Family Functioning and Stress in African American Families: A Strength-Based Approach
Family Functioning and Stress in African American Families: A Strength-Based Approach
research-article2014
JBPXXX10.1177/0095798413520451Journal of Black PsychologyPollock et al.
Article
Journal of Black Psychology
1–26
Family Functioning © The Author(s) 2014
Reprints and permissions:
and Stress in African sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0095798413520451
American Families: jbp.sagepub.com
A Strength-Based
Approach
Abstract
Having social support substantially reduces the effects of stressful
experiences. Family relationships are central components of social support
for African Americans. In a community-based sample of African Americans
(n = 255), the relationship between family functioning and stress was
examined, as well as possible mediators of this relationship, independent
of demographic variables. Using multiple regression analysis, close and
flexible family relationships were linked to lower perceived stress levels. The
association of family functioning and stress operated through the internal
processes of anxiety, depression, daily hassles, and higher hardiness and
explained more than half of the variance in stress levels. These findings also
remained above and beyond the known stressor of discrimination and the
known stress reducer of spirituality. These findings suggest that expanding
traditional stress management programs to include strategies for bolstering
family functioning could have significant benefits.
Keywords
family systems, stress, anxiety, depression, hardiness
Corresponding Author:
Elizabeth Davenport Pollock, Department of Military & Emergency Medicine, Uniformed
Services University of the Health Sciences, 4301 Jones Bridge Road, Bethesda, MD 20814, USA.
Email: [email protected]
and cohesion, while the broader context includes cultural, psychological, and
psychosocial factors. The focus throughout is on applications to African
Americans in particular.
Family Functioning
The present study is informed by family systems theory, which views the
family as an identifiable system and not just a collection of individuals: It is
an interacting system and an entity itself. A key component of systems theory
is that “the whole is greater than the sum of its parts” (Whitchurch &
Constantine, 2004, p. 328). Thus, one must examine systemic concepts that
demonstrate interactions within the system. Constructs such as flexibility and
closeness demonstrate systemic interactions that form overall family
functioning.
As a system, families function with two predominant characteristics: flex-
ibility (moving from rigid to flexible) and cohesion (from distant to close;
Olson, 2000). Optimal family functioning is considered to be a balance of
cohesion and flexibility, with suboptimal functioning at the extremes (i.e., too
close or too disengaged; too chaotic or too rigid; Kane, 2000; Olson, 2000).
Flexible families who remain close tend to have the highest functioning mem-
bers (McCreary & Dancy, 2004). Balanced levels of cohesion and flexibility
help stabilize family systems. According to the concept of homeostasis from
systems theory, individuals strive for stability through balancing the system
(Whitchurch & Constantine, 2004). Cohesion pulls families close while flex-
ibility allows them to adapt to change, simultaneously stabilizing the system.
If systems are not balanced, then they can cause stress as they strive to resta-
bilize (Whitchurch & Constantine, 2004), or conversely, systems that are bal-
anced could help protect its members from extra stress. This is the main
premise of our study. Families that are able to adapt and change while remain-
ing close are theoretically better able to meet the challenge of stressors.
Being close and flexible also tends to be key characteristics of African
American families (Hill, 1998b; Pinderhughes, 2002). For example, rights
and responsibilities in the household tend to be shared with children, and kin-
ship obligations can shift from parents to other family members or close
friends (McLoyd, Cauce, Takeuchi, & Wilson, 2000). Indeed, African
Americans tend to have a more expanded definition of family that demon-
strates flexibility. Hill (1998b) defines African American families as “con-
stellations of households related by blood or marriage or function that
provides basic instrumental and expressive functions of the family to the
members of those networks” (p. 18). These characteristics also lend to family
systems that value interdependence or collectivism (Karenga, 2007; McLoyd
et al., 2000), which theoretically protect family members and reduce stress.
On the other hand, family relationships can also be direct sources of stress
(Neighbors, 1997). Kasper et al. (2008) examined persistent family stress
over 30 years and found that family stress predicted physical health, pain, and
depression among African American women, independent of socioeconomic
status. Additionally, a majority of respondents to the National Survey of
Black Americans Panel Study cited interpersonal difficulties as a major
source of stress (Neighbors, 1997). These findings show a significant link
between family functioning and individual outcomes.
Depression and Anxiety. Stress responses induce depression and anxiety (Kes-
sler, 1997), and correlates of anxiety and depression likely vary by racial
group, along with various cultural, contextual, and family variables (Anesh-
ensel, 2009; Hunter & Schmidt, 2010; McGuire & Miranda, 2008). For
instance, Herman, Ostrander, and Tucker (2007) reported that low family
cohesion was related to depression among African American adolescents.
Dearing (2004) also found racial differences in a study of parenting styles
and depression and reported that whereas restrictive parenting had negative
effects among Caucasian children, it had positive effects among African
American children. Last, in a theoretical review of depression in African
American urban youth, Hammack (2003) highlighted the importance of the
quality of social interactions in combination with individual factors for a
more integrated model to better understand depression.
Hardiness. How one views life and events has a significant impact on stress
levels. For example, having a positive outlook on life and perceiving to be in
Hypotheses
As described above, multiple factors may be related to family functioning
and stress. We currently have a better understanding of the strengths related
to African American family functioning, how different aspects of family
functioning are related to individual outcomes, and of some factors associ-
ated with elevated (or reduced) risk for stress in African Americans. What
remains unknown and the primary aim of this article is to understand how
family functioning affects stress for African Americans. We hypothesized
that flexible and close family functioning would be negatively related to indi-
vidual perceived stress. Furthermore, we speculated that the relationship
between family functioning and stress levels would be influenced by selected
mediator variables: depression, anxiety, daily hassles, and hardiness. Through
examining these mediational hypotheses, we expected to gain a better under-
standing of how family functioning is linked to individual perceived stress in
African American families. We tested our hypotheses by controlling for mul-
tiple factors that can influence family functioning and stress, such as gender,
Variables Percentage
Age, years 43.9 (11.1)
Females 63
Number of childrena 4.2 (1.9)
College degree 34.1
Marital status: Separated 24.7
Marital status: Married 20.8
Participant definition of family
Family of origin 65.1
Partner and/or kids 49.4
Sibling 48.2
Nonbiological kin 29.8
Extended biological family 30.2
Note: Values are percentages except mean (SD); afor families with six or more children,
coded 6, therefore, the mean reported is lower than the actual.
Method
Participants included 255 self-identified African Americans men and women
aged between 18 and 60 years of age. They were recruited through newspaper
ads and community bulletins for a larger experiment addressing health dis-
parities in a large metropolitan city in the mid-Atlantic region. The study was
approved by the university’s institutional review board and written informed
consent was obtained from all participants. Sample characteristics are pre-
sented in Table 1. Most participants were African American (82.9%), fol-
lowed by African (8.9%) and African Caribbean (2.2%). Most participants
were female (63%), and mean age was 44.1 ± 11.1 years. Responses to “who
did you include in your definition of family” were grouped into five catego-
ries (see bottom of Table 1). Most participants included parents/stepparents
(65.1%), but many also included extended biological family (30.2%) and
nonbiological kin (29.8%). These categories of family members were not
mutually exclusive, and 59.6% of participants included individuals from two
or more of the categories.
Table 2. Questionnaire Mean (SD) Scores, Range, and Reliability Coefficient
(n = 255, Except Where Noted).
Measures
The questionnaires for family functioning (independent variable), stress (the
dependent variable), four potential mediators (depression, anxiety, hassles,
hardiness), and additional covariates of interest (discrimination, spirituality)
are described below. Mean values for psychological questionnaires, along
with Cronbach’s alpha, are presented in Table 2.
total score. The scale has good reliability (reliability coefficient = 0.83) and
internal consistency (Bartone, 1995).
Anxiety. Anxiety was measured with the State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI,
Form Y; Speiberger, 1983). The STAI consists of 40 questions, answered on a
4-point Likert-type scale. Half of the items assess state or transitory feelings of
anxiety, and half of them assess trait or stable individual differences in anxiety.
This widely used scale has demonstrated adequate reliability across different
ethnic groups (Novy, Nelson, Goodwin, & Rowzee, 1993; Speiberger, 1983).
Since the STAI was added to the study after it began, 27% of the participants
do not have STAI data. For analyses involving the STAI, demographics of
participants with and without STAI data are compared with each other.
Daily Hassles. Levels of everyday stressors were assessed using the Daily
Hassles Scale (DHS; Kanner et al., 1981), a 117-item inventory of everyday
hassles (e.g., “too many responsibilities,” “care of pets,” etc.). Participants
rate whether they experienced each hassle in the past week, and if they did,
then they rate the hassle’s severity on a 3-point scale (somewhat, moderately,
or extremely severe). The DHS produces a frequency score (a count of has-
sles that were endorsed), along with severity (sum of the severity ratings) and
intensity (severity divided by frequency) scores. The present study used the
frequency score as a measure of daily stressors.
Analysis
Mean values and psychometric properties are reported for each question-
naire, along with participant demographics, with a focus on family structure
(or which participants are considered to be in their family). Multiple regres-
sion analysis was used to test the relationship between family functioning
(FACESIV, TCR) and stress (PSS). All reported regressions controlled for
demographics, which included age, gender, education level, number of chil-
dren in family, and marital status. Education was used to represent socioeco-
nomic status instead of income, because 11.4% of participants preferred not
to report income. Since there can be significant differences between catego-
ries, we wanted to examine the association of our main variables of interest
above and beyond the control variables; therefore, we dichotomized demo-
graphic variables to indicate participants with a college degree or higher,
marital status of divorced or married (coded separately), and last, number of
children was coded as 0 to 5, or, for families with six or more, 6.
Analyses were conducted to determine potential mediators of the relation-
ship between family functioning and stress, which included depression
(BDI), anxiety (STAI), hardiness (DRS), and hassles (DHS). Mediation was
tested using Baron and Kenny’s (1986) steps: (1) the independent variable (in
this case, family functioning) was regressed onto the dependent variable
(stress); (2) in a separate model, the independent variable was regressed onto
the mediator (in this case, hardiness, depression, anxiety, and hassles); (3) in
yet another model, the mediator was regressed onto the dependent variable;
and (4) in a final model, the independent variable was regressed onto the
dependent variable while controlling for the mediator. The Sobel test (Sobel,
1982) was used to assess whether the mediation effect was statistically sig-
nificant based on the coefficients and standard errors from Steps (2) and (3)
above. If the effect was significant (based on the Sobel test), the percentage
of the relationship explained by the mediator was assessed based on the find-
ings from Steps (1) and (4) above (Szklo & Nieto, 2006). Last, discrimination
(GED) and spirituality (DSE) were assessed as covariates, in order to deter-
mine whether they affected the mediation analysis above. Additionally, their
relationship to family functioning and stress was also assessed.
Results
Family Functioning and Stress
Family functioning was negatively associated with stress, β = −.32, t(255) =
−5.00, p < .001, R2 = .12, when controlling for demographics variables (age,
sex, education, family structure, and marital status). Regression results are
presented in Table 3.
Mediator Variables
Mediation was assessed by determining (1) whether family functioning was
associated with the mediator variable, (2) whether family functioning was asso-
ciated with the dependent stress levels, and (3) when adding the mediator vari-
able into the model with the family functioning and stress, whether the mediator
variable was associated with the dependent variable. The following mediators
were tested: depression, anxiety, daily hassles, and hardiness. Summary results
from mediation analyses are graphically depicted in Figure 1.
Anxiety
Out of the 255 participants, 63 did not complete the measured of anxiety
(STAI). Compared with the rest of the sample, participants with missing anx-
iety data did not differ along the other psychological questionnaires, but they
Note: “DV” = dependent variable; “→” indicates which DV is used; n = 255 unless noted
(^n = 192 for models involving Anxiety), independent of control variables, demographics
(age, gender, number of children, marital status), and education level. **Significant at p < .001.
Each line represents a unique regression model. Labels are as follows in the diagram: a = the
association of family functioning (FF) and stress, independent of control variables; b = FF and
mediator; c = mediator and stress; a2 = the association of FF and stress, including the impact
of the mediator. Sobel equation used to determine mediation significance.
Figure 1. The association of family functioning and stress: Percent mediated by
anxiety, depression, daily hassles, and hardiness, independent of demographics.
Note: Percent mediation determined by Sobel equation, presented in %; in parentheses, next
to percent mediation, is the standardized regression coefficient for the association of family
functioning and stress, including the mediator and control variables; in standardized regression
coefficients for the relationship between the variables, the arrow is moving from and pointing
toward is in italics.
were on average younger (41.2 ± 11.6 vs. 44.7 ± 10.9, t = 2.2, p < .05), were
more likely to have a college degree (46% vs. 30%, χ2[1, N = 255] = 5.3, p <
.05), and were more likely to be married/living together (35% vs. 16%, χ2[1,
N = 255] = 10.2, p < .05). Within this sample, family functioning was related
to anxiety (β = −.37, t[192] = −4.63, p < .001), and anxiety was related to
stress (β = .61, t[192] = 11.3, p < .005). Anxiety was a significant mediator
(Sobel test = −3.53, p < .001), and after controlling for anxiety, the relation-
ship between family functioning and stress decreased (β = −.17, t[255] =
−2.88, p < .005, R2 = .47), mediating 30.9% of the relationship.
Depression
Family functioning was related to depression (β = −.27, t[254] = −4.27, p <
.001), and depression was related to stress (β = 0.65, t[254] = 12.87, p <
.001). Depression was a significant mediator (Sobel test = −4.85, p < .001),
and after controlling for depression, the relationship between family func-
tioning and stress decreased (β = −.15, t[254] = −2.92, p < .05, R2 = .44),
mediating 31.7% of the relationship.
Hassles
Family functioning was related to daily hassles (β = −.22, t[254] = −3.52, p <
.001), and daily hassles were related to stress (β = .51, t[254] = 9.06, p <
.001). Daily hassles was a significant mediator (Sobel test = −3.25, p < .001),
and after controlling for daily hassles, the relationship between family func-
tioning and stress decreased (β = −.21, t[254] = −3.70, p < .001, R2 = .31),
mediating 20.1% of the relationship.
Hardiness
Family functioning was related to hardiness (β =.32, t[254] = 5.09, p < .001),
and hardiness was negatively related to stress (β = −.44, t[254] = −7.6, p <
.001). Hardiness was a significant mediator (Sobel test = −4.2, p < .001), and
after controlling hardiness, the relationship between family functioning and
stress decreased (β = −.20, t[254] = −3.16, p < .002, R2 = .22), mediating
23.5% of the relationship.
Discrimination
Mean scores on the GED scale (46.3 ± 20.8; past year: 41.6 ± 15.8; lifetime:
41.6 ± 15.8) were comparable to those reported from other African American
samples (Landrine et al., 2006). Controlling for demographic variables,
appraised discrimination was not related to family functioning (β = −.12, p =
.06). As expected, appraised discrimination was related to perceived stress
(β = .24, p < .001), and controlling for discrimination somewhat reduced the
effect size of family functioning on stress (from β = −.32 to β = −.28, p <
.005), indicating that it accounted for some, but not much, of the relationship
between family functioning and stress. Adding discrimination did not signifi-
cantly alter any of the mediation analyses above. Discrimination emerged as
a significant predictor of stress in the hassles (β = .12, p < .05) and hardiness
(β = .20, p < .005) mediational models, but this had a minimal effect on the
effect sizes of the other variables within those models.
Spirituality
Spirituality was related to both stress (β = .22, p < .001) and family function-
ing (β = −.16, p < .01; Note: low scores indicate high spirituality). Similar to
appraised discrimination, controlling for spirituality somewhat reduced the
effect size of family functioning (β = −.32 to β = −.29, p < .005), indicating
that it accounted for some but not much of the relationship between family
functioning and stress. When it was added to the mediational analyses, the
DSE did not have a significant effect on the model. It did come out as a sig-
nificant predictor of stress in the hassles mediational model (β = 0.13, p <
.05), but this had a minimal effect on the effect sizes of the variables within
that model.
Discussion
Following a family systems approach, the association of family functioning
and individual stress levels was examined, as were factors that could influ-
ence this relationship. Findings from the current study build and extend pre-
vious research (Barbarin et al., 1999; Kane, 2000; Littlejohn-Blake &
Darling, 1993; McAdoo, 1982; Robbins et al., 2003), which has found that
specific aspects of family functioning influence individual outcomes (Johnson
& Jennison, 1994; McCabe et al., 1999). As hypothesized, we found that
close and flexible family relationships were linked to lower individual per-
ceived stress levels. This relationship also remained above and beyond the
known stress maximizer of discrimination and the known stress reducer of
spirituality. Our findings also provide an additional understanding of the
pathway from family functioning to stress levels, taking into account indi-
vidual processes, as our hypotheses were supported for mediation.
Specifically, the impact of family functioning on stress operated through the
internal processes of anxiety, depression and daily hassles, and the buffering
of hardiness.
Dancy, 2004; Robbins et al., 2003). Our findings also suggest that closeness and
flexibility are important stress-reducing properties for African Americans. The
relationship between positive (i.e., close and flexible) family functioning and
stress highlights the positive impact that specific characteristics of family rela-
tionships can have on individual stress levels. Family relationships can provide
valuable support (i.e., closeness) while adapting to changes (i.e., flexibility),
whereas families that are more distant and rigid do not confer these helpful
stress-reducing functions. Our findings clearly support that family relationships
can be “stress-absorbing systems” for African Americans (McAdoo, 1982, p.
479)—when balanced with closeness and flexibility.
Our measure of family functioning (FAVESIV) covered a continuum of
functioning, allowing for the distinction between those individuals from fam-
ilies with balanced levels of closeness and flexibility and those from families
with apparently distant and rigid family dynamics. This spectrum offers a
more thorough understanding of family functioning than dichotomizing as
positive versus negative family functioning and shows that family function-
ing and relationships can have both positive and negative effects on stress
(Neighbors, 1997). Respondents in our study who reported being from a fam-
ily with more balanced levels reported lower stress. Indeed, other research
has defined positive or “effective” family functioning within African
American families as being affectionate, communicative (sharing, advising,
and encouraging), doing things as a family, helping each other, and appropri-
ate parenting (McCreary & Dancy, 2004)—characteristics of closeness and
flexibility. Hill (1998a) emphasizes that positive communication, social
engagement, and flexible functioning are key resilience-building characteris-
tics for African American families.
Practical Applications
Our findings identify key factors related to stress that can be focused on for
programming and practical applications. First, positive family functioning
can have multiple benefits, given that it was related to many individual pro-
cesses. It could be helpful to expand traditional stress management programs
and psycho-education about stress to include the potential positive effect of
family functioning. Second, family functioning may act through individual
processes as well. Helping families balance flexibility and closeness might
confer valuable stress-buffering qualities that are twofold—help individual
and family processes. And since families operate as a system, the stress-
reduction properties could reverberate throughout the family—providing
stress relief for more than one individual. Indeed, Brody et al. (2010) sug-
gested that positive, supportive relationships can help “block the effects of
life stress” by helping individuals be able to downregulate negative emotions,
increase positive emotions, and work toward goal accomplishment. Close
and flexible family relationships can be fostered through focusing on the fol-
lowing behaviors/aspects: being emotionally nurturing and affectionate to
each other; sharing, advising, and encouraging each other verbally or behav-
iorally; doing activities as a family; being helpful in tangible ways; and par-
enting children effectively (McCreary & Dancy, 2004). Practitioners who
work with families can focus on building positive functioning through these
enhancing behaviors.
Last, multiple levels of stressors and resources were examined in our
study. Our findings that hardiness and family functioning are linked highlight
Limitations
Our findings should also be considered within the context of the study’s limi-
tations. Since our sample is community based, it is not a representative sam-
ple and only generalizable to African Americans in our large, metropolitan
area. Additionally, examining multiple individuals in the same family would
help us better examine family functioning from multiple perspectives. Also,
what is unknown, and possibly a topic for future research, is how multiple
positive resources, such as family relationships and spirituality, work together
to impact individual processes and combat stressors in daily life.
on a specific dyad within the family system. Our findings indicate that close
and flexible family relationships are linked to lower stress levels, whereas
individuals from distant and rigid families report higher stress levels.
Additionally, the association of healthy family functioning on lower stress
operated through internal processes—lower anxiety, depression, and daily
hassles and higher hardiness—independent of control factors. By examining
individual, family, and contextual factors, we were able to have a better
global understanding of how family functioning affects individual processes
that in turn affect stress levels. Our findings suggest that expanding tradi-
tional stress management programs to include bolstering and enhancing fam-
ily relationships could have significant benefits. Last, our study fills a gap in
the literature with regard to increasing the understanding of family function-
ing within African American families from a strength-based perspective.
Authors’ Note
The opinions and assertions expressed herein are those of the authors and should not
be construed as reflecting those of the Uniformed Services University of the Health
Sciences or the Department of Defense.
Funding
The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research,
authorship, and/or publication of this article: The project was funded by NCMHD,
National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland, Establishing Exploratory NCMHD
Research Centers of Excellence (P20), RFA-MD-07-001.
References
Aneshensel, C. S. (2009). Towards explaining mental health disparities. Journal of
Health and Human Behavior, 50, 377-394.
Barbarin, O., Whitten, C., Bond, S., & Conner-Warren, R. (1999). The social and cul-
tural context of coping with sickle cell disease: III. Stress, coping tasks, family
functioning, and children’s adjustment. Journal of Black Psychology, 25, 356-377.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction
in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical consider-
ations. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182.
Barrera, M. (2000). Social support research in community psychology. In J. S. Rappaport
& E. Seidman (Eds.), Handbook of community psychology (pp. 214-247). New
York, NY: Kluwer Academic.
McCabe, K., Clark, R., & Barnett, D. (1999). Family protective factors among urban
African American youth. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 28, 137-150.
McCreary, L. L., & Dancy, B. L. (2004). Dimensions of family functioning:
Perspectives of low-income AA single-parent families. Journal of Marriage and
Family, 66, 690-701.
McGuire, T. G., & Miranda, J. (2008). New evidence regarding racial and ethnic
disparities in mental health: policy implications. Health Affairs (Millwood), 27,
393-403.
McLoyd, V., Cauce, A., Takeuchi, D., & Wilson, L. (2000). Marital processes and
parental socialization in families of color: A decade review of research. Journal
of Marriage and the Family, 62, 1070-1093.
Mosley-Howard, G. S., & Burgan Evans, C. (2000). Relationships and contemporary
experiences of the AA family: An ethnographic case study. Journal of Black
Studies, 30, 428-452.
Murry, V., Brown, P., Brody, G., Cutrona, C., & Simons, R. (2001). Racial dis-
crimination as a moderator of the links among stress, maternal psychological
functioning, and family relationships. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63,
915-926.
Murry, V., Bynum, M., Brody, G., Willert, A., & Stephens, D. (2001). African
American single mothers and children in context: A review of studies on risk and
resilience. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 4, 133-155.
Murry, V., Smith, E., & Hill, N. (2001). Race, ethnicity, and culture in studies of
families in context. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63, 911-914.
Neighbors, H. (1997). Husbands, wives, family, and friends: Sources of stress, sources
of support. In R. Taylor, J. Jackson, & L. Chatters (Eds.), Family life in Black
America (pp. 279-294). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Novy, D., Nelson, D., Goodwin, J., & Rowzee, R. (1993). Psychometric comparabil-
ity of the STAI for different ethnic subpopulations. Psychological Assessment,
5, 343-349.
Olson, D. (2000). Circumplex model of marital and family systems. Journal of Family
Therapy, 22, 144-167.
Olson, D. (2011). FACESIV and the circumplex model: Validation study. Journal of
Marital and Family Therapy, 3(1), 64-80.
Petterson, S., & Albers, A. (2001). Effects of poverty and maternal depression on
early child development. Child Development, 72, 1794-1813.
Pieterse, A., & Carter, R. (2007). An examination of the relationship between gen-
eral life stress, racism-related stress, and psychological health among Black men.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 54, 101-109.
Pinderhughes, E. (2002). African American marriage in the 20th century. Family
Process, 41, 269-282.
Robbins, M., Szapocznik, J., Tejeda, M., Samuels, D., Ironson, G., & Antoni, M.
(2003). The protective role of the family and social support network in a sample
of HIV-positive African American women: Results of a pilot study. Journal of
Black Psychology, 29, 17-37.
Sashidharan, T., Pawlow, L., & Pettibone, J. (2012). An examination of racial bias
in the Beck Depression Inventory-II. Cultural Diversity & Ethnic Minority
Psychology, 18, 203-209.
Sobel, M. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural
equation models. In S. Leinbardt (Ed.), Sociological methodology (pp. 290-312).
Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Speiberger, C. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Form Y). Menlo
Park, CA: Mind Garden.
Stack, C., & Burton, L. (1993). Kinscripts. Journal of Comparative Family Studies,
24, 157-170.
Szklo, M., & Nieto, F. (2006). Epidemiology: Beyond the basics (2nd ed.). Boston,
MA: Jones & Bartlett.
Taylor, R. (1995). Kinship support and maternal and adolescent well-being in eco-
nomically disadvantaged African-American families. Child Development, 66,
1585-1597.
Taylor, R., Tucker, M., Chatters, L., Jayakody, R., & Jackson, J. (1997). Recent demo-
graphic trends in African American family structure. In R. Taylor, J. Jackson, &
L. Chatters (Eds.), Family life in Black America (pp. 14-62). Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage.
Uchino, B. (2006). Social support and health: A review of physiological processes
potentially underlying links to disease outcomes. Journal of Behavioral Medicine,
29, 377-387.
Uchino, B., Cacioppo, J., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. (1996). The relationship between social
support and physiological processes: A review with emphasis on underlying
mechanisms and implications for health. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 488-531.
Utsey, S., Giesbrecht, N., Hook, J., & Stanard, P. (2008). Cultural, sociofamilial and
psychological resources that inhibit psychological distress in AA exposed to
stressful life events and race-related stress. Journal of Counseling Psychology,
55, 49-62.
Whitchurch, G., & Constantine, L. (2004). Systems theory. In P. Boss, W. Doherty,
R. LaRossa, W. Schumm, & S. Steinmetz (Eds.), Sourcebook of family theories
and methods: A contextual approach (pp. 325-352). New York, NY: Springer.
Williams, D., Yu, Y., Jackson, J., & Anderson, B. (1997). Racial differences in physi-
cal mental health: SES, stress, and discrimination. Journal of Health Psychology,
2, 335-351.