ТГ
ТГ
Introduction to morphology
Points for discussion
• Grammar as a linguistic study. Types of Grammar: descriptive vs prescriptive.
•The history of English Grammars. The basics of word structure.
• Classification of morphemes. Paradigmatic and syntagmatic relations of grammatical units.
Theoretical grammar as the linguistic science
• The subject of theoretical grammar is a systematic study of the grammatical structure of Modern English.
• Its aim is to present a theoretical description of the grammatical system, i.e. to scientifically analyse, define
grammatical categories, and study the mechanisms of grammatical formation of utterances out of words in the
process of speech making.
Morphology (Greek morphe – form, shape, and logos – learning) deals with the word classes, their grammatical
categories and systems of forms (paradigms) in which these categories actually exist.
Syntax (Greek syn – with, and tasso – arrange) examines the way words combine to form sentences and phrases.
Can be studied diachronically and synchronically.
Diachronic description when we describe a language from the perspective of change (historical grammar).
Synchronic description offers a representation of a language at a particular moment of time.
Approaches to grammar
(1) Should we, in our grammatical accounts, describe how English is used by its speakers? (Descriptive approach)
(2) Should we offer rules on how some people think it should be used? (Prescriptive approach)
A popular view of the role of grammar which is reflected in many school grammars is that it should presents a set
of rules for speaking and writing 'correctly' and it is concerned with prescribing the ways in which language should
be used. (Prescriptive grammar)
Modern linguistics is descriptive.
Descriptive grammar is concerned with describing how language is used rather than prescribing how it should be
used. It may distinguish between formal and informal contexts; or it may report whether a certain form is part of
the standard language, whether it is more common in spoken English, or whether it only occurs in certain regions.
(a) [He knew more about it] than I Formal
(b) [He knew more about it] than me Informal
(c) [He knew more about it] than I did Neutral
Prescriptive grammar dictates what one should say and write according to the linguistic norms prevalent at the
time.
Examples of prescriptive rules are:
(1) Do not end a sentence with a preposition. (e.g. Do not say ‘This is the house which he lives in’ (informal
context); say ‘This is the house in which he lives’) (formal context).
(2) Do not split an infinitive. (e.g. Do not say ‘I expect to shortly welcome him here’; say ‘I expect to welcome him
here shortly’).
(3) Say: “yes" not “yeah".
(4) Say "I don't want any", not "I don't want none".
(5) A pronoun must agree in gender, and number with the nouns to which it refers.
The history of English Grammars
English language history has a long tradition. Until the 17th century, the term “grammar” meant “the study of
Latin”.
One of the earliest and the most popular Latin grammars in English by William Lily was published in the first half
of the 16th century.
Among English grammars, which appeared in the first half of the 17th century Ben Johnson’s and Ch. Butler’s
grammars can be mentioned. These grammars are called prenormative. They described different language
phenomena (classification of parts of speech, the problem of the number of cases, the syntactic system of English).
The age of prescriptive grammar begins in the 2nd half of the 18th century. It was Robert Lowth’s “Short
introduction to English Grammar” (1762). The aim was to reduce the English language to rules and set up strict
rules of language usage.
By the end of the 19th century, when prescriptive grammar had reached its highest level of development, the
appearance of the scientific grammar became possible. Henry Sweet’s “New English Grammar. Logical and
Historical” (1891) gave a scientific explanation of the grammatical phenomena.
Structural (or descriptive) grammar, 1940's (American structural grammarians Ch. Fries, L. Bloomfield, H.
Whitehall) began treating the problem of the structure of English.
(TG) Transformational-generative grammar appeared in 1950’s in which transformational rules form an
essential part. These rules are capable of producing an infinite number of grammatical sentences. This theory
grammar was first introduced by Noam Chomsky in his “Syntactic structures (1957)” and it has been developed by
Zellig Harris (both Americans)
The theory of TG begins by making a distinction of kernel sentences and their transforms. Kernel sentences are
the basic elementary sentences from which all else are made. Transforms are derived from the basic sentences by
certain grammatical rules.
E.g., He gave me a book. – He gave me a look. He gives me… He is giving… He will give… (Subject and
predicate from which all else are made).
Generative Semantics is the theory of grammar developed as an alternative to the transformational-generative
model. In this model, semantic structure has a generative role (John Lyons).
Textual Linguistics is the study of “communicative text” rather than grammatical sentences. Its aim is to observe
such devices as coherence and cohesion over unit larger than a sentence (paragraph, page text) needed for the
theoretical description of text.
A morpheme is the smallest meaningful unit into which a word form may be divided. (A word is the biggest.)
L. Bloomfield – A morpheme is a minimal meaningful unit.
e.g. readers
(1) –read, expressing the basic lexical meaning of the word;
(2) -er, expressing the idea of agent performing the action indicated by the root of the word;
(3) -s, indicating number, i.e. showing that more than one person is doing this work
A morph (allomorph) is the smallest meaningful succession of phonemes which cannot be divided into any other
meaningful units and which regularly occur in different utterances characterized by the state of complementary
distribution.
A morpheme is a set of morphs having the same meaning and being in the state of complementary distribution.
Complementary distribution (структура додаткового розподілу) concerns different environments of formally
different morphs, which are united by the same meaning. It means one morph never stands in place of another
within the same class of elements.
For example,
• Inhuman, impossible, immoral, impatient, impolite, illegal, irregular (the meaning of negativeness -in, -im, -il, -ir)
different morphs in one and the same morpheme of negativeness.
• Clear, clears, clearly, clarify, clarity
• Admirable, admiration, admirability
• Probable, probability,
• Liable, liability
• Decide, decision, decisive
Objective verbs are usually associated both with subject and object while subjective verbs are only associated
with nouns denoting the subject, e.g. She got up and kissed him fairly; got up – subjective verb, kissed –
objective verb.
The terms “subjective” and “objective” correlate with the terms “transitive” and “intransitive”. Thus, objective
verbs connected with their objects directly are called transitive verbs; all other verbs are called intransitive.
According to the aspective nature of their lexical meaning verbs may be divided into limitive and unlimitive
(durative). Cf. arrive, come, find, drop / sleep, behave, hope.
The category of person is represented by the 1st, 2nd, and 3rd person, and it expresses the relation between the
speaker, the person or persons addressed, and other persons and things.
The 1st person, of course, expresses the speaker or a group of which the speaker makes a part; the 2nd person, the
person or persons spoken to, and the 3rd, that person or thing (or those persons or things) which are neither the
speaker nor the person(s) spoken to.
From the point of view of the expression of the person, there are 3 groups of verbs:
1) Modal verbs have no personal inflections, the category of person is neutralized with these verbs.
2) The expression of person by a unique lexeme "be" is three different suppletive personal forms: am for the
first person singular, are for the second person and is for the third person.
3) The remaining verbs express person by the opposition of the third person singular against other persons.
The strong member of the opposition is marked by the affix -(e)s, the other two persons remain unmarked.
The category of number expresses the quantity of the subjects (one or more than one). The more or less distinct
morphemic featuring of the category of number can be seen only with the verb "to be", both in the present tense
(am, is, are) and in the past tense (was, were).
Grammatical category of tense
It is the reference of the verb to the moment of its immediate perception or to the "moment of speech“.
Tense is the grammatical expression of verbal time. It represents the opposition of the past tense to the
present tense. The marked member of the opposition is the past form.
English has two tenses – present and past – which are marked inflectionally. English does not have an
inflectional future tense. Shall go and will go are regarded in traditional grammars as representing future tense
auxiliary. However, shall and will are not tense inflections, but rather modal auxiliaries.
There are the following reasons why there is no future tense in English
Tense in English (present/past) is marked by the inflections;
If we want to ‘invent’ a future tense, we would need to take –ll and attach it to the end of verbs: ‘it rain’ll’
Will and shall grammatically belong to the class of modal auxiliaries
Will doesn’t always refer to future time, and when it does, there is always another meaning, e.g. prediction,
command or promise
Though will is very frequent, other forms are used to denote a future tense, e.g. be going to.
The category of aspect
English has two aspects – perfect and progressive, which express contrast.
Perfect: I have sung. /They had walked.
Progressive (continuous): I am singing. /They were walking.
Aspect is concerned with the temporal aspects of an event or situation, such as whether it is 'in progress' and
whether or not it has been completed, i.e.
how an event or state relates to the surrounding time periods
whether an event or state has relevance to another event, state or point in time
The perfect aspect
It involves a situation, which results from the completion of an earlier event or state. The perfect aspect is
expressed by the auxiliary have in conjunction with the following V-en form.
Perfect forms relate past events and states to the present time (present perfect) or to a subsequent point of time in
the past (past perfect). The relationship may involve:
A completed action or state with a relevance to the present (or past): I've hidden the presents.
An action or state lasting up to the present (or past): You’ve drunk enough.
An accumulation of experience repeated series of events: I've met many diplomats/We have visited Norway
every July for 50 years.
The progressive aspect
It presents an event as being 'in progress', as ongoing, that is the event to have an earlier starting point and a later
finishing point.
The progressive aspect is expressed by the auxiliary be in conjunction with a following V-ing form, as in:
He's standing outside the door.
I was talking about something else.
She's been speaking for 45 minutes.
With the present progressive, we would normally understand that the search began earlier than the time of
utterance and will continue into the future.
E.g. So if you're looking for a better tertiary qualification, …
In the case of the past progressive the point in time during which the activity is presented as being in progress is
located in the past rather than in the present.
For example, the past progressive point in the following sentence is the speaker's Tuesday visit:
e.g. John was repairing his fence when I visited him last Tuesday
Here the past progressive presents the repair as in progress at the time of the visit.
The perfect and progressive aspects
The perfect and progressive aspects can of course be combined and can form one more aspect in English, which
is very rare, as in:
He has been making sandwiches for over an hour. /They had been walking.
Another example: I've been looking for you for 200 years.
Here the present perfect progressive combination suggests that over a period of time beginning before the time of
utterance, the search has been in progress, and that in all likelihood it will extend beyond the moment of utterance.
Sometimes the category of aspect allows us to use the habitual aspect (e.g. He used to cycle to work), indicated by
the expression used to as indicating a situation which obtained in the past and followed by an infinitival verb. This
aspect represents states or habits that are characteristic of an entire period.
Grammatical category of voice: definitions
• Voice expresses the relation between the subject and the action (I. Ivanova).
• Voice expresses the relations between the participants of the situation (subject, object) and the process. (M.
Blokh).
The vast majority of the authors of English scientific grammars recognize two voices in English - active and
passive. The marked member of the opposition is the passive form, which consists of the auxiliary "be" with the
past participle (be... –en').
The passive form as the strong member of the opposition expresses reception of the action by the subject, i.e. the
'passive' subject, which now denotes the object of the action.
The active voice shows that the person or thing denoted by the subject is the doer of the action expressed by the
predicate, i.e. the action is performed by its subject.
The category of voice is connected with the category of transitivity/intransitivity. In English, the category of
voice has a much broader representation in the system of verb than in Ukrainian, since in English not only
transitive, but also intransitive verbs including prepositional ones can be used in the passive. The English verb, as
the matter of fact, has lost its fixed transitivity/intransitivity.
I've just been rung by the police.
He was said to have been very wild in his youth.
The child will be looked after all night.
Some verbs are very common in the passive in English, for example:
It was alleged/claimed that . .
Be born (I was born in 19..) only occurs in the passive.
The passive voice is sometimes considered to be derived from the active voice. But it's all together wrong. Thus
the utterances:
(1) Tom beat John;
(2) John was beaten by Tom are not synonymous as different points of view are expressed. In the first sentence,
Tom is in the centre of interests of the speaker, and in the second - John is in the centre of interests. So the subject
of the sentence is the person or thing which is needed to be emphasized.
As O. Jespersen points out, in the sentence 'The doctor was sent for' the speaker is interested in the doctor, i.e.
who was sent for and not in the person who sent or who was sent.
The form of passive voice can be homonymous with compound nominal predicates, which consist of link verbs
“to be” and past participle. The differentiation between them is made on the basis of the fact that passive voice
renders action and compound nominal predicate renders state.
Sometimes it is difficult to determine whether the construction is in the passive voice or a compound nominal
predicate, e.g. I was upset by his news. Here “was upset” denotes state as a result of the action though it has the
strict passive meaning at the same time.
The category of mood
It is expressed by the opposition of the indicative and a number of oblique (indirect) moods.
The traditional grammar distinguishes between the indicative, which represrnts an action as real, the imperative,
subjunctive and the conditional mood which represents the action as non-real.
The indicative mood is viewed as expressing assertions, and it shows that the speaker presents this action as real
but not everything expressed by the verb in indicative verb is necessarily true. The speaker may either be mistaken
or he may be telling a deliberate lie. So, though the action is presented as real, it may not correspond to reality.
The imperative mood presents the action as ordered or recommended, addressed immediately to the second
person. Its formal expression uses the infinitive without “to” but with the verb “be”: “Be quick!”
Its negative form is expressed by “don’t”: Don’t worry!
Imperative mood forms are used in imperative sentences, which are characterized by the absence of subject. The
missing subject is usually interpreted as “you”, singular or plural. The use of “you” as a subject of the imperative
sentences is stylistically marked: e.g. You come here! You get out of here!
One of the forms of imperative is that with a modal verb “let”. This form of the imperative can be addressed to the
first and third person (e.g. Let us meet. Let him come.)
The subjunctive mood expresses suppositional or desired activity. It may have several types:
The first type is known as the “present subjunctive”
It is used in that-clauses, when the main clause contains an expression of recommendation, resolution, demand to
suggest a future ‘desired’ state or action (the ‘mandative’ subjunctive) (suggest, demand, recommend, insist, it is
necessary, it is strange, it is advisable, etc.). In the subjunctive sentenced below, the verb has no ending.
I insist that he leaves tomorrow morning. (formal)
I suggest he leave immediately. (formal)
With be, the present subjunctive is the same for all persons (‘be’).
It is important that you be there on time. (formal)
It is necessary that he (should) be informed of the rules.
It is also used in the clause of purpose after the conjunction “lest”:
They covered the goods with canvas lest it (should) be damaged by rain.
The second type is known as “conditional”
It uses the forms homonymous to Past Simple and Past Perfect. With be, the past subjunctive is the same for all
persons (‘were’), and this is a formal use. The was-form is possible in informal styles. These forms express the
degree of possibility of the action.
This type involves conditional and concessive clauses typically introduced by conjunctions if, if only and as though
(e.g. If I were a rich man…; It’s all as though he were looking for trouble), and also subordinate clauses with the
verb wish to express a hypothetical idea (e.g. I wish he had been here then).
Instead of if-clause inversion can be used:
Were I to see hem tomorrow, I would ask hem about it.
Had I seen him then, I would ask him about it.
The third type is “modal subjunctive”.
It uses analytical forms with modal verbs “would, might, could” + infinitive or perfect infinitive in the main
clauses. It depends on the degree of possibility of the consequence of unreal condition. These forms are different
from the use of modal verbs in the indicative mood, because in the subjunctive the contracted forms can be used:
e.g. If he did it, we’d know about it.
The fourth type is “formulaic subjunctive”
It consists of the infinitive without “to”, but is only used in certain fixed phrases expressing wishes:
Come what may, we will go ahead.
God save the Queen!
Long live the King (the Queen).
Suffice it to say that…
Be that as it may…
Heaven forbid that…
May he rest in peace.
Lecture 5. Non-finite verbs (verbids)
1. The Infinitive
2. The Gerund
3. Participles
Non-finite verbs (verbids) are the forms of the verb, which in many of their lexico-grammatical features are
between the verb and the non-actional parts of speech. Their mixed nature is revealed in their meaning,
morphological form, combinability and syntactic functions. They render processes as peculiar kinds of substances
and properties. They are formed by special morphemic elements, which do not express either grammatical tense or
mood. They can be combined with verbs like non-verbal lexemes and they can be combined with non-actional
lexemes like verbs.
The infinitive is the non-finite form of the verb, which combines the properties of the verb with those of the noun,
serving as the verbal name of a process.
The infinitive is used in three types of functions:
1) As a notional syntactic part of the sentence;
2) As the constituent of a compound verbal predicate, (e.g. He can swim. He began to learn English 2 years
ago.);
3) As the notional constituent of the analytical form of the verb, (e.g. will go, shall go etc)
The first use, as professor M.Blokh states, is grammatically “free”, the second is grammatically “half-free”, the
third – grammatically “bound”.
Two presentational forms of the infinitive
One of them, characteristic of the free uses of the infinitive, is distinguished by the pre- positional marker “to”.
This form is traditionally called the “to-infinitive” or “marked infinitive”.
The other form, characteristic of the bound uses of the infinitive, does not employ the marker “to”, which is called
“"the bare infinitive” or “unmarked infinitive”
The unmarked infinitive is used:
a. With modal verbs (except the modals ought to, used to, have to, be to);
b. With verbs of physical perception, e.g. We heard hem play the violin;
c. With the verbs “let”, “make”, “help”, e.g. What made you do so?
d. With the verb “know” in the sense of “experience”, e.g. I have never known him be cruel with animals;
e. With a few verbal phrases of modal nature (had better, would rather, would have, etc.);
f. With the pronoun “why”, e.g. Why not go and do it?
Forms of Infinitives
Combinability
The verb-type combinability of the infinitive is displayed in its combining:
1) With nouns expressing the object of action (to read books);
2) With nouns expressing the subject of the action (I want Mary to go there);
3) With modifying adverbs (to read quickly);
4) With verbs of semi-functional nature forming a verbal predicate (can read);
5) With auxiliary verbs in the analytical form (will read).
The noun-type combinability of the infinitive is displayed it its combing:
1) With finite notional verbs as the object of the action (like to read);
2) With finite notional verbs when the infinitive is the subject of the action (To live means…).
Predicative constructions
1. Objective-with-the-infinitive construction (Complex Object),
e.g. I like her to sing; I heard Charlie play the piano
The category of modality exposes the speaker's attitude toward the situation being described. In Linguistic
Perspectives on English Grammar (2010) Martin J. Endley suggests that modality is "the simplest way to explain
modality is to say that it has to do with the stance the speaker adopts toward some situation expressed in an
utterance".
As prof. M. Blokh states, modality, as different from predication, is not specifically confined to the sentence.
Modality is a broader category revealed both in the grammatical elements of language and in its lexical, purely
nominative elements. In this sense, every word expressing a definite correlation between the substance and
objective reality should be recognized as modal. Here belong such words as "probability", "necessity", "perhaps",
"may be", "just", and modal verbs. As for predication, it represents only syntactic modality.
Types of modality
'Objective' modality and 'subjective' modality
The objective modality can be subdivided into epistemic modality of reality / irreality (It would be nice to live by
the sea. (I don't live by the sea)), likelihood or necessity of situations and events as in You must be right, modality
of duty (must), possibility (may, possible, probably etc.), ability (can) etc.
The subjective (emotional) modality is expressed by such modal words as fortunately, unfortunately, hopefully,
deliberately etc. and reflects the speaker's emotional attitude to the information contained in the utterance.
Classification of sentences
According to the types of communication, there are four classes:
1) Declarative;
2) Interrogative;
3) Imperative;
4) Exclamative sentences.
Declarative
The traditional function of declaratives is as statements, that is, conveying information, feelings, etc. e.g. It’s ten
o’clock. I like it.
The sentence with the question mark is called a declarative question,
e.g. The door is open?
It is also possible with wh-words to give special emphasis, e.g. to express surprise or ask for repetition, e.g. She
said what? He sat where?
Interrogatives
Interrogatives or direct questions have three major subtypes:
(1) Yes/no questions: e.g. Can you swim? including a special subtype, tag questions, e.g. You can swim, can't you?
(2) Wh-questions: e.g. Where is he going?
(3) Alternative questions with two subtypes:
- Would you like blue, white, or red? – resembles a yes/no question
- Which colour would you like? Blue, white, or red? – resembles a wh-question
Questions also have a few minor types:
(a) Exclamatory questions inviting agreement, e.g. Am I tired! Isn't she beautiful!
(b) Rhetorical questions expecting no answer, e.g. Isn't this elementary? Haven't you got anything better to do?
How should I know?
Imperatives
The formal characteristic of imperatives is that no subject is mentioned. The basic form of the verb is used: Take
care. Be quiet.
There is also an emphatic imperative with do: Oh, do be quiet.
Negative imperatives are formed with do not (or don't) followed by an infinitive: Don't laugh at me.
This also applies to be: Don't be scared.
Exclamatives
Exclamatives are sentences expressing a strong emotion. In writing, they are usually signalled by an exclamation
mark. These are different from WH-interrogatives in that they do not contain subject-operator inversion:
What a nice day it is!
How quickly they reacted!
What a fool I was!
How much he looks like you!
Classification of sentences
According to sentence structure, the following two main types are singled out: simple sentences and composite
sentences (or 'multiple' sentences).
A simple sentence is one consisting of only one clause, while a multiple sentence consists of more than one
clause.
Multiple sentences are of two types: compound sentences and complex sentences. These are two types of
multiple sentences, reflecting the two main ways in which clauses are combined.
Simple two-member sentences
Simple sentences are classified into two-member and one-member sentences. Sentences, which contain a word or
a phrase denoting the doer of the action and another word or phrase denoting the action, are termed two-member
sentences. They have the subject and the predicate and can be extended and unextended. Cf. John ran. John ran
quickly to me.
There are sentences, which have only one main part, the other main part is implied. These two-member sentences
are called elliptical. E.g. No birds singing in the dawn (with either subject or predicate is omitted).
Hope you are well. (I hope ...)
(So are you coming?)- Depends on the weather. (It depends...)
(Why did he do it?) – Don't know. (I don't … )
Simple one-member sentences
One-member sentences can be of two types: nominal and verbal.
Nominal sentences are those in which the principal part is expressed by a noun. They may be unextended (e.g.
Silence. Summer.) and extended (e.g. The grass, this good, soft grass).
Verbal sentences are those in which the principal part can be expressed by the verb in the imperative mood, or by
the infinitive.
Imperative sentences with no subject of the action mentioned are also to be classed among one-member
sentences, e.g. Get away.
Another type of one-member sentences are infinitive sentences with an infinitive as the main part, e.g. To be
alive! These are one-member infinitive exclamatory sentences. Another type of infinitive sentences are
interrogative sentences beginning with "why" followed by an infinitive without the particle "to", and sometimes
preceded by the particle "not", e.g. Why not go there!
Sentences expressing confirmation and negation (Yes; No) and some formulas of courtesy (Hello! Good-bye. See
you tonight etc.) are also one-member sentences.
Lecture 7. The Constituent Structure of the Simple Sentence
Points for discussion
1. The problem of sentence parts.
2. The main parts of the sentence.
3. Secondary parts of the sentence.
It is common in grammatical theory to distinguish between the main and the secondary parts of a sentence. Besides
main and secondary parts, there are elements, which are said to stand outside the sentence structure. They are
detachment, apposition, and parenthesis. e.g. A famous linguist, namely Noam Chomsky, will speak tomorrow
night. You are not afraid, I hope.
The division into main and secondary parts of a sentence reflects their participation in the forming of the
predicative nucleus of the sentence.
Groups of sentence parts
1. Subject and predicate. They are interdependent and independent members in relation to any other part of the
sentence. They are two obligatory constituents in the sentence.
There are three theories of the hierarchy of the main parts of a sentence.
According to one of them (supported by Otto Jespersen) subject is the absolutely predominant part of a sentence.
The second point of view implies equality of the two main parts of the sentence (Hryhoriy Pocheptsov). The
third point of view states thạt the predicate (the 'verb') is the only predominant part of the sentence (Lucien
Tesnière). He positioned the verb as the root of all clause structure, and his conception was called 'verb centrality'
theory.
Groups of sentence parts
The second group of parts of a sentence is represented by objects. As a rule, they depend upon verb. Objects are
often obligatory constituents for the structural and semantic completeness of the sentence. For example, we cannot
omit object and adverbial modifiers in the sentences She treated Daddy like a child. My brother often invited her to
his parties. If we erase the objects from the sentence, this leads to sentences of questionable acceptance.
Adverbial modifiers. In most cases, they are optional because such circumstantial information is not obligatory
and can be omitted without causing ungrammaticality.
I left it on the table.
I fell asleep because I was tired.
However, there are a few situations where adverbials are obligatory:
I lay on the grass.
He put it in his pocket.
Here on the grass and in his pocket are necessary to make a clause; you could not say 'I lay' or 'l put it'.
The fourth group includes attributes. They are syntactically connected with nouns and can be omitted without
destroying the structural scheme of the sentence.
Two other men passed across his window.
Subject
The subject and the predicate are the main parts of the sentence. They can be defined according to three criteria:
the meaning, syntactic relations in the sentence and morphological realization.
Definition: the subject is one of the two main parts of the sentence, which denotes the thing or person whose
action or characteristic is expressed by the predicate, which is not dependent on any other part of the sentence but
predicate.
Apart from some special types of sentences, the position of the subject in English is rather fixed. Subjects occur
immediately before the verb phrase, with only certain adverbials being allowed to intervene, and in English,
subjects are obligatory.
The subject may be expressed by:
1) A noun in the common case (A swallow flew by. Mary gave her a book);
2) A pronoun (I objected; Everyone agreed; Nothing had changed; Something had disturbed him);
3) A numeral (The first was my friend);
4) An infinitive (To see you is a pleasure);
5) A gerund (Smoking is prohibited at the Institute);
6) A phrase, sometimes rather complex (The Port of Hew York Authority ticket book sales office is over there.
Their defeat and subsequent surrender means that the war is over.);
7) Clauses themselves can also be the subject in other clauses (Who can do it is not known; What you did is
unforgivable)
8) The anticipatory "it" (It is necessary to go home.).
Two views have been put forward concerning such sentences.
(1) the pronoun "it" at the beginning of the sentence is the formal subject, and the real subject is the infinitive.
(2) "it" is the subject of the sentence, and the infinitive – an apposition to it.
The predicate is one of the two main parts of the sentence, which denotes the action, or property of the thing
expressed by the subject. It is not dependent on other parts of the sentence but subject. Predicate (or simply the
term 'verb’) usually refers to everything in a sentence that says something about the subject.
Traditional classification of predicates
Simple verbal predicate consists of a notional verb in any tense, aspect, voice or mood form and it can also be
expressed by phraseological units and phrases.
e.g. His words frightened me.
We are having lunch now.
They have been taking care of him.
Compound verbal predicate can be of two types:
(1) The compound verbal modal predicate
(2) The compound verbal aspect predicate
The compound verbal modal predicate consists of the verb with a modal meaning or a modal expression and the
infinitive.
e.g. He can't say a word, he can't even apologize.
What am I to do?
Are you willing to prove that?
The compound verbal aspect predicate consists of a notional verb with an aspective meaning and the infinitive
or gerund. The first component expresses:
1. The beginning, the duration and the end of the action (to begin, to start, to commence, to come, to go on, to
proceed, to continue, to stop, to finish, to cease, to give up). Here also belong would (+inf.), used (+inf.) denoting a
repeated action in the past. e.g. She kept on smiling. Give up smoking. I used to dance when I was young.
2. Verbs of chancing and seeming (unexpected occurrence) (to chance, to appear, to seem, to happen, to prove,
to turn out). E.g. They happened to meet at the bus stop. They seemed to understand everything I said. He proved
to be a clever man.
3. Verbs denoting motion and position (to be in/out/away) e.g. Mother is out shopping.
Compound nominal predicate consists of link verbs (to be, to become, to get, to grow, to turn, to look, to seem)
and a predicative (=a nominal part) commonly expressed by a noun, an adjective, a pronoun, a numeral, an
infinitive, a gerund, a participle, a prepositional phrase or a clause.
e.g. The man was difficult to convince. I’m the first. She is on our side. That’s what I think. The leaves are turning
yellow.
Object is usually defined as a secondary part of the sentence, denoting a think to which the action passes on, which
is a result of the action, in reference to which an action is committed or a property
Classifications of objects
Purely structural classification of objects into prepositional and non-prepositional was proposed by prof.
A.Smyrnytskyi. But the basis of such classification is not very important, as it is not connected with the meaning.
Some linguists divide objects into direct, indirect and prepositional. There are sentence, which include both
direct and indirect objects. These are sentences in which the predicate is expressed by the verbs "send, show, give"
and the like. These verbs take two different kinds of objects simultaneously:
1) an object expressing the thing which is sent, shown, given, etc.;
2) the person or persons to whom the thing is sent, shown, given, etc:
e.g. We sent them a present.
He told me (direct) about his success.
The division of objects into direct
Classifications of objects
Prof. Pocheptsov distinguishes:
1) object complement
2) subject complement
3) addressee complement.
He uses the term "complement" in order to avoid tautology.
The object complement denotes the object of the action or its property. It can be both prepositional and non-
prepositional (Cf. He saw me – He looked at me).
The addressee complement denotes the person or thing to which the action is addressed. It can be prepositional
and non-prepositional as well (e.g., He offered me the book – He offered it to me).
The subject complement denotes the bearer of the action expressed by the verb. The verb then is in the passive
voice. The subject complement is always prepositional: by/with
Attribute is a secondary part of the sentence characterizing the thing as to its quality or property. Attributes can he
prepositional (when they precede the part of the sentence which they modify), or postpositional (when they
follow the word they modify).
e.g. a beautiful girl, from the time immemorial
The number of attributes in the attributive chain can be large.
Adjectival order is fixed and should be as follows:
1) opinion
2) shape
3) age
4) colour
5) origin
6) material
A nice long new black British plastic pen
Adverbial modifiers (or 'adjuncts') are defined as constituents that provide information about the circumstances of
the action denoted by the verb and its subject and objects. Such circumstantial information may, for example,
concern time, location, manner, cause or purpose. Thus, adverbial modifiers can be of different types: time, place,
condition, purpose, manner, concession, comparison, etc.
Lecture 8. The composite sentence
Points for discussion
1. The definition of the composite sentence.
2. Intermediate types of composite sentences.
3. Compound sentences.
4. Complex sentences.
The composite sentence expresses a complicated act of thought. The composite sentence in contrast to the simple
sentence is polypredicative, i.e. it has more than one predicative line. In terms of situations and events, this means
that the composite sentence reflects two or more elementary situations making up a unity.
Each predicative unit in a composite sentence makes up a clause in it.
e.g. I quickly shut the door before the animal could escape.
The means of combining clauses into a polypredicative sentence are divided into:
1) Syndetic, i.e. with the clauses linked by conjunctions and conjunctive words (adverbs or pronouns)
E.g., they had a little quarrel, which (but) he soon forgot.
2) Asyndetic, i.e. without any connectors (e.g. The poorer they became the more desperate they grew)
Coordination and subordination
Within a composite sentence clauses may be joined by means of coordination and subordination, thus forming a
compound and complex sentence respectively.
In coordination, the clauses are of equal syntactic status, with neither being contained within the other.
e.g. The door of Henry's room opened and two men came in.
In subordination, the clauses are of unequal status, with the lower status clause being embedded within the
structure of the other.
e.g. He always knew what was the right thing.
Sometimes coordination and subordination can be combined within one sentence. In this case, we have compound-
complex or complex-compound sentences.
e.g. I know that she hates me, but I'll make her love me.
It's a compound-complex sentence because it consists of two subordinate clauses in which one of them is complex
in structure.
A complex-compound sentence has two or more subordinate clauses connected by coordination.
e.g. He realized that he had a terrific responsibility and the best thing to do was to keep his mouth shut.
!!!!!! 2. Intermediate types of composite sentences. (d-learn)
In the compound sentences, clauses are connected by coordination as units of syntactically equal rank.
The authors of prescriptive arid scientific grammars considered the compound sentence to be the weak link in the
structural classification of sentences as the compound sentence contains syntactically independent coordinated
clauses.
The coordinating connectors are divided into conjunctions (and, but, or, nor, for, either ... or, neither... nor, etc.)
and connectors of adverbial character (then, yet, so, thus, moreover, however, etc.).
The coordinating conjunctions differ from each other in definiteness of meaning.
Cf.: 1. The old lady had recognized Ellen's handwriting and her fat little mouth was pursed in a frightened way ...
(M.Mitchel).
2. The bazaar had taken place Monday night and today was only Thursday.
In (1) there is a shade of meaning of cause-result due to the meanings of the words recognized and pursed in a
frightened way.
In (2) there is something like an adversative shade of meaning and this is due to the relation in meaning between
the word Monday in the first clause and that of the word only Thursday in the second.
Types of coordination between the clauses of a compound sentence
1. Copulative coordination. It implies that two events or ideas conveyed by coordinate clauses are merely joined
in time and place. The copulative connectors are: the conjunctions and, nor, neither … nor, not only...but also,
as well as, the conjunctive adverbs then, moreover.
E.g., I didn't recognize the girl, nor I remember her name.
Note. The same kind of relations may be expressed by a complex sentence with a subordinate clause of cause
or result (because, as, so ... that). The difference is that the connection between the subordinate and principal
clauses. The cause or result expressed in coordination is added as a kind of afterthought, both clauses being
independent and separated by a comma or semicolon.
1. The composite sentence expresses a complicated act of thought. The composite sentence in contrast
to the simple sentence is polypredicative, i.e. it has more than one predicative line. In terms of situations and
events this means that the composite sentence reflects two or more elementary situations making up a unity.
Each predicative unit in a composite sentence makes up a clause in it.
e.g. I quickly shut the door before the animal could escape.
This sentence includes two clauses, which are related to one another.
While classifying composite sentences, first of all the way in which the parts of a composite sentence are
joined together must be taken into account. The means of combining clauses into a polypredicative sentence are
divided into
syndetic, i.e. with the clauses linked by conjunctions and conjunctive words (adverbs or pronouns). If
this joining word is a conjunction, it has no other function in the sentence but that of joining the clauses
together. If it is a pronoun or an adverb it, on the one hand, is a part of the clause, which is joined (a subject,
object, adverbial modifier etc.), and on the other hand, it has a joining function as well. e.g. They had a little
quarrel which (but) he soon forgot.
and asyndetic, i.e. without any connectors (e.g. The poorer they became the more desperate they grew)
Sometimes coordination and subordination can be combined within one sentence. In this case we have
compound-complex or complex-compound sentences.
e.g. I know that she hates me, but I’ll make her love me.
It’s a compound-complex sentence because it consists of two subordinate clauses in which one of them is
complex in structure.
A complex-compound sentence has two or more subordinate clauses connected by coordination.
e.g. He realized that he had a terrific responsibility and the best thing to do was to keep his mouth shut.
2. Prof. B.Ilyish, M.Blokh and some other linguists consider that some sentences occupy an
intermediate position between the simple and the composite sentences M. Blokh calls them "semi-composite
sentences".
Among such transitional sentences between the simple and the composite
sentences prof. Ilyish distinguishes;
1) sentences with homogeneous parts;
2) sentences with a dependent appendix;
3) sentences with secondary predication.
Sentences with homogeneous parts can't be called compound because they can't be separated into two
classes, they have one separate subject.
e.g. Scarlett stood in her apple-green "second-day" dress in the parlor of Twelve Oaks amid the blaze of
hundred of candles, jostled by the Same throng as the night before, and saw the plain little face of Melanie
Hamilton glow into beauty... (K. Mitchell).
This sentence cannot be described as a compound one because it has only one subject, but it cannot very
well be described as a simple sentence either, as prof. Ilyish considers, because the predicates are different and
each of them is accompanied by a set of secondary parts. On this basis prof. Ilyish considers it safe to say that
this sentence stands somewhere between simple and compound sentences.
Among sentences with a dependent appendix B.Ilyish distinguishes sentences:
1) with conjunction "than" + noun, pronoun, e.g. ...I've known many ladies who were prettier than you...
(K. Mitchell);
2) sentences containing an adjective or adverb, which may be preceded by the adverb "as" + additional
part consisting of the conjunction "as" + adjective, or noun, or an adverb, e.g. The conduct of a widow must be
twice as circumspect as that of a matron (K. Mitchell). Old Marley was as dead as a door-nail (Ch. Dickens);
3) a subordinating conjunction "as though", "though" + a phrase, e.g. Catherine, though a little
disappointed, had too much good nature to make any opposition.
All these sentences, as prof. Ilyish considers, contain something which doesn't fit into the pattern of a
simple sentence, yet, at the same time they lack something that is necessary to make the sentence complex.
The third group of transitional sentences singled out by prof. Ilyish are sentences with secondary
predication, i.e. predication which is not between the subject and the predicate of the sentence.
O. Jespersen proposed the term "nexus" for every predicative grouping of words. There are several ways
of expressing secondary predication in modern English:
1. The complex object, e.g. We expect you to visit us. The primary predication in this sentence is
between the subject "we" and predicate "expect". The predication between "you - to visit" is obviously a
secondary one. Here belong such objective predicative constructions as
I saw him running (Part.l); I found him ill (adj.); I found him asleep (stative).
2. Another type of secondary predication may be seen in the so-called absolute construction. Absolute
participial constructions are the most frequent among absolute constructions. e.g. The preliminary greetings
spoken, Denis found an empty chair and sat down.
2. Syndetic composite sentences are divided into compound and complex. In the compound sentences
clauses are connected by coordination as units of syntactically equal rank.
A sequential clause in a compound sentence refers to the whole of the leading clause, whereas a
subordinate clause in a complex sentence, as a rule, refers to one notional constituent in a principal clause.
The authors of prescriptive arid scientific grammars considered the compound sentence to be the weak
link in the structural classification of sentences as the compound sentence contains syntactically independent
coordinated clauses or sentences.
The coordinating connectors are divided into conjunctions (and, but, or, nor, for, either … or, neither …
nor, etc.) and connectors of adverbial character (then, yet, so, thus, moreover, however, etc.).
We can represent the constituent structure of the compound sentence with the coordinator as an
immediate constituent of the coordination in the following way.
S
e
The coordinating conjunctions differ from each other in definiteness of meaning. Cf.: 1. The old lady had
recognized Ellen’s handwriting and her fat little mouth was pursed in a frightened way … (M.Mitchel).
2. The bazaar had taken place Monday night and today was only Thursday.
The first sentence has a shade of meaning of cause-result, and this is obviously due to the meanings of
the words recognized and pursed in a frightened way. In the second sentence there is something like an
adversative shade of meaning and this is due to the relation in meaning between the word Monday in the first
clause and that of the word only Thursday in the second.
There are few conjunctions generally classed as subordinating which in certain, conditions tend to
become coordinating. So the sentences in which they occur may be considered to be compound rather than
complex, or, to be more exact, the distinction between coordination and subordination, and consequently that
between compound and complex sentences is in such cases neutralized. This concerns the conjunction while.
The conjunction while expresses simultaneity of an action with another action. However; this meaning of
simultaneity can change into a different meaning.
E.g. 1. Magnus briefly outlined the case for the independent sovereignty of Scotland, while Frieda
listened without any remarkable interest.
2. He sat by the fire while they drank tea.
It is clear that the while-clause in the first example does not express the time when the action of the first
clause took place. It rather expresses adversative relations and it serves to characterize the doer of the action.
In such cases the reasons for calling the while-clause a subordinate one are very much weakened. We
might here put the conjunction and instead of while and the actual meaning would be the same.
There the following types of coordination between the clauses of a compound sentence:
1. Copulative coordination. It implies that two events or ideas conveyed by coordinate clauses are
merely joined in time and place. The copulative connectors are: the conjunctions and, nor, nether … nor, not
only…but also, as well as, the conjunctive adverbs then, moreover.
e.g. I didn’t recognize the girl, nor I remember her name.
2. Adversative coordination joins clauses containing opposition, contradiction or contrast. Adversative
connectors are: the conjunctions but, while, whereas, the conjunctive adverbs yet, still, nevertheless.
e.g. The story was amusing, but nobody laughed. It was midsummer,
yet it was rather chilly.
3. Causative-consecutive coordination joins clauses connected in such a way that one of them contains a
reason and the other – a consequence. The second clause may contain either the reason or the result of the event
conveyed by the previous clause. The conjunctions are: for, so, so that, therefore, hence.
e.g. The days became longer, for it was now springtime. Everything is all
right, so you needn’t worry.
Note. The same kind of relations may be expressed by a complex sentence with a subordinate clause of
cause or result (because, as, so … that). The difference is that the connection between the subordinate and
principal clauses. The cause or result expressed in coordination is added as a kind of afterthought, both clauses
being independent and separated by a comma or semicolon.
Cf. The windows were open, for it was hot. (coordination) The windows were
open because it was hot.
It was so hot that the windows were open.
4. The complex sentence is a polypredicative construction built up on the principle of subordination. In
the complex sentences clauses are connected by subordination as units of unequal rank, one being categorically
dominated by the other. Subordination is a non-symmetrical relation between two clauses one of which is a
constituent part of the other. Compare the coordination in "I like John and John likes me" with the subordination
in "I like John because John likes me”.
The clause within which a subordinate clause is embedded is said to be superordinate to one or more
other clauses, so that a hierarchy of clauses, one within another, may be built up. A superordinate clause may be
a main clause but it does not have to be.
A relatively simple example is: e.g. I think that you can do it if you try.
Here the subordinate clause If you try is an immediate constituent of the
superordinate clause You can do it if you try, and this clause in turn is an immediate constituent of the
superordinate (and main) clause I think that you can do it if you try. The relationships may be seen more clearly
in a tree diagram.
Se – sentence S – subject
NP – noun phrase P – predicate
VP – verb phrase O – object
A – adjunct
SCl – subordinate clause
S
e
S:NPP:V A:S
P Cl
O
d
Thus, the complex sentence consists of the principal (main) clause and the subordinate clause. But terms
"main" and "subordinate" are rather conventional ones. There are complex sentences, in which the main clause is
not represented even by a part of a sentence, but its component: e.g. What he learnt was that they had never
arrived.
Subordinate clauses correlate with the parts of the sentence, i.e. with words, but unlike the words they
render a situation. Subordinate clauses can be classified according to two principles:
l) according to their correlation with the parts of the sentence, subordinate clauses are divided into
subject clauses, predicative clauses, object clauses, attributive clauses and adverbial clauses. Prof. M. Blokh
calls this classification "the functional" one, because clauses are classed on the functional principle, i.e. on the
analogy of the positional parts of the simple sentence.
2) the second classification is on the basis of correlation with the parts of speech: subordinate clauses
substantial, adjectival, adverbial. M. Blokh calls this classification a categorial one, i.e. the classification on the
categorial principle.
The traditional definition of the subject clause is the following a clause which performs within a
complex sentence the same function that the subject performs within a simple sentence. It quite fits for the cases
like. e.g. What had happened was that I had spent too much time in the library.
Things are somewhat more difficult and controversial in sentences like: It had seemed certain that their
meeting was fortunate. There are two views upon such sentences: l) It is formal subject, subordinate clause is
real subject; 2) It is real subject; subordinate clause is appositional clause referring to the subject of the main
clause.
Predicative clauses are clauses of the type: This was exactly what she had expected him to say... The
reason for calling these clauses predicative is that if they are dropped the sentence will be unfinished: there will
be the link verb, but the predicative will be missing.
We must also consider under the heading of predicative clauses the following type: It's because he's weak
that he needs me. Here the predicative subordinate clause is included within the construction "it is ... that".
The easiest cases of complex sentences with the object subordinate clause are those in which the
subordinate clause can be replaced by a noun which would then be an object in a simple sentence. These are the
sentences of the type. e.g. He bought what he wanted.
Object clauses introduced by prepositions are termed prepositional object clauses. Most verbs which
combine with a preposition in a simple sentence do so in a complex sentence as well. e.g. It depends on what
you will say.
But we must note that a prepositional object in a simple sentence does not always correspond to a
prepositional object clause, e.g. the verb "insist" which always combines with the preposition "on" (or upon) in
& simple sentence never has this preposition when followed by an object clause, e.g. Strickland insisted that he
should take the picture.
In some cases an object in a simple sentence may have a synonymous object clause. e.g. I heard of his
arrival – I heard that he had arrived. The meaning of the two sentences in each case is exactly the same, but
there is a certain stylistic difference: the simple sentence with the prepositional object sounds more literary or
even bookish than the complex sentence with the object clause which is fit for any sort of style.
Most usually attributive clause comes immediately after its head word. But sometimes it comes after
some other word or phrase. e.g. He wanted Ann to die, whom his son passionately loved.
1. The word-group is a separate language unit that qualitatively differs from other language units (a
word, a clause, a sentence). To better understand the phenomenon of the word-group, let us consider it in
opposition to the sentence.
1) The phrase is a nominative unit, denoting a complex referent (phenomenon of reality), which can be a
concrete thing, an action, a quality, or a whole situation (a picturesque village, to start with a joke, extremely
difficult, the unexpected arrival of the chief). The phrase is further analyzed into its component elements with
various relations between them.
2) A word-group and a sentence refer to different sublevels within syntax. A word-group is non-
communicative, may only be a part of an utterance, while the sentence is the basic communicative unit, stands
separately in speech in the form of an utterance.
3) A word-group and a sentence differ greatly in the formal aspect. Within a word-group it is quite
possible to change the paradigmatic form of the constituents, without destroying the identity of the word-group
(e.g. to read a book – to be reading a book, to have read a book, to read books). Things are different with
reference to a sentence (e.g. The book was read). The change of the paradigmatic form of the constituents is sure
to result in destroying the structure of the sentence and will lead to unmarkedness of the structure. A word-group
does not have an intonation of its own, while the intonation is one of the essential properties of a sentence.
There exist many definitions of a word phrase. The traditional view is that a word phrase is a combination
of two or more notional words. Prof. B. Ilyish defines phrase as every combination of two or more words which
is a grammatical unit but is not an analytical form of some word (for instance, the perfect forms of verbs), even
“prep + noun”, “conj + noun” etc. are considered to be a word phrase.
Thus, there are “noun+noun”, “adjective+noun”, “verb+noun”, “verb+adverb”, “adverb+adjective”,
“adverb+adverb” types of phrases. Phrases are made up not only by notional words but also by functional words,
e.g.: “in accordance with”, “due to”, “apart form”, “as soon as” – such phrases function in the sentence like
prepositions and conjunctions.
The theory of word phrase appeared abroad only in the 30s of the XXth century. The American linguist
L. Bloomfield treats a word phrase widely as any syntactically organized group. He divides word phrases into
two groups: 1) exocentric or headed (from Gr. endon ‘inside’) and exocentric or non-headed (from Gr. exo
‘outside’). Endocentric structures are such structures in which one of the constituents can function in the same
way as the whole structure, e.g. poor John is the endocentric word phrase because "John" can substitute the
whole word phrase in the
sentence Poor John ran away – John ran away. In exocentric constructions neither of the constituents can
substitute the whole word-combination, e.g. John ran.
There are four slots where certain kinds of elements can go. The central element of the phrase, the noun, is
in slot 3, while slot 2 is optionally occupied by an adjective, and slot 1 is occupied by an article (the, a), a
demonstrative (this) or a possessive (his, their). Note that this slot can also remain empty, as for example in
[people who knock on the door]. Slot 4 can either remain empty or may contain smaller or larger constituents.
The most important element of a phrase is its head which names the phrases after their heads. Thus we have
examples of noun phrases (abbreviated as ‘NPs’), i.e. of phrases headed by a noun. As we can see in the
following examples, other kinds of phrases have other kinds of head.
1. As each sentence is a sign of situation, we can speak about situational semantics of the sentence. The
famous French linguist Lucien Tesnière compared the sentence with a theatrical performance having its
participants and circumstances. Like a drama, it obligatorily involves a process and most often actors and
circumstances. Transferred from the theatre to structural syntax, the process, the actors, and the circumstances
become respectively the verb, the actants, and the circumstants (L. Tesnière, Elements of Structural Syntax,
2015, p. 97).
When we analyze the sentence as it is represented in speech we can speak about its surface structure
which can be described in terms of subject, object, predicate, attribute and adverbial modifier.
On the other hand, when we analyze the sentence as a sign of situation we speak about its deep structure.
e.g. The role of Phaedra was played by a beautiful French actress madam Besancon.
The most widely spread semantic or thematic roles include agent, nominative, patient, factitive,
instrument, location, source, temporative.
Agent is defined as an animate thing which fulfils the action denoted by the
verb.
e.g. The audience rose to the feet. John
broke the windshield.
In the surface structure it is conveyed by the subject or subject complement
(e.g. Tom kicked the ball; The ball was kicked by Tom).
Nominative is a semantic role which correlates with the agent but unlike the agent it is expressed by an
inanimate noun (e.g. His eyes twinkled. Mountains frightened him. The wind broke the windshield.)
Patient can be described as
(a) an entity viewed as existing in a state, located or moving
e.g. The sky is blue The stone
moved
(b) an entity affected by the action or state, it is rendered by the object or object complement in the
surface structure. (e.g. I gave to children some sweets. The surgeons operated on her for several hours).
Factitive is the entity that didn’t exist before but come into being as a result of the action (e.g.
Frankenstein created a monster. Tom dug the hole. Mary baked a cake);
Instrument is the object (usually inanimate) with which the act is accomplished (e.g. He got beaten by a
gang; Tom cut a rose with a knife; John opened the door with the crowbar).
The location role has different subtypes because there are many locative meanings expressed by the
prepositions on, in, over, above, under etc. Hence there are locatives of starting point (e.g. He jumped from the
rock), locatives of terminal (e.g. He jumped into the sea).
The location role can also be played by the phrase that designates the state at or in which something is at
a particular time (e.g. John is in a foul humor. John is in his evening wear). Locatives usually correspond to the
adverbial modifier in the surface structure but sometimes it may be denoted by the subject (e.g. The bus holds
forty people. Forty people can sit in the bus).
The source role is played by the phrase that indicates the place or state to which something moves:
e.g. John went from New York to New Orleans.
his bed his bath
silly serious
Temporative usually corresponds to the adverbial modifier of time in the surface structure but
sometimes it may correspond to the subject (e.g. Yesterday was a busy day. Tomorrow is his birthday).
Purpose denotes the purpose of an activity (e.g. He went to the café for some take-away).
Manner means the way in which an activity is done or the way in which a change of state takes place
(e.g. She spoke to us politely).
2. The most important feature of the sentence is the reaction of the speaker to some reality. All declarative
sentences contain two basic content elements:
(1) a statement and (2) an element about which the statement is made.
If a sentence is to be formulated clearly, especially in writing, we should make a clear-cut distinction
between these two basic elements, i.e. we should employ a functional sentence perspective (FSP), theme-rheme
structure or actual division.
The actual division of the sentence is the division of it into the part, which carries the data, known before,
what the sentence is about, the theme of the sentence;
and the new information, what is said about the theme, the rheme. Actual division is such communicative
organization of the sentence which is predetermined by the speaker’s will who singles out introductory
information which as a rule is connected with the previous sentence and a new information. Thus, actual division
of the sentence is its informational arrangement.
Of the numerous terms used to designate the constituents of information units, we deal with the two most
wide-spread, i.e. “theme” and “rheme”. Theme comes from a Greek root “the” which means “to establish”, “to
set”. Rheme also comes from Greek “rhe” which denotes “to say”, “to tell”.
The prevailing view is that theme as an item of discourse denotes something that is spoken about,
something that is known or at least obvious in the given situation.
Rheme is what one says about the theme, it conveys the most important information already established
and whose expression is the purpose of communication.
e.g. John gave a book to Mary.
In this sentence Mary is the rheme when we can ask the question: Whom did John give a book? But if we
are interested in Who gave a book to Mary? Then, John is a rheme or Mary (T) was given a book by John (R).
Thus, the theme denotes the starting point of the communication, an object or phenomenon about
something is reported, The rheme expresses the main information of the sentence.
It is possible to determine the theme and the rheme by using the context caused by extra- and
intralinguistic factors. The extralinguistic factors are situational referents, underlying questions or disagreement
arising from the situation, the communicant’s intentions and presuppositions.
(1) ‘Smell!’ said Pollard, suffing the wind.
(2) An old woman went to pick berries.
In (1), the extralinguistic factor is the situational referents – what the speaker smells, while the rheme is
smell.
In (2), the extralinguistic factor is a presupposition – an expectation of some action, the rheme being the
whole sentence (an old woman … berries).
(b) Лісництво (T) було недалеко від села (R). Недалеко від села
(T) було лісництво (R). The forestry (T) was near the village
(R). There was a forestry (R) near the village (T).
The words дівчина and лісництво are the theme in Ukrainian when they stand at the beginning of the
sentence and the rheme when they are in the end position. In English this difference is made clear by the use of
the articles and the structure with “there is”.
(4) syntactic constructions. In Modern English there are several ways of showing that a word or phrase
corresponds either to the rheme or to the theme. These are the constructions it is … that, it is … who, it is …
which with the word or phrase representing the rheme being enclosed between the words it is and the word that
(who, which etc.). Such sentences are called cleft sentences.
e.g. John wore his best suit for the dance last night.
In this sentence it is possible to stress any part with the help of the cleft sentence.
Variants: