Socioanalytic Theory: Basic Concepts, Supporting Evidences, and Practical Implications

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 41

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/303913679

Socioanalytic Theory: Basic concepts, supporting evidences, and practical


implications.

Chapter · June 2016

CITATIONS READS

30 3,463

2 authors, including:

Gerhard Blickle
University of Bonn
268 PUBLICATIONS   4,144 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Socioanalytic Theory of Personality at work View project

Successful psychopathy at work View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Gerhard Blickle on 12 June 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


1

This work is scheduled to appear in 2017 in

The SAGE Handbook of Personality and Individual Differences


This manuscript may not exactly replicate the final version published in the handbook
Cite the following reference:

Hogan, R. & Blickle, G. (in press). Socioanalytic Theory: Basic concepts, supporting eviden-
ces, and practical implications. In V. Zeigler-Hill, & T. K. Shackelford (Eds.), The SAGE
Handbook of Personality and Individual Differences. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
2

Socioanalytic Theory: Basic Concepts, Supporting Evidences, and Practical Implications

Robert Hogan & Gerhard Blickle

Hogan Assessment Systems, Tulsa, USA, University of Bonn, Germany

To be published in

The SAGE Handbook of Personality and Individual Differences

Edited by

Virgil Zeigler-Hill & Todd K. Shackelford

SAGE Publications Inc., 2455 Teller Road, Thousand Oaks, CA 91320

Abstract

Socioanalytic theory of personality provides a perspective on human nature based on insights

from: Charles Darwin about human evolution; Sigmund Freud about unconscious motivation;

and George Herbert Mead about the dynamics of social interaction. This chapter presents the

basic assumptions of socioanalytic personality theory, reviews supporting empirical evidence

and practical implications of the theory in the fields of leadership and faking in personnel

selection. Finally, socioanalytic theory is positioned in the context of trait and clinical theories

of personality. Socioanalytic theory differs from the other two theories of personality

primarily by rejecting introspection as a valid source of data. In addition, trait theory has a

“pure science” agenda with minimal concern for applications, whereas clinical theories and

socioanalytic theory have an applied agenda—helping people improve their lives.


3

Socioanalytic theory provides a perspective on human nature based on insights from:

(1) Charles Darwin (1871) about human evolution; (2) Sigmund Freud (1913) about

unconscious motivation; and (3) George Herbert Mead (1934) about the dynamics of social

interaction. All three writers assume that, because we evolved as group living animals, we

have deep, organic needs for the universal features of human culture—collective rituals,

family relations, authority structures, morality, etc.. When evaluating theories of personality

(or anything else), the first question to ask is: “What question is the theory trying to answer?”

The pioneers of personality psychology (e.g., Freud, Jung, Adler, Horney, Maslow), for

example, wanted to explain the causes of psychopathology. Trait theory wants to identify the

structure of the trait descriptive universe and trace its neurological foundations. Socioanalytic

theory wants to predict and explain individual differences in peoples’ ability to: (1) acquire

social acceptance and support; (2) attain power and the control of resources; and (3) find

purpose and meaning in life (the theory applies to Maoris, chimpanzees, musicians,

politicians, scientists, drug dealers, etc.). The first part of this chapter presents the basic

assumptions of socioanalytic theory; the second part reviews supporting empirical evidence

and certain practical implications of the theory.

Basic assumptions

Two adaptations gave early humans an advantage over chimpanzees, baboons, and

other competitors: an upright stance and tool use. The upright stance freed hands to grasp

tools. Superior tools ─ clubs, spears, and sharp stones ─ gave early humans an advantage in

hunting and fighting with other primates. Better weapons continue to give human groups an

advantage when competing with other groups. The arms race associated with tool creation ─

and culture in general ─ stimulated the development of larger brains, which then facilitated

the development of more sophisticated tools and weapons.

Eibl-Eibesfeldt (1989) notes that "The basic principles of human society are fully

developed in hunter-gatherer peoples" (p. 615). Hunter-gatherers live in groups of 20 to 50


4

people, groups that contain grandparents, parents, and children. They live in fixed territories

which they must defend against other groups. They maintain morale and solidarity by gift

giving and other forms of exchange, and by ritualized social interaction ─ which includes

festivals and religious ceremonies. Some groups are egalitarian, some are authoritarian, but all

have status hierarchies. Most believe that they are a special people and distrust foreigners.

Finally, disputes inside the groups and warfare between the groups are constant. Groups with

superior technology and social organization overwhelm, enslave, or destroy groups with

inferior technology and social organization. Technology and social organization are the keys

to group survival, which makes it important for the younger generation to learn the culture of

its group.

These themes reflect about 1,000,000 years of human experience and are the

unconscious background for the development of individual personality. People lived in hunter

gatherer groups until the invention of agriculture about 13,000 years ago; agriculture allowed

much larger communities to develop. Modern industrial society is about 150 years old and has

led to huge urban centers. Life in large cities is easier in some ways than life in a hunter-

gatherer group ─ food, water, and electricity are generally available ─ but more difficult in

other ways ─ we no longer know or trust our neighbors. We are adapted to living in

conditions that no longer exist, and that explains much of the malaise of modern urban living

─ which Durkheim (1897) referred to as anomie.

Basic motives

Sociology, anthropology, and primate research contain three important generalizations

about human nature (Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1989; Fardon, et al., 2012; Mead, 1934). The first is that

people always live in groups; this suggests that they are inherently social, that at a deep and

unconscious level, people need companionship and social acceptance (cf. Baumeister &

Leary, 1995). These tendencies reflect the fact that group living has crucial survival

implications ─ solitary primates and humans do not live very long. The second generalization
5

is that every human (and primate) group has a status hierarchy; this suggests that, at a deep

and unconscious level, people (and primates) need status—because status permits better

choices in mates, food, and other commodities that promote fitness. Finally, anthropology

tells us that religion is an ancient human practice and a cultural universal. This suggests that

people need predictability—to understand how the world works, and their place in it. The

need for predictability leads to religion, culture, and technology; the need for predictability

has obvious evolutionary significance.

People have physiological needs (food, air, water, and sleep), but they are not

distinctively human. The three motivational themes described above answer the question,

“What do people really want,” and what people really want reflects what early humans

needed for reproductive success. However, the fact that people are motivated by biologically

based impulses does not mean that consciousness is irrelevant. Biology sets life’s problems,

but consciousness allows us to solve the problems. A model of motivation based on

evolutionary theory answers the question of how people are all alike. But we must also

account for the individual differences among people. People differ most importantly in terms

of the strategies they have developed to deal with the problems of getting along, getting

ahead, and finding meaning, and some strategies are more effective than others.

As we noted, at a deep and often unconscious level, people are motivated by needs for:

(1) attention, approval, and acceptance; (2) status, power, and the control of resources; and (3)

predictability and order in their everyday lives. We refer to these themes as “getting along,”

“getting ahead,” and “finding meaning.” These are powerful motives ─ people will kill to

gain acceptance or status, they will kill to avoid losing them, and they will sometimes kill

themselves when they think they have lost a sense of meaning and purpose. These needs

represent the universal themes in human affairs ─ e.g., “It’s still the same old story, the fight

for love and glory….as time goes by.” (Hupfeld, 1931)


6

The first two motives are familiar themes. In social theory, David Bakan (1966)

argued that participation in larger social units (communion/getting along) and efforts at self-

promotion (agency/getting ahead) are universal themes in human affairs (cf. Abele &

Wojciszke, 2014). In evolutionary psychology, Buss (2015) observed that successful

reproduction depends on forming alliances (getting along) and negotiating status hierarchies

(getting ahead). In anthropology, Redfield (1960) noted that the survival of any social group

depends on its members living together and getting a living. In sociology, Parsons and Bales

(1955) demonstrate that every human group depends on successfully completing two

activities: socio-emotional tasks (getting along) and tasks related to group survival (getting

ahead).

In academic psychology, McAdams (2001) showed that the stories that people tell to

support their identities can be organized around two themes that he calls intimacy and power.

Similarly, social exchange theory (Foa & Foa, 1980) argued that social interaction is

organized around the exchange of love and status, and Wiggins and Trapnell (1996) proposed

that the exchange of love and status is the principal dynamic in social life.

There are two major consequences associated with getting along and getting ahead.

First, these issues make social interaction unavoidable—because we receive attention and

status during interaction. Second, if we are successful, others will resent us even as they

congratulate us; conversely, to be accepted by others, we must conform to their expectations

─ which makes high achievement difficult. Thus, there is an inherent tension beneath the

surface of social life as people try to advance themselves without alienating others.

The need for predictability and order also has a long history in psychology. Pavlov

(1927) showed that dogs become neurotic when they are required to perform in unpredictable

circumstances. Hebb and Thompson (1954) pointed out that chimpanzees have a strong need

for predictability, are highly sensitive to small changes in the behavior of others, and become

upset when others deviate even slightly from their normal behavior. George Kelly’s personal
7

construct theory (1955) begins by assuming that people need to be able to predict how others

will respond to them. Finally, Durkheim’s (1897) key concept, anomie, refers to peoples’

psychological experience when their lives no longer make sense in traditional terms ─ i.e., the

concept of anomie assumes that people need the predictability and meaning provided by

established culture and tradition. There is also a tragic tension associated with the need for

meaning; people want it and there is none.

Over time, people with more status, social support, and control in their lives had a

reproductive advantage ─ they had better choices of marital partners, food supplies, shelter,

and other resources (Daly & Wilson, 1988). With some qualifications, this is still true today.

To summarize this discussion, at a deep and often unconscious level, people need structure

and predictability, attention and approval, and status and control of resources. People meet

these needs during social interaction ─ the unique features of human evolution compel people

to interact. But because human nature is rooted in biology, individual differences are

inevitable. Thus, some people need more social acceptance than others (e.g., actors versus

hermits), some people need more status than others (e.g., politicians versus social workers),

and some people need more predictability and meaning than others (e.g., the religious

orthodox versus nonobservant). Finally, some people are more successful than others in

attaining these goals, and these individual differences are what socioanalytic theory tries to

explain.

Identity

Following G. H. Mead (1934), socioanalytic theory postulates that the self (how we

view ourselves) develops based on feedback from others during social interaction. Once the

self is formed, it guides our actions vis-à-vis others, and it is further shaped by subsequent

feedback from others. The self, in this sense, is the same as our identity.

We think of human development in terms of four broad stages which are cross-cultural

universals—they occur in every culture. The first stage concerns the individual forming an
8

“attachment bond” (Bowlby, 1969). Normal parents are programed to want to care for their

babies, and the babies need caregivers. This typically leads to the development of an

"attachment bond" (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978), a strong emotional link

between an infant and its caretakers. The quality of the mother-child relationship is the basis

for an infant’s sense of security and well-being, and the platform on which subsequent social

relationships are built.

Children expect their parents to coddle them, and the experience of being coddled

creates childish egocentrism. In the second phase of development, children must learn to

interact with their peers. In order for children to interact successfully, they must outgrow their

infantile egocentrism. The opposite of egocentrism is social sensitivity, and this develops, as

George Herbert Mead (1934) pointed out, through playing games in childhood, "By playing

together in the children’s group the members learn what aggravates others and which rules

they must obey” (Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1989, p. 601). Children learn how to interact with their

parents and other adults by following adult rules; in contrast, they learn how to interact with

other children by anticipating their expectations, and they learn to do this by playing games.

The third phase of development (adolescence) concerns developing a sense of identity

(Erikson, 1968). Our identities are idealized views of ourselves that answer the question of

who and what we are, what we stand for, and how we should be treated. During adolescence

young people try out a variety of identities ─ musician, cheerleader, mystic. Feedback from

the peer group ─ e.g., "Who do you think you are?" ─ gradually determines the final choice.

Our identity guides our behavior during social interaction by controlling the roles we are

willing to play and how we play them. For example, the people in a college classroom are all

in the role of student, but they play the role differently depending on their identity. The two

major tasks of adolescence, therefore, concern choosing an identity and developing the skills

needed to survive as an adult.


9

Adulthood is the final developmental phase. Adult social life consists of an extended

series of interactions, usually with the same people over and over, in the process of trying to

have a career. As noted above, the goal of adult interaction is the pursuit of status and social

acceptance (i.e., a career), and there are two major strategies for interaction: competing and

trying to outperform others, and affiliating and trying to build coalitions. Acquiring status

depends on the support of others, which involves building relationships. But status also

depends on aggression, on the desire to outcompete others and to defend one’s position

against other challengers.

As Wiggins (1996) noted, social interaction is an exchange process: after each

interaction ends, there is an accounting process which results in each person gaining or losing

a little bit of status and a little bit of acceptance. Interacting with others is a skilled

performance; athletes, actors, and politicians are only as good as their last performance, and

the same is true for the rest of us. Because interaction is so central to adult social life, it is

useful to ask what is needed in order to interact? The answer is, there are two requirements:

(a) a pretext for the interaction ─ e.g., let’s get together and watch football ─ and (b) roles for

the participants to play ─ e.g., you be the host and I will bring some beer.

Note that engaging in play provides children with attention in a structured and

predictable manner. Note also that adults do not simply get together and bask in one another’s

company, they get together and pretend to "do something" ─ have a cup of coffee, drink a

beer, talk something over ─ and these interactions are essentially identical to the way 3-year

olds play. To interact with another person, we need a pretext and a role to play. Outside of our

roles, we have nothing to say to one another. Persons with good social skill have a talent for

inventing pretexts for interaction and for negotiating who plays what role.

Where do identities come from? Generally speaking, other people teach us who we

are. This is true, but we also choose our identities from the menus that are available in our

cultures. The menu is usually found in the entertainment world—in movies, books, TV shows,
10

etc. But the larger point is that our identities—the persons we think are real—come from

menus provided by society. We may reject the identities that are available in our culture, but

we will have to find substitutes before we can join the game of life and begin interacting with

others. Our identities include our values, and our values are tied to our social class. For

example, wealthy people with working class values seem working class, whereas working

class people with good taste and civil manners seem “well bred.” Consequently, personality─

identity─ is linked to social class through our values (Allport, 1961).

Reputation

For socioanalytic theory, the core components of personality are identity and

reputation. Our identity guides our behavior during social interaction. Other people evaluate

our behavior during social interactions and their evaluations create our reputation. Identities

are personality from the perspective of the actor ─ our identities concern the person that we

think we are. Reputations, on the other hand, are personality from the perspective of the

observer ─ reputations concern the person whom others think we are. Successful people pay

close attention to their reputations.

Reputations are important for the study of personality for five reasons. First, they

develop quickly and are stable over time ─ different observers tend to agree substantially

about a person's reputation ─ which means that reputations can be studied objectively. In

contrast, identities are more fluid and much harder to study. Second, most people spend a

great deal of time and energy trying to establish and maintain their reputations. Third, because

the best predictor of future behavior is past behavior, and because reputations reflect a

person's past behavior, reputations are the best data source we have for predicting most

aspects of social performance ─ e.g., academic achievement, occupational success,

delinquency, and career choice. Fourth, we have a well-defined taxonomy of reputations: it is

the Five-Factor Model (FFM; Hogan, 1996). We think about and describe other people in

terms of five categories or dimensions ─ self-confidence, social presence, likeability,


11

conscientiousness, and curiosity/creativity. Finally, our reputations reflect the amount of

social acceptance and status we have in our communities. Successful people know how to

manage their reputations, and they manage them during social interaction all the time

(Goffman, 1959).

Allport (1961) defined personality psychology as the study of identity and dismissed

reputation as an epiphenomenon. It is worth noting, however, that after 100 years of research

on identity, there is no taxonomy, no measurement base, and few significant generalizations

to report. In contrast, researchers have been studying reputation for about 20 years. There is a

reliable taxonomy of reputation (the Five-Factor Model), a measurement base, and a

cornucopia of robust research findings. We know a lot about personality (defined as

reputation) and variety of outcomes, including marital satisfaction, occupational success,

substance abuse, academic performance, health status, etc.

Social skill

What is self-knowledge? Self-knowledge should be defined from two perspectives ─

the actor’s and the observer’s. Self-knowledge from the actor’s perspective concerns what we

think we know about ourselves; self-knowledge from the observer’s perspective concerns

what other people actually know about us. Self-knowledge from the actor’s perspective

concerns understanding our identity and becoming mindful of how we interact with others.

Self-knowledge from the observer’s perspective concerns understanding our reputation and

what we did to create it. Identity and reputation are related in interesting ways. For example,

the person we think we are may not be the person that others think we are; to the degree that

this is true, we will have trouble achieving our goals in life.

Identities are idealized views of ourselves, and we typically try to convince others that

these idealized views are true. Some people are better at this than others because their social

behavior is more convincing and effective. Hogan and Shelton (1998) argued that the ability

successfully to translate one’s identity into a desired reputation is moderated by social skill.
12

They defined social skill as competent impression management—the ability to control the

impressions that others form of us. The alignment between identity and reputation depends on

actor’s social skills, of which self-knowledge and empathy are important facets. The success

of impression management also depends on selecting audiences appropriately, being sensitive

to the emotional cues of others, using correct language-style, and controlling non-verbal cues

appropriately. Empirical research supports the idea that socially skilled individuals more

quickly identify and attend to emotional cues in others and are better able to choose

appropriate facial expressions, hand gestures, body postures, voice textures, and other

paralinguistic cues (Gangestad & Snyder, 2000; Momm, Blickle, & Liu, 2010).

The unconscious

Freud argued that we are typically unaware of the reasons for our actions, and his view

is supported by modern research in cognitive psychology. We believe there are at least four

sources of unconscious influence on our behavior.

The first set of unconscious influences comes from biology. As noted above, we need

attention and approval, we need status and control of resources, and we need order and

predictability. Although much of our everyday behavior concerns pursuing these needs, we

are rarely conscious of this fact. Loneliness usually indicates a need for interaction;

depression indicates lost status and control; and anxiety is caused by chaos and

unpredictability. These needs become conscious primarily when they are unfulfilled. We

would also include temperament and mood in this set of unconscious biological processes.

Our temperament produces our characteristic mood states which serve as a filter for our

attention and shape our perceptions in ways that are simultaneously profound, idiosyncratic,

and unconscious.

Our natural egocentrism creates a second set of unconscious influences. That is, most

of us tend to ignore what others expect or believe during interactions. For example, when we

ask managers to describe how their staff evaluates them, they are surprisingly inaccurate—
13

managers typically see themselves in much more positive terms than their staff describes

them. Similarly, couples in close relationships are unable accurately to describe how their

partners perceive them. Although we constantly interact with others, the data suggest that

these interactions are based on superficial mutual understanding. This raises an interesting

question about how social interaction is even possible.

One answer is that the rules governing interaction are prewired in our nervous

systems, so that our responses to others do not depend on understanding what they expect.

Human social interaction seems to resemble the mating dance of dragonflies, wherein

gestures, colors, smells, and posture trigger corresponding gestures, postures, and behavior.

Eibl-Eibesfeldt (1989) argues that the rules of social interaction are inherited, that there is "...a

universal grammar of social behavior according to which verbal and nonverbal interactions

are similarly structured (p. 499)." The result is that "many of the basic strategies of social

interaction...follow their course automatically according to phylogenetically evolved

programs” (p. 516). For example, people all over the world respond to the same nonverbal

gestures (e.g., sticking one's tongue out is a universal sign of impertinence) and recognize the

same emotions in others. Eibl-Eibesfeldt further suggests that "...we can speak of a universal

grammar of human social behavior….With the discovery of the universal interactional

strategies....We have recognized the existence of a universal…regulating system governing all

interactions” (1989, p. 517). Consequently, it seems that much of our behavior during social

interaction ─ posture, facial expressions, gaze, pace, etc. ─ runs off automatically and is

therefore unconscious. Politicians, actors, and other entertainers understand this and often go

through elaborate coaching in order to master and control these very subtle and otherwise

unconscious interpersonal behaviors.

The third set of unconscious influences on social behavior are the values, customs, and

norms of our culture that we assume are true and that we rarely question or challenge. These

include rules about what we should eat, how we should dress, how women are treated, how
14

members of minority groups should be treated, and how our life style is superior to that of

others. This third form of unconscious influence is much more powerful than we typically

realize.

Finally, our brains evolved so as to handle routine activities with automatic processes

and use conscious attention to solve novel problems (Johnson & Hogan, 2006). Routine

behaviors demand far less attention than new skills, so human brains are designed for a high

level of automatic activity, and these automatic mental routines give regularity and

consistency to human behavior. When people are learning new tasks (e.g., how to type or

drive a car) their first efforts are typically halting and clumsy. With practice their performance

becomes automatic, consistent, and largely unconscious. This is also true for the way we

interact with others, and these processes can lead to self-defeating behavior. Self-defeating

behaviors are usually adaptive when they first develop, but when circumstances change,

automatic and unconscious behaviors may produce short-term benefits but may create long-

term costs (i.e., they cloud judgement and hamper performance; Kaiser, LeBreton, & Hogan,

2015; Stanovich, West, & Toplak, 2013). Using feedback, people can become aware of what

triggers their automated behaviors (Kaiser & Kaplan, 2006).

We can summarize this discussion as follows. Although social behavior is largely

guided by forces outside our awareness, many of these nonconscious influences can become

conscious through social feedback, education, and self-reflection. To the degree that they

remain unconscious, we are liable to act in ways that are foolish, self-defeating, or even

immoral ─ e.g., engage in selfish, racist, and/or sexist behavior.

Caveat

One problem with the foregoing model of personality should be mentioned. Brewer

and Caporael (1991) observed that human evolutionary theory is often put to political rather

than scientific purposes. Specifically, evolutionary arguments have been used to support

racism, sexism, and even genocide. The pseudo-logical argument is often constructed in the
15

following way: Someone notes that there are consistent differences between two groups (e.g.,

men and women) and then argues that the characteristics that typify the group that is in power

are innately superior to the characteristics of the other group, precisely because one's group is

in power (Padilla, Hogan, & Kaiser, 2007). One is then allowed to stigmatize the other group

on the grounds of one's natural superiority. We, of course, deplore these sorts of arguments

because they are pseudo-scientific, circular, and hateful. To argue that what is should also be,

is the naturalistic fallacy (Moore, 1903).

Supporting empirical evidences

The following discussion presents evidence regarding the primacy of the motives of

getting along, getting ahead, and finding meaning. It also presents evidence regarding how

identity determines the division of labor within groups, and how social skills support the

expression of identity. Finally, we review research on the links between reputation and career

success.

Basic motives

Empirical research supports the importance of the three basic (and usually

unconscious) motives emphasized by socioanalytic theory. Within groups there are typically

individual differences in the desire to get along and get ahead. Because culture (the rules of

interaction) is constant within groups, the need for stability, predictability, and meaning

becomes more salient in comparisons between groups with different cultures.

Getting along and getting ahead. Digman (1997) analyzed personality trait ratings

from 14 studies ─ five based on children and adolescents, nine on adults. Seven of the studies

used observer ratings of personality traits and seven used self-ratings. Digman analyzed the

data using exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses. In all 14 studies, two higher-order

factors emerged. The alpha-factor contained the traits of agreeableness, conscientiousness,

and emotional stability; the beta-factor contained the traits of extraversion and intellect (viz.,

openness to experience). These two factors have been labeled social interest and superiority
16

striving by Adler (1939), communion and agency by Wiggins (1991), intimacy and power

motivation by McAdams (1992), and popularity and status striving by Hogan (1982). These

two meta-traits represent the motives of getting along and ahead. Recently, Ashton and Lee

have proposed adding a factor they call Honesty-Humility, defined by the terms sincere, fair,

and unassuming versus sly, greedy, and pretentious (Ashton & Lee, 2005). We think this

factor fits nicely with the getting along motivation factor.

Finding meaning. Socioanalytic theory assumes that stable, predictable, and

meaningful social interactions are essential for our psychological well-being. Chaotic social

interaction is deeply stressful, and if it continues long enough, anyone will break down

(Hogan, 1982). Basically, culture defines the rules which provide interaction with stability,

predictability, and meaning (Hogan & Bond, 2009). Jahoda (1981) first proposed that

employment in industrial societies is an important source of structure and meaning in daily

life (cf. Paul & Batinic, 2009) and therefore promotes psychological well-being. Barrick,

Mount, and Li (2013) also propose that striving for meaning is an important (unconscious)

motive at work.

In a cross-national study of over 80 nations, Oishi and Diener (2013) report that

economic scarcity is associated with lower social status, fewer educational chances, poorer

health care, and lower levels of life satisfaction, but also with more religiosity and perceived

meaning in life. This suggests that the stress of poverty enhances the salience of peoples’

needs for meaning and predictability, a need that is filled by strong religious convictions.

Conversely, when people live in societies that provide employment, education, and health

care, the need for stability, predictability, and meaning is less salient.

Identity

Socioanalytic theory assumes that people must interact, that they use their identities to

structure interaction (identity is the generic role people carry with them across situations), and
17

that social skill enhances the ability to express one’s identity in different situational contexts.

We now review some evidence to support this claim.

Identity and the division of labor within groups. Holland (1996) developed a

widely accepted taxonomy of vocational types; every job in the Dictionary of Occupational

Titles can be classified using the Holland model, and it is an indispensable starting point for

any discussion of careers (see Figure 1). There are six occupational types: Realistic,

Investigative, Artistic, Social, Enterprising, and Conventional. Thus the Holland model is

often referred to as “the RIASEC model”. Realistic types (engineers) prefer concrete practical

activities: building, maintaining, and repairing equipment. Investigative types (scientists)

prefer research and problem solving activities. Artistic types (artists) prefer creative work in

art, music, and design. Social types (teachers) prefer activities designed to help others.

Enterprising types (entrepreneurs) prefer money-making and status enhancing activities.

Finally, Conventional types (accountants) prefer activities associated with organizing and

regulating financial and other details.

----------------------------------

Insert Figure 1 about here

-----------------------------------

These occupational (personality) types can be organized using the two dimensions of

sociability and conformity (Figure 1; Hogan, 1982). Conventional types score high on

conformity and artistic types score low on conformity. Enterprising types score high on

conformity and sociability. Realistic types score high on conformity and low on sociability.

Social types score high on sociability and low on conformity factor. And Investigative types

score low on conformity and sociability. According to Holland (1996), each occupational type

prefers to work in a specific environment containing specific demands and rewards: Artistic

types need freedom, Enterprising types need financial opportunities, etc.


18

Socioanalytic theory suggests that the Holland types represent six key roles in

successful human groups (Hogan, 1982). Enterprising types move into leader roles.

Conventional types implement group decisions. Realistic types build and repair tools.

Investigative types invent tools (and technology). Artistic types entertain people around the

camp fire (and challenge dysfunctional rules and obsolete traditions). And Social types

maintain group morale and cohesion.

Consistent with Holland’s model, meta-analyses find that the closer peoples’

(RIASEC) personality types fit their work environments, the greater their training success, job

satisfaction, job tenure, and job performance (Assouline & Meir, 1987; Van Iddekinge, Roth,

Putka, & Lanivich, 2011). Hogan and Blake (1999) explain these findings in terms of

“person-environment fit”, where the environment is defined by the personalities of the other

people in that environment. That is, people work more productively when they share the

values of their co-workers.

It is interesting to note that the personality dimension of neuroticism is not part of the

Holland model (which is designed to predict occupational success), but neuroticism is a

strong predictor of job performance (Hogan & Holland, 2003). Hogan (1982) notes that for

neurotics, “…anxiety, depression, and physical complaints are part of a general self-

presentational program where the message is, ‘I am sick, I need special attention and

consideration, and don’t expect too much of me under these circumstances.’” (p. 80). Meta-

analytic research strongly supports the view that neuroticism predicts reduced career success

(Lodi-Smith & Roberts, 2007; Ng, Eby, Sorensen, & Feldman, 2005).

Mainstream personality research ignores ambition—e.g., ambition is not part of the

FFM. In contrast, socioanalytic theory regards ambition as essential for career success (i.e.,

getting ahead) because ambition is about persistent and generalized striving for success,

attainment, and accomplishment. Consistent with this claim, Judge and Kammeyer-Mueller

(2012) show, in a predictive study over 30 years with 717 participants, that individual
19

differences in ambition predict educational attainment and prestige, occupational income and

prestige, and life satisfaction.

Identity and social skill. Hogan and Shelton (1998) suggested that social skill

translates interpersonal aspirations into action. More specifically, social skill translates

identity into reputation—social skill allows people to portray themselves positively during

social interaction. Social skill allows people to achieve their interpersonal goals just as hand-

eye coordination allows people to hit tennis balls accurately. Social skill is trainable and has

the following features (Hogan & Shelton, 1998): being sensitive and responsive to others’

needs and moods, being flexible and adaptable, being persuasive, being able to instill trust,

being consistent across social interactions, being accountable, and being able to listen to and

communicate with a wide range of audiences. Self-presentational behavior guided by good

social skill maximizes the approval and minimizes the disapproval of an actor’s social

performance.

Numerous empirical studies strongly support this proposition. High scores on a

measure of social skill combined with a measure of wanting to get along with peers and

supervisors predicted higher supervisors’ ratings of an employee’s cooperation, job

performance, and promotion potential (Blickle et al., 2011). High scores on these two

measures predicted higher income and marketability of new employees after two years

(Blickle, Momm, Liu, Witzki, & Steinmayr, 2011). High scores on a measure of social skill

and a measure of achievement striving are associated with supervisory ratings of leadership

emergence (Marinova, Moon, & Kamdar, 2013). Finally, in a sample of 510 school

headmasters, high scores on a measure of social skill and a measure of wanting to get ahead

predicted success in leadership as rated by three to four teachers (N = 1,881) who reported to

these headmasters (Ewen et al., 2014).

In conclusion, social skill enhances the ability of people to present themselves and

control their reputation during social interaction. People with social skill are better able to
20

restrain, calibrate, and adjust their behavior in different and changing social contexts. This

adaptability allows them to exercise interpersonal influence by gaining the trust and

confidence of those with whom they interact. After every interaction, the participants evaluate

the performance of one another. These evaluations ultimately turn into performance appraisals

(Hogan & Shelton, 1998). On what do these evaluations depend? They primarily reflect the

degree to which people are rewarding during social interactions. Being rewarding involves

helping others advance their agendas, being compliant and attentive, and fitting with the

culture of the group. Being rewarding has to do with making another person feel and look

good in his or her role.

Reputation

One important claim of socioanalytic theory is that measures of reputation should

predict performance better than so-called self-report measures of personality. “Reputation is a

perceptual identity formed from the collective perceptions of others, which is reflective of the

complex combination of salient personal characteristics and accomplishments, demonstrated

behavior, and intended images presented over some period of time as observed directly and/or

reported from secondary sources, which reduces ambiguity about expected future behavior”

(Zinko, Ferris, Blass, & Laird, 2007, p. 165). Consequences of a positive reputation include:

elbowroom ─ having more discretion to act; power ─ others will defer to one’s judgment;

improved performance ─ having more discretion and power to get things done; enhanced

performance assessments; and better compensation. Other useful consequences of having a

positive reputation include career success and enhanced subjective well-being (Zinko et al.,

2007).

Observers use trait terms to describe and evaluate other people, and these

descriptions/evaluations turn into peoples’ reputations—which are encoded in trait words.

Reputations, encoded in trait words, evaluate a person’s contribution to the success of the

groups to which the person belongs ─ tribe, family, combat-unit, or work team. Trait terms are
21

the units of reputation and the FFM (Wiggins, 1996) is a robust and widely accepted

taxonomy of reputation. Briefly the dimensions of the FFM are: (1) Emotional stability—low

volatility and performance under pressure; (2) Extraversion—sociability and assertiveness; (3)

Openness—curiosity and flexibility; (4) Agreeableness—warmth, tact, and charm; (5)

Conscientiousness—trustworthiness and integrity. Reputation can be assessed directly or

indirectly. Direct measures of reputation use observer ratings to assess personality, whereas

indirect measures of reputation use self-ratings of personality.

Direct personality measures of reputation and success. Empirical data strongly

support the view that observer ratings of personality (reputation) are significantly more valid

that self-reports of personality (identity) in predicting social behavior. Kluemper, McLarty,

and Bing (2015) report that observer ratings of the FFM personality dimensions add

significant validity to self-reports when predicting work place deviance. Kholin et al. (2016)

found that peer-ratings of students’ learning approach predicted academic performance in

MINT-subjects (Mathematics ─ Informatics ─ Natural Sciences ─ Technology) much better

than self-ratings of learning approach. Oh, Wang, and Mount (2011) used meta-analysis to

compare the operational validities of FFM traits based on self- and other-ratings. As predicted

by socioanalytic theory, they found that the validities of observer ratings are significantly

higher than those based on self-report ratings. In addition, observer ratings of FFM traits

yielded incremental validity over self-reports of the same dimensions in predicting

performance, but the reverse was not true.

Connelly and Ones (2010) also conducted a meta-analytic study of the links between

observer ratings of actors’ personalities and ratings for job performance. Their results strongly

support those reported by Oh et al. (2011). How well does reputation (observers’ ratings of

personality) predict job performance ─ compared to other assessment procedures? Schmidt

and Hunter (1998) report that General Mental Ability is the best single predictor of overall job

performance (ρ = .51; for a more accurate estimate, see Schmidt, Shaffer, & Oh, 2008) and
22

work sample tests (ρ = .54; for an alternative estimate, see Roth, Bobko, & McFarland, 2005).

Structured employment interviews (ρ = .51) are the best non-test predictors of job

performance. However, Connelly and Ones (2010) found that observer ratings of

conscientiousness are slightly more valid overall (true score correlations ρ = .55; for

operational validity estimates, see Oh et al., 2011). For comparison purposes, other popular

procedures yield the following validity coefficients: reference checks (ρ = .26); biographical

data measures (ρ = .35); assessment center performance (ρ = .37); unstructured employment

interview (ρ = .38); and integrity tests (ρ = .41). In sum, socioanalytic theory predicts

individual differences in peoples’ career success.

Successful people use superior social skills to manage their reputations. Empirical

research also supports this claim. For example, peer ratings (indices of reputation) of

academic aptitude significantly predict the academic performance of university students with

good social skills over more than one year (ß = .43) but not among students with low social

skills (ß = .02; Kholin et al., 2016). In many, if not most cases, career success depends on

peoples’ reputation and the manner in which they present themselves to others. People who

are successful in the game of life are mindful of these issues.

Indirect personality measures of reputation and success. Several personality

inventories based on the FFM are available to researchers. These inventories are typically

called self-report measures. Trait theorists assume that, when respondents complete these

inventories, they consult their memories and then report on their “true” thoughts, feelings, and

past behaviors (Allport, 1961). In contrast, socioanalytic theory assumes that, when

respondents complete these inventories, they respond in ways that tell others how they want to

be regarded. That is, respondents do not provide self-reports, they provide self-presentations.

We believe people use items on personality questionnaires to express idealized views of

themselves and hope others will believe accept these views. In addition, socioanalytic theory
23

argues that these self-presentations are not necessarily conscious because, over time, self-

presentation tends to become automatic role behavior.

Additionally, others find us likable or disagreeable depending on the degree to which

we support the identity they want to project. Thus, employees who comply with the requests

of their supervisors and respect their status will be seen as rewarding because they help their

supervisors look good. Supervisors rarely think about their subordinates in terms of the

subordinate’s goals, fears, and aspirations; rather, supervisors think about subordinates in

terms of how rewarding they are—defined in terms of protecting, supporting, and enhancing

supervisors’ identity. The same is true for peers.

Social skills are also needed in order to control others by counseling, persuading, and

suggesting rather than by ordering, criticizing, and coercing them (Blickle, Kane-Frieder, et

al., 2013). Although identity is rather stable, social skills are, in principle, trainable.

Moreover, good social skills can coexist with deeply flawed identities ─ where flawed is

defined in terms of insecurity, selfishness, strange and irrational goals, and a disposition

toward treachery and deceit (Leary, 1995).

As socioanalytic theory predicts, many studies show that social skill moderates the

predictive validity of standard personality measures, including the validity of measures of:

agreeableness (Blickle et al., 2008); conscientiousness (Witt & Ferris, 2003); extraversion

(Blickle, Wendel, & Ferris, 2010); openness to experience (Blickle et al., 2013); honesty-

humility (Diekmann, Blickle, Hafner, & Peters, 2015); trait sincerity (Meurs, Perrewé, &

Ferris, 2011); and proactive personality (Sun & van Emmerik, 2015). Socioanalytic theory

regards these personality dimensions as “bright side” characteristics (Hogan & Hogan, 2001)

because they appear during social interactions when people are self-consciously trying to get

along and get ahead.

Socioanalytic theory distinguishes between “bright side” and “dark side” behavior.

Bright side behavior is what we see when people are behaving themselves; the FFM is a
24

taxonomy of bright side behavior. In contrast with bright side characteristics, dark side

characteristics (e.g., narcissism, psychopathy) emerge when people let down their guard—

when they are angry, tired, or “just being themselves”, and usually when they are dealing with

subordinates or people with less power. We also assume (Hogan & Shelton, 1998) that good

social skills can compensate for dark side personality tendencies. Recent research supports

this assumption.

Narcissism is a dark side characteristic defined by a grandiose but fragile sense of self,

feelings of entitlement, preoccupation with success, and demands for admiration (e.g., Owens,

Wallace, & Feldman, 2015). It also includes being self-centered, self-absorbed, extremely

self-confident, and exploitive. O’Boyle, Forsyth, Banks, and McDaniel (2012) found in a

meta-analysis that narcissism predicts counterproductive work behavior (r = .35). However,

Owens et al. (2015) also report that, when narcissism is combined with humility (i.e., social

skill) ─ admitting when one makes mistakes, being aware of others’ strengths—such leaders

receive high ratings of effectiveness and followers show high levels of job engagement and

objective job performance.

Primary psychopathy (Lykken, 1995) is characterized by fearlessness, attention

seeking, manipulative tendencies, and an inability to learn from experience. Lilienfeld and

Widows (2005) refer to psychopathy as “fearless dominance”. Based on socioanalytic theory,

Schütte et al. (2015) suggested that: (1) people with high scores for both psychopathy and

social skill should succeed in organizations; whereas (2) people with high scores for

psychopathy and low scores for social skill should fail. Schütte et al. (2015) report that people

with high scores for psychopathy and low scores for social skill displayed high levels of

counterproductive behavior and low levels of productive performance at work. Conversely,

people with high scores for both psychopathy and social skill demonstrated low levels of

counterproductive behavior and high levels of productive performance.


25

In sum, people with social skill are flexible, able to establish rapport via an

unassuming and effective communication style, and able to behave in ways that are situation-

specific, appropriate, and influential. Thus, people with extensive dark side tendencies who

have good social skills are often able to control their negative behaviors and be successful.

However, these studies of narcissism, psychopathy, and social skill are cross-sectional.

We need longitudinal studies to determine whether persons with strong dark side tendencies

use social skill to deceive others while selfishly extracting resources from the group (Jones,

2014). Some parasitic animals use complex deception tactics based on slow resource

extraction, careful integration into the community, and avoidance of detection, and their

behavior has severe long-term negative consequences for the host community.

Practical implications of Socioanalytic theory

Socioanalytic theory has many practical implications; in the following we review two

that substantially impact our daily lives, namely leadership and faking in personnel selection.

Personality and Leadership

Sociologists, political scientists, and historians argue that leadership is the function of

existing organizational and historical circumstances. Socioanalytic theory, however, argues

that leadership is a function of personality and that some people have more talent for

leadership than others. Good leadership contributes to the long-term success of groups and

organizations (Kaiser, Hogan, Craig, 2008; Van Vugt, Hogan, Kaiser, 2008). Successful

leadership involves persuading group members to set aside, for a limited time, their selfish,

short-term interests and contribute to long-term group goals of their groups or organizations

(Padilla, Hogan, & Kaiser, 2007). Charles Darwin (1871) noted: “A tribe including many

members who … were always ready to aid one another, and to sacrifice themselves for the

common good, would be victorious over most other tribes, and this would be natural

selection” (p. 132).


26

To evaluate the links between personality and leadership, one needs scores for

individual leaders’ reputation, and quantitative indices of their performance in leadership

roles. Judge, Bono, Ilies, and Gerhardt (2002) aggregated the results of 222 correlations from

73 studies of personality and leadership performance. Their sample contained more than

25,000 managers from every level in 5,000 organizations across every industry sector. They

report that four of the five dimensions of the FFM significantly predict leadership

performance, with Adjustment/Emotional Stability as the best predictor (.33), and

Agreeableness/Interpersonal Sensitivity the weakest predictor (.07). In this study,

conscientiousness/prudence, extraversion, and openness were also significantly correlated

with leadership (.29, .27, and .21, respectively), and the multiple correlation between

personality and leadership was .53. For people who believe in data, this study definitively

settles the argument that personality predicts leadership performance across all organizational

levels and industry sectors, and does so more powerfully than any known alternative.

Faking

Faking on personality inventories involves intentionally responding to items in a way

that will enhance one’s scores (Hogan & Blickle, 2013). Many psychologists think personality

assessment lacks any inherent validity because, they argue, it is easy for people to fake their

scores. Because personality assessment is the indispensable research method for personality

psychology, the faking criticism represents an existential threat to the discipline.

Socioanalytic theory interprets item responses in terms of impression management: people use

the items on personality measures to tell others how they want to be regarded. This, then,

suggests an alternative way to understand faking in the assessment process: “Deception is a

conscious, deliberate deviation from typical forms of self-presentation, a deviation that

acquaintances would describe as uncharacteristic behavior. This view of deception contrasts

with the view that deception involves acting in a way that is inconsistent with a single ‘true

self’ hidden inside of us” (Johnson & Hogan, 2006, p. 211). “When individuals try to act in
27

deceptive ways in everyday life (e.g., introverts try to act like extraverts) their natural

tendencies ‘leak through’ and observers readily detect them … Only good actors can make

atypical performances seem convincing …” (Johnson & Hogan, 2006, pp. 210-211).

Consider the process of child rearing. Small children act in ways that reflect their real

desires and urges. Socialization primarily involves training children to hide their real desires

and to behave in ways that are consistent with the norms of adult behavior. For a traditional

view on faking, socialization involves training children to fake. For impression management

theory, socialization involves training children in the appropriate forms of self-expression.

Items on well-constructed personality measures sample ordinary socialized adult behavior.

Most adults know the rules of conduct and respond to the items in terms of social norms

rather than in terms of their real desires. Criminals and other rebels respond in ways that are

closer to their real desires—in ways that are consistent with their typical behavior.

The point is that it is nearly impossible to distinguish faking from socialized behavior.

Johnson and Hogan (2006) report on a study using six unlikely virtue scales. Each scale

corresponded to one of six personality scales. The following is an unlikely virtue item for the

Inquisitive scale: “In my own way, I am an intellectual giant”, and the following is for the

Adjustment scale: “I have no psychological problems whatsoever.” Students completed the

personality scale and the unlikely virtue scales. In addition, two people who knew each

student rated that student on the six personality dimensions. Most students endorsed unlikely

virtue items proportional to their scores on the personality scales. Thus, each unlikely virtue

scales was most highly correlated with its corresponding personality scale and with the peer

ratings for the same dimension. This implies that although the students sometimes

exaggerated by endorsing specific unlikely virtue items, their exaggerated self-presentations

were consistent with their rated reputations. Thus, endorsing unlikely virtue items provides

information that predicts job performance because the endorsements are consistent with the
28

respondents’ typical self-presentations; the exaggerations are deviations that acquaintances

still describe as characteristic of the person.

It is also possible to test the faking criticism directly. In the only proper study of

faking in a real world context, Hogan, Barrett, and Hogan (2007) tested several thousand

applicants for a government job using a well validated measure of the FFM, of which a

subsample of applicants were rejected. Six months later, 5,266 of the rejected applicants

reapplied for the job. Because they were denied employment based on the inventory, and

because they wanted the job, they were motivated to improve their scores by faking. The

results indicated that 5.2% changed their scores on the second trial, but that scores improved

for 2.6% of the sample, and scores declined for 2.6% of the sample. These results clearly

show that only a small minority (about 5% of the sample) tried to change their scores, and of

those who tried, as many lowered their scores as raised their scores. Faking (i.e., a deliberate

deviation from typical individual forms of self-presentation) on personality measures is

theoretically possible, but rarely happens in practice.

Summary and Conclusion

Today personality psychology consists of three major theoretical clusters, each with a

distinctive focus and intent. The first cluster, clinical theories of personality (e.g.,

Psychoanalysis), use introspection (statements about identity) to identify the sources of

individual neurosis and perhaps overcome its affects. The second cluster, trait theory (Allport,

1961), uses introspection (self-report data) to identify the structure of self-report data and

trace its neurological underpinnings. The third, socioanalytic theory uses reputation to predict

important life outcomes, usually in the form of career success (or failure). Trait theory has a

“pure science” agenda with minimal concern for applications, whereas clinical theories and

socioanalytic theory have an applied agenda—helping people improve their lives.

Socioanalytic theory differs from the other two theories of personality primarily by

rejecting introspection as a valid source of data. We reject introspection for three reasons.
29

First, the capacity for (and interest in) introspection is normally distributed. A portion of the

population (usually people with neurotic tendencies) introspect constantly with clinical

psychologists possibly being part of this group. Many people engage in introspection from

time to time. But a significant portion of the population is incapable of introspection—they

never do it and they are unable to do it. Two examples of unusually successful people who

were famously incapable of introspection are Ronald Reagan, former President of the United

States (Cannon, 2000), and Margaret Thatcher, former Prime Minister of England (Campbell,

2011). The fact that neurotics enjoy introspection whereas highly successful politicians are

unable to introspect raises the question of why introspection matters.

The second reason for rejecting introspection concerns the universality of self-

deception. As Freud noted, people lie to themselves about their true motives and agendas and

then believe their lies. When they introspect, they dredge up invented memories and stories

which they then relay to us. The third reason for rejecting introspection concerns the question

of how hard it is for us, as listeners, to verify the introspective claims of other people. We

focus on reputation (observers’ ratings) as our data source.

Socioanalytic theory differs from trait theory (Allport, 1961) in an important way.

Trait theory uses trait words (honest, brave, creative, hostile) to describe peoples’ behavior,

and then uses trait words to explain peoples’ behavior: e.g., Mike Tyson (an American former

professional boxer) is aggressive (description) because he has a trait for aggression

(explanation). This is a tautology, a logical fallacy at the core of trait theory. In our view, we

do not have traits, we have agendas, goals, intentions, and our behavior reflects these agendas.

Other people watch us and then assign trait labels to our behavior so that they can predict our

future behavior. Traits exist in the minds of observers and in the observed behavior of actors.

Traits are an inference that other people make about our behavior, and they are powerful

sources of data to predict our future behavior. But it is a fundamental logical error to use traits

to predict and then explain behavior.


30

Finally, socioanalytic theory agrees with Freudian psychoanalysis in four ways. First,

we believe that, to understand human nature (personality), we need to study human origins

because the evolutionary history of our species holds the keys to understanding modern

behavior. Second, we believe that most social behavior is unconsciously motivated, that

people typically do not know why they do what they do when they do it, but they are good at

inventing explanations which they also believe. Third, we believe that development matters

such that experiences early in life have a greater impact on personality than experiences later

in life. And finally, Freud defined maturity as the capacity to love and to work. Similarly, we

think maturity is the capacity to build and maintain healthy relationships and to have a

successful career.
31

References

Abele, A., & Wojciszke, B. (2014). Communal and agentic content. A dual perspective

model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 50, 198-255.

Adler, A. (1939). Social Interest. New York: Plenum.

Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment.

Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Allport, G. W. (1961). Pattern and growth in personality. New York: Holt, Rhinehart, &

Winston.

Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2005). Honesty-humility, the Big Five, and the Five-Factor model.

Journal of Personality, 73, 1321-1353.

Assouline, M., & Meir, E.I. (1987). Meta-analysis of the relationship between congruence and

well-being measures. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 31, 319-332.

Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human experience. Boston, MA: Beacon.

Barrick, M. R., Mount, M. K., & Li, N. (2013). The theory of purposeful work behavior: The

role of personality, higher-order goals, and job characteristics. Academy of

Management Review, 38, 132-153.

Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M.R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal

attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497-

529.

Blickle, G., Fröhlich, J. Ehlert, S., Pirner, K, Dietl, E., Hanes T. J., & Ferris, G. R. (2011).

Socioanalytic theory and work behavior: Roles of work values and political skill in job

performance and promotability assessment. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 78, 136-

148.

Blickle, G., Kane-Frieder, R. E., Oerder, K., Wihler, A., von Below, A., Schütte, N.,

Matanovic, A., Mudlagk, D., Kokudeva, T. & Ferris, G. R. (2013). Leader behaviors
32

as mediators of the leader characteristics – follower satisfaction relationship. Group &

Organization Management, 38, 601-628.

Blickle, G., Meurs, J. A., Wihler, A., Ewen, C., Plies, A. & Günther, S. (2013). The

interactive effects of conscientiousness, openness to experience, and political skill on

job performance in complex jobs: The importance of context. Journal of

Organizational Behavior, 34, 1145-1164.

Blickle, G., Meurs, J. A., Zettler, I, Solga, J., Noethen, D., Kramer, J, & Ferris, G. R. (2008).

Personality, political skill, and job performance. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 72,

377-387.

Blickle, G., Momm, T., Liu, Y., Witzki, A. & Steinmayr, R. (2011). Construct validation of

the Test of Emotional Intelligence (TEMINT): A two-study investigation. European

Journal of Psychological Assessment, 27, 282-289.

Blickle, G., Wendel, S. & Ferris, G. R. (2010). Political skill as moderator of personality - job

performance relationships in socioanalytic theory: Test of the getting ahead motive in

automobile sales. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 76, 326-335.

Bowlby, J. (1969). Atttachment and loss, Vol. I. New York: Basic Books.

Bowlby, J. (1971). Attachment and loss, Vol. II. New York: Basic Books.

Brewer, M. B., & Caporeal, L. R. (1990). Selfish genes versus selfish people: Sociobiology as

origin myth. Motivation and Emotion, 14, 237-243.

Buss, D. M. (2015). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind (5th edition).

Boston: Pearson.

Campbell, J. (2011). Margaret Thatcher. NY: Random House.

Cannon, L. (2000). President Ronald Reagan. NY: PublicAffairs.

Connelly, B. S., & Ones, D. S. (2010). An other perspective on personality: Meta-analytic

integration of observers’ accuracy and predictive validity. Psychological Bulletin, 136,

1092-1122.
33

Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1988). Homicide. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

Darwin, C. R. (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. London: John

Murray.

Diekmann, C., Blickle, G., Hafner, K. & Peters, L. (2015). Trick or trait? The combined

effects of employee impression management modesty and trait modesty on supervisor

evaluations. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 89, 120-129.

Digman, J. M. (1997). Higher-order factors of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 73, 1246-1256.

Durkheim, E. (1897). Le suicide. Paris: F. Alcan.

Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1989). Human ethology. Chicago: Aldine.

Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity, youth, and crisis. New York: Norton.

Ewen, C., Wihler, A., Frieder, R. E., Blickle, G., Hogan, R. & Ferris, G. R. (2014). Leader

advancement motive, political skill, leader behavior, and effectiveness: A moderated

mediation extension of Socioanalytic Theory. Human Performance, 27, 373-392.

Fardon, R., et al. (Eds.). (2012). The SAGE handbook of social anthropology (2 Vols.).

London: Sage Publications.

Foa, E. B., & Foa, U. G. (1980). Resource theory of social exchange. In J. W. Thibaut, J. T.

Spence, & R. C. Carson (Eds.), Contemporary topics in social psychology.

Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.

Freud, S. (1913). Totem und Tabu. Wien: Heller.

Gangestad, S. W., & Snyder, M. (2000). Self-monitoring: Appraisal and reappraisal.

Psychological Bulletin, 126, 530-555.

Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. New York: Anchor.

Hebb, D. O., & Thompson, W. R. (1954). The social significance of animal studies. In D. T.

Gilbert, & G. Lindzay (Eds.), Handbook of Social Psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 532-561).

Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.


34

Hogan, J., Barrett, P., & Hogan, R. (2007). Personality measurement, faking, and employment

selection. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 1270-1285

Hogan, J., & Holland, B. (2003). Using theory to evaluate personality and job-performance

relations: A socioanalytic perspective. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 100-112.

Hogan, R. (1982). A socioanalytic theory of personality. In M. M. Page (Ed.), Nebraska

Symposium on Motivation (pp. 55-89). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.

Hogan, R. (1996). A socioanalytic interpretation of the Five-Factor Model. In J. Wiggins

(Ed.), The Five-factor model of personality (pp. 163-179). New York: Guilford.

Hogan, R., & Blake, R. (1999). John Holland’s vocational typology and personality theory.

Journal of Vocational Behavior, 55, 41-56.

Hogan, R. & Blickle, G. (2013). Socioanalytic theory. In N. D. Christiansen & R. P. Tett

(Eds.), Handbook of personality at work (pp. 53-70). New York: Routledge.

Hogan, R., & Bond, M. H. (2009). Culture and personality. In P. J. Corr, & G. Matthews

(Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of personality psychology (pp. 577-588).

Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Hogan, R., & Hogan, J. (2001). Assessing leadership: A view from the dark side.

International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 9, 40-51.

Hogan, R., & Shelton. D. (1998). A socioanalytic perspective on job performance. Human

Performance, 11, 129-144.

Holland, J. L. (1996). Exploring careers with a typology. American Psychologist, 51, 397-

406.

Hupfeld, H. (1931). Song: As time goes by. Lyrics and music.

https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Herman_Hupfeld (retrieved June-4, 2016).

Jahoda, M. (1981). Work, employment, and unemployment. Values, theories, and approaches

in social research. American Psychologist, 36, 184-191.


35

Johnson, J. A., & Hogan, R. (2006). A socioanalytic view of faking. In R. Griffith & H. M.

Peterson (Eds.), A closer examination of applicant faking (pp. 207-229). Greenwich,

CT: Information Age.

Jones, D. N. (2014). Predatory personalities as behavior mimics and parasites: Mimicry-

deception theory. Psychological Science, 9, 445-451.

Judge, T.A., Bono, J.E., Ilies, R., & Gerhardt, M. (2002). Personality and leadership. Journal

of Applied Psychology, 87, 765-780.

Judge, T. A., & Kammeyer-Mueller, J. D. (2012). On the value of aiming high: The causes

and consequences of ambition. Journal of Applied Psychology, 97, 758-775.

Kaiser, R. B., Hogan, R., & Craig, S. B. (2008). Leadership and the fate of organizations

American Psychologist, 63, 96-110.

Kaiser, R. B., & Kaplan, R. B. (2006). The deeper work of executive development:

Outgrowing sensitivities. Academy of Management: Learning & Education, 5, 463-

483.

Kaiser, R. B., LeBreton, J. M., & Hogan, J. (2015). The dark side of personality and extreme

leader behavior. Applied Psychology: In International Review, 64, 55-92.

Kelly, G. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs. London: Routledge.

Kholin, M., Meurs, J., Blickle, G., Wihler, A., Ewen, C. & Momm, T. (2016). Refining the

openness – performance relationship: Construct specificity, contextualization, social

skill, and the combination of trait self- and other-ratings. Journal of Personality

Assessment, 98, 277-288.

Kluemper, D. H., McLarty, B. D., & Bing, M. N. (2015). Acquaintance ratings of the Big

Five personality traits: Incremental validity beyond and interactive effects with self-

reports in the prediction of workplace deviance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100,

237-248.
36

Leary, M. R. (1995). Self-Presentation: Impression management and interpersonal behavior.

Boulder, CO: Westview.

Lilienfeld, S. O., & Widows, M. R. (2005). Psychological assessment inventory-revised (PPI-

R). Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Lodi-Smith, J., & Roberts, B. W. (2007). Social investment and personality: A meta-analysis

of the relationship of personality traits to investment in work, family, religion, and

volunteerism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11, 68-86.

Lykken, D. T. (1995). The antisocial personalities. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Marinova, S. V., Moon, H., & Kamdar, D. (2013). Getting ahead or getting along? The two-

facet conceptualization of conscientiousness and leadership emergence. Organization

Science, 24, 1257-1276.

McAdam, D. P. (1992). The five-factor model in personality. Journal of Personality, 60, 329-

361.

McAdams, D. P. (2001). The psychology of life stories. Review of General Psychology, 5,

100-122.

Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, & society. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Meurs, J. A., Perrewé, P. L., & Ferris, G. R. (2011). Political skill as moderator of the trait

sincerity – task performance relationship: A socioanalytic, narrow trait perspective.

Human Performance, 24, 119-134.

Momm, T., Blickle, G., & Liu, Y. (2010). Political skill and emotional cue learning.

Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 396-401.

Moore, G. E. (1903). Principia Ethica. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Ng, T. W. H., Eby, L. T., Sorensen, K. L., & Feldman, D.C. (2005). Predictors of objective

and subjective career success: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 58, 367-408.

O'Boyle Jr, E. H., Forsyth, D. R., Banks, G. C., & McDaniel, M. A. (2012). A meta-analysis

of the dark triad and work behavior: A social exchange perspective. Journal of
37

Applied Psychology, 97, 557-579.

Oh, I.-S., Wang, G., & Mount, M. K. (2011). Validity of observer ratings of the Five-Factor

Model of personality traits: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology. 96, 762-

773.

Oishi, S., & Diener, E. (2014). Residents of poor nations have more meaning in life than

residents of wealthy nations. Psychological Science, 25, 422-430.

Owens, B. P., Wallace, A. S., & Waldman, D. A. (2015). Leader narcissism and follower

outcomes: The counterbalancing effect of leader humility. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 100, 1203-1213.

Padilla, A., Hogan, R., & Kaiser, R. B. (2007). The toxic triangle: Destructive leaders,

susceptible followers, and conductive environments. The Leadership Quarterly, 18,

176-194.

Parsons, T., & Bales, R. F. (1955). Family, socialization and interaction process. Glencoe,

IL: Free Press.

Paul, K. I., & Batinic, B. (2009). The need for work: Jahoda’s latent functions of employment

in a representative sample of the German population. Journal of Organizational

Behavior, 31, 45-64.

Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned reflexes. London: Oxford University Press.

Redfield, R. (1960). How society operates. In H. L. Shapiro (Ed.), Man, culture, and society

(pp. 345-368). New York: Oxford University Press.

Roth, P. L., Bobko, P., & McFarland, L. A. (2005). A meta-analysis of work sample test

validity: Updating and integrating some classic literature. Personnel Psychology, 58,

1009-1037.

Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1998). The validity and utility of selection methods in

personnel psychology: Practical and theoretical implications of 85 years of research

findings. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 437-454.


38

Schmidt, F. L., Shaffer, J. A., & Oh, I.-S. (2008). Increased accuracy for range restriction

corrections: Implications for the role of personality and general mental ability in job

and training performance. Personnel Psychology, 61, 827-868.

Schütte, N., Blickle, G., Frieder, R., Wihler, A., Schnitzler, F., Heupel, J. & Zettler, I. (2015).

The role of interpersonal influence in counterbalancing psychopathic personality trait

facets at work. Journal of Management. DOI: 10.1177/014920635607967.

Stanovich, K. E., West, R. F., & Toplak, M. E. (2013). Myside bias, rational thinking, and

intelligence. Psychological Science, 22, 259-261.

Sun, S., & van Emmerik, H. H. (2015). Are proactive personalities always beneficial?

Political skill as a moderator. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100, 966-975.

Van Iddekinge, C. H., Roth, P. L., Putka, D. J., & Lanivich, S. E. (2011). Are you interested?

A meta-analysis of the relations between vocational interests and employee

performance and turnover. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96, 1167-1194.

Van Vugt, M., Hogan, R., & Kaiser, R. B. (2008). Leadership, followership, and evolution:

Some lessons from the past. American Psychologist, 63, 182-196.

Wiggins, J. S. (1991). Agency and communion as conceptual coordinates for the

understanding and measurement of interpersonal behavior. In W. M. Grove, & D.

Cicchetti (Eds.), Thinking clearly about psychology, Vol. 2: Personality and

psychopathology (pp. 89-113). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minneapolis Press.

Wiggins, J. S. (Ed.). (1996). The five-factor model of personality. New York: Guilford.

Wiggins, J. S., & Trapnell, P. E. (1996). A dyadic-interactional perspective on the Five-Factor

model. In J. S. Wiggins (Ed.). The five-factor model of personality (pp. 88-162). New

York: Guilford.

Witt, L. A., & Ferris, G. R. (2003). Social skill as moderator of the conscientiousness-

performance relationship: Convergent results across four studies. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 88, 809-820.


39

Zinko, R., Ferris, G. R., Blass, F. R., & Laird, M. D. (2007). Toward a theory of reputation in

organizations. In J. J. Martocchio (Ed.), Research in personnel and human resources

management, Vol. 26 (pp. 163-204). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.


40

High
CONVENTIONAL TYPE
concern for orderliness and routines;
prf. for predictable demands and
specified standards

ENTERPRISING TYPE REALISTIC TYPE

CONFORMITY
seeking for power; prf. for bargaining, prf. for practical activities, use of
leading, and manipulating others to machines, tools, and materials
attain personal or collective goals

High SOCIABILITY Low

SOCIAL TYPE INVESTIGATIVE TYPE


concern for the welfare of others and prf. for intellectual activities aimed at
a prf. for working with others in the creation and use of knowledge
helpful and facilitative ways

ARTISTIC TYPE
preference for creative work in
music, writing, performance,
or unstructured intellectual
endeavors
Low

Figure 1

Identity and the division of labor within groups

View publication stats

You might also like