraldinsight.com/1463-578X.htm More information A fully searchable subject specific database, brought to you
about the journal is also available at by Emerald Management Reviews, providing article reviews
www.emeraldinsight.com/ jpif.htm from the world’s top management journals.
Our liberal institution-wide licence allows everyone within your Research register
institution to access your journal electronically, making your A web-based research forum that provides insider
subscription more cost effective. Our Web site has been information on research activity world-wide located at
designed to provide you with a comprehensive, simple system www.emeraldinsight.com/researchregister You can
that needs only minimum administration. Access is available also register your research activity here.
via IP authentication or username and password.
User services
Comprehensive librarian and user toolkits have been created to
Key features of Emerald electronic journals help you get the most from your journal subscription.
For further information about what is available
Automatic permission to make up to 25 copies of individual visit www.emeraldinsight.com/usagetoolkit
articles
This facility can be used for training purposes, course notes, Choice of access
seminars etc. This only applies to articles of which Emerald Electronic access to this journal is available via a number of
owns copyright. For further details visit channels. Our Web site www.emeraldinsight.com is the
www.emeraldinsight.com/ copyright recommended means of electronic access, as it provides fully
searchable and value added access to the complete content
Online publishing and archiving of the journal. However, you can also access and search the
As well as current volumes of the journal, you can also gain article content of this journal through the following journal
access to past volumes on the internet via Emerald Fulltext. delivery services:
You can browse or search the database for relevant articles.
EBSCOHost Electronic Journals
Service ejournals.ebsco.com
Non-article content
Huber E-Journals
Material in our journals such as product information, industry e-journals.hanshuber.com/english/index.htm
trends, company news, conferences, etc. is available online
and can be accessed by users. Ingenta
www.ingenta.com
Key readings Minerva Electronic Online
This feature provides abstracts of related articles chosen by Services www.minerva.at
the journal editor, selected to provide readers with current OCLC FirstSearch
awareness of interesting articles from other publications in the www.oclc.org/firstsearch
field. SilverLinker
www.ovid.com
Reference linking SwetsWise
Direct links from the journal article references to abstracts of www.swetswise.com
the most influential articles cited. Where possible, this link is
to the full text of the article. Emerald Customer Support
For customer support and technical help contact:
E-mail an article E-mail [email protected]
Allows users to e-mail links to relevant and interesting articles to Web www.emeraldinsight.com/customercharter
another computer for later use, reference or printing purposes. Tel +44 (0) 1274 785278 Fax +44 (0) 1274
785204
EDITORIAL ADVISORY BOARD Editorial
Professor Alastair Adair Dr Yu Shi Ming advisory board
University of Ulster, Northern Ireland National University of Singapore
Dr Richard Barkham Dr Seow Eng Ong
Grosvenor Estate, London, UK National University of Singapore
Professor Andrew Baum Professor John Ratcliffe
The University of Reading, UK Dublin Institute of Technology, Eire
291
Peter Byrne Dr Tim Richards
The University of Reading, UK Co´ras Iompair Eireann, Dublin, Eire
Professor Neil Crosby Professor Jon Robinson
The University of Reading, UK University of Melbourne, Australia
Eamonn D’Arcy Professor Stephen Roulac
The University of Reading, UK Roulac Group, USA and University of Ulster,
Professor Julian Diaz III Northern Ireland
Georgia State University, Atlanta, USA Dr Ed Schuck
Dr Tim Dixon Frank Russell Company New Zealand
College of Estate Management, Reading, UK Dr Karen Sieracki
Professor Paul Gallimore KASPAR Associates, Tunbridge Wells, UK
Nottingham Trent University, UK Andrew Smith
Professor Robin Goodchild Aberdeen Property Investors, London, UK
LaSalle Investment Management, London, UK Professor Dogan Tirtiroglu
Dr Liow Kim Hiang Concordia University, Quebec, Canada
National University of Singapore, Singapore Sotiris Tsolacos
Professor Martin Hoesli Jones Lang LaSalle
University of Geneva, Switzerland and University Professor Charles W.R. Ward
of Aberdeen, UK The University of Reading, UK
Geoff Keogh Professor James R. Webb
University of Aberdeen, UK Cleveland State University, USA
Tony Key Dr Larry Wofford
City University, UK C&L Systems Corp., Tulsa, USA
Professor Colin Lizieri Professor Elaine Worzala
The University of Reading, UK University of San Diego, USA
Professor Bryan MacGregor Dr Peter Wyatt
University of Aberdeen, UK University of the West of England, Bristol, UK
George Matysiak
The University of Reading, UK
References
Adams, D., Disberry, A., Hutchison, N. and Munjoma, T. (2001), “Urban redevelopment:
contextual influences and landowner behaviour”, Journal of Property Research, Vol.
18 No. 3, pp. 217-34.
Diaz, J. III (1999), “The first decade of behavioral research in the discipline of property”,
Journal of Property Investment & Finance, Vol. 17 No. 4, pp. 326-32.
Diaz, J. III and Hansz, J.A. (2001), “The use of reference points in valuation judgement”,
Journal of Property Research, Vol. 18 No. 2, pp. 141-8.
Gallimore, P. (1999), “Editorial”, Journal of Property Investment & Finance, Vol. 17 No. 4, p. 1. Editorial
Gallimore, P. and Gray, A. (2002), “The role of investor sentiment in property investment
decisions”, Journal of Property Research, Vol. 19 No. 2, pp. 111-20.
Gallimore, P. and Wolverton, M. (2000), “The objective in valuation: a study of the influence
of client feedback”, Journal of Property Research, Vol. 17 No. 1, pp. 47-57.
Gallimore, P., Hansz, J.A. and Gray, A. (2000), “Decision making in small property
companies”, Journal of Property Investment & Finance, Vol. 18 No. 6, pp. 602-12. 297
Green, L. and Kagel, J.H. (1987), Advances in Behavioral Economics, Ablex Publishing,
Westport, CT.
Hansz, J.A. and Diaz, J. III (2001), “Valuation bias and commercial appraisal: a transaction
price feedback experiment”, Real Estate Economics, Vol. 29 No. 4, pp. 553-65.
Havard, T.M. (2001), “An experimental evaluation of the effect of data presentation on heuristic bias
in commercial valuation”, Journal of Property Research, Vol. 18 No. 1, pp. 51-67.
Kassajian, H.H. and Robertson, T.S. (1973), Perspectives in Consumer Behavior, rev. ed.,
Scott, Foresman and Company, Glenview, IL.
Levy, D. and Henry, M. (2001), “An analysis of published property research methodologies
and methods”, paper presented at “Cutting Edge 2001” Property Research Conference of
the Royal Institution of Chartered Surveyors, Oxford.
Levy, D. and Lee, C.C.-K. (2000), “Family influence in decision making processes for
housing”, paper presented at “Cutting Edge 2000” Property Research Conference of the
Royal Institution of Chartered Surveyors, London.
Levy, D. and Schuck, E. (1999), “The influence of clients on valuations”, Journal of Property
Investment & Finance, Vol. 17 No. 4, pp. 380-400.
McFadyen, J. and Wood, H. (1991), Economics – Psychological Aspects, Elsevier Science
Publishers, Amsterdam.
Shiller, R.J. (2000), Irrational Exuberance, Princeton University Press, Princeton NJ.
Webb, J.R. (2000), “An inquiry into the professional self image of real estate agents”, Journal
of Real Estate Research, Vol. 20 Nos 1/2, pp. 153-77.
Wolverton, M.L. and Gallimore, P. (1999), “Client feedback the role of the appraiser”, Journal
of Real Estate Research, Vol. 18 No. 3, pp. 415-31.
The Emerald Research Register for this journal is available at The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available at
www.emeraldinsight.com/researchregister www.emeraldinsight.com/1463-578X.htm
JPIF
22,4 Identifying the dimensions to retail
centre image
Valerie Kupke
298 School of International Business, University of South Australia,
Adelaide, Australia
Received January 2002 Keywords Perception, Retail marketing, Shopping centres
Accepted December 2002
Abstract First feelings and first impressions largely govern and delimit the kinds of
experiences one expects and seeks. This paper reviews the application of multidimensional
scaling (MDS) to retail market research and discusses how MDS is able to map these
impressions and hence explain the retail centre image held by consumers. It proposes that
retail centre image is not just relevant to marketers and retailers, but is also of importance to
property managers who need an informed and holistic consumer orientation in order to
create and sustain property value. By means of illustration MDS is applied to data based on
shopper evaluations of seven retail centre items and an interpretation the results is offered.
Introduction
Zajonc (1980) argued that “we are never wrong about what we like or dislike” and
that once formed, an evaluation is not readily revoked. He argued against the notion,
that before we can like something we must know what it is, that objects must be
understood before they can be evaluated. Instead Zajonc suggested it was possible to
like something without knowing precisely what it was. Newell and Simon (1972, in
Hardin, 1999) have suggested that while an individual may interpret a task and try to
formulate it into a known item, through comprehension and problem space definition,
processes Zajonc describes as “cold cognition”, they also recognise that information
processing has an important preconscious component. Information, they suggest, is
processed preconsciously even before humans are conscious of the presence of
information (Hardin, 1999). Ittelson (1974) argues that first feelings and first
impressions largely govern the directions taken and delimits the kinds of experiences
one expects and seeks.
These first feelings and impressions are a key factor in understanding retail consumer
behaviour. The limited nature of the human attention span results in the need for the
selective processing of information especially within retail environments that offer
multiple stimuli and choices (Lindquist, 1973; Phillips et al., 1997). Thus initial
perceptions play an important role in explaining the images held by consumers of retail
outlets, images which in turn account for customer evaluations and patronage. This paper
reviews multidimensional scaling (MDS) as a market research methodology that is capable
of mapping out the “feelings” or perceptions held by shoppers in terms of retail images.
MDS attempts to detect the constructs that frame the highly selective shopping process by
identifying the key elements of a store image which are retained by the consumer and
Journal of Property Investment &
influence their purchaser behaviour. This paper emphasises that a research methodology
Finance which offers a better understanding of store image should correspond to a more holistic
Vol. 22 No. 4, 2004
pp. 298-306 approach to the management of retail centres, management which offers competitive
q Emerald Group Publishing advantage by being more consumer focused. It proposes that retail centre image is not just
Limited 1463-578X
DOI 10.1108/14635780410550858 relevant to marketers and retailers, but is
also of increasing importance to property managers who need an informed consumer Retail centre
orientation in order to create and sustain property value. The paper begins by image
reviewing the contribution of research within human geography to behavioural studies
of property market decision making as well as acknowledging the behavioural focus
of recent property research.
Literature review
299
The importance of the preconscious and the emotional has been recognised within the
context of property markets by human geographers over a number of years. This discipline
group offers useful insights for those interested in the application of behavioural studies to
real estate decision making as it focuses on spatial outcomes and built environments.
Writers such as Brown and Moore (1970) and Clark and Cadwallader (1973) have
postulated the subjective and idiosyncratic nature of housing market search and residential
mobility. Simon (1952, 1957) offered the framework of bounded rationality and decision
making as “satisficing” behaviour. Clark and Smith (1982) argued that the household
search for a new home is very selective, based primarily on existing feelings for, and
awareness of a known locality or action space and is aimed at satisfying rather than
optimising utility. Brown and Moore (1970) postulated the classic three stage model of
stress, search and evaluation as an explanation for housing location choice. Smith and
Mertz (1980) defined a decision frontier to include change in preferences and
opportunities throughout the housing search period with special reference to the influence
of real estate agents. Potter (1977) applied Horton and Reynolds’ (1971) distinction
between action space and activity space to shopping behaviour to theorise on consumer
information fields versus usage fields. Downs (1970) argued that in terms of explaining
shopping centre patronage store image was very complex and that the tangible and
intangible elements of a centre could not be separated easily. Rushton (1969, 1976)
developed the revealed space preference approach which proposed that consumers rank
alternative shopping centres on a scale of internally constructed “preferredness”. Rushton
argued that shopping behaviour could be predicted from a knowledge of preferences and
that these preferences could be measured by means of MDS of pair wise comparison data.
Timmermans et al. (1982) used the MDS approach to suggest that size and accessibility
were the aspects of shopping centres that individuals consider most often.
303
Figure 1.
Aggregated perception
map Shopping Centre A
Figure 2.
Aggregated perception
map Shopping Centre B
JPIF dimension. Along Dimension 2 prices and atmosphere are positive while service is
22,4 strongly negative with fast food and opening hours in the middle range. This would
suggest that Dimension 1 is depicting the functional or physical aspects of the centre
based primarily on accessibility issues. Dimension 2 which captures atmosphere
relative to service, would appear to represent the more intangible qualities of the centre
based on the emotional response of customers to the centre. This interpretation is
304 supported by Martineau’s (1958) evaluation of a centre image or “personality” based on
a functional dimension linked to service items and on a psychological dimension linked
to feelings of comfort and friendliness. It also ties in with explanations by Lindquist
(1973) and Mazursky and Jacoby (1986) who have linked retail image to an external
dimension based on parking and location items and an internal dimension related to
visual content and internal experience of the store. One of the strongest impressions
Centre A is making on shoppers is in terms of accessibility, either good or bad
depending on the item. Another strong component of retail image is based upon the
quality of the internal shopping experience which again is either positive or negative
depending on the item.
Figure 2 shows that for Shopping Centre B parking is a strong but single positive
along Dimension 1 with all the other attributes positioned closely together on the
negative side. Along Dimension 2 service is strongly positive, followed by price with
atmosphere strongly negative. In this case Dimension 2 is likely to be depicting the
more subjective aspects of the centre. However Dimension 1 is less easily identified.
Again it may be capturing the functional qualities of the centre. The quality of the
internal shopping experience or “feel” of the centre would appear to be an extremely
important in terms of Centre B. Again depending on the item this can be strongly
positive as with service, or strongly negative as for atmosphere. The functional
aspects of the centre in term of location and access do not appear to be as important in
terms of retail image.
Thus the dimensions which are the elements of retail image are similar for both
outlets. However shopper impressions of Centre A are more strongly aligned to
functional form including access and location, while the image of Centre B is linked
strongly to the less tangible aspects of the retail centre. Both these images tie in with
the built form of the centre; A has only internal shopping while B retains some
external opportunities. This serves to support the validity of MDS in capturing
shopper perception. In terms of ongoing management it is important to recognise that
for shoppers using Centre A accessibility leaves a particularly strong impression
either good or bad. For those managing Centre B it is should be important to
appreciate that the internal shopping experience at this centre leaves customers with a
very strong feeling either positive or negative. Such interpretations would be
especially useful for managers if images could be compared over time across
competitive centres and between tenants and customers. The impact of changes such
as tenant mix, refits or new stores on shopper evaluation could be monitored and
mapped along with levels of patronage.
Conclusion
MDS requires caution in interpretation but can offer interesting insights into market
evaluations of property attributes. It is recognised that for retail centre participants
MDS is only a first step in appraising the imagery or “in the head” constructs of these
environments but it does provide guidelines as to the direction of further research and Retail centre
should be accepted at least as an exploratory research tool. It enables a feel for the image
less tangible aspects of a market with findings and suggestions that can be explored
using other techniques such as conjoint or correspondence analysis. Property
management which is not only tenant, but also customer oriented, will recognise that
MDS provides an important step in identifying people’s first impressions of real
estate environments, of how they “feel” about property attributes. This evaluation of 305
attributes is important where rationality may not necessarily determine choice.
References
Birtwistle, G., Clarke, I. and Freathy, P. (1999), “Store image in the UK fashion sector:
consumer versus retailer perceptions”, The International Review of Retail,
Distribution & Consumer Research, Vol. 9 No. 1, pp. 1-16.
Brown, L.A. and Moore, E.G. (1970), “The intra urban migration process, a perspective”,
Geografiska Annaler, Vol. 52B, pp. 1-13.
Burt, S. and Carralero-Encinas, J. (2000), “The role of store image in retail internationalism”,
International Marketing Review, Vol. 17 No. 1, pp. 433-53.
Clark, B. (2000), “Managerial perceptions of marketing performance: efficiency, adaptability,
effectiveness and satisfaction”, Journal of Strategic Marketing, Vol. 8 No. 1, pp. 3-25.
Clark, W. and Cadwallader, M. (1973), “Locational stress and residential mobility”, Environment
& Behaviour, Vol. 5 No. 1, pp. 29-41.
Clark, W. and Smith, T.R. (1982), “Housing market search behaviour and expected utility theory:
the process of search”, Environment & Planning A, Vol. 14, pp. 717-38.
Downs, R.M. (1970), “The cognitive structure of an urban shopping centre”, Environment &
Behaviour, Vol. 2, pp. 13-39.
Ferguson, E. and Kerrin, M. (1997), “The use of multidimensional scaling: a cognitive mapping
technique in occupational settings”, Journal of Managerial Psychology, Vol. 12 No. 3,
pp. 204-14.
Hair, J., Anderson, R., Tatham, R. and Black, W. (1998), Multivariate Data Analysis, 5th ed.,
Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.
Hansen, R. and Deutscher, T. (1977), “An empirical investigation of attribute importance in
retail store selection”, Journal of Retailing, Vol. 53 No. 4, pp. 59-72, 95.
Hardin, W. (1999), “Behavioural research into heurisitics and bias as an academic pursuit”,
Journal of Property Investment & Finance, Vol. 17 No. 4, pp. 333-52.
Horner, P.H. (1984), “Research into vacant land values Burbank-Crafers arterial road”, The
Valuer, Vol. 28 No. 1, pp. 2-12.
Horton, F.E. and Reynolds, D.R. (1971), “Effects of urban spatial structure on individual
behaviour”, Economic Geography, Vol. 47 No. 1, pp. 36-48.
Hourihan, K. (1979), “The evaluation of urban neighbourhoods”, Environment & Planning,
Vol. 11, pp. 1355-67.
Howard, E. (1997), “The management of shopping centres: conflict or collaboration?”, The International
Review of Retail, Distribution & Consumer Research, Vol. 7 No. 3, pp. 263-85.
Ittelson, W.H. (1974), An Introduction to Environmental Psychology, Holt Rinehart &
Winston, New York, NY.
Jain, A. and Etgar, M. (1977), “Measuring store image through multidimensional scaling of
free response data”, Journal of Retailing, Vol. 52 No. 4, pp. 61-71.
JPIF Jones, K. and Biasiotto, M. (1999), “Internet retailing: current hype or future reality?”, The International
Review of Retail, Distribution and Consumer Research, Vol. 9 No. 1, pp. 69-79.
22,4
Levy, D.S. (1995), “Modern marketing research techniques and the property professional”,
Property Management, Vol. 13 No. 3, pp. 33-40.
Lindquist, J.D. (1973), “Meaning of image”, Journal of Retailing, Vol. 50, pp. 29-38.
Martineau, P. (1958), “The personality of the retail store”, Harvard Business Review, Vol.
306 36 No. 1, pp. 47-55.
May, E.G. (1973), Management Application of Retail Image Research, Massachusetts
Marketing Science Institute, Cambridge, MA.
Mazursky, D. and Jacoby, J. (1986), “Exploring the development of store image”, Journal of
Retailing, Vol. 62 No. 2, pp. 145-65.
Norusis, M.J. (1993) SPSS for Windows Professional Statistics Release 6.0, SPSS, Chicago, IL.
Oppewal, H. and Timmermans, H. (1997), “Retailer self-perceived store image and competitive
position”, The International Review of Retail, Distribution and Consumer
Research, Vol. 7 No. 1, pp. 41-59.
Parker, D. (1997), “Spatial relationships between determinants of the capitalization rate”, paper
presented at the 13th Annual American Real Estate Society Meeting, Sarasota, FL.
Phillips, H., Broderick, A. and Thompson, P. (1997), “Perception, selectivity and decision
making at the point of sale”, The International Review of Retail, Distribution and
Consumer Research, Vol. 7 No. 1, pp. 79-89.
Potter, R.B. (1977), “The nature of consumer usage fields in an urban environment: theoretical
and empirical perspectives”, Tijdschrift voor Economische en Sociale Geografie,
Vol. 68, pp. 168-76.
Rushton, G. (1969), “Analysis of behavior by revealed space preference”, Annals of the
Association of American Geographer, Vol. 59, pp. 391-400.
Rushton, G. (1976), “Decomposition of space preference functions”, in Golledge, R.G. and
Rushton, G. (Eds), Spatial Choices and Spatial Behaviour, Ohio State University
Press, Colombus, OH, pp. 119-34.
Simon, H.A. (1952), “A behavioral model of rational choice”, Quarterly Journal of
Economics, Vol. 69, pp. 99-118.
Simon, H.A. (1957), Models of Man: Social and Rational, Wiley, New York, NY.
Smith, T.R. and Mertz, F. (1980), “An analysis of the effects of information revision on the
outcomes of housing market search, with special reference to the influence of realty
agents”, Environment & Planning A, Vol. 12, pp. 155-74.
Timmermans, H., Van Der Heijden, R. and Westerveld, H. (1982), “Cognition of urban
retailing structures: a Dutch case study”, Tijdschrift voor Economische en Sociale
Geografie, Vol. 73, pp. 2-12.
Zajonc, R.B. (1980), “Feeling and thinking; preferences need no inferences”, American
Psychologist, Vol. 35 No. 2, pp. 151-75.
The Emerald Research Register for this journal is available at The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available at
www.emeraldinsight.com/researchregister www.emeraldinsight.com/1463-578X.htm
Decision-making
The decision-making behaviour of behaviour
office occupiers
Chris Leishman
Department of Building Engineering and Surveying, Heriot-Watt 307
University, Riccarton, UK
Craig Watkins Received January 2002
Accepted December 2002
Department of Land Economy, University of Aberdeen, Kings College,
Aberdeen, UK
Keywords Decision making, Office buildings
Abstract Typically, studies of the occupiers’ choice of office property have focused on the
influence of location. Following the standard behavioural assumptions of neo-classical
economics, the firm is assumed to make the rational profit-maximising decision on the basis of full
information. All firms are implicitly assumed to be homogeneous. This general approach
eliminates much of the complexity from the decision-making process. This paper uses evidence
from a survey of over 100 office occupiers in Edinburgh to examine the influence of a broader
range of factors on individual firms’ choice of office. Using logistic regression techniques on the
survey data, the empirical analysis shows that by taking account of heterogeneity of firms, it is
possible to identify the type of property occupied. Firms’ decisions are closely related to their size,
business type and whether the market they serve is local, regional or national.
1. Introduction
From a contents analysis of the major US and UK real estate journals, Levy and
Henry (2001) provide evidence of the increasing methodological domination of
applied neo-classical economics. Although, in a recent review article, Diaz (1999)
highlighted the mutli-disciplinary nature of UK property research, and, in particular,
work on the development process which adopts institutionalist and structure-agency
perspectives, this new evidence suggests that published work in British outlets is
starting to follow the lead from the USA where the neo-classical approach has
dominated for three decades.
Clapp and Myers (2000) outline the extent to which urban property research has
undergone a Kuhnian paradigm shift. The rich tradition of applied “real estate and urban
land economics” that can traced from James A. Graaskamp, through Richard Ratcliff,
Homer Hoyt and Ernest Fisher back to Richard T. Ely was overthrown by the urban
economics paradigm (or the new urban economics (NUE)) in the 1960s and 1970s (Weiss,
2000). This change heralded the substitution of inductive methods, based on the direct
observation of facts, by deductive modes of thought that deploy a set of assumptions in
order to deduce logical conclusions. The adoption of this mode of reasoning imposes
constraints on the types of research questions that might be asked and the methods that can
usefully be applied. Consequently researchers have focused on property market outcomes Journal of Property Investment &
(such as prices, and levels of development activity) and have sought to derive and test Finance
Vol. 22 No. 4, 2004
parsimonious explanations of these observed phenomena. Typically this has been based on pp. 307-319
the development of increasingly sophisticated econometric models. The process, by which q Emerald Group Publishing Limited
1463-578X
observed prices and output DOI 10.1108/14635780410550876
JPIF levels are established, is treated as a black box, with human behaviour reduced to a
22,4 number of simplifying assumptions. The neo-classical model is predicated on the
notion that the market comprises rational actors operating with perfect information in
an environment of costless transactions. It is also assumed that property can be treated
as a homogenous commodity and that consumers of space are also homogenous.
McMaster and Watkins (2000) are critical of the extent to which this approach
308 circumscribes the scope of real estate research. They explore the limitations of the
NUE and suggest that it might be fruitful to reintroduce some of the ideas from Ely’s
work. Specifically they highlight the policy impact of the analytical work of Grigsby,
Rapkin and others, which is in the Ely tradition (see Grigsby, 1963). They argue that
real estate analysts need to learn more about market process and, in particular,
highlight the need to examine the role of agents in the market, the property search
process, consumer decision making, the nature and flow of market information, and
the way in which prices are set. This assessment is reflective of the concerns of
institutional economists and the evolving applied behavioural property research
agenda (see D’Arcy and Keogh, 2000). The behavioural agenda, in particular, has
begun raise new questions about the search process in real estate markets (Baryla et
al., 2000); role and influence of agents (Zumpano et al., 1996); the use of market
information in the negotiation process (Black and Diaz, 1996); and the influence of
actors in the valuation process (Gallimore, 1994; Diaz, 1990).
This paper seeks to add to the behavioural agenda by examining the decisions
made by office occupiers. To date studies of office occupiers’ choices have taken the
form of location choice models. These studies have been based on the textbook
theoretical proposition that office location decisions are influenced by factor costs,
transport and communication costs, the quality of the urban environment and
agglomeration economies (Ball et al., 1998; Evans, 1985; Goddard, 1975). Empirical
research has confirmed that the desire of office occupiers to locate in the core of the
city has tended to push up rents in the central business district, giving rise to a
negatively sloped bid-rent curve (Dunse et al., forthcoming; Bollinger et al., 1998;
Mills, 1992). These studies, however, have stripped out the complexity of the
decision-making process and, consequently, ignore the importance of factors other
than rent and location in shaping decisions. The approach used in this paper combines
survey evidence with logistic regression methods in order to assess the relative
importance of a range of factors, including the characteristics of the firm, in
determining the choice of office space to be occupied. The model developed allows us
to identify the firms’ choice of property type from its size and business profile and
can be used as a marketing device for agents to match office users to available space.
The paper is organised as follows. Section 2 seeks to establish the motivation for
this research. It begins by presenting the standard neo-classical treatment of the office
location decision and then considers the extent to which behavioural changes have
begun to present a challenge to this model. The review concludes by highlighting the
need to develop an understanding of factors influencing individual firms’ choice of
property in the office market. In section 3 we outline the data and research methods
used in the paper. Section 4 summarises the empirical results. Using a detailed dataset
of physical office characteristics matched to occupier profiles, we consider the extent
to which it is possible to establish the office location decisions of different groups of
occupiers by allowing for the heterogeneity of firms. In the final section of the paper,
in addition to summarising our findings, we make the case for further research based on Decision-making
survey data. Following Kummerow (2000) we note that real estate research would benefit behaviour
from the combination of different research techniques. Indeed there is a clear need for a
multi-disciplinary and methodologically diverse real estate research agenda.
Variable Description
PSIZE Property size (square metres)
RENT Rent (per square metre)
ATTSCORE1 Property specification score (lighting type, heating
type, lifts, ventilation and so on)
Table IV. ATTSCORE2 Property secondary specification score (number of
Description of continuous floors, layout of reception area and so on)
variables DIST Distance from the city centre (metres)
in the recruitment and training sector. This variable primarily describes floorspace Decision-making
layout and the standard of the reception area. behaviour
This simple descriptive analysis tends to suggest that there are some significant
differences between the physical and quality aspects of office space targeted by firms
in different economic sectors. In the remainder of this section we concentrate on the
construction of a simple predictive model of occupiers’ choice of office space. There
are two basic steps to the construction of the model. In the first, we employ cluster 313
analysis in order to draw the 119 office properties into broadly homogenous “property
type” groups using the eight physical attribute categorical variables. In the second
step we estimate a simple multinomial logistic regression model in which the
probability that firms will locate in each of the property types is postulated as a
function of their business activity and geographical focus.
Table V sets out the results of the k-means cluster analysis. Based on the results of
an earlier round of hierarchical cluster analysis (not shown here for the sake of
brevity), the data are drawn into four clusters.
Table VI describes the inherent characteristics of office properties in each of the
four clusters.
Tables VIII and IX show the results of the multinomial logistic regression analysis.
The model is specified as follows:
Cluster
Variable 1 2 3 4
CT 2 5 6 7
PCOND 5 6 5 5
FINTY 4 6 3 6
SUBTYPE 1 3 4 3
AGEBAND 1 5 5 5
SIZEBAND 3 2 2 2
SPECSC1 4 1 1 1 Table V.
SPECSC2 1 2 1 1 Results of cluster
Cases 11 39 48 21 analysis
Cluster Description
314 where:
p
ij probability that the ith occupier will locate in the jth property type cluster
( j ¼ 1; 2; 3; 4).
x1 occupier market size ( k ¼ 0; 1; . . .; 3).
x2 occupier size proxy (l ¼ 0; 1; 2).
x3 occupier business category (m ¼ 0; 1; . . .; 5).
a constant (intercept); specific to the jth property type cluster.
b1 parameter on the occupier market size variable (x1); specific to the jth cluster
and the ith value of x1.
b2 parameter on the occupier size proxy variable (x2); specific to the jth cluster
and the ith value of x2.
b3 parameter on the occupier business category variable (x3); specific to the jth
cluster and the ith value of x3.
The model includes three independent categorical variables. These relate to market
size (MKT) which is defined as international, national, regional or urban; a proxy for
firm size (ENTRIES) based on a simple categorisation of the number of yellow page
entries for each firm; and business type (BUSTYPE) which is defined in the list of
business categories of owner above. The normalisation is achieved by setting the
fourth alternative (property type ¼ 4) as a benchmark such that a4 ¼ b4 ¼ 0.
In Table VII the likelihood ratio test is for the hypothesis that the parameters of the
independent variables are equal to zero. This null hypothesis is easily rejected. The
Cox and Snell, Nagelkerke and McFadden statistics are so-called pseudo-R-square
statistics and are standard SPSS output. The Nagelkerke measure is often preferred
since it ranges from 0 to 1 while Cox and Snell, for example, has a maximum that is
less than 1. The pseudo-R-square measures indicate a reasonable level of fit. Table
VIII sets out the parameter estimates.
The empirical results are shown for property type categories 1, 2 and 3. The
benchmark is therefore category 4. The exponentiated parameter estimates (Exp(B))
(2) Firms are less likely to locate in type 1 property than type 4 if:
.their market is regional; and
(3) Firms are more likely to locate in type 2 property than type 4 if:
.their market is international;
(4) Firms are less likely to locate in type 2 property than type 4 if:
.their main business is in professional services; and
(5) Firms are more likely to locate in type 3 property than type 4 if:
.they have two or more Yellow Page entries
Overall, these results are broadly in line with expectations. One surprising aspect of
the results, however, is that several of the business type categories are not significant.
Nevertheless, the results indicate that even simplistic categorisation of occupiers
provides some ability to predict their behaviour with respect to location decisions and
space consumption. As Table IX indicates, the model may be used to predict the
property type selected by the firms included in the sample with the model predicting
correctly 58 per cent of the time.
5. Conclusions
In mainstream neo-classical economic analyses urban office markets are depicted as
coherent, unitary entities. Individual firms are assumed to be rational, profit maximisers
whose selection of office property will be dominated by a trade-off between
Predicted
Observed 1 2 3 4 Percent correct
1 6 2 3 0 54.6
2 1 23 10 5 59.0
Table IX. 3 0 15 29 4 60.4
Classification/prediction 4 1 4 5 11 52.4
table Overall % 6.7 37.0 39.5 16.8 58.0
accessibility and space. There will be an observable negatively sloped rent gradient Decision-making
that can be explained by the influence of agglomeration economies and transport behaviour
costs.
In reality, however, the process by which firms match themselves to office units is
much more complex than a simple location choice decision. Some observers have
noted the tendency towards decentralisation with the growing importance of IT
requirements and flexible working practices (Ball et al., 1998). This implies that the 317
influence of rent and location may now be dampened by additional concerns. Other
observers highlight the tendency of consumers to engage in satisficing behaviour and
to enter into sub-optimal location decisions (Alexander, 1979).
In the empirical part of this paper, we seek to take a less restrictive view of the way
in which firms match themselves to particular units. We do this by analysing evidence
from a survey of 119 office occupiers in Edinburgh. Using cluster analysis
techniques, we construct a classification of property types in the city. We then test
whether the choice of property type can be identified from knowledge about the
characteristics of the firm. The results suggest that firms’ choice of property type will
be contingent on their size, type of business and the geographical extent of their
market. This innovative approach provides new empirical evidence of the way firm
characteristics influence their decision choice. Although it is unclear whether the
results are specific to our case study city, by relaxing the assumption that firms are
homogenous, the empirical work augments existing insights (from mainstream
location decision models) about the structure and operation of urban office markets.
At a general level, this paper seeks to add to the growing “behavioural property”
literature. In focusing on office users’ choice of units, the paper provides an extension
to the existing behavioural studies that have tended to focus on the valuation process
and the role of agents. To a lesser extent, the paper also seeks to make a contribution
to ongoing methodological debates. In discussing the limits inherent in the dominant
quantitative research paradigm, Kummerow (2000) notes that, a partial remedy for
coping with complexity and uncertainty in real estate markets is to combine research
methods. Although Kummerow specifically suggests the combination quantitative and
qualitative techniques to improve information in local applications, this observation
could equally be applied as a rationale for combining data collected by survey
methods with rigorous methods of statistical analysis. This paper is illustrative of the
way in which the analysis of data of this sort can allow us to augment existing
empirical facts. It is also illustrative of the principle that a multi-disciplinary
behavioural research agenda should be developed in order to develop a better
appreciation of the market process occurring in the “black box” that tends to be
assumed away in most quantitative studies of market outcomes.
Note
1. The survey was supported by a research grant from the RICS Education Trust.
References
Alexander, I. (1979), Office Location and Public Policy, Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Ball, M., Lizieri, C. and MacGregor, B. (1998), Economics of Commercial Property
Markets, Routledge, London.
JPIF Baryla, E., Zumpano, L. and Elder, H. (2000), “An investigation of buyer search in the
residential real estate market under different market conditions”, Journal of Real
22,4 Estate Research, Vol. 20 No. 1, pp. 75-91.
Black, R.T. and Diaz, J. III (1996), “The use of information versus asking price in the real property
negotiation process”, Journal of Property Research, Vol. 13 No. 4, pp. 287-97.
Bollinger, R., Ihlanfeldt, K. and Bowes, R. (1998), “Spatial variation in office rents within the
318 Atlanta region”, Urban Studies, Vol. 35, pp. 1097-118.
Bourassa, S., Hamelink, F., Hoesli, M. and MacGregor, B. (1999), “Defining housing
submarkets”, Journal of Housing Economics, Vol. 8, pp. 160-83.
Clapp, J. and Myers, D. (2000), “Graaskamp and the definition of rigorous research”, in
Delisle, J.R. and Worzala, E. (Eds), Essays in Honour of James A Graaskamp: Ten
Years After, Kluwer, New York, NY.
Daniels, P. (1991), Services and Metropolitan Development, Routledge, London.
D’Arcy, E. and Keogh, G. (2000), “Graaskamp, institutional economics and the real estate
market”, in Delisle, J.R. and Worzala, E. (Eds), Essays in Honour of James A
Graaskamp: Ten Years After, Kluwer, New York, NY.
Diaz, J. III (1990), “How appraisers do their work: a test of the appraisal process and the development of a
descriptive model”, Journal of Real Estate Research, Vol. 5 No. 1, pp. 1-15.
Diaz, J. III (1999), “The first decade of behavioural research in the discipline of property”,
Journal of Property Investment & Finance, Vol. 17 No. 4, pp. 326-32.
Dunse, N., Leishman, C. and Watkins, C. (2001a), “Classifying office submarkets”, Journal of
Property Investment & Finance, Vol. 19 No. 3, pp. 236-50.
Dunse, N., Leishman, C. and Watkins, C. (forthcoming), “Testing for office submarkets”,
Urban Studies.
Dunse, N.A., Leishman, C., Warren, F.J. and Watkins, C. (2001b), “Office space requirements:
comparing occupiers’ preferences with agents’ perceptions”, paper presented to the
ESRC Property Economics and Finance Research Network Seminar Group, Corporate
Real Estate Workshop, University of Aberdeen, Aberdeen.
Evans, A.W. (1985), Urban Economics, Macmillan, London.
Gallimore, P. (1994), “Aspects of information processing in valuation judgement and choice”,
Journal of Property Research, Vol. 11 No. 2, pp. 97-110.
Gibson, V. and Lizieri, C. (1998), Business Reorganisation and Working Practices, RICS
Research Papers, RICS, London.
Gibson, V. and Lizieri, C. (1999), “New business practices and the corporate portfolio
approach: how responsive is the UK property market?”, Journal of Property Research,
Vol. 16 No. 3, pp. 201-18.
Goddard, J. (1975), Office Location in Urban and Regional Development, Oxford
University Press, Oxford.
Grigsby, W.G. (1963), Housing Markets and Public Policy, Johns Hopkins Press, Baltimore, MD.
Hoesli, M., Lizieri, C. and MacGregor, B. (1997), “The spatial dimensions of the investment
performance of UK commercial property”, Urban Studies, Vol. 34, pp. 1475-94.
Jackson, C. (2002), “Classifying local retail property markets on the basis of rental growth
rates”, Urban Studies, Vol. 39 No. 8, p. 1417.
Keeble, D. and Tyler, P. (1995), Enterprising Behaviour and the Urban-Rural Shift, ESRC
Business Research, Working Paper No. 4 Vol. 4, University of Cambridge, Cambridge.
Kummerow, M. (2000) in Delisle, J.R. and Worzala, E. (Eds), Essays in Honour of James Decision-making
A. Graaskamp: Ten Years After, Kluwer, New York, NY.
behaviour
Levy, D. and Henry, M. (2001), “The evolving property research agenda: perspectives,
methods and methodologies”, paper presented at the University of Aberdeen, Aberdeen.
McMaster, R. and Watkins, C. (2000), The Economics of Urban Land and Housing: Richard T
Ely and the “Land Economy” School Reconsidered, Aberdeen Papers in Land Economy,
No. 00-10, , University of Aberdeen, Aberdeen. 319
Mills, E. (1992), “Office rent determinants in the Chicago area”, AREUEA Journal, Vol. 20
No. 2, pp. 273-87.
O’Roarty, B. (2001), “Deconstructing real estate values”, paper presented at the ESRC Property
Economics and Finance Research Network Seminar Group Meeting, Corporate Real
Estate Workshop, Aberdeen, April.
Weiss, M. (2000), “James A Graaskamp, Richard T. Ely and the tradition of real estate and urban
land economics at the University of Wisconsin”, in Delisle, J.R. and Worzala, E. (Eds),
Essays in Honour of James A Graaskamp: Ten Years After, Kluwer, New York, NY.
Wyatt, P. (1999), “Can a geographical analysis of property values aid business location?”,
paper presented to the RICS Cutting Edge Conference, Cambridge.
Zumpano, L., Elder, H. and Baryla, E. (1996), “Buying a house and the decision to use a real estate
broker”, Journal of Real Estate Finance and Economics, Vol. 13 No. 2, pp. 169-81.
The Emerald Research Register for this journal is available at The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available at
www.emeraldinsight.com/researchregister www.emeraldinsight.com/1463-578X.htm
JPIF
22,4 The influence of family members on
housing purchase decisions
Deborah S. Levy
320 Department of Property, The University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
Christina Kwai-Choi Lee
Received January 2002 Department of Marketing, The University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
Accepted December 2002
Keywords Family, Decision making, Influence, Real estate
Abstract Families and households make up a significant proportion of the real estate market.
There is, however, little information in mainstream real estate literature on the impact of
family behaviour on real estate decisions. This paper clarifies some of these issues by
analysing and expanding on many of the findings from the marketing literature, in particular
the topic of influence between different family members in the purchase of a new home.
This paper presents some important issues to be considered when examining family
decision-making. These include the roles played by different family members and their
influence at different stages of the decision-making process. It also reports on the findings
of a study involving a series of in-depth interviews with real estate agents to determine their
perception of the family decision-making process in relation to a house purchase decision.
This culminates in a conceptual framework on family decision making specifically for the
purchase of residential real estate, before discussing the implications of these findings to
the general real estate market, including service, promotion and valuation.
1. Introduction
Traditionally the study of real estate has been based on neoclassical economic theory that
assumes individuals make rational economic decisions with a view to maximising utility.
This is reflected in property valuations which are primarily based on physical
characteristics rather than more intangible non-financial factors which are often important
to the purchasers of real estate (Smith et al., 1992) and thus fail to take into account
behavioural issues and processes. To date, research relating to the behavioural issues
relating to the purchase of residential real estate has largely been confined to areas such as
buyer search duration and location and tenure choice (Anglin, 1997; Baryla and Zumpano,
1995; Elder and Zumpano, 1991). Generally these studies do not focus on the dynamics
relating to the decision-making process within the family.
Traditional or neoclassical theory in the past has either ignored the family as an
institution or treated it as if it was a single individual or as if the paternal head of the
household personified the family as a whole (Hodgson, 1999). Hodgson (1999)
suggests that the only mainstream attempt to model the family was undertaken by
Becker (1976, 1991), however Becker’s model assumed a market where marriage and
relationships are formed under contract. Hodgson (1999) asserts that both traditional
Journal of Property Investment &
theory and mainstream attempts have ignored moral, cultural and institutional
Finance distinction. The implication therefore is that different techniques are required in order
Vol. 22 No. 4, 2004
pp. 320-338 to explore family decision-making behaviour.
q Emerald Group Publishing
Limited 1463-578X
The study of consumer behaviour within the marketing literature however, has
DOI 10.1108/14635780410550885 examined many issues regarding the purchasing behaviour of families. Our belief is
that the knowledge gained from this line of research may assist in a better Housing
understanding and prediction of decision-makers’ actions in the real estate market purchase
(Gibler and Nelson, 1998). The focus of this study is, therefore, to determine whether
the extant literature relating to family decision making and family member influence decisions
mirror the decision making process in the purchase of a family home. The purpose is
not to produce definitive conclusions, or to provide generalisable results, but to
provide a basis from which academics and practitioners in real estate can begin to 321
understand and explore this under-researched area.
Part 2 of the paper examines the extant literature relating to family member
influence in the area of purchase decisions and the implications for real estate
purchase decisions. Part 3 reports on the results of a series of individual in-depth
interviews with experienced real estate agents in the Auckland area. From the review
of the literature and information collected from the interviews a revised model of
family decision making relevant to the real estate purchase decision is proposed. This
is followed by a discussion of the implications to the marketing and valuation of real
estate and explores possible avenues for future research.
322
Figure 1.
Framework of family
decision making as it
relates to the purchase of a
family home
(Sheth, 1974). Conflict occurs when there is a disagreement among the family
members, and this is directly related to the desire to influence other members to
accept one’s own point of view.
The amount of influence exerted by different family members is dependent on how
interested or involved the individual member is in the purchase. For example, if the
woman in the household is interested in the local residential property market and
keeps up to date with recent sales, it is quite possible that she will have most influence
in the housing decision. Similarly, children may not be as interested in the purchase of
the family home and hence may not have a strong influence on the decision.
The following section reports on the outcome of a number of consumer behaviour
studies relating to the different elements of the conceptual framework as presented in
Figure 1; specifically the impact of family characteristics, stages in the decision-
making process, decision-making roles and influence strategies.
3. Research approach
In order to reflect the family decision-making process accurately and to identify factors
from the literature that may be applicable to the real estate environment in-depth
interviews were carried out. The use of in-depth interviews encourages interviewees to
speak openly and frankly and also allows the interviewer to explore different areas with a
large degree of freedom. This approach concurs with the recommendations of Anastas
(1988) who suggests that in-depth interviews should be utilised in situations
JPIF of sensitive subject matter and complex decision-making processes. Several other
22,4 advantages of this type of interview include the encouragement of personal thought,
maintenance and attentiveness of respondents to questions and the interviewer’s
consequent ability to sense non-verbal feedback (Sokolow, 1985).
The interviews were carried out by one of the authors and once completed were
transcribed and independently examined and audited by both authors. This audit
326 included an examination of the transcripts to identify the main issues highlighted by
the interviewees in order to assist in a deeper understanding of the family decision
making process and how it may work in the light of the purchase of a family home.
F
I
P
J
4
,
2
2
Summary of interviews
General model
Roles and Real Estate
Group decision influence Decision Family member’s Factors affecting
process structure Process Group decision member’s role and direct influence structure indirect influence influence
Problem , Initiator Whether to Woman more predominant especially with young family Children, taking into Family characteristics
recognition purchase a Affected more by the way the house fulfils the emotional and account the changing Stage of life cycle
family home functional needs of the family needs of the children Experience
Man more predominant in older couples considers more the Sex role orientation
practical aspects of the home i.e. too much maintenance
Both more predominant when no children and in particular
in the purchase of the first home
Product , User/ General location Bank/Financial consultant may be required to assess price Woman for more Individual
specification stakeholder Price range range and suitability of properties. Seems to be more Children’s specific characteristics
Main attributes prevalent in families of lower socio-economic status needs, e.g. number of Gender (women more
Man seems to have more influence in determining the bedrooms, play area expressive roles, men
general area of the property, associated with prestige and etc. (indirect influence) more instrumental
resale value roles)
When main income earner Family characteristics
Both when parents see closeness to school is a priority Sex role orientation
Woman more concerned about issues relating to how the including resource
family will live in the house, whereas man more concerned contribution
with practical aspects such as structure materials
maintenance, resale value etc.
Children will voice their requirements, e.g. younger children
will want a tree in the garden to climb, older children will
want their own space and be close to their friends (direct
influence)
Information , Information Information gathering Woman when not working, especially in higher Children will again Family characteristics
search gatherer and about properties on socio-economic families have indirect influence Socio-economic status
gatekeeper the market Either (tending towards the woman) in lower socio-economic in parents anticipating Sex role orientation
Point of contact with families depends on the time available to the individual their requirements
real estate agent partners. Male participation on the increase
First inspection Open homes have encouraged more joint inspections
(Continued)
General model
Roles and Real Estate
Group decision influence Decision Family member’s Factors affecting
process structure Process Group decision member’s role and direct influence structure indirect influence influence
Alternative , Influencer Subsequent Both partners will inspect the property at this time Children’s needs taken Family characteristics
evaluation inspections of the Children may then be introduced to the property at this stage into account Socio-economic status
property Over 15 year olds may not find the time to come as they have Family life cycle (age
a hectic social life and see the family home as short-term of children)
accommodation
Children more influence in higher price range
Children more influential in Asian families
Family and friends may also be introduced at this stage
Family and relatives more influential in Pacific Island and
Asian families, Caucasian families more independent
Family more influence in first home purchase
Final choice , Decision Negotiation of price Financier will need to approve the deal and thus influence Individual
maker and purchase of the price and suitability of property characteristics
property Both partners must agree to buy Personality
Either or Both Information
The spouse liking the home more will be pushing more for Involvement
the purchase (emotional
Either, the personality of the partner will determine who attachment)
pushes the deal through more Sex role orientation
Either – the main income earner or the one who has the most
information about the market
Table I.
329
H
si
is
g
n
u
o
u
p
n
o
e
c
s
s
r
i
JPIF This information-gathering stage is then followed by the evaluation of alternatives.
22,4 From the study, the main action taken by the family at this stage is the inspection of
alternative properties until a suitable home is found. This stage is followed by final
choice, which includes the negotiation of the price and contract and the purchase of
the property. Figure 2 sets out this process in more detail and includes the concept of
favourable and unfavourable outcomes to the different stages of the process.
330
4.3. Decision-making roles
The roles of the individuals in the decision-making group can best be understood
when related to the decision-making process. As the decision-making process
progresses different roles are played out by different members of the family. In the
problem recognition stage an initiator will become apparent.
Figure 2.
Role players and the
family decision process
when purchasing a family
home
The interviews suggested that the woman tends to be more dominant as the initiator, Housing
especially in the case of a family with young children, where the mother recognises purchase
the changing needs of the family. The influence exerted by the man may prevail in
cases where he feels the home is too big or requires too much maintenance. In the decisions
case of couples with no children, and in particular when embarking on the purchase of
a first home, there may be equal influence from both parties. The reason for this may
be because the couple has not yet taken up individual roles (Filiatrault and Ritchie, 331
1980), but also may reflect the high perceived risk of purchasing a first home,
especially when neither partner has had experience in such a purchase previously.
These results indicate that the family life cycle is an important factor in determining
who influences and how they influence the decision at this stage.
The interviews also suggest that even in this early part of the decision-making
process the wife plays a more expressive role when determining the requirements of a
home, she is taking into account the emotional needs of the family. The husband,
however, tends to take a more instrumental role. These differences are demonstrated
further in the next phases of the decision-making process.
In the product specification stage, the man, woman, children and stakeholder will
be involved in determining the main requirements of the house, general location and
price range. At this phase evidence seems to suggest that the man has more influence
in determining the general location of the property, issues that seem to be important to
him at this stage are the prestige to be gained by the property and the resale value.
Both parents will be equally as influential in the location decision when closeness to a
certain school is their priority.
When determining the main physical requirements of the house, men and women tend
to take on different roles. Women will be more concerned with issues relating to how the
family will function in the home, for example women with young families tend to be more
concerned with the functional aspects of the kitchen, including appropriate size and site
lines to the play area. However if the man is the main cook (especially evident in higher
socio-economic families where both partners work) he may also be interested in the
kitchen from a functional aspect of cooking. Men in general are more concerned with
attributes such as a double garage and workshop. Children, at this stage, may also have a
direct influence, requesting space of their own or a location near to their friends. They may
also hold a substantial amount of indirect influence as parents will be anticipating their
needs thus reflecting such attributes as the number of bedrooms they require, the closeness
to schools and a safe and secure play area.
The bank or financier may be the person who will determine how much a family
will be able to pay for a property. This is especially prevalent for families of lower
socio-economic status. There is evidence to show that the main income earner
(usually the man) will be more dominant in determining how much the family will put
aside for the purchase of the home. The concept of the main income earner
determining the amount to be spent endorses the concept of the resource contribution
theory (Blood and Wolfe, 1960). This theory proposes that the influence over
decisions comes from the resources the individual can provide to meet the need of the
other partner. Other more recent research also endorses this concept (e.g. Lee and
Beatty, 2002; Strober and Weinberg, 1977; Weinberg and Winer, 1983). The current
study also suggests a more democratic process in circumstances where the wife is the
person keeping track of the family’s finances.
JPIF The information search phase as perceived by the real estate agents in the current
22,4 study reflect families’ socio-economic status and sex role orientation. The interviews
suggest that in families of higher socio-economic status, where the woman is not
working that she will take on the role of information gatherer and gatekeeper. She will
make the initial inquiry to the real estate agent and become the main point of contact;
she will also make an initial inspection of the property. In lower socio-economic
332 families the choice of partner undertaking this role will depend on the time available
to each partner. There appears to be an overall trend of increased male participation in
this role, which has been encouraged by the growing number and scheduling of “open
homes” making inspections more accessible to both partners.
The alternative evaluation stage includes subsequent inspections of the property.
Here there is the opportunity for other family group members to influence the final
decision. At this stage both partners will inspect the property. Children may also play
a direct role. Their ability to influence decisions seems to depend on the family’s
socio-economic status, cultural background and age of children, with children from
families purchasing more expensive homes having more say in the purchase of the
home. Children from Asian families tend to be more influential in the decision than
families from a Caucasian background. Children over 15 years may not find time to
inspect the property due to “a hectic social life” and the fact that they consider the
family home as only short term accommodation before they move to their own
residence. Children from approximately nine to 15 years will have the greatest impact,
although they will most likely be overruled if the parents are keen on buying a
property. However, parents may be prepared to enter into a bargaining situation with
them to keep them happy. For example: “once we have bought the property we will
see about putting in a swimming pool”.
The alternative evaluation stage in many cases introduces family and friends into
the process. These players are more influential in Pacific Island and Asian families,
with Caucasian families being more independent in their decision making. The study
also indicated that parents of couples purchasing their first home are influential at this
stage of the process.
The final stage of the process includes the negotiation of the price, the terms and
the purchase of the property. The players influencing this stage of the process will
include both partners. Overall the final choice will be a joint decision and in most
cases both partners will have to sign the sale and purchase agreement. There may be
situations where one spouse may push for the completion of the purchase more than
the other partner, this will tend to be the one liking the home more or with the
personality to push the purchase through. There is evidence to suggest that in some
families the partner with the most influence at this stage will be the one who is the
main income earner or the one who has collected and assimilated the most
information about the market and comparable properties. Another influential player at
this stage will be the person financing the purchase, as they will need to approve the
loan required to complete the deal.
Overall, however, the study suggests that either husband or wife may be dominant
depending on a number of family, individual and situational characteristics as set out
in Figure 3. This tends to support Engel et al.’s (1986) contention that there is a
blurring of role specialisations between males and females.
Housing
purchase
decisions
333
Figure 3.
Revised framework of
family decision making in
the purchase of a family
home
Note
1. As it relates to physical aspects rather than the how it works for the family living together.
References
Anastas, M. (1988), “For sensitive research studies, one-to-one interviews work best”, Bank
Marketing, Vol. 20 No. 7, pp. 18-22.
Anglin, P.M. (1997), “Determinant of buyer search in a housing market”, Real Estate
Economics, Vol. 25 No. 4, pp. 567-89.
Assael, H. (1987), Consumer Behavior and Marketing Action, Thomas Nelson, Melbourne.
Atkin, C.K. (1978), “Observation of parent-child interaction in supermarket decision-making;”,
Journal of Marketing, Vol. 42 October, pp. 41-5.
Baryla, E.A. and Zumpano, L.V. (1995), “Buyer search duration in the residential real estate
market: the role of the real estate agent”, Journal of Real Estate Research, Vol. 10
No. 1, pp. 1-13.
Beatty, S.E. and Talpade, S. (1994), “Adolescent influence in family decision making: a replication
with extension”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 21 September, pp. 332-40.
Becker, G.S. (1976), The Economic Approach to Human Behavior, University of Chicago
Press, Chicago, IL.
Becker, G.S. (1991), A Treatise on the Family, 2nd ed., Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.
Blood, R.D. and Wolfe, D.L. (1960), Husbands and Wives, The Free Press, New York, NY.
Brinberg, D. and Schwenk, N. (1985), “Husband-wife decision making: an exploratory study of the
interaction process”, in Hirschman, E.C. and Holbrook, M.B. (Eds), Advances in Consumer
Research, Vol. 12, Association for Consumer Research, Provo, UT, pp. 487-91.
JPIF Davis, H. (1970), “Dimensions of marital roles in consumer decision making”, Journal of
Marketing Research, Vol. 7 May, pp. 169-77.
22,4
Davis, H. (1971), “Measurement of husband-wife influence in consumer decision making”,
Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 8 August, pp. 305-12.
Davis, H. (1976), “Decision making within the household”, Journal of Consumer Research,
Vol. 2 March, pp. 241-60.
336 Davis, H. and Rigaux, B.P. (1974), “Husband-wife influence in family purchasing behavior”,
Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 1 June, pp. 51-62.
Eisenhardt, K.M. (1989), “Building theories from case research”, Academy of Management
Review, Vol. 14 No. 4, pp. 532-50.
Elder, H.W. and Zumpano, V. (1991), “Tenure choice, housing demand and residential
location”, Journal of Real Estate Research, Vol. 6 No. 3, pp. 341-56.
Engel, J.F., Blackwell, R.D. and Miniard, P.W. (1986), Consumer Behavior, 5th ed., CBS
College Press, Orlando, FL.
Ferber, R. and Lee, L. (1974), “Husband-wife influence in family purchasing behavior”,
Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 1 June, pp. 43-50.
Filiatrault, P. and Ritchie, J.R.B. (1980), “Joint purchasing decisions: a comparison of
influence structure in family and couple decision making units”, Journal of Consumer
Research, Vol. 7 September, pp. 131-40.
Fisher, B.A. (1970), “Decision emergence: phases in group decision making”, Speech
Monographs, Vol. 37, pp. 53-66.
Foxman, E. and Tansuhaj, P. (1988), “Adolescents’ and mothers’ perceptions of relative
influence in family purchase decisions: patterns of agreement and disagreement”, in
Houston, M.J. (Ed.), Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 15, Association for
Consumer Research, Provo, UT, pp. 449-53.
Foxman, E., Tansuhaj, P. and Elstrom, K. (1989), “Family members’ perception of adolescents’
influence in family decision making”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 15 March,
pp. 482-91.
Gibler, K.M. and Nelson, S. (1998), “Consumer behavior research applications to real estate”,
paper presented at the American Real Estate Meeting, April.
Granbois, D.H. (1963), “The role of communication in the family decision making process”, in
Greyser, S.A. (Ed.), Proceedings of the American Marketing Association
Educators Conference, pp. 44-57.
Granbois, D.H. (1971), “A multilevel approach to family role structure research”, in Gardner,
D.M. (Ed.), Advances in Consumer Research, Association for Consumer Research,
College Park, MD, pp. 99-107.
Green, R.T. and Cunningham, I.C.M. (1975), “Feminine role perceptions and family
purchasing decisions”, Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 12 August, pp. 325-32.
Gersick, C.J.G. (1988), “Time and transition in work teams: toward a new model of group
development”, Academy of Management Journal, Vol. 31 No. 1, pp. 9-41.
Haley, Overholser and Associates, Inc. (1975), Purchase Influence: Measures of Husband/Wife
Influence on Buying Decisions, Haley, Overholser and Associates, New Haven, CT.
Hempel, D.J. (1974), “Family buying decisions”, Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 11,
pp. 295-302.
Hodgson, G.M. (1999), Economics and Utopia; Why the Learning Economy Is Not the
End of History, Routledge, London.
Jenkins, R.L. (1979), “The influence of children in family decision-making: parents’ Housing
perceptions”, in Wilkie, W.L. (Ed.), Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 16,
Association for Consumer Research, Ann Arbor, MI, pp. 413-8. purchase
Komarovsky, M. (1961), “Class differences in family decision making on expenditures”, in decisions
Foote, N. (Ed.), Household Decision Making, NY University Press, New York, NY.
Lawson, R., Tidwell, P., Rainbird, P., Loudon, D. and Della Bitta, A. (1996), Consumer
Behaviour in Australia and New Zealand, McGraw-Hill Book Company, Sydney. 337
Lee, C.K.C. (1992), “A model of family buyer behaviour”, New Zealand Journal of
Business, Vol. 14, pp. 50-60.
Lee, C.K.C. and Beatty, S.E. (2002), “Family structure and influence in family decision
making”, Journal of Consumer Marketing, Vol. 19 No. 1, pp. 24-41.
Lee, C.K.C. and Collins, B.A. (2000), “Family decision making and coalition patterns”,
European Journal of Marketing, Vol. 34 Nos 9/10, pp. 1181-98.
Lee, C.K.C. and Marshall, R. (1998), “Measuring influence in the family decision making
process using a process using an observational method”, Qualitative Market Research:
An International Journal, Vol. 15 March, pp. 88-98.
Lee, C.K.C., Brown, R. and Wong, M.Y. (1997), “The impact of Confucian dynamism on the
family decision making process”, in Sidin, S.M. and Manrai, A.K. (Eds), Eighth World
Marketing Congress, Vol. 8, Academy of Marketing Science, Vancouver, pp. 202-6.
Mangleburg, T.F. (1990), “Children’s influence in purchase decisions: a review and critique”,
in Goldberg, M.E., Gorn, G. and Pollay, R.W. (Eds), Advances in Consumer
Research, Vol. 17, Association for Consumer Research, Provo, UT, pp. 159-69.
Miles, M.B. and Huberman, A.M. (1994), Qualitative Data Analysis: An Expanded Source
Book, 2nd ed., Sage Publications, Thousand Oaks, CA.
Morse, J.M. (Ed.) (1994), Critical Issues in Qualitative Research Methods, Sage, Thousand
Oaks, CA.
Moschis, G.P. and Mitchell, L.G. (1986), “Television advertising and interpersonal influence on
teenagers’ participation in family consumer decisions”, in Lutz, R.J. (Ed.), Advances in
Consumer Research, Vol. 13, Assocaition for Consumer Research, Provo, UT, pp. 181-6.
Na, W.B., Son, Y.S. and Marshall, R. (1998), “An empirical study of the purchase role structure in
Korean families”, Psychology and Marketing, Vol. 15 September, pp. 563-76.
Pervan, S. and Lee, C.K.C. (1998), “An observational study of the family decision making
process of Chinese immigrant families”, in Hung, K. and Moore, K.B. (Eds), Asia
Pacific Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 3, Association for Consumer
Research, Provo, UT, pp. 20-5.
Qualls, W.J. (1987), “Household decision behavior: the impact of husbands’ and wives’ sex
role orientation”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 4 September, pp. 264-79.
Qualls, W.J. and Jaffe, F. (1992), “Measuring conflict in household decision behavior: read my lips
and read my mind”, in Sherry, J.F. Jr and Sternthal, B. (Eds), Advances in Consumer
Research, Vol. 19, Association for Consumer Research, Provo, UT, pp. 522-31.
Robinson, P.J., Faris, C.W. and Wind, Y. (1967), Understanding the Industrial Buyer, Vol. 3
November, Marketing Science Unit, Cambridge, MA.
Rossiter, J.R. (1978), “Children’ consumer research”, in Wilkie, W.L. (Ed.), Advances in Consumer
Research, Vol. 6, Assocaition for Consumer Research, Ann Arbor, MI, pp. 424-6.
Sheth, J.N. (1973), “A model of industrial buying behaviour”, Journal of Marketing, Vol. 37
October, pp. 50-6.
JPIF Sheth, J.N. (1974), “A theory of family buying decisions”, in Sheth, J.N. (Ed.), Models of
Buyer Behavior, Harper & Row, New York, NY, pp. 17-33.
22,4
Slama, M.E. and Tahchian, A. (1985), “Selected socioeconomic and demographic characteristics
associated with purchasing involvement”, Journal of Marketing, Vol. 49, pp. 72-82.
Smith, C.A., Garbarino, L.N. and Martini, J. (1992), “Analyzing the leasing criteria of retail
tenants”, Journal of Property Management, Vol. 57 No. 6, pp. 40-3.
338 Sokolow, H. (1985), “In-depth interviews increasing in importance”, Markeitng News, Vol.
19 No. 19, p. 26, 31.
Solomon, M.R. (1999), Consumer Behaviour, 4th ed., Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.
Spiro, R. (1983), “Persuasion in family decision making”, Journal of Consumer Research,
Vol. 9 March, pp. 393-402.
Strober, M.H. and Weinberg, C.B. (1977), “Working wives and major family expenditures”,
Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 4 December, pp. 141-7.
Szybillo, G.J. and Sosanie, A.K. (1977), “Family decision making”, Journal of Consumer
Research, Vol. 9 March, pp. 393-402.
Webster, F.E. Jr and Wind, Y. (1972), Organisational Buying Behaviour, Prentice Hall,
Englewood Cliffs, NJ.
Weinberg, C.B. and Winer, R.S. (1983), “Working wives and major family expenditures:
replication and extension”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 10 September, pp. 259-63.
Woodside, A.G. and Motes, W.H. (1979), “Perceptions of marital roles in consumer decsion
processes for six products”, American Marketing Association Proceedings, American
Marketing Association, Chicago, IL.
Further reading
Cartwright, D. (1959), Studies in Social Power, Research Center for Group Dynamics,
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI.
Hague, P. (1992), The Industrial Market Research Handbook, Kogan Page, London.
Hempel, D.J. (1975), “Family role structure and housing decisions”, in Schlinger, M.J. (Ed.),
Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 2, Association for Consumer Research,
Chicago, IL, pp. 71-80.
Kirk, J. and Miller, M.L. (1986), Reliability and Validity in Qualitative Research, Qualitative
Research Methods Series, a Sage University paper, Sage Publications, Beverly Hills, CA.
Lee, C.K.C. and Marshall, R. (1993), “Who do we ask and when? A pilot study about research
in family decision making”, in Levy, M. and Grewal, D. (Eds), Developments in
Marketing Science, Proceedings of the Annual Conference of the Academy of
Marketing Science, Miami, FL, Vol. 16, pp. 30-5.
Roberts, M.L., Wortzel, L.H. and Berkeley, R.L. (1981), “Mothers’ attitudes and perceptions of
children’s influence and their effect on family consumption”, in Anderson, B.B. (Ed.),
Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 3, Association for Consumer Research, Ann
Arbor, MI, pp. 730-5.
Taylor, S.J. and Bogdan, R. (1998), Introduction to Qualitative Research Methods: A
Guidebook and Resource, John Wiley and Sons, New York, NY.
Vuchinich, S., Emery, R.E. and Cassidy, J. (1988), “Family members as third parties in dyadic
family conflict: strategies, alliances and outcomes”, Child Development, Vol. 59, pp.
1293-302.
The Emerald Research Register for this journal is available at The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available at
www.emeraldinsight.com/researchregister www.emeraldinsight.com/1463-578X.htm
Valuation
Multicultural examination of behaviour
valuation behaviour
Julian Diaz III
Georgia State University, Atlanta, Georgia, USA 339
Paul Gallimore
The Nottingham Trent University, Nottingham, UK Received January 2002
Accepted December 2002
Deborah Levy
The University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
Keywords Value analysis, Individual behaviour, United Kingdom, United States of
America, New Zealand
Abstract A series of experiments were conducted to examine valuation behaviour in the UK, the
USA, and New Zealand (NZ). Professional valuers from all three countries participated in the
study whose findings support the notion that the US normative model is cognitively demanding
and that greater departures from it result in reduced cognitive effort. The study also concluded
that subjects from cultures requiring disclosure (USA and NZ) examined a significantly greater
number of sales than did subjects from the UK where disclosure is uncommon. Finally, while
valuers perhaps ought to increase sales search in unfamiliar markets, this research revealed no
evidence that they do so. These findings are consistent with the need to seek cognitive efficiency
and reduce cognitive effort even at the expense of performance quality.
Introduction
Valuation behaviour has been the focus of an expanding literature recently reviewed
in Diaz (1999) and Hardin (1999). For example the processes actually followed by
valuers, as contrasted with those prescribed to them, have been investigated in Diaz
(1990a). Here evidence was developed that residential appraisers from the USA do
not follow prescribed (normative) processes. This finding was expanded in Diaz et al.
(2000), who concluded that the actual valuation processes of New Zealand (NZ), US
and UK valuers deviated from processes prescribed in the USA, but that NZ valuer
processes were more normative than US valuer processes, which were in turn more
normative than those of UK valuers. Further, the actual processes of NZ valuers did
not vary significantly from US valuer processes, but both varied significantly from the
processes actually employed by UK valuers.
Diaz (1990a) argued that normative processes are cognitively demanding and that
valuers depart from them in a subconscious search for efficiency. Compelled by
cognitive limitations characterising all humans, this search for efficiency is ubiquitous
in all human problem solving. Because increasing cognitive efficiency is associated
with decreasing time on task, an expectation is that the more normative (cognitively
demanding) a valuation process is, the greater the time on task. Finding evidence
supporting this expectation is one goal of this research. Journal of Property Investment &
The process of selecting comparable sales has been studied in Diaz (1990b), Finance
Vol. 22 No. 4, 2004
Wolverton (1996), and Gallimore and Wolverton (1997). Diaz modelled the sales pp. 339-346
selection processes of expert US residential appraisers and contrasted these processes q Emerald Group Publishing Limited
1463-578X
with those of novices. Wolverton discovered that knowledge of subject transaction DOI 10.1108/14635780410550894
JPIF price could bias the comparable sales selected by US residential appraisers. Gallimore
22,4 and Wolverton extended this study to the UK and concluded that UK valuers were
less susceptible to price knowledge biases during sales selection than were their US
counterparts probably because of cultural differences in the transparency of
comparable sale selection. In the USA, comparable sales used to form a value opinion
must be disclosed. Such disclosure or transparency is not common in the UK.
340 Gallimore and Wolverton also discovered that in their experiments, UK valuers
engaged in significantly less sales search effort than did US subjects. Once again this
was attributed to transparency (disclosure). Disclosure requirements of NZ valuers are
similar to those of US appraisers. This suggests that sales search effort expended,
measured by number of sales examined, by NZ valuers should be similar to US
appraisers but significantly different than that of UK valuers. Examining this
expectation is the second goal of this investigation.
Some research has indicated that valuer behaviour may change when property
settings change from familiar geographic settings to unfamiliar ones. For example
Diaz (1997) discovered that US appraisers valuing properties in areas familiar to them
were not susceptible to the value opinions of other anonymous experts, but Diaz and
Hansz (1997) reported that US appraisers valuing property in unfamiliar areas were
susceptible to such influences. Diaz et al. (2000) rejected the hypothesis that valuer
processes would be driven toward normative models when valuers value property in
areas unfamiliar to them. Nevertheless a compelling prescription for optimal valuation
performance suggests that valuers unfamiliar with the geographic area of the subject
being appraised need to expend more effort to become knowledgeable about an
unknown market, that is, prescriptively they should examine more sales to value
property in unfamiliar markets than in familiar markets. Arguing against this
expectation is the need for cognitive efficiency and the associated reduced time on
task as well as the routinisation of production rules such as “to value property you
need about three sales.” Providing evidence of sales search effort in familiar versus
unfamiliar markets is the third goal of this research.
Research hypotheses
The goals of this investigation suggest three research hypotheses. The first derives
from the expectation that the more normative a valuation process is, the more
cognitively demanding it is and therefore the more time on task it will require. From
Diaz et al. (2000) comes the finding that while NZ, US, and UK valuation processes
are not US normative, NZ processes are slightly more normative than US processes
which are significantly more normative than UK processes. Research hypothesis 1
(H1) is therefore that NZ valuers will spend more time on valuation tasks than will
US valuers (although perhaps not significantly more) who will spend more time on
valuation tasks than will UK valuers:
where
T ¼ time on task;
NZ ¼ New Zealand valuers;
US ¼ US valuers; and Valuation
UK ¼ UK valuers. behaviour
The second goal of this research is to examine the expectation that comparable sales
disclosure, labelled transparency by Gallimore and Wolverton, is associated with
sales search, that the number of sales examined is smaller for valuation tasks in
cultures not requiring disclosure. Since New Zealand and the USA require similar 341
levels of disclosure which are uncommon in the UK, research hypothesis 2 (H2) is
that the number of sales examined by NZ and US valuers will be similar to each other
and greater than the number examined by UK valuers:
SNZ ¼ SUS . SUK ð2Þ
S S
NZf ¼ NZu ð3aÞ
S S
USf ¼ USu ð3bÞ
S S
UKf ¼ UKu ð3cÞ
where:
f ¼ subject properties in familiar markets; and
u ¼ subject properties in unfamiliar markets.
Methods
To examine the three research hypotheses, data were developed from one-factor, repeated
measures experiments modeled after Diaz (1990a). The one-factor, repeated measures
design reduces unwanted variability (“noise”) by focusing on one variable of interest
thereby maximising the probability of finding statistical significance. The factor
examined, geographic familiarity, was fixed at two levels, high positive geographic
familiarity and high negative geographic familiarity. While there is no scale or precise
measure of geographic familiarity, generally valuers will be highly familiar with areas in
which they routinely value property and unfamiliar with areas in which they have never
valued property. Therefore to operationalise the two factor levels, two appraisal cases
were constructed for the UK experiment and two for the NZ experiment. High positive
familiarity was implemented for the UK experiment with a case developed from
Nottingham, UK, the city in which participating UK experimental subjects practice, and
high negative familiarity was implemented with a case set near the city of Atlanta, USA,
an area unfamiliar to these subjects. For the NZ experiment,
JPIF experts were recruited from the city of Auckland and an Auckland property served as
22,4 the high positive familiarity case with the Atlanta property used as the high negative
familiarity case. Slight differences in the Atlanta case used in the UK experiment
versus the Atlanta case used in the NZ experiment were due to differences in the
respective vernacular.
The experimental cases were constructed to be representative of real world
342 residential appraisal assignments and were developed from actual appraisal
assignments and their resulting reports. Several expert appraisers were used to
validate the representativeness of the cases. Geographically familiar cases are
representative of the type of assignments encountered in practice by the participating
experts. Geographically unfamiliar cases, while constructed to be representative of
real world assignments, are by design not representative of realistic assignments faced
by participating experts. They are therefore realistic problems, but for the
participating expert, not realistic assignments. The objective of this design is to see if
expert behaviour will vary when experts are taken out of their area of expertise. Will
experts seek and consider more sales data when confronted with realistic valuation
problems in areas unfamiliar (unrealistic) to them compared to realistic valuation
problems in areas familiar (realistic) to them?
A total of 12 expert valuers participated in the UK experiment whereas ten experts
participated in the NZ study. Also 12 experts participated in the original US study
(Diaz, 1990a). Experts were defined as practising appraisers of no less than five years
valuation experience with appropriate professional credentials. These experts
performed both the familiar and unfamiliar tasks. Each task required the subject to
estimate market value based upon the available information. Sessions were initiated
by presenting the subject with a brief written introduction and request for basic
demographic information. When the demographic information sheet was returned to
the experimenter, the experimenter gave the subject detailed written instructions for
the experiment followed by a worksheet restating briefly the instructions and
providing a list of available data (cues). These alphabetically ordered cue labels
represented the information suggested by the US normative model and therefore
available to the subject during the experiment.
The subject was required to select verbally a desired cue label. The experimenter
then recorded the request, provided the subject with the requested information, and
began to time cue usage. When finished with the information, the subject returned it
to the experimenter and requested the next desired information. The experimenter
refiled the used information, recorded the time of usage, recorded the new request,
provided the newly requested information, and began to time usage of the newly
acquired cue. Among the recordings made by the experimenter was the number of
sales examined. A total of four packets containing three sales each or a total of 12
comparable sales were available for each case. Subjects were free to examine as many
or as few sales packets as they desired. To minimize deviation in the response
variable due to extraneous factors, strict quality control was observed. The same
written instructions were used for all study subjects. All verbal communications
between experimenter and subject were kept to a minimum during experimental
sessions. The first experimental session in Nottingham occurred on July 8, 1999 and
the final one on January 11, 2000. The Auckland sessions began on July 21, 1998 and
ended on July 30, 1998. US data came from the original Diaz (1990a) experiments.
Results Valuation
Data were analysed using the most statistically powerful tests appropriate. Parametric behaviour
tests are generally more powerful than non-parametric analogues, that is, given equal
sample sizes, parametric tests are more likely to detect true significance than similar
non-parametric tests. Parametric tests are not always appropriate however. For
example, parametric tests require continuous data. Discrete data must be analysed
with non-parametric tests. Further, parametric tests have more restrictive 343
assumptions. To ensure that assumptions are generally met, sample sizes of 30 or
more are generally needed to use parametric tests. Databases of fewer than 30
observations may meet parametric assumptions, but they may not. Since the data of
these experiments are in small samples, both powerful parametric tests, whose
assumptions may not be met by the data, and the less powerful non-parametric
analogues whose assumptions are met by the data are employed when appropriate in
the study.
Total time, in seconds, spent on geographically familiar tasks is reported in Table I.
This table records the time taken by each subject to complete the experimental task
including gathering all the desired data and performing whatever analysis the subject
deemed necessary to determine an estimate of value. This variable does not include
time taken to write a report or document or support the value judgement. Once the
valuer determined a value estimate, time measurement ceased. A quick check of
the mean time for task completion across the three cultures reveals results consistent
with H1 (equation (1)). For all NZ subjects when performing valuation tasks in familiar
locations, the mean time to complete is 2534.4 seconds, for US experts 2,091.1 seconds,
and for UK experts 1,846.4 seconds. To determine whether these differences are
significant, a parametric analysis of variance and a non-parametric Kruskal-Wallis
test were conducted. P-values for these tests are 0.002 and 0.001 respectively indicating
that there is an overall significant difference in central tendency among these data.
A series of two-sample comparisons were made to clarify where the significant
differences are found. The parametric t-test and the non-parametric Mann-Whitney
test were employed. The difference between total time on task for NZ versus
US subjects, while in the anticipated direction, is not significant ( p-values of 0.186
and 0.385). The difference between US and UK time on task is significant ( p-values
1 12 9 3 3 9 12
2 6 6 9 12 12 12
3 12 12 9 6 3 6
4 12 9 12 12 6 6
5 9 6 12 12 9 12
6 12 12 12 12 6 6
7 6 6 12 9 9 9
8 12 9 6 6 6 3
9 12 6 6 12 3 3
10 12 12 6 6 6 9
Table II. 11 12 12 6 3
Number of sales 12 6 6 6 3
examined Mean 10.5 8.7 8.75 9 6.75 7
level. Tests based on individual level comparisons are more powerful than those based Valuation
on group level comparisons. Test were conducted for the NZ data (familiar versus behaviour
unfamiliar) the US data (familiar versus unfamiliar) and the UK data (familiar versus
unfamiliar). The results of the tests are mixed. None support the prescription of greater
sales search in unfamiliar markets. Two, the US data and the UK data (both with
p-values of 0.705), support the no difference hypothesis. The significant difference in
the NZ data (p 7 value ¼ 0:034) is in the wrong direction to support the suggestion that 345
valuers should engage in greater sales search in unknown markets. New Zealand
valuers participating in the experiment examined significantly fewer sales in the
unfamiliar case.
Conclusions
The amount of time spent on task by valuers from the three cultures represented in this
investigation support the notion that the US normative model is cognitively
demanding and that greater departures from it result in greater reductions in effort.
The processes of NZ valuers which were closest to the US normative model, were
associated with the greatest time on task. The US processes, less normative than the
NZ processes but not significantly so, resulted in less time on task compared to the NZ
valuers but again not significantly so. The UK processes, significantly less normative
than those of US valuers, resulted in significantly less time on task.
Data from this investigation generally are consistent with the idea that disclosure
requirements (transparency) are associated with sales search effort. Subjects from
cultures requiring disclosure, the USA and NZ, tended to examine a greater amount of
sales than did subjects from the UK where disclosure is uncommon. Valuers from the
US and UK did not significantly increase the number of sales examined when moving
from familiar to unfamiliar geographic settings. NZ valuers significantly decreased the
number of sales examined when moving to unfamiliar markets. While valuers perhaps
ought to increase sales search in unfamiliar markets, this research does not find any
evidence that they do. Rather the findings are consistent with the ubiquitous need to
seek cognitive efficiency and reduce cognitive effort even at the expense of
performance quality.
Other factors not specifically addressed in the experiments probably play a role in
the time-on-task and number-of-sales-examined behaviours discovered in this study.
Fee levels may be important. The general perception among experts in all three
cultures is that fee levels for valuation services are low across all property types and
while stable in nominal terms, are eroding in real terms. Information availability,
greater in the USA and NZ than in the UK, likely exacerbates the impact of
transparency and may contribute to cultural differences. The opinion among some
experts is that the UK will move closer to the USA in terms of reporting requirements
although there is no current sign that UK education is anticipating this by more
explicit promotion of formalised approaches to valuation procedures that embody
aspects of the US normative model. Such a move toward greater transparency will
likely be accompanied by increased information availability as comparable sales
information moves into the public domain. These movements may require greater
time-on-task and sales-search from UK experts. The current UK fee structure, already
perceived as low, and the lack of an education response are factors that contravening
factors.
JPIF Why NZ valuers participating in the experiment, unlike those from either the USA
22,4 or the UK, examined significantly fewer sales in the unfamiliar case is a matter for
speculation. There are no normative reasons for this behaviour, but it is consistent with
a strategy to minimise cognitive effort by reducing sales search through early closure
perhaps by anchoring onto comparables encountered at the beginning of the search
process. The finding is clearly worthy of more investigation.
346 This study involved valuation experts valuing single family homes. Further
research is needed to extend these conclusions into commercial settings. Nevertheless
these results add to our growing understanding of actual valuation behaviour and the
role of distinct cultural environments. Of future interest is the quality of performance
among processes of varying cognitive efficiency. In a shrinking world characterised
by a global economy, greater understanding of the differing valuation processes and
their impact on valuation performance is an acute need.
References
Diaz, J. (1990a), “How appraisers do their work: a test of the appraisal process and the
development of a descriptive model”, The Journal of Real Estate Research, Vol. 5
No. 1, pp. 1-15.
Diaz, J. (1990b), “The process of selecting comparable sales”, The Appraisal Journal, Vol. 58
October, pp. 533-40.
Diaz, J. (1997), “An investigation into the impact of previous expert value estimates on
appraisal judgment”, Journal of Real Estate Research, Vol. 13 No. 1, pp. 57-66.
Diaz, J. (1999), “The first decade of behavioral research in the discipline of property”, Journal
of Property Investment & Finance, Vol. 17 No. 4, pp. 326-32.
Diaz, J. and Hansz, J.A. (1997), “How valuers use the value opinions of others”, Journal of
Property Valuation and Investment, Vol. 15 No. 3, pp. 256-60.
Diaz, J., Gallimore, P. and Levy, D. (2000), “Residential valuation behaviour in the United
States, the United Kingdom, and New Zealand”, paper presented to the RICS Cutting
Edge Property Research Conference, London.
Gallimore, P. and Wolverton, M. (1997), “Price-knowledge-induced bias: a cross-cultural
comparison”, Journal of Property Valuation and Investment, Vol. 15 No. 3, pp. 261-73.
Hardin, W. (1999), “Behavioral research into heuristics and bias as an academic pursuit:
lessons from other disciplines and implications for real estate”, Journal of Property
Investment & Finance, Vol. 17 No. 4, pp. 333-52.
Wolverton, M. (1996), “Investigation into price knowledge induced comparable sale selection
bias”, dissertation, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA.