Critical Media Literacy and Gender
Critical Media Literacy and Gender
Critical Media Literacy and Gender
org
Abstract
This study examined the effectiveness of the implementation of a small-scale critical media literacy curriculum
unit focused on gender stereotypes, especially as they pertain to occupations. The research question was
whether students exposed to the critical media literacy (CML) curriculum were more likely than students not
exposed to believe: that women experience discrimination in the workplace; that the media constructs
stereotypical messages about women and men, especially regarding occupations; and that the media influences
people’s thinking. Participants were students in 5 seventh grade classes, who were exposed to a 4-workshop
curriculum, and students in 5 eighth grades classes, who were not. Methods included a 14-question pre-post
survey and interviews with 4 students before and after implementation of the CML unit. Quantitative and
qualitative findings indicate that the CML unit was generally successful at increasing the seventh graders’
understanding of target issues.
Keywords: media literacy, social justice, gender, occupations, stereotyping, middle-school, research
Introduction
Critical media literacy focuses on unpacking hidden power messages in the media and learning to
resist the messages (Alvermann and Hagood 2000; Share 2009). Although wide calls for critical media
literacy exist, as well as recommendations for how to teach it, there is little empirical research testing the
ability of critical media literacy to educate students about social justice issues. The current study addresses
this gap by examining the effectiveness of the implementation of a small-scale critical media literacy
curriculum unit focused on gender stereotypes, especially as they pertain to occupations. In this paper, we
discuss the results from a field experiment that measured whether students exposed to a critical media
literacy (CML) curriculum were more likely than students not exposed to believe: that women experience
discrimination in the workplace; that the media constructs stereotypical messages about women and men,
especially regarding occupations; and that the media influences people’s thinking.
Our participants were students in 5 seventh grade classes, who were exposed to a 4-workshop
curriculum, and students in 5 eighth grades classes, who were not. The goals of the critical media literacy
curriculum were twofold: (1) show students that stereotypes about men and women are socially constructed
and contribute to the discrimination that women experience in the workplace; (2) convey that the media
creates and perpetuates some of those gender stereotypes. Our hope was that education about discrimination
23
L. Puchner, L. Markowitz & M. Hedley / Journal of Media Literacy Education 7(2), 23 - 34
and gender stereotypes in the media could influence students’ attitudes about the entrance of males and
females into gender atypical professions.
We frame our study using critical media literacy, or media literacy aimed at exposing social injustice.
Our focus was sexism. Defining sexism in the media is a challenging task. Gill (2007) cites how trends such
as sexualization of women, for example, can be seen both positively, as movement away from earlier
repressive media representations of women, or negatively, as commodification and objectification of female
bodies. Although areas of contestation exist, media literacy scholars recognize that racist, sexist, classist, and
anti-democratic messages abound in the media. Giroux (1994) calls for studying racism, sexism, classism
and other forms of oppression within media to provide “a sense of agency in students based on a
commitment to changing oppressive contexts by understanding the relations of power that inform them”
(297). While media audiences are not passive consumers of such messages (Jenkins 1992), oppressive
messages still play a large role in forming the beliefs of youth (Giroux 1994, 1995). Jenkins, Ford, and Green
(2013) call for media consumers to “exercise greater power over the decisions which impact the quality of
our everyday lives as citizens” by being political participants in media (156). The issue we are addressing is
to what extent do students learn to be critical consumers of the media through CML.
Literature Review
When in occupations, females are often presented in fields perceived as nurturing: teachers, nurses, social
workers, sex workers or secretaries (Gainer 2010). Giroux (1994, 1995) provides analyses of both racism and
sexism in Disney and other films. Further, the films and writings of Jean Kilbourne (c.f. Kilbourne 2000;
Kilbourne, Jhally and Rabinovitz 2010) chronicle the damaging nature of gender stereotypes and sexism in
advertising. Others have written about the damaging messages in video games, magazines, the internet, TV,
and music (Alvermann and Hagood 2000).
student production of media that challenges dominant narratives, meaning that educators need to teach
students the skills necessary for production (Giroux 1995), so that the creation of alternative media, and not
just deconstruction of what already exists, is incorporated into the educational process (Kellner and Share
2005). The social activism aspect of CML means using media deconstruction to promote political
engagement (Giroux 2001; Jenkins et al 2013), such that students use what they learn and see in media to
understand and act on wider social issues outside the media domain. The ultimate goal of CML theorists is to
create an active citizenry that feels connected to broad democratic ideas rather than ignorant of and alienated
by them.
The literature on critical media literacy (CML) is clear about the first goal required to develop
participatory-democratic skills: expose the social construction of media and the ideologies behind them. The
question is how to develop a successful curriculum to meet this goal. A limited number of studies have
assessed the impact of media literacy instruction on student cognition and skills (c.f., Hobbs and Frost 2003),
and some empirical, quantitative work has tested the effectiveness of social justice curricula (Weisgram and
Bigler 2007; Phalke, Bigler, and Green 2010). However, there are few studies that bring social justice and
media literacy aspects together to test the application of a practical critical media literacy curriculum. Indeed,
the empirical CML studies to date have mostly been qualitative, applying a post-modern theoretical approach
to understanding how students make sense of popular culture. For example, Cherland (2008) provides a post-
structural deconstruction of how children read the Harry Potter books. Cullen and Sandy (2009) discuss the
reactions of young students to the remaking of a lesbian Cinderella story. In their books, Share (2009) and
Alvermann, Moon, and Hagood (1999) describe many cases of how teachers applied post-modern
deconstruction tools in their classrooms. Such qualitative studies as the ones described above are important
in that they provide rich information about how innovative teachers attempt to unsettle traditional notions of
power relations in society. Yet, the individual case description approach to the studies demonstrates the
difficulty of developing and widely disseminating a post-modern CML curriculum.
Indeed, despite calls by Kellner and Share (2007a,b) and others (c.f., Giroux 1994) for incorporation
of CML into school curricula, the field of education in the US has not embraced CML (Giroux 1994; Kellner
and Share 2007a,b). Giroux (1994) argues that educators tend to pride themselves as being “scientific and
objective,” and “practical,” and are thus somewhat suspicious of the ideological objectives of cultural studies
(279). One way to make CML seem more appropriate in the eyes of educators might be to use more practical
and positivist approaches to demonstrate the efficacy of CML. Many in education worry about
representations of girls and women in the media (c.f., Gray 2005; Gannon 2007; Aina and Petronella 2011).
Using more traditional methods of study to show how CML is effective at counteracting some of those
problems may be a way to make CML more palatable to teachers and others in the field of education. Thus as
part of the wider CML effort to help teachers and other educators see the value of CML, we developed a
pretest-posttest comparison group design, in addition to using qualitative methods, to examine the
effectiveness of CML instruction.
(3) the media portrays stereotypical messages about women’s interests in men and family and men’s interest
in careers; (4) the media portrays stereotypical images about women and men and in the context of
occupations; and (5) the media influences their own behavior and occupational choices.
To gain additional information about what students learned in the workshops, we conducted semi-structured
interviews with four students before and after their participation in the curriculum unit. The purpose of the
interviews was to examine differences in student responses to questions about gender stereotypes in the
media before and after exposure to the unit, and to find out what students felt they had learned in the
workshops.
Methods
Study Context
We participated in a grant-funded project to develop and test a critical media literacy curriculum unit
aimed at middle school students (12 to 14-year olds). We worked with two language arts teachers to develop
the 4-lesson unit in Fall 2012 and early Spring 2013, and taught the unit in the teachers’ classes in March-
May of 2013. As a culminating assignment the students worked in groups to create video advertisements that
challenged gender stereotypes.
With the help of a media consultant, we developed four workshops to teach students to be media
literate. The 45-minute lessons comprised discussion and activities revolving around multiple examples of
print and video ads. In Workshop 1, we introduced, demonstrated, and discussed the following: gender
stereotypes, causes of stereotypes, the relationship between stereotypes and performance in
school/occupations, media disruption, and the concept of critical media literacy. To facilitate understanding
of these concepts and ideas, we showed students various provocative visual media, such as ads and posters,
and asked students to discuss, in small and large group format, questions related to sexism, stereotypes, and
the messages in the media. For example, after projecting and discussing two backpack ads, one geared
towards girls that read “Light as a feather tough as long division” and one geared towards boys that read,
“Superlight, super hero tough,” we asked students to generate stereotypes about men and women which we
documented in charts at the front of the room.
In Workshop 2, we discussed gender ideas about occupations, how those ideas can come from the
media and how gender ideas influence access to economic opportunities. Activities in this workshop included
discussing the results of a questionnaire they had completed indicating whether various jobs were typically
associated with men, women, or both. We tabulated the students’ responses, and students worked to cross-
reference a salary sheet with the results to answer questions about which occupations had higher salaries and
whether such jobs were typically associated with men or women.
In Workshop 3, students worked together to deconstruct television commercials and print ads. The
lesson began with discussion of two video commercials advertising vacuum cleaners, one traditional,
containing many negative occupation-related gender stereotypes and one alternative and compelling ad that
is counter-stereotypical. In this Workshop students also worked in groups to deconstruct magazine ads that
we gave them, and to construct alternative, counter-stereotypical ads, which they presented to the class.
During Workshop 3, we observed that students identified sexist messages in the ads that we, the
facilitators, had not even noticed. For example, an ad selling car insurance showed a white, male
mathematician having written an equation on the chalkboard. Students noticed that the formula suggested
that girls were bad drivers as the pictorial graph essentially said, “putting on mascara plus the square root of
deer on a rainy night equals car wreck.”
At the end of the third lesson, students received a handout describing their final video ad assignment,
which was to work in groups to create a video ad that related to occupations in some way and that challenged
gender stereotypes. Students spent four class periods between the third and fourth lesson creating the videos
27
L. Puchner, L. Markowitz & M. Hedley / Journal of Media Literacy Education 7(2), 23 - 34
and then in the final lesson of the unit, the teacher played each video and students, teacher, and researchers
discussed them.
We taught the lessons on Fridays and they were spaced one week apart except for the first two, which
were two weeks apart. The researchers taught the first three lessons with the classroom teacher present. The
teacher and the researchers co-facilitated the final lesson in which each student-produced video was shown
and discussed.
Findings
Survey Results
We analyzed students’ responses to fourteen items on both the pre-test and post-test regarding gender
discrimination in the workplace and stereotypes in the media. Given that we were unable to establish our
groups through randomization, we conducted preliminary t-tests (pretest measures by condition) to assess the
degree of exogenous differences between groups. On the pretest the control group (eighth grade students)
expressed significantly more disagreement with the item, “In the media, I am just as likely to see female
scientists as I am to see male scientists.” The remaining pretest questions regarding stereotypes in the media
were not significantly different.
Our primary statistical analyses examined whether students in the two conditions showed similar
attitudinal changes from pretest to posttest. Nine of the 14 items showed significant interactions for time of
testing by condition, all in expected directions.
In terms of beliefs about discrimination in the workplace, we found that the seventh grade treatment
condition expressed significantly more increase in agreement pretest to posttest than the eighth grade control
condition to the three items that asked about gender discrimination in the workplace. The three items were:
women tend to have jobs that get paid less (F 1,236=39.095, p<.001), men tend to have more jobs available to
them (F1,237=15.914, p,.001) and men tend to have jobs that are perceived as more important (F1,235=31.051,
p<001).
We also found that students exposed to the unit were significantly more likely than students not
exposed to believe that the media influences the way people think about men and women. Specifically, the
28
L. Puchner, L. Markowitz & M. Hedley / Journal of Media Literacy Education 7(2), 23 - 34
treatment group showed more pre-post change toward agreement that the media has hidden messages about
gender (F1,237=18.009, p<.001), that the media generally influences the way people think about men (F 1,236
=28.252, p<.001), and that the media generally influences the way people think about women (F
1,235=18.136, p<.001).
Findings regarding student beliefs related to whether the media portrays stereotypical messages about
women’s interests in men and family and men’s interest in careers were mixed. We did find that students in
the treatment group showed more change in agreement that the media is likely to present women as sexy and
in need of men’s attention (F2,235=9.005, p<.01). However, students did not significantly change their opinion
about whether the media often presents women as moms or whether men are more likely than women to be
shown as concerned with their careers.
We did find that students in the treatment group showed more pre-post change in beliefs about
whether the media portrays stereotypical portrayals about women and men in the context of occupations.
Specifically, students in the treatment group expressed significantly more increase in disagreement pretest to
posttest than the eighth grade control group with the item, “In the media, I am just as likely to see female
scientists as I am to see male scientists” (F 1,237=31.288, p<.001) and agreement with the item, “In the media,
men are more likely than women to be shown as mathematicians” (F 1,237=24.533, p<.001).
We did not find that students exposed to the curriculum unit were more likely to believe that the
media influences their own behavior and occupational choices. The treatment group was not significantly
more likely after the treatment to agree with the statements “Media messages have an impact on how I
behave” and “Media messages may impact the occupation (job) I choose.”
Interview Findings
We transcribed the interviews and then read and reread them with a focus on interviewees’
understandings and beliefs about stereotypes, on differences in understanding prior to and after the
workshops, and on what they reported about the workshops in the second interview. Two findings emerged
from the interview data. First, the one student who displayed the least amount of knowledge of gender
stereotypes in analyzing the print ad in the pre-unit interview displayed much more knowledge in the second
interview. Specifically, prior to the workshops, three of the students spoke knowledgeably about the basic
male and female stereotypes in the Toys R Us gift certificate ad we showed them. Gail, for example, said:
“The little girl dressed as a fairy and the boy has a light saber, so pretty stereotypical.” In addition to
identifying the stereotypical nature of the ad, these three spoke negatively about the stereotypes and how they
do not give an accurate portrayal of all girls and boys. However, one of the four students, Anna, spoke less
knowledgeably than the others. When asked what messages about boys and girls the ad gives us, she stated
that boys and girls are different. However, she did not express any concern about the messages, and when
asked what she thought of them, she said: “Well I think it is a good thing for younger kids so they [parents]
know what the child likes.” During the second interview, however, Anna’s response to the different toy ad
we showed was much more sophisticated: “Well, I’ll say this is very stereotypical…They got like a boy there
making crazy monsters and aliens and things, but they got the girl over here making flowers and pretty
rainbows. But some girls do like the alien shows that come on…and then they don’t show the boy liking
flowers and they don’t show the girl liking kooky things…So they should have said both girls and boys love
kooky and pretty things, that would make it less stereotypical.”
A second finding from the interviews is that when asked questions about what they got out of the
workshops, all four provided a variety of responses consistent with important intended workshop learning
objectives. For example, when asked what seemed most important to them from the workshops, both boys
said it was how negatively the media portrays girls. Allan’s answer was “About how badly they put like girls
in like positions like as always putting men as the better gender,” while Warren said: “The way that the
media looks at females, probably. I think they sometimes are looking very disrespectfully and like you were
talking about using them as sex objects, that part stood out the most…” In the case of Gail, since she had
29
L. Puchner, L. Markowitz & M. Hedley / Journal of Media Literacy Education 7(2), 23 - 34
appeared fairly confident and knowledgeable about gender stereotypes in her first interview, in the second
interview the interviewer asked her whether she had learned anything in the workshops that she had not been
aware of before. Referring to a Workshop 1 discussion about a print ad in which a woman is standing in a
submissive posture behind her “husband,” she responded: “Yes. Like for the commercial on how they were
posing I didn’t think that that was one [a gender message].” She added “and like that one ad that we had
where it was on a chalkboard and how that was even a stereotype and all that stuff.” In this latter excerpt she
is referring to the insurance ad described earlier showing a math teacher/insurance agent with an equation on
the blackboard about a driver putting on makeup and getting into an accident.
When Anna was asked what stuck out most to her in the unit, she discussed the fact that media
contain hidden messages:
Well, the messages is what hit me the most, because it is so crazy how you can say, “Oh, it’s just a piece of
paper,” but when you go really into detail it’s like, “Oh, I didn’t even see the message, you had a deep
message in there, you couldn’t even see,” and I’m like “Oh, I didn’t even see.” It’s like a code kind of, that
they don’t even want people to see…
Anna also said that an important point she got from the workshops was to avoid judging males who dress and
act in a stereotypically female manner. She spoke about a friend of her mother’s “that is a man but he’s kind
of a girl but it’s okay because he’s very nice, he’s very funny and I love him…And it’s very good to think
like because it is good.”
These interviews were brief, and student responses to the interviewer’s questions were likely
influenced by a desire to please. As a supplement to the questionnaire results, however, the interview
findings show that at least some students were able to independently and in an open-ended format articulate
important knowledge they had gained or that at the very least had been reinforced in the curriculum unit.
While the points raised above by Anna, Warren, and Allan pertained to CML ideas about discrimination and
media messages that cut across the lessons, excerpts from Gail show that she remembered specific details
from discussions that had occurred approximately two and five weeks earlier.
To nurture active citizens, we must provide children with analytical tools that allow them to
deconstruct hidden messages in the media. In this particular study, we were interested in students’ learning
about media messages regarding gender, specifically, could a CML curriculum teach students about
discrimination women experience inside and outside the media?
We asked first whether students exposed to the CML unit would show greater pre-post change in responses
to questions about gender discrimination in the workplace. We found that the seventh grade treatment group
did show greater pre-post change in agreement with the three job discrimination statements: women tend to
get paid less, have less access to jobs and have jobs that are less valued. These were all questions about
beliefs and attitudes. Although we cannot tell from these survey answers how deeply-held these beliefs were,
our findings indicate that the unit was at least somewhat effective in teaching students about gender
discrimination and is consistent with previous studies showing that education does change students’ attitudes
about whether gender discrimination exists (Weisgram and Bigler 2007; Weisgram et al. 2010).
We also found that after the workshop students were more likely to report believing that the media
influences the way people think about men and women. This finding is important because developing an
understanding of the role of hidden media messages is a fundamental goal of both media literacy education
and critical media literacy.
Interestingly, despite their agreement with the notion that media influences the way people in general
think, students in the treatment group did not shift in their beliefs that their own behaviors would be affected
by those hidden messages. During the workshops, we had heavily emphasized that the media has an
influence on all of us, so from a methodological perspective one implication of this finding is that in
30
L. Puchner, L. Markowitz & M. Hedley / Journal of Media Literacy Education 7(2), 23 - 34
completing the survey the students were not simply putting down answers that they knew we would like,
since in this case their responses contradicted one of our major messages. From an educational perspective,
however, this finding is unfortunate in that changing the effects of media on gender stereotypes in the
workplace and beyond can only happen when people see themselves as responsible for learning and
perpetuating gender stereotypes. Although disappointing, this finding is consistent with research supporting
Davison’s (1983) third person effect theory, or the idea that people simply do not believe the media
influences them the way it influences others (Golan and Day 2008). Third person effect theory tends to apply
across the life span, but middle and high school age may be an even more difficult age span than others to
convince students that the media affects them personally due to the tendency towards feelings of invincibility
that characterize adolescence (Steinberg 2003).
Another question addressed in the study was whether students exposed to the critical media literacy
curriculum unit were more likely to believe that the media portrays stereotypical messages about women’s
interests in men and family and men’s interest in careers. Students in the treatment group did show more
change in agreement that the media is likely to present women as sexy and in need of men’s attention, a
finding that makes sense in light of the high levels of interest students displayed when we showed examples
of and discussed the portrayal of women as sex objects in the media. However, treatment group students
were not significantly more likely after the workshops to believe that women are presented more as moms
and men are presented more as careerists. A possible reason for the lack of significant difference in pre-post
change on these items may be that many students already recognized, pre-workshops, that the media tends to
present women as moms and men as careerists. Indeed, there was a change in pre-post mean for the treatment
group in the expected directions, which was not true for the control group, whose means actually decreased a
small amount pre-post. However, the pre-test scores for all students were relatively high on these two items,
thus it may have been harder to show a statistically significant pre-post difference.
We found that students exposed to a critical media literacy curriculum were more likely to report
believing that the media portrays stereotypical images about women and men in the context of scientific and
mathematic occupations. The finding for the scientist item is especially interesting given that on the pretest
the eighth grade control group was found to be significantly more in disagreement than the seventh grade
treatment group with the same item. In other words, the eighth grade control group results showed a greater
understanding of the reality of this issue prior to the workshops, but after the workshops the seventh grade
treatment group changed their belief about the media presentation of women in science. The workshop may
have been effective, then, at making students aware that the media is less likely to present women in STEM
professions.
The pre-post test results of this study are promising. However, quantitative studies using a pre- and
post-test Likert-style format are limited, as the ability to correctly respond to closed-format multiple choice
questions in the context of school does not necessarily translate into more authentic skills of knowledgeably
talking about and acting in a socially responsible manner regarding gender issues and the media. Our
interviews with four students before and after the implementation of the unit provide some triangulation, as
they indicate that the interviewees were able to describe ideas they learned from the curriculum unit. We
don’t know how convinced the two boys were that gender discrimination is important, but their responses
indicate that they at least learned that some people do believe this, and they each expressed the concept of
gender discrimination in their own words (i.e., using language that was not simply a parroting of what we
presenters had said). Gail had come into the pre-interview already equipped with the understanding that the
word “stereotype” was appropriate for describing the toy ad we showed them, and Gail and both boys had
realized that expressing disapproval for the stereotypical nature of the ad was appropriate in that interview
context. Anna did neither of these things in her pre-interview, but confidently did both of them in the post-
interview, indicating that she may have learned to think somewhat differently about certain types of media.
Of course we don’t know how broadly any of the students will apply this thinking, but it corroborates the
survey results in a promising manner.
31
L. Puchner, L. Markowitz & M. Hedley / Journal of Media Literacy Education 7(2), 23 - 34
Although the study results are promising, the study has many limitations. First, as alluded to earlier,
survey responses may be indicators of surface level beliefs and understandings, but do not necessarily
indicate depth of understanding or internalization of the beliefs and ideas that we were trying to teach. The
curriculum unit was short, and the interview sample was very small. Also, although the interviews allowed
for open-ended responses, we have no way of knowing from those responses either the extent to which
students really understood or believed what they were telling us. We can say that the interview results
indicate that students may have learned about negative stereotypes from the curriculum unit, but we cannot
say for sure. Further, even if small changes in attitudes and knowledge occurred, we have no evidence that
they would lead to changes in behavior.
Proponents of CML have been calling for its integration into education curricula for many years,
however progress has been slow. One reason for this is that education has a strong focus on scientific
evidence, practical concerns, and testing accountability. These concerns don’t match well with CML which
has a post-structural focus inconsistent with testing. Our testing here of a CML curriculum via quantitative
means might be more convincing for some educators, by showing evidence, in a practical manner, that
supports CML’s effectiveness at changing students’ understandings and beliefs.
In a small-scale curriculum initiative like this one, it is necessary to focus on a specific social justice
issue. We chose to focus on an issue that is of current concern in the field of education: the lack of girls
entering STEM fields. Our ultimate hope is that if boys and girls understand how media provides sexist
messages about gender and jobs that influence girls’ and boys’ entrance into various professions, then both
boys and girls will be better able to resist and counter such messages. Understanding that the sexist messages
exist and that they influence people will only be helpful if students understand sex discrimination more
broadly.
References
Allan, K., and S. Coltrane. 1996. “Gender Displaying Television Commercials: A Comparative Study of
Television Commercials in the 1950’s and 1980’s.” Sex Roles, 35: 185-204.
Aina, O.E., and A. C. Petronella. 2011. “Why Does Gender Matter? Counteracting Stereotypes with Young
Children.” Dimensions of Early Childhood, 39 (3): 11-20.
32
L. Puchner, L. Markowitz & M. Hedley / Journal of Media Literacy Education 7(2), 23 - 34
Alvermann, Donna. E., J. Moon and Margaret C. Hagood. 1999. Popular culture in the classroom: Teaching
and researching media literacy. Chicago, IL: National Reading Conference.
Alvermann. Donna and Margaret Hagood. 2000. “Critical Media Literacy: Research, Theory, and Practice in
‘New Times.’” Journal of Educational Research, 93(3) 193-205.
Bem, Sandra. 1981. “Gender Schema Theory: A Cognitive Account of Sex Typing.” Psychological Review,
88: 354-364.
Carr, P.R. 2009. “Political Conscientization and Media (II) Literacy: Critiquing the Mainstream Media as a
Form of Democratic Engagement.” Multicultural Education, 17 (1): 2-10.
Cherland, M. 2008. “Harry’s Girls: Harry Potter and the Discourse of Gender.” Journal of Adolescent and
Adult Literacy, 52: 273-282.
Cullen, F. and L. Sandy. 2009. “Lesbian Cinderella and Other Stories: Telling Tales and Researching
Sexualities Equalities in Primary Schools.” Sex Education, 9: 141:154.
D’Acci, Julie. 2004. “Television, Representation, and Gender.” In The Television Studies Reader, ed. Robert
C. Allen and Annette Hill, 373-385. London: Routledge.
Davison, W. P. 1983. The Third-Person Effect in Communication. Public Opinion Quarterly, 47: 1-15.
Dole, Janice A. & Sinatra, Gale M. 2011. Reconceptualizing Change in the Cognitive Conception of
Knowledge. Educational Psychologist, 33 (2/3): 109-128.
Evans, C.D., and A.B. Diekman. 2009. “On Motivated Role Selection: Gender Beliefs, Distant Goals, and
Career Preferences.” Psychology of Women Quarterly, 33: 235-249.
Gainer, J.S. 2010. “Critical Media Literacy in Middle School: Exploring the Politics of Representation.”
Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy, 53 (5): 364-373.
Gannon, S. 2007. “Laptops and Lipsticks: Feminizing Technology.” Learning, Media and Technology, 32
(1): 53-67.
Gill, Rosalind. 2007. Gender and the Media. Cambridge, UK: Polity Press.
Giroux, Henry A. 1994. “Doing Cultural Studies: Youth and the Challenge of Pedagogy.” Harvard
Educational Review, 64 (3): 278-308.
Giroux, Henry A. 1995. “When You Wish Upon a Star It Makes a Difference Who You Are: Children’s
Culture and the Wonderful World of Disney.” International Journal of Educational Reform, 4 (1):
79-83.
Giroux, Henry A. 2001. “Breaking into the Movies: Pedagogy and the Politics of Film. JAC: A Journal of
Composition Theory, 21 (3): 583-98.
Golan, Guy and Anita Day. 2008. “The First-Person Effect and Its Behavioral Consequences: A New Trend
in the Twenty-Five Year History of Third-Person Effect Research” Mass Communication and
Society, 11: 539–556.
Gray, J. B. 2005. “Sugar and Spice and Science: Encouraging Girls through Media Mentoring.” Current
Issues in Education, 8 (18): http://cie.ed.asu.edu/volume8/number18/ (accessed January 30, 2014).
Hall, Stuart. 1980. “Encoding/Decoding.” In Media Studies, ed. Stuart Hall, Dorothy Hobson, Andrew Lowe
and Paul Willis, 128-137. London: Hutchinson.
Hobbs, Renee and A. Jensen. 2009. “The Past, Present and Future of Media Literacy Education.” Journal of
Media Literacy Education, 1: 1-11.
Hobbs, Renee, and Richard Frost. 2003. “Measuring the Acquisition of Media-Literacy Skills.” Reading
Research Quarterly, 38 (3): 330-355.
Jagacinski, C. 2013. “Women Engineering Students: Competence Perceptions and Achievement Goals in the
Freshman Engineering Course.” Sex Roles, 69 (11/12): 644-657.
Jenkins, Henry, 1992. Textual Poachers: Television Fans and Participatory Culture. New York: Routledge.
Jenkins, Henry, Sam Ford, and Joshua Green. 2013. Spreadable Media: Creating Value and Meaning in a
Networked Culture. New York: New York University Press.
Kellner, Douglas. and Kim, Gooyang. 2010. “Youtube, Critical Pedagogy and Media Activism.” Review of Education,
33
L. Puchner, L. Markowitz & M. Hedley / Journal of Media Literacy Education 7(2), 23 - 34
34