"Why Be Normal?": Language and Identity Practices in A Community of Nerd Girls
"Why Be Normal?": Language and Identity Practices in A Community of Nerd Girls
ABSTRACT
The idea that the social world is best viewed as a set of practices is not new.
Praxis is a foundational concept of Marxism, and more recently Giddens 1979
For Certeau, by contrast, the individual is much more agentive, because the
focus of investigation is subversion as well as reproduction of the social order.
But like Bourdieu, Certeau finds inspiration for his project in linguistics:
Our investigation . . . can use as its theoretical model the construction of
individual sentences with an established vocabulary and syntax. In linguis-
tics, “performance” and “competence” are different: the act of speaking (with
all the enunciative strategies that implies) is not reducible to a knowledge of
the language. By adopting the point of view of enunciation – which is the
subject of our study – we privilege the act of speaking; according to that point
of view, speaking operates within the field of a linguistic system; it effects an
appropriation, or reappropriation, of language by its speakers; it establishes a
present relative to a time and place; and it posits a contract with the
other (the interlocutor) in a network of places and relations. These four char-
acteristics of the speech act can be found in many other practices (walking,
cooking, etc.). (1984:xiii; original emphasis)
Certeau here makes the link between language and other social practices even
more explicit than did Bourdieu before him. Certeau sees all social practices,
both linguistic and non-linguistic, as similar in their social effects. But where
Bourdieu considers practice to be a reproduction of social structure, Certeau views
it as an appropriation, an act of agency. The point, then, is to understand how
culturally shared resources (such as language) are made to serve the specific
social needs of individuals. These needs may enforce the social status quo, but
they may just as easily challenge or revise it.
A third theory of practice has been developed within anthropology by Ortner
1996, who criticizes earlier scholarship on the grounds that it fails to take seri-
ously the practices of women. Making the female agent central in the project of
practice theory, Ortner constructs a framework that has room for both structure
and agency. Although language is not a guiding concept in Ortner’s work as it is
for Bourdieu and Certeau, she views structure itself as textual in nature – the
“field of a linguistic system”, in Certeau’s words – within which an individual act
of speaking operates. Thus a complete analysis of gender, and especially of lan-
guage and gender, cannot focus on texts alone. As Ortner argues (1996:2),
Studies of the ways in which some set of “texts” – media productions, literary
creations, medical writings, religious discourses, and so on – “constructs” cat-
egories, identities, or subject positions, are incomplete and misleading unless
they ask to what degree those texts successfully impose themselves on real
people (and which people) in real time. Similarly, studies of the ways in which
people resist, negotiate, or appropriate some feature of their world are also
inadequate and misleading without careful analysis of the cultural meanings
and structural arrangements that construct and constrain their “agency”, and
that limit the transformative potential of all such intentionalized activity.
206 Language in Society 28:2 (1999)
“WHY BE NORMAL?”: LANGUAGE AND IDENTITY PRACTICES
The possibility – and the reality – of such unified analyses within language and
gender studies is offered by the community of practice framework. More than any
previous approach in sociolinguistics, the community of practice allows research-
ers to examine, in a theoretically adequate way, both the actions of individuals
and the structures that are thereby produced and reproduced, resisted and subverted.
G E N D E R , T H E S P E E C H C O M M U N I T Y,
AND THE COMMUNITY OF PRACTICE
The inadequacies of the speech community model for scholars of language and
gender are overcome in the theory of the community of practice as articulated by
Eckert & McConnell-Ginet 1992, 1995.3 Rather than investing language with a
special analytic status, the community of practice framework considers language
as one of many social practices in which participants engage. By defining the
community as a group of people oriented to the same practice, though not nec-
essarily in the same way, the community of practice model treats difference and
conflict, not uniformity and consensus, as the ordinary state of affairs. The in-
herent heterogeneity of the community of practice also brings marginal members
to the forefront of analysis. One reason for this shift to the margins is that some
peripheral members are recognized as novices, as in Lave & Wenger’s original
formulation (1991). More importantly, however, the community of practice, un-
like the speech community, may be constituted around any social or linguistic
practice, no matter how marginal from the perspective of the traditional speech
community. Likewise, by focusing on individuals as well as groups, the theory of
the community of practice integrates structure with agency. And because identi-
ties are rooted in actions rather than categories, the community of practice model
can capture the multiplicity of identities at work in specific speech situations
more fully than is possible within the speech community framework. Such nu-
anced description is also facilitated by Eckert & McConnell-Ginet’s intrinsically
ethnographic approach to language and gender research. The remainder of this
article draws on the above characteristics of the community of practice to dem-
210 Language in Society 28:2 (1999)
“WHY BE NORMAL?”: LANGUAGE AND IDENTITY PRACTICES
a
In Bucholtz 1998 I offer a fuller discussion of the phonological and syntactic patterns of nerds. The
present article focuses primarily on lexicon and on discursive identity practices. The variables (uw)
and (ow) are part of a vowel shift that is characteristic of California teenagers (Hinton et al. 1987,
Luthin 1987). It is stereotypically associated with trendy and cool youth identities.
McConnell-Ginet’s finding runs counter to the sociolinguistic tenet that “in sta-
ble variables, women use fewer non-standard variants than men of the same so-
cial class and age under the same circumstances” (Chambers 1995:112).4 The
researchers argue that the vowels employed by the Burned-Out Burnout girls are
resources through which they construct their identities as tough and streetwise;
unlike the boys, who can display their toughness through physical confronta-
tions, female Burnouts must index their identities semiotically, because fighting
is viewed as inappropriate for girls. Thus Burnout girls and boys share an orien-
tation toward toughness in their community of practice, but the practice of tough-
ness is achieved in different ways by each gender. By viewing language as
equivalent to other social practices like fighting, Eckert & McConnell-Ginet are
able to explain the ethnographic meaning of the Burnout girls’ vowel systems,
and to show how, as symbolic capital (Bourdieu 1978), language can acquire the
empowering authority of physical force itself.
Nerds, of course, attain empowerment in very different ways than either Burn-
outs or Jocks. One of the primary ways they differ from these other, more trend-
conscious groups is through the high value they place on individuality. Compared
to both Jocks and Burnouts – who must toe the subcultural line in dress, language,
friendship choices, and other social practices – nerds are somewhat less con-
strained by peer-group sanctions.
For girls, nerd identity also offers an alternative to the pressures of hegemonic
femininity – an ideological construct that is at best incompatible with, and at
worst hostile to, female intellectual ability. Nerd girls’ conscious opposition to
this ideology is evident in every aspect of their lives, from language to hexis to
other aspects of self-presentation. Where cool girls aim for either cuteness or
sophistication in their personal style, nerd girls aim for silliness. Cool girls play
soccer or basketball; nerd girls play badminton. Cool girls read fashion maga-
zines; nerd girls read novels. Cool girls wear tight T-shirts, and either very tight
or very baggy jeans; nerd girls wear shirts and jeans that are neither tight nor
extremely baggy. Cool girls wear pastels or dark tones; nerd girls wear bright
primary colors. But these practices are specific to individuals; they are engaged
in by particular nerd girls, not all of them.
The community of practice model accommodates the individuality that is par-
amount in the nerd social identity, without overlooking the strong community ties
that unify the nerd girls in this study. The community of practice also allows us to
look at nerd girls in the same way that Eckert & McConnell-Ginet 1999 view the
Burnout girls: as speakers and social actors, as individuals and members of
communities, and as both resisting and responding to cultural ideologies of gender.
Carrie’s question in line 1 creates the conditions for intellectual display. Although
the humor of the question is acknowledged through laughter (line 2), it receives
immediate, serious uptake from two participants, Bob and Fred (lines 4–5). Car-
rie’s subsequent question (line 6), however, forces an admission of ignorance
from Fred (line 7).
Because knowledge is symbolic capital within the nerd community of prac-
tice, Fred’s admission results in some loss of face. She recovers from this (minor)
social setback by invoking the authority of a reference book (I’ll look it up for
you, line 11). In this way Fred can safely assure her interlocutor that, although she
does not yet know the answer, she soon will. She is also able to one-up Bob, who
has misidentified the bagel seeds (line 4) and continues to show some skepticism
about Fred’s classification of them (Sesame:, line 8). Fred tracks this indirect
challenge for five lines, through her own turn and Carrie’s next question; rather
than continuing to participate in the series of adjacency pairs that Carrie has
initiated (lines 12–13), she responds to Bob (line 14). Fred thus succeeds in dis-
playing both actual knowledge, about the type of seeds under discussion, and
potential knowledge, about the appearance of sesame plants.
Claims to knowledge are, however, often disputed in this community of prac-
tice. After Bob provides an incorrect answer to Carrie and receives a correction
from Fred, she continues to exhibit doubt about Fred’s knowledge (line 15). She
offers a second incorrect identification of the seeds in line 16, but this time she
interrupts herself and self-corrects (lines 17–18), in an effort to prevent further
other-correction. She does not succeed, however; and when Carrie explains why
Language in Society 28:2 (1999) 215
M A R Y B U C H O LT Z
Bob is mistaken, the latter overlaps with her, offering three quick acknowledg-
ments that are designed to cut off Carrie’s turn (lines 22–24).
This passage shows several deviations from the preference organization of
repair in conversation (Schegloff et al. 1977), according to which self-initiation
and self-repair are preferred over initiation and repair by another. Bob twice
initiates dispreferred repairs of Fred’s turns (lines 8, 15), and she even begins to
carry out the repair itself in line 16. When Bob initiates a repair of her own
utterance through self-interruption in the same line, Carrie performs the repair
despite Bob’s efforts to prevent her from doing so (lines 21–24). The frequent
apparent violations of repair organization suggest that, in this community of prac-
tice, self-repair is preferred only by the speaker; the listener’s positive face (the
desire to be viewed as intelligent) wars against and often overrides consideration
of the speaker’s negative face (the desire not to be viewed as unintelligent).
Bob’s loss of face in ex. 1 leads her, in ex. 2, to initiate a new conversational
direction:
Bob here enters into the unfeminine spirit of Carrie’s narrative, even outdoing
Carrie with her repeated insistence on her own immunity from “gross” subjects
like crustiness (lines 73, 81). A competitive tone is also evident in the multiple
challenges she issues to Carrie throughout the latter’s narrative (lines 65, 73). As
questions, these challenges echo Carrie’s earlier questions (lines 1, 6, 12–13); but
whereas Carrie’s appeared to be genuine information-seeking questions, Bob’s
are not. Carrie’s recognition of this fact is shown by her failure to respond at all
to the first question, and by her answering the second question with an equally
challenging question of her own (Would you want to live in a castle full of crust?,
lines 76, 78). Bob’s face-threatening response (I mi:ght, line 81) perpetuates the
jocular-combative tone. In ex. 5, however, this combativeness becomes not a
shared resource for joint identity construction, but a marker of social division.
The positive identity practices that dominate in the earlier part of the interaction
are replaced by negative identity practices, as community members experience a
threat not only to their face but also to their identities.
These questions display Bob’s nerd identity through her use of puns on the word
crust (lines 85, 87). Punning, as a discourse practice that orients to linguistic
form, is characteristic of nerds’ discourse style (see Table 1). Carrie’s refusal (line
88) to participate in Bob’s punning thus constitutes a negative identity practice –
one which, moreover, indexes a rejection of nerd identity as it has been con-
structed through preceding interactional practices. The refusal is made more
evident by her exploitation (lines 86, 88–90) of Bob’s syntactic template. By
conforming to the syntactic form of Bob’s turn, while failing to conform to the
218 Language in Society 28:2 (1999)
“WHY BE NORMAL?”: LANGUAGE AND IDENTITY PRACTICES
discourse practice of punning, Carrie separates herself from Bob at a point when
the latter is fully engaged in nerdy identity practices.
This analysis is confirmed by Carrie’s choice of upgraded adjective in line
89. Bootsy is a slang term with a negative evaluative sense; it is not used by
other members of the Random Reigns Supreme Club. The introduction of youth
slang into a group that explicitly rejects such linguistic forms is part of a strongly
negative identity practice, and the reactions of Carrie’s interlocutors are corre-
spondingly negative: Bob’s response (lines 92–95) jokingly concedes the point,
while underscoring that Carrie has violated the rules of nerdy argument by
appealing to the authority of cool youth culture. Fred’s mocking repetition of
the term (line 96) demonstrates that the use of slang is itself worthy of com-
ment. With Carrie’s narrative entirely derailed – it never becomes clear how it
is connected to the earlier discussion – she soon afterward moves away from
the group.
The complex interaction presented above reveals Carrie’s peripheral status in
this community of practice. As a non-core member, she moves between friend-
ship groups – in fact, the interaction occurred when Carrie approached the core
group in the middle of lunch period. Carrie’s social flexibility has made her a
cultural and linguistic broker for the Random Reigns Supreme Club, whose mem-
bers become aware of current youth slang in large part through contact with her.
Hence many slang terms that circulate widely in the “cool” groups are labeled by
club members as “Carrie words”.
Yet Carrie also demonstrates her ability and willingness to participate in the
group’s positive identity practices. She does so most obviously by engaging in
sound play in recounting her poem (crusty king, line 44; a boat in the moat, line
60). More significant, though, is the subtle shift in her speech practices at the
beginning of the interaction. Thus Carrie’s question Is anybody here knowledge-
able about (.) the seeds on top of bagels? (lines 12–13) draws on the formal
register through her choice of the word knowledgeable. Among nerds, this reg-
ister projects a speaker’s persona as smart and highly educated. But the use of the
formal register is strategic, not a mechanical result of membership in a particular
social category. This point is supported by the fact that Carrie employs the nerd
identity practice only after she asks two related questions in colloquial register
(lines 1, 6). Her unwillingness to overlap her turn with Fred’s (lines 9, 10) further
suggests that the question is a performance of nerdiness, not just a manifestation
of it; she does not produce her utterance until she is assured of an attentive audi-
ence. That is, Carrie is simultaneously displaying and commenting on nerd prac-
tice – showing her awareness of nerdy linguistic forms, and announcing her
willingness to enter a nerdy interactional space by carefully gauging her utter-
ance to match the group’s practices. Thus Carrie’s performance of nerdiness places
her within the community of practice; but her use of slang, as the other members
are quick to let her know, moves her outside it. Such adjustments at interactional
boundaries may reflect adjustments at community boundaries.
Language in Society 28:2 (1999) 219
M A R Y B U C H O LT Z
CONCLUSION
Because all the participants in the above exchange are middle-class European
American girls from the same California city, the traditional sociolinguistic per-
spective would classify them unproblematically as members of the same speech
community. Such an analysis would overlook the details of greatest interest to
language and gender researchers: the performances of identity, and the struggles
over it, which are achieved through language. However, by viewing the inter-
action as the product of a community of practice, we can avoid this problem, as
well as others associated with the speech community model.
The ethnographic method brings into view the social meanings with which
participants invest their practices. These meanings emerge on the ground in local
contexts; thus what it means to display academic knowledge, or to use slang,
depends not on fixed identity categories but on where one is standing. Nor do
participants necessarily agree on the meanings of their actions; nerdiness, like all
identities, is a contested domain in which speakers struggle both over control of
shared values, via positive identity practices (Who’s better at being a nerd?), and
over control of identity itself, via negative identity practices (Who counts as a
nerd?). Such conflicts reveal the heterogeneity of membership in the community
of practice – its constitution through the work of central and peripheral members
alike. In this project, the interactional choices of specific individuals matter. Thus
Carrie’s identity is on display – and at risk – in a way that Loden’s, for example,
is not. These actions must be seen as choices, not as the outputs of interactional
algorithms. While some practices reproduce the existing local social structure (as
does Carrie’s use of the formal register), others undermine it (e.g. her use of
slang). Likewise, some nerdy practices (such as being good students) comply
with the larger social order, while others (such as rejecting femininity) resist it.
Linguistic practices, moreover, have no special status in this process. Instead,
they work in conjunction with other social practices to produce meanings and
identities. Bob’s interactional work to distance herself from hegemonic feminin-
ity, for instance, is part of her overall participation in anti-feminine practices and
her non-participation in feminine practices, as evidenced also by her physical
self-presentation.
For sociolinguists, the community of practice represents an improvement over
the speech community in that it addresses itself to both the social and the linguis-
tic aspects of the discipline. As a well-grounded framework with currency in a
number of fields, practice theory in general, in particular the community of prac-
tice, revitalizes social theory within sociolinguistics. What is more, it does so at
a sufficiently general level to accommodate multiple dimensions of social analy-
sis – including both structure and agency, both ideology and identity, both norms
and interactions. The community of practice also provides an avenue for a more
complete sociolinguistic investigation of identity. Although introduced for gender-
based research, the community of practice has never been restricted to the analy-
sis of a single element of identity. Indeed, it lends itself to the simultaneous
220 Language in Society 28:2 (1999)
“WHY BE NORMAL?”: LANGUAGE AND IDENTITY PRACTICES
investigation of multiple aspects of the self, from those at the macro level – like
gender, ethnicity, and class – to micro-identities like Jocks, Burnouts, or nerds.
The framework also allows for the study of interaction between levels of identity.
The concepts of positive and negative identity practices, as proposed in this ar-
ticle, are intended as one way to develop the potential of the community of prac-
tice in this arena.
In addition to its benefits for social analysis, the community of practice offers
an integrated approach to linguistic analysis. By understanding all socially mean-
ingful language use as practices tied to various communities, the model enables
researchers to provide more complete linguistic descriptions – along with social
explanations – of particular social groups. Moreover, the community of practice
provides a way to bring qualitative and quantitative research closer together.
Because both kinds of linguistic data emerge from practice, both can be included
in a single analysis. This richly contextualized approach to both language and
society is one of the great strengths of the community of practice as a sociolin-
guistic framework.
The community of practice, having revolutionized the field of language and
gender almost as soon as it was first proposed, enables researchers of socially
situated language use to view language within the context of social practice.
Perhaps the most valuable feature is that the community of practice admits a
range of social and linguistic phenomena that are not analyzed in other theoretical
models. Local identities, and the linguistic practices that produce them, become
visible to sociolinguistic analysis as the purposeful choices of agentive individ-
uals, operating within (and alongside and outside) the constraints of the social
structure. To describe and explain such complexity must be the next step not only
for language and gender scholars, but for all sociolinguists concerned with the
linguistic construction of the social world.
NOTES
* My thanks to Janet Holmes, Chris Holcomb, Stephanie Stanbro, and members of the Ethnography/
Theory Group at Texas A&M University for comments on and discussion of the ideas in this article.
1
The work of Barbara Horvath on immigrants in Sydney’s speech community (Horvath 1985,
Horvath & Sankoff 1987) has done a great deal to correct this omission.
2
Santa Ana & Parodi’s model of nested speech communities (1998) is a recent attempt to address
this problem.
3
A fuller discussion of the advantages of practice theory for language and gender research is
provided by Bucholtz 1999.
4
Eckert 1989b calls this simple formulation into question; see also Labov 1990 for a response.
5
Though this is not its actual name, it preserves the flavor of the original. All other names are
pseudonyms chosen by the speakers.
6
Transcription conventions are as follows:
. end of intonation unit; falling intonation
, end of intonation unit; fall-rise intonation
? end of intonation unit; rising intonation
– self-interruption
: length
REFERENCES
_ (1996). The linguistic individual: Self-expression in language and linguistics. Oxford & New
York: Oxford University Press.
_ , & Bean, Judith Mattson (1997). Self-expression and linguistic variation. Language in Soci-
ety 26:221– 46.
Kinney, David A. (1993). From nerds to normals: The recovery of identity among adolescents from
middle school to high school. Sociology of Education 66:1.21– 40.
Labov, William (1966). The social stratification of English in New York City. Washington, DC: Center
for Applied Linguistics.
_ (1972). The reflection of social processes in linguistic structures. In his Sociolinguistic pat-
terns, 110–21. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
_ (1990). The intersection of sex and social class in the course of linguistic change. Language
Variation and Change 2:205–54.
Lave, Jean (1988). Cognition in practice: Mind, mathematics, and culture in everyday life. Cam-
bridge & New York: Cambridge University Press.
_ , & Wenger, Etienne (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. Cam-
bridge & New York: Cambridge University Press.
Luthin, Herbert W. (1987). The story of California (ow): The coming-of-age of English in California.
In Keith M. Denning et al. (eds.), Variation in language: NWAV-XV at Stanford, 312–24. Stanford,
CA: Department of Linguistics, Stanford University.
Milroy, James (1992). Linguistic variation and change: On the historical sociolinguistics of English.
Oxford: Blackwell.
Morgan, Marcyliena (1999). No woman, no cry: Claiming African American women’s place. In Mary
Bucholtz et al. (eds.), Reinventing identities (to appear).
Ortner, Sherry (1996). Making gender: The politics and erotics of culture. Boston: Beacon.
Pratt, Mary Louise (1987). Linguistic utopias. In Nigel Fabb et al. (eds.), The linguistics of writing:
Arguments between language and literature, 48– 66. New York: Methuen.
Rickford, John R. (1986). The need for new approaches to social class analysis in sociolinguistics.
Language and Communication 6:3.215–21.
Romaine, Suzanne (1982). What is a speech community? In Suzanne Romaine (ed.), Sociolinguistic
variation in speech communities, 13–24. London: Arnold.
Santa Ana, Otto, & Parodi, Claudia (1998). Modeling the speech community: Configuration and
variable types in the Mexican Spanish setting. Language in Society 27:23-51.
Schegloff, Emanuel; Jefferson, Gail; & Sacks, Harvey (1977). The preference for self-correction in
the organization of repair in conversation. Language 53:361–82.
Silverstein, Michael (1996). Encountering languages and languages of encounter in North American
ethnohistory. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 6:126– 44.
Tolone, W. L., & C. R. Tieman (1990). Drugs, delinquency and nerds: Are loners deviant? Journal of
Drug Education 20:2.153– 62.
Wenger, Etienne (1998). Communities of practice. Cambridge & New York: Cambridge University
Press.
Williams, Glyn (1992). Sociolinguistics: A sociological critique. London: Routledge.