Socioanalytic Theory: Basic Concepts, Supporting Evidences, and Practical Implications
Socioanalytic Theory: Basic Concepts, Supporting Evidences, and Practical Implications
Socioanalytic Theory: Basic Concepts, Supporting Evidences, and Practical Implications
net/publication/303913679
CITATIONS READS
30 3,463
2 authors, including:
Gerhard Blickle
University of Bonn
268 PUBLICATIONS 4,144 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Gerhard Blickle on 12 June 2016.
Hogan, R. & Blickle, G. (in press). Socioanalytic Theory: Basic concepts, supporting eviden-
ces, and practical implications. In V. Zeigler-Hill, & T. K. Shackelford (Eds.), The SAGE
Handbook of Personality and Individual Differences. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
2
To be published in
Edited by
Abstract
from: Charles Darwin about human evolution; Sigmund Freud about unconscious motivation;
and George Herbert Mead about the dynamics of social interaction. This chapter presents the
and practical implications of the theory in the fields of leadership and faking in personnel
selection. Finally, socioanalytic theory is positioned in the context of trait and clinical theories
of personality. Socioanalytic theory differs from the other two theories of personality
primarily by rejecting introspection as a valid source of data. In addition, trait theory has a
“pure science” agenda with minimal concern for applications, whereas clinical theories and
(1) Charles Darwin (1871) about human evolution; (2) Sigmund Freud (1913) about
unconscious motivation; and (3) George Herbert Mead (1934) about the dynamics of social
interaction. All three writers assume that, because we evolved as group living animals, we
have deep, organic needs for the universal features of human culture—collective rituals,
family relations, authority structures, morality, etc.. When evaluating theories of personality
(or anything else), the first question to ask is: “What question is the theory trying to answer?”
The pioneers of personality psychology (e.g., Freud, Jung, Adler, Horney, Maslow), for
example, wanted to explain the causes of psychopathology. Trait theory wants to identify the
structure of the trait descriptive universe and trace its neurological foundations. Socioanalytic
theory wants to predict and explain individual differences in peoples’ ability to: (1) acquire
social acceptance and support; (2) attain power and the control of resources; and (3) find
purpose and meaning in life (the theory applies to Maoris, chimpanzees, musicians,
politicians, scientists, drug dealers, etc.). The first part of this chapter presents the basic
assumptions of socioanalytic theory; the second part reviews supporting empirical evidence
Basic assumptions
Two adaptations gave early humans an advantage over chimpanzees, baboons, and
other competitors: an upright stance and tool use. The upright stance freed hands to grasp
tools. Superior tools ─ clubs, spears, and sharp stones ─ gave early humans an advantage in
hunting and fighting with other primates. Better weapons continue to give human groups an
advantage when competing with other groups. The arms race associated with tool creation ─
and culture in general ─ stimulated the development of larger brains, which then facilitated
Eibl-Eibesfeldt (1989) notes that "The basic principles of human society are fully
people, groups that contain grandparents, parents, and children. They live in fixed territories
which they must defend against other groups. They maintain morale and solidarity by gift
giving and other forms of exchange, and by ritualized social interaction ─ which includes
festivals and religious ceremonies. Some groups are egalitarian, some are authoritarian, but all
have status hierarchies. Most believe that they are a special people and distrust foreigners.
Finally, disputes inside the groups and warfare between the groups are constant. Groups with
superior technology and social organization overwhelm, enslave, or destroy groups with
inferior technology and social organization. Technology and social organization are the keys
to group survival, which makes it important for the younger generation to learn the culture of
its group.
These themes reflect about 1,000,000 years of human experience and are the
unconscious background for the development of individual personality. People lived in hunter
gatherer groups until the invention of agriculture about 13,000 years ago; agriculture allowed
much larger communities to develop. Modern industrial society is about 150 years old and has
led to huge urban centers. Life in large cities is easier in some ways than life in a hunter-
gatherer group ─ food, water, and electricity are generally available ─ but more difficult in
other ways ─ we no longer know or trust our neighbors. We are adapted to living in
conditions that no longer exist, and that explains much of the malaise of modern urban living
Basic motives
about human nature (Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1989; Fardon, et al., 2012; Mead, 1934). The first is that
people always live in groups; this suggests that they are inherently social, that at a deep and
unconscious level, people need companionship and social acceptance (cf. Baumeister &
Leary, 1995). These tendencies reflect the fact that group living has crucial survival
implications ─ solitary primates and humans do not live very long. The second generalization
5
is that every human (and primate) group has a status hierarchy; this suggests that, at a deep
and unconscious level, people (and primates) need status—because status permits better
choices in mates, food, and other commodities that promote fitness. Finally, anthropology
tells us that religion is an ancient human practice and a cultural universal. This suggests that
people need predictability—to understand how the world works, and their place in it. The
need for predictability leads to religion, culture, and technology; the need for predictability
People have physiological needs (food, air, water, and sleep), but they are not
distinctively human. The three motivational themes described above answer the question,
“What do people really want,” and what people really want reflects what early humans
needed for reproductive success. However, the fact that people are motivated by biologically
based impulses does not mean that consciousness is irrelevant. Biology sets life’s problems,
evolutionary theory answers the question of how people are all alike. But we must also
account for the individual differences among people. People differ most importantly in terms
of the strategies they have developed to deal with the problems of getting along, getting
ahead, and finding meaning, and some strategies are more effective than others.
As we noted, at a deep and often unconscious level, people are motivated by needs for:
(1) attention, approval, and acceptance; (2) status, power, and the control of resources; and (3)
predictability and order in their everyday lives. We refer to these themes as “getting along,”
“getting ahead,” and “finding meaning.” These are powerful motives ─ people will kill to
gain acceptance or status, they will kill to avoid losing them, and they will sometimes kill
themselves when they think they have lost a sense of meaning and purpose. These needs
represent the universal themes in human affairs ─ e.g., “It’s still the same old story, the fight
The first two motives are familiar themes. In social theory, David Bakan (1966)
argued that participation in larger social units (communion/getting along) and efforts at self-
promotion (agency/getting ahead) are universal themes in human affairs (cf. Abele &
reproduction depends on forming alliances (getting along) and negotiating status hierarchies
(getting ahead). In anthropology, Redfield (1960) noted that the survival of any social group
depends on its members living together and getting a living. In sociology, Parsons and Bales
(1955) demonstrate that every human group depends on successfully completing two
activities: socio-emotional tasks (getting along) and tasks related to group survival (getting
ahead).
In academic psychology, McAdams (2001) showed that the stories that people tell to
support their identities can be organized around two themes that he calls intimacy and power.
Similarly, social exchange theory (Foa & Foa, 1980) argued that social interaction is
organized around the exchange of love and status, and Wiggins and Trapnell (1996) proposed
that the exchange of love and status is the principal dynamic in social life.
There are two major consequences associated with getting along and getting ahead.
First, these issues make social interaction unavoidable—because we receive attention and
status during interaction. Second, if we are successful, others will resent us even as they
─ which makes high achievement difficult. Thus, there is an inherent tension beneath the
surface of social life as people try to advance themselves without alienating others.
The need for predictability and order also has a long history in psychology. Pavlov
(1927) showed that dogs become neurotic when they are required to perform in unpredictable
circumstances. Hebb and Thompson (1954) pointed out that chimpanzees have a strong need
for predictability, are highly sensitive to small changes in the behavior of others, and become
upset when others deviate even slightly from their normal behavior. George Kelly’s personal
7
construct theory (1955) begins by assuming that people need to be able to predict how others
will respond to them. Finally, Durkheim’s (1897) key concept, anomie, refers to peoples’
psychological experience when their lives no longer make sense in traditional terms ─ i.e., the
concept of anomie assumes that people need the predictability and meaning provided by
established culture and tradition. There is also a tragic tension associated with the need for
Over time, people with more status, social support, and control in their lives had a
reproductive advantage ─ they had better choices of marital partners, food supplies, shelter,
and other resources (Daly & Wilson, 1988). With some qualifications, this is still true today.
To summarize this discussion, at a deep and often unconscious level, people need structure
and predictability, attention and approval, and status and control of resources. People meet
these needs during social interaction ─ the unique features of human evolution compel people
to interact. But because human nature is rooted in biology, individual differences are
inevitable. Thus, some people need more social acceptance than others (e.g., actors versus
hermits), some people need more status than others (e.g., politicians versus social workers),
and some people need more predictability and meaning than others (e.g., the religious
orthodox versus nonobservant). Finally, some people are more successful than others in
attaining these goals, and these individual differences are what socioanalytic theory tries to
explain.
Identity
Following G. H. Mead (1934), socioanalytic theory postulates that the self (how we
view ourselves) develops based on feedback from others during social interaction. Once the
self is formed, it guides our actions vis-à-vis others, and it is further shaped by subsequent
feedback from others. The self, in this sense, is the same as our identity.
We think of human development in terms of four broad stages which are cross-cultural
universals—they occur in every culture. The first stage concerns the individual forming an
8
“attachment bond” (Bowlby, 1969). Normal parents are programed to want to care for their
babies, and the babies need caregivers. This typically leads to the development of an
"attachment bond" (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978), a strong emotional link
between an infant and its caretakers. The quality of the mother-child relationship is the basis
for an infant’s sense of security and well-being, and the platform on which subsequent social
Children expect their parents to coddle them, and the experience of being coddled
creates childish egocentrism. In the second phase of development, children must learn to
interact with their peers. In order for children to interact successfully, they must outgrow their
infantile egocentrism. The opposite of egocentrism is social sensitivity, and this develops, as
George Herbert Mead (1934) pointed out, through playing games in childhood, "By playing
together in the children’s group the members learn what aggravates others and which rules
they must obey” (Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1989, p. 601). Children learn how to interact with their
parents and other adults by following adult rules; in contrast, they learn how to interact with
other children by anticipating their expectations, and they learn to do this by playing games.
(Erikson, 1968). Our identities are idealized views of ourselves that answer the question of
who and what we are, what we stand for, and how we should be treated. During adolescence
young people try out a variety of identities ─ musician, cheerleader, mystic. Feedback from
the peer group ─ e.g., "Who do you think you are?" ─ gradually determines the final choice.
Our identity guides our behavior during social interaction by controlling the roles we are
willing to play and how we play them. For example, the people in a college classroom are all
in the role of student, but they play the role differently depending on their identity. The two
major tasks of adolescence, therefore, concern choosing an identity and developing the skills
Adulthood is the final developmental phase. Adult social life consists of an extended
series of interactions, usually with the same people over and over, in the process of trying to
have a career. As noted above, the goal of adult interaction is the pursuit of status and social
acceptance (i.e., a career), and there are two major strategies for interaction: competing and
trying to outperform others, and affiliating and trying to build coalitions. Acquiring status
depends on the support of others, which involves building relationships. But status also
depends on aggression, on the desire to outcompete others and to defend one’s position
interaction ends, there is an accounting process which results in each person gaining or losing
a little bit of status and a little bit of acceptance. Interacting with others is a skilled
performance; athletes, actors, and politicians are only as good as their last performance, and
the same is true for the rest of us. Because interaction is so central to adult social life, it is
useful to ask what is needed in order to interact? The answer is, there are two requirements:
(a) a pretext for the interaction ─ e.g., let’s get together and watch football ─ and (b) roles for
the participants to play ─ e.g., you be the host and I will bring some beer.
Note that engaging in play provides children with attention in a structured and
predictable manner. Note also that adults do not simply get together and bask in one another’s
company, they get together and pretend to "do something" ─ have a cup of coffee, drink a
beer, talk something over ─ and these interactions are essentially identical to the way 3-year
olds play. To interact with another person, we need a pretext and a role to play. Outside of our
roles, we have nothing to say to one another. Persons with good social skill have a talent for
inventing pretexts for interaction and for negotiating who plays what role.
Where do identities come from? Generally speaking, other people teach us who we
are. This is true, but we also choose our identities from the menus that are available in our
cultures. The menu is usually found in the entertainment world—in movies, books, TV shows,
10
etc. But the larger point is that our identities—the persons we think are real—come from
menus provided by society. We may reject the identities that are available in our culture, but
we will have to find substitutes before we can join the game of life and begin interacting with
others. Our identities include our values, and our values are tied to our social class. For
example, wealthy people with working class values seem working class, whereas working
class people with good taste and civil manners seem “well bred.” Consequently, personality─
Reputation
For socioanalytic theory, the core components of personality are identity and
reputation. Our identity guides our behavior during social interaction. Other people evaluate
our behavior during social interactions and their evaluations create our reputation. Identities
are personality from the perspective of the actor ─ our identities concern the person that we
think we are. Reputations, on the other hand, are personality from the perspective of the
observer ─ reputations concern the person whom others think we are. Successful people pay
Reputations are important for the study of personality for five reasons. First, they
develop quickly and are stable over time ─ different observers tend to agree substantially
about a person's reputation ─ which means that reputations can be studied objectively. In
contrast, identities are more fluid and much harder to study. Second, most people spend a
great deal of time and energy trying to establish and maintain their reputations. Third, because
the best predictor of future behavior is past behavior, and because reputations reflect a
person's past behavior, reputations are the best data source we have for predicting most
the Five-Factor Model (FFM; Hogan, 1996). We think about and describe other people in
social acceptance and status we have in our communities. Successful people know how to
manage their reputations, and they manage them during social interaction all the time
(Goffman, 1959).
Allport (1961) defined personality psychology as the study of identity and dismissed
reputation as an epiphenomenon. It is worth noting, however, that after 100 years of research
to report. In contrast, researchers have been studying reputation for about 20 years. There is a
Social skill
the actor’s and the observer’s. Self-knowledge from the actor’s perspective concerns what we
think we know about ourselves; self-knowledge from the observer’s perspective concerns
what other people actually know about us. Self-knowledge from the actor’s perspective
concerns understanding our identity and becoming mindful of how we interact with others.
Self-knowledge from the observer’s perspective concerns understanding our reputation and
what we did to create it. Identity and reputation are related in interesting ways. For example,
the person we think we are may not be the person that others think we are; to the degree that
Identities are idealized views of ourselves, and we typically try to convince others that
these idealized views are true. Some people are better at this than others because their social
behavior is more convincing and effective. Hogan and Shelton (1998) argued that the ability
successfully to translate one’s identity into a desired reputation is moderated by social skill.
12
They defined social skill as competent impression management—the ability to control the
impressions that others form of us. The alignment between identity and reputation depends on
actor’s social skills, of which self-knowledge and empathy are important facets. The success
to the emotional cues of others, using correct language-style, and controlling non-verbal cues
appropriately. Empirical research supports the idea that socially skilled individuals more
quickly identify and attend to emotional cues in others and are better able to choose
appropriate facial expressions, hand gestures, body postures, voice textures, and other
paralinguistic cues (Gangestad & Snyder, 2000; Momm, Blickle, & Liu, 2010).
The unconscious
Freud argued that we are typically unaware of the reasons for our actions, and his view
is supported by modern research in cognitive psychology. We believe there are at least four
The first set of unconscious influences comes from biology. As noted above, we need
attention and approval, we need status and control of resources, and we need order and
predictability. Although much of our everyday behavior concerns pursuing these needs, we
are rarely conscious of this fact. Loneliness usually indicates a need for interaction;
depression indicates lost status and control; and anxiety is caused by chaos and
unpredictability. These needs become conscious primarily when they are unfulfilled. We
would also include temperament and mood in this set of unconscious biological processes.
Our temperament produces our characteristic mood states which serve as a filter for our
attention and shape our perceptions in ways that are simultaneously profound, idiosyncratic,
and unconscious.
Our natural egocentrism creates a second set of unconscious influences. That is, most
of us tend to ignore what others expect or believe during interactions. For example, when we
ask managers to describe how their staff evaluates them, they are surprisingly inaccurate—
13
managers typically see themselves in much more positive terms than their staff describes
them. Similarly, couples in close relationships are unable accurately to describe how their
partners perceive them. Although we constantly interact with others, the data suggest that
these interactions are based on superficial mutual understanding. This raises an interesting
One answer is that the rules governing interaction are prewired in our nervous
systems, so that our responses to others do not depend on understanding what they expect.
Human social interaction seems to resemble the mating dance of dragonflies, wherein
gestures, colors, smells, and posture trigger corresponding gestures, postures, and behavior.
Eibl-Eibesfeldt (1989) argues that the rules of social interaction are inherited, that there is "...a
universal grammar of social behavior according to which verbal and nonverbal interactions
are similarly structured (p. 499)." The result is that "many of the basic strategies of social
programs” (p. 516). For example, people all over the world respond to the same nonverbal
gestures (e.g., sticking one's tongue out is a universal sign of impertinence) and recognize the
same emotions in others. Eibl-Eibesfeldt further suggests that "...we can speak of a universal
interactions” (1989, p. 517). Consequently, it seems that much of our behavior during social
interaction ─ posture, facial expressions, gaze, pace, etc. ─ runs off automatically and is
therefore unconscious. Politicians, actors, and other entertainers understand this and often go
through elaborate coaching in order to master and control these very subtle and otherwise
The third set of unconscious influences on social behavior are the values, customs, and
norms of our culture that we assume are true and that we rarely question or challenge. These
include rules about what we should eat, how we should dress, how women are treated, how
14
members of minority groups should be treated, and how our life style is superior to that of
others. This third form of unconscious influence is much more powerful than we typically
realize.
Finally, our brains evolved so as to handle routine activities with automatic processes
and use conscious attention to solve novel problems (Johnson & Hogan, 2006). Routine
behaviors demand far less attention than new skills, so human brains are designed for a high
level of automatic activity, and these automatic mental routines give regularity and
consistency to human behavior. When people are learning new tasks (e.g., how to type or
drive a car) their first efforts are typically halting and clumsy. With practice their performance
becomes automatic, consistent, and largely unconscious. This is also true for the way we
interact with others, and these processes can lead to self-defeating behavior. Self-defeating
behaviors are usually adaptive when they first develop, but when circumstances change,
automatic and unconscious behaviors may produce short-term benefits but may create long-
term costs (i.e., they cloud judgement and hamper performance; Kaiser, LeBreton, & Hogan,
2015; Stanovich, West, & Toplak, 2013). Using feedback, people can become aware of what
guided by forces outside our awareness, many of these nonconscious influences can become
conscious through social feedback, education, and self-reflection. To the degree that they
remain unconscious, we are liable to act in ways that are foolish, self-defeating, or even
Caveat
One problem with the foregoing model of personality should be mentioned. Brewer
and Caporael (1991) observed that human evolutionary theory is often put to political rather
than scientific purposes. Specifically, evolutionary arguments have been used to support
racism, sexism, and even genocide. The pseudo-logical argument is often constructed in the
15
following way: Someone notes that there are consistent differences between two groups (e.g.,
men and women) and then argues that the characteristics that typify the group that is in power
are innately superior to the characteristics of the other group, precisely because one's group is
in power (Padilla, Hogan, & Kaiser, 2007). One is then allowed to stigmatize the other group
on the grounds of one's natural superiority. We, of course, deplore these sorts of arguments
because they are pseudo-scientific, circular, and hateful. To argue that what is should also be,
The following discussion presents evidence regarding the primacy of the motives of
getting along, getting ahead, and finding meaning. It also presents evidence regarding how
identity determines the division of labor within groups, and how social skills support the
expression of identity. Finally, we review research on the links between reputation and career
success.
Basic motives
Empirical research supports the importance of the three basic (and usually
unconscious) motives emphasized by socioanalytic theory. Within groups there are typically
individual differences in the desire to get along and get ahead. Because culture (the rules of
interaction) is constant within groups, the need for stability, predictability, and meaning
Getting along and getting ahead. Digman (1997) analyzed personality trait ratings
from 14 studies ─ five based on children and adolescents, nine on adults. Seven of the studies
used observer ratings of personality traits and seven used self-ratings. Digman analyzed the
data using exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses. In all 14 studies, two higher-order
and emotional stability; the beta-factor contained the traits of extraversion and intellect (viz.,
openness to experience). These two factors have been labeled social interest and superiority
16
striving by Adler (1939), communion and agency by Wiggins (1991), intimacy and power
motivation by McAdams (1992), and popularity and status striving by Hogan (1982). These
two meta-traits represent the motives of getting along and ahead. Recently, Ashton and Lee
have proposed adding a factor they call Honesty-Humility, defined by the terms sincere, fair,
and unassuming versus sly, greedy, and pretentious (Ashton & Lee, 2005). We think this
meaningful social interactions are essential for our psychological well-being. Chaotic social
interaction is deeply stressful, and if it continues long enough, anyone will break down
(Hogan, 1982). Basically, culture defines the rules which provide interaction with stability,
predictability, and meaning (Hogan & Bond, 2009). Jahoda (1981) first proposed that
life (cf. Paul & Batinic, 2009) and therefore promotes psychological well-being. Barrick,
Mount, and Li (2013) also propose that striving for meaning is an important (unconscious)
motive at work.
In a cross-national study of over 80 nations, Oishi and Diener (2013) report that
economic scarcity is associated with lower social status, fewer educational chances, poorer
health care, and lower levels of life satisfaction, but also with more religiosity and perceived
meaning in life. This suggests that the stress of poverty enhances the salience of peoples’
needs for meaning and predictability, a need that is filled by strong religious convictions.
Conversely, when people live in societies that provide employment, education, and health
care, the need for stability, predictability, and meaning is less salient.
Identity
Socioanalytic theory assumes that people must interact, that they use their identities to
structure interaction (identity is the generic role people carry with them across situations), and
17
that social skill enhances the ability to express one’s identity in different situational contexts.
Identity and the division of labor within groups. Holland (1996) developed a
widely accepted taxonomy of vocational types; every job in the Dictionary of Occupational
Titles can be classified using the Holland model, and it is an indispensable starting point for
any discussion of careers (see Figure 1). There are six occupational types: Realistic,
Investigative, Artistic, Social, Enterprising, and Conventional. Thus the Holland model is
often referred to as “the RIASEC model”. Realistic types (engineers) prefer concrete practical
prefer research and problem solving activities. Artistic types (artists) prefer creative work in
art, music, and design. Social types (teachers) prefer activities designed to help others.
Finally, Conventional types (accountants) prefer activities associated with organizing and
----------------------------------
-----------------------------------
These occupational (personality) types can be organized using the two dimensions of
sociability and conformity (Figure 1; Hogan, 1982). Conventional types score high on
conformity and artistic types score low on conformity. Enterprising types score high on
conformity and sociability. Realistic types score high on conformity and low on sociability.
Social types score high on sociability and low on conformity factor. And Investigative types
score low on conformity and sociability. According to Holland (1996), each occupational type
prefers to work in a specific environment containing specific demands and rewards: Artistic
Socioanalytic theory suggests that the Holland types represent six key roles in
successful human groups (Hogan, 1982). Enterprising types move into leader roles.
Conventional types implement group decisions. Realistic types build and repair tools.
Investigative types invent tools (and technology). Artistic types entertain people around the
camp fire (and challenge dysfunctional rules and obsolete traditions). And Social types
Consistent with Holland’s model, meta-analyses find that the closer peoples’
(RIASEC) personality types fit their work environments, the greater their training success, job
satisfaction, job tenure, and job performance (Assouline & Meir, 1987; Van Iddekinge, Roth,
Putka, & Lanivich, 2011). Hogan and Blake (1999) explain these findings in terms of
“person-environment fit”, where the environment is defined by the personalities of the other
people in that environment. That is, people work more productively when they share the
It is interesting to note that the personality dimension of neuroticism is not part of the
strong predictor of job performance (Hogan & Holland, 2003). Hogan (1982) notes that for
neurotics, “…anxiety, depression, and physical complaints are part of a general self-
presentational program where the message is, ‘I am sick, I need special attention and
consideration, and don’t expect too much of me under these circumstances.’” (p. 80). Meta-
analytic research strongly supports the view that neuroticism predicts reduced career success
(Lodi-Smith & Roberts, 2007; Ng, Eby, Sorensen, & Feldman, 2005).
FFM. In contrast, socioanalytic theory regards ambition as essential for career success (i.e.,
getting ahead) because ambition is about persistent and generalized striving for success,
attainment, and accomplishment. Consistent with this claim, Judge and Kammeyer-Mueller
(2012) show, in a predictive study over 30 years with 717 participants, that individual
19
differences in ambition predict educational attainment and prestige, occupational income and
Identity and social skill. Hogan and Shelton (1998) suggested that social skill
translates interpersonal aspirations into action. More specifically, social skill translates
identity into reputation—social skill allows people to portray themselves positively during
social interaction. Social skill allows people to achieve their interpersonal goals just as hand-
eye coordination allows people to hit tennis balls accurately. Social skill is trainable and has
the following features (Hogan & Shelton, 1998): being sensitive and responsive to others’
needs and moods, being flexible and adaptable, being persuasive, being able to instill trust,
being consistent across social interactions, being accountable, and being able to listen to and
social skill maximizes the approval and minimizes the disapproval of an actor’s social
performance.
measure of social skill combined with a measure of wanting to get along with peers and
performance, and promotion potential (Blickle et al., 2011). High scores on these two
measures predicted higher income and marketability of new employees after two years
(Blickle, Momm, Liu, Witzki, & Steinmayr, 2011). High scores on a measure of social skill
and a measure of achievement striving are associated with supervisory ratings of leadership
emergence (Marinova, Moon, & Kamdar, 2013). Finally, in a sample of 510 school
headmasters, high scores on a measure of social skill and a measure of wanting to get ahead
predicted success in leadership as rated by three to four teachers (N = 1,881) who reported to
In conclusion, social skill enhances the ability of people to present themselves and
control their reputation during social interaction. People with social skill are better able to
20
restrain, calibrate, and adjust their behavior in different and changing social contexts. This
adaptability allows them to exercise interpersonal influence by gaining the trust and
confidence of those with whom they interact. After every interaction, the participants evaluate
the performance of one another. These evaluations ultimately turn into performance appraisals
(Hogan & Shelton, 1998). On what do these evaluations depend? They primarily reflect the
degree to which people are rewarding during social interactions. Being rewarding involves
helping others advance their agendas, being compliant and attentive, and fitting with the
culture of the group. Being rewarding has to do with making another person feel and look
Reputation
perceptual identity formed from the collective perceptions of others, which is reflective of the
behavior, and intended images presented over some period of time as observed directly and/or
reported from secondary sources, which reduces ambiguity about expected future behavior”
(Zinko, Ferris, Blass, & Laird, 2007, p. 165). Consequences of a positive reputation include:
elbowroom ─ having more discretion to act; power ─ others will defer to one’s judgment;
improved performance ─ having more discretion and power to get things done; enhanced
positive reputation include career success and enhanced subjective well-being (Zinko et al.,
2007).
Observers use trait terms to describe and evaluate other people, and these
Reputations, encoded in trait words, evaluate a person’s contribution to the success of the
groups to which the person belongs ─ tribe, family, combat-unit, or work team. Trait terms are
21
the units of reputation and the FFM (Wiggins, 1996) is a robust and widely accepted
taxonomy of reputation. Briefly the dimensions of the FFM are: (1) Emotional stability—low
volatility and performance under pressure; (2) Extraversion—sociability and assertiveness; (3)
indirectly. Direct measures of reputation use observer ratings to assess personality, whereas
support the view that observer ratings of personality (reputation) are significantly more valid
and Bing (2015) report that observer ratings of the FFM personality dimensions add
significant validity to self-reports when predicting work place deviance. Kholin et al. (2016)
than self-ratings of learning approach. Oh, Wang, and Mount (2011) used meta-analysis to
compare the operational validities of FFM traits based on self- and other-ratings. As predicted
by socioanalytic theory, they found that the validities of observer ratings are significantly
higher than those based on self-report ratings. In addition, observer ratings of FFM traits
Connelly and Ones (2010) also conducted a meta-analytic study of the links between
observer ratings of actors’ personalities and ratings for job performance. Their results strongly
support those reported by Oh et al. (2011). How well does reputation (observers’ ratings of
and Hunter (1998) report that General Mental Ability is the best single predictor of overall job
performance (ρ = .51; for a more accurate estimate, see Schmidt, Shaffer, & Oh, 2008) and
22
work sample tests (ρ = .54; for an alternative estimate, see Roth, Bobko, & McFarland, 2005).
Structured employment interviews (ρ = .51) are the best non-test predictors of job
performance. However, Connelly and Ones (2010) found that observer ratings of
conscientiousness are slightly more valid overall (true score correlations ρ = .55; for
operational validity estimates, see Oh et al., 2011). For comparison purposes, other popular
procedures yield the following validity coefficients: reference checks (ρ = .26); biographical
interview (ρ = .38); and integrity tests (ρ = .41). In sum, socioanalytic theory predicts
Successful people use superior social skills to manage their reputations. Empirical
research also supports this claim. For example, peer ratings (indices of reputation) of
academic aptitude significantly predict the academic performance of university students with
good social skills over more than one year (ß = .43) but not among students with low social
skills (ß = .02; Kholin et al., 2016). In many, if not most cases, career success depends on
peoples’ reputation and the manner in which they present themselves to others. People who
inventories based on the FFM are available to researchers. These inventories are typically
called self-report measures. Trait theorists assume that, when respondents complete these
inventories, they consult their memories and then report on their “true” thoughts, feelings, and
past behaviors (Allport, 1961). In contrast, socioanalytic theory assumes that, when
respondents complete these inventories, they respond in ways that tell others how they want to
be regarded. That is, respondents do not provide self-reports, they provide self-presentations.
themselves and hope others will believe accept these views. In addition, socioanalytic theory
23
argues that these self-presentations are not necessarily conscious because, over time, self-
we support the identity they want to project. Thus, employees who comply with the requests
of their supervisors and respect their status will be seen as rewarding because they help their
supervisors look good. Supervisors rarely think about their subordinates in terms of the
subordinate’s goals, fears, and aspirations; rather, supervisors think about subordinates in
terms of how rewarding they are—defined in terms of protecting, supporting, and enhancing
Social skills are also needed in order to control others by counseling, persuading, and
suggesting rather than by ordering, criticizing, and coercing them (Blickle, Kane-Frieder, et
al., 2013). Although identity is rather stable, social skills are, in principle, trainable.
Moreover, good social skills can coexist with deeply flawed identities ─ where flawed is
defined in terms of insecurity, selfishness, strange and irrational goals, and a disposition
As socioanalytic theory predicts, many studies show that social skill moderates the
predictive validity of standard personality measures, including the validity of measures of:
agreeableness (Blickle et al., 2008); conscientiousness (Witt & Ferris, 2003); extraversion
(Blickle, Wendel, & Ferris, 2010); openness to experience (Blickle et al., 2013); honesty-
humility (Diekmann, Blickle, Hafner, & Peters, 2015); trait sincerity (Meurs, Perrewé, &
Ferris, 2011); and proactive personality (Sun & van Emmerik, 2015). Socioanalytic theory
regards these personality dimensions as “bright side” characteristics (Hogan & Hogan, 2001)
because they appear during social interactions when people are self-consciously trying to get
Socioanalytic theory distinguishes between “bright side” and “dark side” behavior.
Bright side behavior is what we see when people are behaving themselves; the FFM is a
24
taxonomy of bright side behavior. In contrast with bright side characteristics, dark side
characteristics (e.g., narcissism, psychopathy) emerge when people let down their guard—
when they are angry, tired, or “just being themselves”, and usually when they are dealing with
subordinates or people with less power. We also assume (Hogan & Shelton, 1998) that good
social skills can compensate for dark side personality tendencies. Recent research supports
this assumption.
Narcissism is a dark side characteristic defined by a grandiose but fragile sense of self,
feelings of entitlement, preoccupation with success, and demands for admiration (e.g., Owens,
Wallace, & Feldman, 2015). It also includes being self-centered, self-absorbed, extremely
self-confident, and exploitive. O’Boyle, Forsyth, Banks, and McDaniel (2012) found in a
Owens et al. (2015) also report that, when narcissism is combined with humility (i.e., social
skill) ─ admitting when one makes mistakes, being aware of others’ strengths—such leaders
receive high ratings of effectiveness and followers show high levels of job engagement and
seeking, manipulative tendencies, and an inability to learn from experience. Lilienfeld and
Schütte et al. (2015) suggested that: (1) people with high scores for both psychopathy and
social skill should succeed in organizations; whereas (2) people with high scores for
psychopathy and low scores for social skill should fail. Schütte et al. (2015) report that people
with high scores for psychopathy and low scores for social skill displayed high levels of
people with high scores for both psychopathy and social skill demonstrated low levels of
In sum, people with social skill are flexible, able to establish rapport via an
unassuming and effective communication style, and able to behave in ways that are situation-
specific, appropriate, and influential. Thus, people with extensive dark side tendencies who
have good social skills are often able to control their negative behaviors and be successful.
However, these studies of narcissism, psychopathy, and social skill are cross-sectional.
We need longitudinal studies to determine whether persons with strong dark side tendencies
use social skill to deceive others while selfishly extracting resources from the group (Jones,
2014). Some parasitic animals use complex deception tactics based on slow resource
extraction, careful integration into the community, and avoidance of detection, and their
behavior has severe long-term negative consequences for the host community.
Socioanalytic theory has many practical implications; in the following we review two
that substantially impact our daily lives, namely leadership and faking in personnel selection.
Sociologists, political scientists, and historians argue that leadership is the function of
that leadership is a function of personality and that some people have more talent for
leadership than others. Good leadership contributes to the long-term success of groups and
organizations (Kaiser, Hogan, Craig, 2008; Van Vugt, Hogan, Kaiser, 2008). Successful
leadership involves persuading group members to set aside, for a limited time, their selfish,
short-term interests and contribute to long-term group goals of their groups or organizations
(Padilla, Hogan, & Kaiser, 2007). Charles Darwin (1871) noted: “A tribe including many
members who … were always ready to aid one another, and to sacrifice themselves for the
common good, would be victorious over most other tribes, and this would be natural
To evaluate the links between personality and leadership, one needs scores for
roles. Judge, Bono, Ilies, and Gerhardt (2002) aggregated the results of 222 correlations from
73 studies of personality and leadership performance. Their sample contained more than
25,000 managers from every level in 5,000 organizations across every industry sector. They
report that four of the five dimensions of the FFM significantly predict leadership
with leadership (.29, .27, and .21, respectively), and the multiple correlation between
personality and leadership was .53. For people who believe in data, this study definitively
settles the argument that personality predicts leadership performance across all organizational
levels and industry sectors, and does so more powerfully than any known alternative.
Faking
that will enhance one’s scores (Hogan & Blickle, 2013). Many psychologists think personality
assessment lacks any inherent validity because, they argue, it is easy for people to fake their
scores. Because personality assessment is the indispensable research method for personality
Socioanalytic theory interprets item responses in terms of impression management: people use
the items on personality measures to tell others how they want to be regarded. This, then,
with the view that deception involves acting in a way that is inconsistent with a single ‘true
self’ hidden inside of us” (Johnson & Hogan, 2006, p. 211). “When individuals try to act in
27
deceptive ways in everyday life (e.g., introverts try to act like extraverts) their natural
tendencies ‘leak through’ and observers readily detect them … Only good actors can make
atypical performances seem convincing …” (Johnson & Hogan, 2006, pp. 210-211).
Consider the process of child rearing. Small children act in ways that reflect their real
desires and urges. Socialization primarily involves training children to hide their real desires
and to behave in ways that are consistent with the norms of adult behavior. For a traditional
view on faking, socialization involves training children to fake. For impression management
Most adults know the rules of conduct and respond to the items in terms of social norms
rather than in terms of their real desires. Criminals and other rebels respond in ways that are
closer to their real desires—in ways that are consistent with their typical behavior.
The point is that it is nearly impossible to distinguish faking from socialized behavior.
Johnson and Hogan (2006) report on a study using six unlikely virtue scales. Each scale
corresponded to one of six personality scales. The following is an unlikely virtue item for the
Inquisitive scale: “In my own way, I am an intellectual giant”, and the following is for the
personality scale and the unlikely virtue scales. In addition, two people who knew each
student rated that student on the six personality dimensions. Most students endorsed unlikely
virtue items proportional to their scores on the personality scales. Thus, each unlikely virtue
scales was most highly correlated with its corresponding personality scale and with the peer
ratings for the same dimension. This implies that although the students sometimes
were consistent with their rated reputations. Thus, endorsing unlikely virtue items provides
information that predicts job performance because the endorsements are consistent with the
28
It is also possible to test the faking criticism directly. In the only proper study of
faking in a real world context, Hogan, Barrett, and Hogan (2007) tested several thousand
applicants for a government job using a well validated measure of the FFM, of which a
subsample of applicants were rejected. Six months later, 5,266 of the rejected applicants
reapplied for the job. Because they were denied employment based on the inventory, and
because they wanted the job, they were motivated to improve their scores by faking. The
results indicated that 5.2% changed their scores on the second trial, but that scores improved
for 2.6% of the sample, and scores declined for 2.6% of the sample. These results clearly
show that only a small minority (about 5% of the sample) tried to change their scores, and of
those who tried, as many lowered their scores as raised their scores. Faking (i.e., a deliberate
Today personality psychology consists of three major theoretical clusters, each with a
distinctive focus and intent. The first cluster, clinical theories of personality (e.g.,
individual neurosis and perhaps overcome its affects. The second cluster, trait theory (Allport,
1961), uses introspection (self-report data) to identify the structure of self-report data and
trace its neurological underpinnings. The third, socioanalytic theory uses reputation to predict
important life outcomes, usually in the form of career success (or failure). Trait theory has a
“pure science” agenda with minimal concern for applications, whereas clinical theories and
Socioanalytic theory differs from the other two theories of personality primarily by
rejecting introspection as a valid source of data. We reject introspection for three reasons.
29
First, the capacity for (and interest in) introspection is normally distributed. A portion of the
population (usually people with neurotic tendencies) introspect constantly with clinical
psychologists possibly being part of this group. Many people engage in introspection from
never do it and they are unable to do it. Two examples of unusually successful people who
were famously incapable of introspection are Ronald Reagan, former President of the United
States (Cannon, 2000), and Margaret Thatcher, former Prime Minister of England (Campbell,
2011). The fact that neurotics enjoy introspection whereas highly successful politicians are
The second reason for rejecting introspection concerns the universality of self-
deception. As Freud noted, people lie to themselves about their true motives and agendas and
then believe their lies. When they introspect, they dredge up invented memories and stories
which they then relay to us. The third reason for rejecting introspection concerns the question
of how hard it is for us, as listeners, to verify the introspective claims of other people. We
Socioanalytic theory differs from trait theory (Allport, 1961) in an important way.
Trait theory uses trait words (honest, brave, creative, hostile) to describe peoples’ behavior,
and then uses trait words to explain peoples’ behavior: e.g., Mike Tyson (an American former
(explanation). This is a tautology, a logical fallacy at the core of trait theory. In our view, we
do not have traits, we have agendas, goals, intentions, and our behavior reflects these agendas.
Other people watch us and then assign trait labels to our behavior so that they can predict our
future behavior. Traits exist in the minds of observers and in the observed behavior of actors.
Traits are an inference that other people make about our behavior, and they are powerful
sources of data to predict our future behavior. But it is a fundamental logical error to use traits
Finally, socioanalytic theory agrees with Freudian psychoanalysis in four ways. First,
we believe that, to understand human nature (personality), we need to study human origins
because the evolutionary history of our species holds the keys to understanding modern
behavior. Second, we believe that most social behavior is unconsciously motivated, that
people typically do not know why they do what they do when they do it, but they are good at
inventing explanations which they also believe. Third, we believe that development matters
such that experiences early in life have a greater impact on personality than experiences later
in life. And finally, Freud defined maturity as the capacity to love and to work. Similarly, we
think maturity is the capacity to build and maintain healthy relationships and to have a
successful career.
31
References
Abele, A., & Wojciszke, B. (2014). Communal and agentic content. A dual perspective
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment.
Allport, G. W. (1961). Pattern and growth in personality. New York: Holt, Rhinehart, &
Winston.
Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2005). Honesty-humility, the Big Five, and the Five-Factor model.
Assouline, M., & Meir, E.I. (1987). Meta-analysis of the relationship between congruence and
Barrick, M. R., Mount, M. K., & Li, N. (2013). The theory of purposeful work behavior: The
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M.R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal
529.
Blickle, G., Fröhlich, J. Ehlert, S., Pirner, K, Dietl, E., Hanes T. J., & Ferris, G. R. (2011).
Socioanalytic theory and work behavior: Roles of work values and political skill in job
148.
Blickle, G., Kane-Frieder, R. E., Oerder, K., Wihler, A., von Below, A., Schütte, N.,
Matanovic, A., Mudlagk, D., Kokudeva, T. & Ferris, G. R. (2013). Leader behaviors
32
Blickle, G., Meurs, J. A., Wihler, A., Ewen, C., Plies, A. & Günther, S. (2013). The
Blickle, G., Meurs, J. A., Zettler, I, Solga, J., Noethen, D., Kramer, J, & Ferris, G. R. (2008).
Personality, political skill, and job performance. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 72,
377-387.
Blickle, G., Momm, T., Liu, Y., Witzki, A. & Steinmayr, R. (2011). Construct validation of
Blickle, G., Wendel, S. & Ferris, G. R. (2010). Political skill as moderator of personality - job
Bowlby, J. (1969). Atttachment and loss, Vol. I. New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1971). Attachment and loss, Vol. II. New York: Basic Books.
Brewer, M. B., & Caporeal, L. R. (1990). Selfish genes versus selfish people: Sociobiology as
Buss, D. M. (2015). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind (5th edition).
Boston: Pearson.
1092-1122.
33
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1988). Homicide. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
Darwin, C. R. (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. London: John
Murray.
Diekmann, C., Blickle, G., Hafner, K. & Peters, L. (2015). Trick or trait? The combined
Digman, J. M. (1997). Higher-order factors of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social
Ewen, C., Wihler, A., Frieder, R. E., Blickle, G., Hogan, R. & Ferris, G. R. (2014). Leader
Fardon, R., et al. (Eds.). (2012). The SAGE handbook of social anthropology (2 Vols.).
Foa, E. B., & Foa, U. G. (1980). Resource theory of social exchange. In J. W. Thibaut, J. T.
Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. New York: Anchor.
Hebb, D. O., & Thompson, W. R. (1954). The social significance of animal studies. In D. T.
Gilbert, & G. Lindzay (Eds.), Handbook of Social Psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 532-561).
Hogan, J., Barrett, P., & Hogan, R. (2007). Personality measurement, faking, and employment
Hogan, J., & Holland, B. (2003). Using theory to evaluate personality and job-performance
(Ed.), The Five-factor model of personality (pp. 163-179). New York: Guilford.
Hogan, R., & Blake, R. (1999). John Holland’s vocational typology and personality theory.
Hogan, R., & Bond, M. H. (2009). Culture and personality. In P. J. Corr, & G. Matthews
Hogan, R., & Hogan, J. (2001). Assessing leadership: A view from the dark side.
Hogan, R., & Shelton. D. (1998). A socioanalytic perspective on job performance. Human
Holland, J. L. (1996). Exploring careers with a typology. American Psychologist, 51, 397-
406.
Jahoda, M. (1981). Work, employment, and unemployment. Values, theories, and approaches
Johnson, J. A., & Hogan, R. (2006). A socioanalytic view of faking. In R. Griffith & H. M.
Judge, T.A., Bono, J.E., Ilies, R., & Gerhardt, M. (2002). Personality and leadership. Journal
Judge, T. A., & Kammeyer-Mueller, J. D. (2012). On the value of aiming high: The causes
Kaiser, R. B., Hogan, R., & Craig, S. B. (2008). Leadership and the fate of organizations
Kaiser, R. B., & Kaplan, R. B. (2006). The deeper work of executive development:
483.
Kaiser, R. B., LeBreton, J. M., & Hogan, J. (2015). The dark side of personality and extreme
Kholin, M., Meurs, J., Blickle, G., Wihler, A., Ewen, C. & Momm, T. (2016). Refining the
skill, and the combination of trait self- and other-ratings. Journal of Personality
Kluemper, D. H., McLarty, B. D., & Bing, M. N. (2015). Acquaintance ratings of the Big
Five personality traits: Incremental validity beyond and interactive effects with self-
237-248.
36
Lodi-Smith, J., & Roberts, B. W. (2007). Social investment and personality: A meta-analysis
Marinova, S. V., Moon, H., & Kamdar, D. (2013). Getting ahead or getting along? The two-
McAdam, D. P. (1992). The five-factor model in personality. Journal of Personality, 60, 329-
361.
100-122.
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, & society. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Meurs, J. A., Perrewé, P. L., & Ferris, G. R. (2011). Political skill as moderator of the trait
Momm, T., Blickle, G., & Liu, Y. (2010). Political skill and emotional cue learning.
Ng, T. W. H., Eby, L. T., Sorensen, K. L., & Feldman, D.C. (2005). Predictors of objective
O'Boyle Jr, E. H., Forsyth, D. R., Banks, G. C., & McDaniel, M. A. (2012). A meta-analysis
of the dark triad and work behavior: A social exchange perspective. Journal of
37
Oh, I.-S., Wang, G., & Mount, M. K. (2011). Validity of observer ratings of the Five-Factor
773.
Oishi, S., & Diener, E. (2014). Residents of poor nations have more meaning in life than
Owens, B. P., Wallace, A. S., & Waldman, D. A. (2015). Leader narcissism and follower
Padilla, A., Hogan, R., & Kaiser, R. B. (2007). The toxic triangle: Destructive leaders,
176-194.
Parsons, T., & Bales, R. F. (1955). Family, socialization and interaction process. Glencoe,
Paul, K. I., & Batinic, B. (2009). The need for work: Jahoda’s latent functions of employment
Redfield, R. (1960). How society operates. In H. L. Shapiro (Ed.), Man, culture, and society
Roth, P. L., Bobko, P., & McFarland, L. A. (2005). A meta-analysis of work sample test
validity: Updating and integrating some classic literature. Personnel Psychology, 58,
1009-1037.
Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1998). The validity and utility of selection methods in
Schmidt, F. L., Shaffer, J. A., & Oh, I.-S. (2008). Increased accuracy for range restriction
corrections: Implications for the role of personality and general mental ability in job
Schütte, N., Blickle, G., Frieder, R., Wihler, A., Schnitzler, F., Heupel, J. & Zettler, I. (2015).
Stanovich, K. E., West, R. F., & Toplak, M. E. (2013). Myside bias, rational thinking, and
Sun, S., & van Emmerik, H. H. (2015). Are proactive personalities always beneficial?
Van Iddekinge, C. H., Roth, P. L., Putka, D. J., & Lanivich, S. E. (2011). Are you interested?
Van Vugt, M., Hogan, R., & Kaiser, R. B. (2008). Leadership, followership, and evolution:
Wiggins, J. S. (Ed.). (1996). The five-factor model of personality. New York: Guilford.
model. In J. S. Wiggins (Ed.). The five-factor model of personality (pp. 88-162). New
York: Guilford.
Witt, L. A., & Ferris, G. R. (2003). Social skill as moderator of the conscientiousness-
Zinko, R., Ferris, G. R., Blass, F. R., & Laird, M. D. (2007). Toward a theory of reputation in
High
CONVENTIONAL TYPE
concern for orderliness and routines;
prf. for predictable demands and
specified standards
CONFORMITY
seeking for power; prf. for bargaining, prf. for practical activities, use of
leading, and manipulating others to machines, tools, and materials
attain personal or collective goals
ARTISTIC TYPE
preference for creative work in
music, writing, performance,
or unstructured intellectual
endeavors
Low
Figure 1