Language Learning - 2015 - Vandergrift - Learner Variables in Second Language Listening Comprehension An Exploratory Path
Language Learning - 2015 - Vandergrift - Learner Variables in Second Language Listening Comprehension An Exploratory Path
Language Learning - 2015 - Vandergrift - Learner Variables in Second Language Listening Comprehension An Exploratory Path
Listening comprehension plays a key role in language acquisition, yet little is known
about the variables that contribute to the development of second language (L2) lis-
tening ability. This study sought to obtain empirical evidence for the impact of some
of the learner variables and the degree to which they might predict success in L2 lis-
tening. The learner variables of interest included: first language (L1) listening ability,
L1 vocabulary knowledge, L2 vocabulary knowledge, auditory discrimination ability,
metacognitive awareness of listening, and working memory capacity. Data from 157
Grade Seven students in the first year of a French immersion program indicated a sig-
nificant relationship among most of the variables and L2 listening ability. A number
of path analyses were then conducted, based on hypothetical relationships suggested
by current theory and research, in order to uncover relationships between the variables
in determining L2 listening comprehension ability. The best fit to the data supported
a model in which general skills (auditory discrimination and working memory) are
initially important, leading to more specific language skills (L1 and L2 vocabulary) in
determining L2 listening comprehension. In positing a provisional model, this study
opens up useful avenues for further research on model building in L2 listening.
We are especially grateful to teacher Jodi Kathron who so graciously opened up her two Grade
Seven classrooms for research three years in a row. She also helped us facilitate work with the
teachers of other subjects so that one-on-one data collection for the vocabulary and working
memory tests could continue with the students throughout the school day. We are also grateful to
Cara Jelly and Lucille Aubin for their expert management of the data collection process, as well
as data collection; and to Simon DesRoberts and David Davidson who helped them collect the
data for most of the one-on-one tasks. Finally, we are thankful for the input of three anonymous
Language Learning reviewers whose thoughtful comments, probing questions, and recommended
revisions attest to the power of the peer review process to significantly improve a manuscript for
publication purposes. The elicitation instruments used for this study can be accessed by readers in
the IRIS digital repository (http://www.iris-database.org).
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to: Larry Vandergrift, University
of Ottawa—OLBI, 70 Laurier E Ottawa, Ontario K1N 6N5, Canada. E-mail: [email protected]
DOI: 10.1111/lang.12105
Vandergrift and Baker L2 Listening Comprehension
Introduction
Listening comprehension is a key component of language acquisition and an
important foundation for success in language immersion programs; yet little
is known about the listener characteristics that contribute to successful second
language (L2) listening comprehension. Given this clear gap in knowledge,
the primary objective of this study was to obtain empirical evidence for some
of the listener variables that might contribute to listening and the degree to
which these factors might predict success in L2 listening comprehension. A
more precise understanding of the learner variables involved in L2 listening
comprehension can potentially help to explain some of the difficulties faced by
L2 learners and, thereby, to inform listening instruction and potential remedia-
tion. The present study provides evidence for some of the learner variables that
might contribute to listening and proposes a provisional model that allows us
to observe how these variables might interact in L2 listening comprehension.
The theoretical motivation for this study is grounded in earlier research in
L2 reading, in particular, the body of research that began with an examination
of the relationships between L2 proficiency, L2 reading, and first language (L1)
reading. We present a brief overview of these areas and then link this body of
research with L2 listening, highlighting similarities and differences between
the comprehension processes underlying these two skills. We then review the
literature pertaining to the variables under investigation in the present study
and what we know about their relationship to L2 listening comprehension.
In order to determine the relationship between the variables of interest and
L2 listening comprehension ability, we first perform a correlational analysis.
Second, we perform a path analysis to test for theoretical causal relations
among the variables by determining if correlational data are consistent with the
imposed causal structure. We conclude with some pedagogical implications of
our findings.
L2 Listening Comprehension
For the purposes of this study, we operationalize L2 listening comprehension
the same way as Buck (2001, p. 114), that is, as
the ability to 1) process extended samples of realistic spoken language,
automatically and in real time; 2) understand the linguistic information
that is unequivocally included in the text; and, 3) make whatever
inferences are unambiguously implicated by the content of the passage.
This default construct is sufficiently flexible and broad to fit most contexts
and to allow listeners to demonstrate their comprehension ability in real-life
listening contexts.
As a comprehension process, listening shares many important characteris-
tics with reading (Bae & Bachman, 1998; Kintsch, 1998; Samuels, 1987). Both
require receptive language processing, which involves decoding and interpre-
tation. Both processes, therefore, use two basic knowledge sources: linguistic
knowledge (e.g., vocabulary and syntax) and world knowledge (e.g., topic, text
structure, schema, and culture) for purposes of text comprehension. Like read-
ing, listening also entails top-down and bottom-up processing to apply these
knowledge sources to the language input during comprehension. Both listening
and reading necessitate cognitive processing that is flexible and adaptable to
L1 Listening Comprehension
In an initial exploration of Alderson’s (1984) ability/proficiency question for
L2 listening, Vandergrift (2006a) examined the relative contributions of L1
listening and L2 proficiency to success in L2 listening comprehension with 75
English-speaking students of French. Results indicated that together both L1
(English) listening and L2 (French) proficiency accounted for about 39% of the
common variance. L2 proficiency was the stronger factor, accounting for about
25%. L1 listening accounted for about 14%, surprisingly similar to the results
for L1 in the reading studies. Based on these results, we can posit a contributing
role for L1 listening comprehension ability to L2 listening comprehension.
L1 Vocabulary Knowledge
To our knowledge, the role of L1 vocabulary knowledge in L2 listening com-
prehension has not yet been investigated. However, given the high number of
cognates (about 40,000) shared by French and English (Bragg, 2006), the po-
tential for transfer between the two languages is relatively strong. Ringbom
and Jarvis (2009) point out that the possibility of lexical transfer is particularly
high when (1) languages are related, (2) the word occurs in similar contexts,
(3) the focus is on comprehension, and, (4) learners are in early stages of lan-
guage learning. Given these factors, we can hypothesize a potential effect of
L1 vocabulary knowledge on the L2 listening comprehension of Anglophone
learners of French.
L2 Vocabulary Knowledge
The contribution of L2 vocabulary knowledge to L2 listening ability was
investigated by Mecartty (2000) in a study examining the relative contributions
of vocabulary and syntactic (grammar) knowledge to L2 listening and reading
comprehension. Whereas grammar knowledge failed to emerge as a predictor
for both skills, vocabulary knowledge did emerge as a significant predictor,
explaining about 25% of reading ability and about 14% of listening ability.
Metacognition
Metacognition refers to listener awareness of the cognitive processes involved
in comprehension, and the capacity to oversee, regulate, and direct these pro-
cesses (Vandergrift & Goh, 2012). Much of what we know about the relationship
between metacognition and successful L2 listening comes from research into
the strategies of skilled listeners. Using a think-aloud methodology (tapping the
thought processes of listeners while they are actually engaged in the listening
event) researchers record, transcribe, and analyze the think-alouds of skilled
and less-skilled listeners for evidence of strategy use (Goh, 2002; O’Malley,
Chamot, & Küpper, 1989; Vandergrift, 2003). Skilled listeners reveal using
Working Memory
The role of working memory is receiving increased attention, particularly as a
variable for explaining individual differences in L2 learning and use (Bloom-
field et al., 2011; Juffs & Harrington, 2011). Working memory involves the
temporary storage and manipulation of information used in complex cognitive
activities such as language processing (Baddeley, 1992). We adopted the mul-
ticomponent model proposed by Baddeley because it is considered the most
influential in second language acquisition (SLA) research (Juffs & Harrington,
2011). This model proposes a central executive component for planning, co-
ordinating the flow of information and retrieving knowledge from long-term
memory. The actual work, however, is carried out by two subsystems: a phono-
logical loop to retain spectral information about the sounds currently being
processed and a visuo-spacial sketchpad to hold nonverbal (visual and spatial)
information. A fourth component, an episodic buffer, was later added to ac-
count for integration of information from all the systems (episodes) for transfer
to long-term memory (Baddeley, 2003).
Of particular interest for listening research are the phonological loop and
the central executive, both limited in capacity. The phonological loop plays
an important role in retention and manipulation of speech; information can
be stored in phonological short-term memory for only so long until it decays
and is replaced by new information. The central executive plays an important
role in (1) controlling the flow of information between the components and
other cognitive processes and (2) maintaining focus and inhibiting distracting
information, behaviors crucial to listening success.
Auditory Discrimination
Although auditory discrimination, the ability to receive, differentiate and pro-
cess information through the ear, has been shown to correlate significantly with
L1 development (e.g., Tsao, Liu, & Kuhl, 2004), there is very little evidence
available regarding the relationship between auditory discrimination ability,
L2 listening ability, and language development. A recent study by Wilson,
Kaneko, Lyddon, Okamoto, and Ginsburg (2011) demonstrated significant cor-
relations between auditory discrimination and several different L2 proficiency
tests with Japanese learners of English. The researchers observed a moderate
Method
Participants
The 157 participants came from intact (late) FI classes, sampled from 3 different
cohorts reflecting the calendar year the particular students entered the program:
2008 25 22 47
2009 24 25 49
2010 29 32 61
Total 78 79 157
2008 (N = 47), 2009 (N = 49), and 2010 (N = 61). Late FI, in this school
jurisdiction, begins in Grade Seven, so participants were about 13 years old.
Late FI is a program of choice; that is, both students and parents agree to
enrolment. Proficiency in French is highly valued in the cultural context in
which this study took place since (French/English) bilingualism is considered
a distinct asset in securing employment in many work sectors and progressing
quickly within the ranks of the public service. Over 60% of the students in
the entire school jurisdiction are enrolled in FI at some point. There are three
different entry points at which students can begin FI instruction. Late immersion
(i.e., beginning in Grade Seven) is the last opportunity for students to begin
a FI program, and it largely enrolls students who have moved into the school
jurisdiction from other parts of Canada, immigrant children who now feel
comfortable in learning a third language (or more) and students who began FI
at an earlier entry point (Kindergarten or Grade Four) but dropped out and now
wish to try it again. All instruction is in French, except for Mathematics.
All three cohorts were taught by the same teacher and data collection took
place near the end of the school year. Although no L2 proficiency measure
was administered, it would be fair to assume that the language proficiency
attained by the students at this point in their FI program would be at the A2
level (Common European Framework of Languages, Council of Europe, 2001)
but definitely not yet at the B1 level. This is roughly the equivalent to the
Intermediate Mid-High ranges in the ACTFL Guidelines often employed in the
United States (Vandergrift, 2006b). Given that the L1 listening test was given in
English, students who did not complete all their schooling in English (Grades
One to Six) were excluded from the participant group. Students were deemed
to have English as L1 if they were either born in Canada or if they completed
all their schooling in English; that is, developing conceptual knowledge in this
language. With regard to gender, as can be seen in Table 1, the distribution
between males and females was virtually equal.
Instruments
The data for the present study were collected using seven different instruments
to measure the cognitive variables of interest: L1 listening comprehension,
L2 listening comprehension, L1 vocabulary knowledge, L2 vocabulary knowl-
edge, auditory discrimination ability, metacognitive awareness of listening, and
working memory capacity.
Questions for both tests followed a similar pattern, as seen in the following
example:
Metacognition
Metacognitive awareness of listening was measured using the MALQ (Vander-
grift et al., 2006), an instrument previously validated with adults and secondary
school students. The MALQ, a self-report instrument, consists of 21 randomly
ordered items related to L2 listening comprehension. The items measure the
perceived use of the strategies and processes underlying five factors related to
the regulation of L2 listening comprehension: Problem-solving, Planning and
Evaluation, Mental Translation, Person Knowledge, and Directed Attention.
Participants respond using a Likert scale from 1 to 6, with 6 signifying full
agreement with the item. For example, the following statement is one of four
items tapping the factor of Directed Attention:
I try to get back on track when I lose concentration.
A global score was created for each participant by adding up the scores for each
of the 21 items. Cronbach’s alpha for internal consistency of this scale was .62.
Validation data for the MALQ revealed internal reliability coefficients ranging
from .74 to .78 for four of the factors and .68 for the fifth (Directed Attention)
factor (Vandergrift et al., 2006).
Working Memory
Working memory capacity was measured using the Working Memory Test Bat-
tery for Children (WMTB-C; Pickering & Gathercole, 2001), an instrument
normed and validated for 5-to 15-year-olds. Due to time constraints related to
the one-on-one nature of the subtests, only the Backward Digit Recall (BDR;
tapping the work of both the Central Executive and the Phonological Loop)
and the Nonword List Recall (NLR, tapping the Phonological Loop only) sub-
tests were administered. For the BDR, participants must maintain a forward
sequence of digits while recalling them in reverse order (last one first), imme-
diately after spoken presentation of the sequence by the research assistant. The
test progresses from presentation of a two-digit span to as many as seven. A
score is established by adding up the number of correct items in each block of
six. The test ends once the participant makes three errors in a given block. For
the NLR, participants must recall a sequence of one-syllable sounds in exactly
the same order in which it was heard. It progresses in much the same way
as the BDR and also stops once the participant makes more than three errors
in a block. The BDR subtest, in particular, is widely used in working memory
research (Juffs & Harrington, 2011; Kormos & Sáfár, 2008). By using digits
and nonsense one-syllable words, both subtests avoid the problem of using an
overt meaningful language component. Both are appropriate tests of working
memory capacity because they avoid the possibility of covert rehearsal by en-
gaging both the storage and processing components as participants execute the
task (Juffs & Harrington, 2011). Scores of the two sub-tests were combined
to generate one score to measure the Working Memory variable. Historical
internal reliability coefficients for the WMTB were not available.
Procedure
Participation was voluntary and both parents and students signed the consent
form. All participants completed a short demographic questionnaire requesting
information concerning previous French instruction, language(s) spoken at
home and language of schooling from senior kindergarten to the present. The
listening tests, auditory ability subtests and metacognition questionnaire were
administered to each intact class. The vocabulary and working memory tests
were conducted one-on-one with a research assistant outside the classroom.
Results
The first goal of this study was to examine the relationship between L2 listen-
ing comprehension and some important learner variables (L1 listening com-
prehension, L1 and L2 vocabulary, auditory discrimination, working memory,
and metacognition). The means, standard deviations and minimum/maximum
scores for each variable are provided in Table 2. The second goal was to explore
the relationships among the variables through a path analysis test in order to
arrive at a model of how these variables might interact in leading to L2 listening
comprehension. A path analysis is essentially a regression procedure where the
Table 2 Means, standard deviations and minimum/maximum scores for the listening
variables
.21 .28
L1 Vocabulary
Auditory Working L2 L2
Discrimination Memory Vocabulary Listening
Metacognition .49
.42 ns
.29
and metacognition) are significant. Thus, the correlational results for these
grade-seven late FI students generally indicate that increases in the variables of
interest are associated with increases in L2 listening comprehension, with the
strongest and most consistent association being with L2 vocabulary.
The positive relationships, however, do not imply causation. For that reason,
the data were further explored for a potential causal relationship between the
predictor variables and L2 listening comprehension. In particular, we wanted
to test for a model that could encapsulate the relationship among the predictor
variables, in order to gain greater insight as to how these variables might interact
in the process leading to L2 listening comprehension.
Table 4 Goodness of fit statistics related to the provisional L2 Listening Comprehension
Model
The results suggested removing the nonsignificant path from working mem-
ory to metacognition. When the nonsignificant path was dropped, there was no
significant improvement to the fit of the model, X2 (1) = 0.05, p = ns. The final
solution suggested no further deletion or addition. Although the relationships
are only weak to moderate, the final version depicts auditory discrimination
positively influencing working memory, which positively impacts L1 vocabu-
lary (but not metacognition). Both L1 vocabulary and metacognition positively
influence L2 listening comprehension through L2 vocabulary, which is a di-
rect precursor to L2 listening comprehension. Thus, the final result suggests a
(provisional) model where more general skills like auditory discrimination and
working memory are important to the development of more specific language
skills leading to L2 listening comprehension. As mentioned above, L1 listen-
ing was not included in the analysis because of the limited sample size for this
variable.
Discussion
Variables Related to L2 Listening Comprehension
The first goal of this study was to explore the relationship between L2 listening
comprehension and the cognitive learner variables chosen for this investigation.
All variables were positively related to L2 listening ability. However, it is
somewhat perplexing that the results of the third cohort do not echo the more
positive results of the first and second cohorts, even though all three simply
represent the same student population sampled at three different calendar times,
2008, 2009, and 2010 but the same curricular time: the beginning of late FI in
grade seven. Even though data collection for the third cohort was directed by a
different research assistant, a careful review of the data collection protocol with
the assistant and the classroom teacher (the same person for all three cohorts)
revealed no discrepancies. Compared to the other two cohorts, the variance in
working memory was slightly higher in the third cohort, while the variance
for L1 vocabulary was lower. Considering the high consistency in correlations
across the first two cohorts, the differences in the third cohort may simply
represent an anomaly. Although the coefficients are smaller, the nature of the
relationships between the cognitive variables and L2 listening comprehension
is the same as the other two cohorts.
The robust role of L2 vocabulary in L2 listening comprehension is cer-
tainly the most significant finding. This reinforces the results obtained by
Staehr (2009) who found a very high correlation between listening success
and vocabulary size (r = .70). Staehr observes, however, that the measurement
tool he used assessed the written form of the word only, noting that the relation-
ship between vocabulary and listening should be based on a measurement tool
that involves an auditory presentation (hearing) of a word rather than a visual
presentation (reading) of its orthographic form. The present study has compen-
sated for this shortcoming by using a measure of oral receptive vocabulary. At
the same time, it should be noted that recognition of an individual word out of
context, as measured by the PPVT, does not necessarily mean that the word is
recognized in rapid, concatenated speech. Nevertheless, recognition of the oral
form of a word is a more valid measurement of word recognition for purposes
of listening, rather than reading and understanding the word.
The significant finding for L2 vocabulary lends credence to the possibility
of a threshold for L2 listening, similar to an L2 reading threshold. A threshold
for L2 listening assumes a relationship between L2 vocabulary knowledge
and listening comprehension success; listeners need to attain a certain level
of vocabulary knowledge before they can efficiently transfer L1 skills to L2
listening tasks. Although there is some evidence for a threshold for L2 reading
(Bossers, 1991; Lee & Shallert, 1997; Schoonen et al., 1998), the existence
of a threshold for L2 listening remains to be explored. The results of the
present study provide further motivation for exploring the possibility of such a
threshold.
Our results provide support for the hypothesis that L1 vocabulary is posi-
tively associated with L2 listening comprehension success. Given the overall
strong relationship between English and French vocabulary, it is not surprising
that these listeners, who are in the early stages of learning French, can transfer
this L1 knowledge to facilitate their L2 listening efforts. However, this finding
needs to be explored for other languages as well, particularly for languages that
are not very cognate rich such as, for example, Chinese and English.
The relationship between L1 listening ability and L2 listening comprehen-
sion is somewhat tenuous in this study. We could not include L1 listening ability
in the first cohort due to the lack of reliability of the measurement instrument
used with that group. There was a moderate relationship with L2 listening
ability in the second cohort, but this relationship was weak in the third cohort.
Thus, there is not enough evidence in this study to confirm earlier findings
on the potential contribution of L1 listening ability to L2 listening success
(Vandergrift, 2006a).
Our proposition about the potential role of auditory discrimination appears
to be confirmed for the overall participant group. This finding supports the
significant role for auditory discrimination observed by Wilson et al. (2011).
Based on the strength of the relationship of auditory discrimination with L2
single study, we can then explore any possible interaction effects between those
variables.
skills (L1 word recognition in particular) could explain up to 58% of the variance
in L2 proficiency. They concluded that L1 word recognition formed the basis
of L2 learning. Although the path to L2 vocabulary in our model is relatively
weak (.28), this suggests some evidence of L1 vocabulary influence.
Whether working memory impacts L2 listening ability only through L1 and
L2 vocabulary is still a matter of speculation. The path in our model is relatively
weak (.21) and a possible, additional path to metacognition was not significant.
Temporary storage, manipulation, and coordination of information involved
in problem solving, which all happens in working memory, should influence
the awareness of cognitive processes. However, our model appears to suggest
that metacognition is an executive function that, on its own, is an important
determinant of L2 vocabulary and L2 listening comprehension, highlighting the
importance of knowledge and control of the L2 listening process on L2 listening
ability. Nevertheless, the lack of a significant path between metacognition and
working memory remains puzzling. Another explanation may be a function of
the instrument used to measure metacognition. Whereas the instruments used
to measure the other variables provided actual performance data, the MALQ
asked listeners to self-report their awareness of listening processes. Given that
the data for metacognition was not based on actual performance, these data
may not represent actual metacognitive activity, resulting in a nonsignificant
path from working memory. By the same token, the path from metacognition
to L2 vocabulary could be either more or less positively related.
L2 vocabulary appears to be influenced by separate, but relatively weak,
paths from L1 vocabulary (.28) and metacognition (.29). The cognates argument
discussed earlier may explain the path from L1 to L2 vocabulary. The role of
metacognition is not totally clear, however. What may be operating here is that
listener metacognition triggers the inference of unknown vocabulary, based on
knowledge sources such as L1, other L2 knowledge, and world knowledge (Lee
& Cai, 2010). Finally, L2 vocabulary leads to L2 listening via a moderately
strong path (.49), reflecting the strength of the relationship in the correlational
analysis.
In sum, the most important contribution of this provisional model is the
proposition that general skills influence more specific language skills. This
evokes the role of aptitude, an important construct in examining what is involved
in the process of language learning and elucidating the nature of that process.
After falling out of favor for a few decades, the role of language aptitude in SLA
is receiving increased interest, with a greater emphasis on the interplay between
aptitude and context (Dörnyei, 2005). Although there is more debate today about
the components of aptitude, DeKeyser and Koeth (2011) suggest that most
cognitive psychologists today would agree that analytical ability (verbal ability
and general intelligence), memory, and phonetic sensitivity would be deemed
the most important. Although we did not measure general intelligence in the
present study, results from our measures for memory and phonetic sensitivity led
to the inclusion of both auditory discrimination and working memory as initial
determinants in our model. In short, our provisional model suggests aptitude as
a precursor to language-specific skills in L2 listening comprehension success.
Conclusion
Claims to be made in this study are limited by the variables under investigation.
First, some learner variables, such as background knowledge and particularly
topic familiarity, which have a discernible role in L2 listening comprehension
success (see, e.g., Leeser, 2004; Long, 1990), were not included in this study.
Other potentially significant learner variables such as reasoning ability, as
measured by IQ (Andringa et al., 2012), were not included. Second, our results
are limited to the languages and proficiency level under investigation, that is,
French learned by students in an English-speaking school context and their
level of L2 proficiency. Results for languages that are more distant would likely
be different as would the results for more advanced proficiency levels. Third,
our results are limited to the age group examined; results for adults, who have
much more life experience or language learning experience, might be quite
different. Finally, due to the lack of sufficient data for the first cohort, the role of
L1 could not be included in the analysis and, given the low reliability of the
initial L1 listening test, future research using a more reliable test (such as the
one used with the second and third cohorts) will be needed; specifically, a test
would be desirable that matches the task- and question-types of the French test.
Although provisional, our model has implications for pedagogy. First of all,
the role of L2 vocabulary listening comprehension achievement is important
information for teachers. Time spent in L2 vocabulary development should bear
fruit and research has established several useful guidelines for the teaching
of vocabulary and efficient learning (e.g., Nation, 2001; 2013). Second, the
nascent role of auditory discrimination in L2 listening success suggests that
some attention to consciousness-raising at the sound segment level would be
useful, particularly with learners at lower levels of language proficiency (e.g.,
Wilson et al., 2011). Third, the critical role of both L1 and L2 vocabulary
suggests that an emphasis of overall literacy development in both languages
can foster L2 listening success (e.g., Dufva &Voeten, 1999; Hulstijn, 2011).
Finally, given that metacognition appears to play a determining role in listening
References
Alderson, J. (1984). Reading in a foreign language: A reading problem or language
problem? In J. Alderson & A. Urquhart (Eds.), Reading in a foreign language
(pp. 1–24). New York: Longman.
Andringa, S., Olsthoorn, N., van Beuningen, C., Schoonen, R., & Hulstijn, J. (2012).
Determinants of success in native and non-native listening comprehension: An
individual differences approach. Language Learning, 62(S2), 49–78.
Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255, 556–559.
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of
Communication Disorders, 36, 189–208.
Bae, J., & Bachman, L. (1998). A latent variable approach to listening and reading:
Testing factorial invariance across two groups of children in the Korean/English
two-way immersion program. Language Testing, 15, 380–414.
Bentler, P.M. (1998). EQS: Structural equations program manual. Encino, CA:
Multivariate Software.
Bernhardt, E. (2005). Progress and procrastination in second language reading. Annual
Review of Applied Linguistics, 25, 133–150.
Bernhardt, E. B., & Kamil, M. L. (1995). Interpreting relationships between L1 and L2
reading: Consolidating the linguistic threshold and the linguistic interdependence
hypotheses. Applied Linguistics, 16, 16–34.
Bloomfield, A., Wayland, S. C., Rhoades, E., Blodgett, A., Linck, J., & Ross, S.
(2011). What makes listening difficult? Factors affecting second language listening
comprehension. College Park: University of Maryland.
Bonk, W. (2000). Second language lexical knowledge and listening comprehension.
International Journal of Listening, 14, 14–31.
Bossers, B. (1991). On threshold, ceilings, and short-circuits: The relation between L1
reading, L2 reading and L2 knowledge. AILA Review, 8, 45–60.
Bragg, M. (2006). The adventure of English: The biography of a language. New York:
Arcade Publishing.
Brunfaut, T., & Revesz, A. (2013). Text characteristics of task input and difficulty in
second language listening comprehension. Studies in Second Language Acquisition,
35, 31–65.
Buck, G. (2001). Assessing listening. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Carrell, P. (1991). Second language reading: Reading ability or language proficiency?
Applied Linguistics, 12, 159–179.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Council of Europe. (2001). A common European framework of reference for languages:
Learning, teaching, assessment. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
DeKeyser, R. M., & Koeth, J. (2011). Cognitive aptitudes for second language
learning. In E. Hinkel (Ed.), Handbook of research in second language teaching and
learning, 2 (pp. 395–406). New York: Routledge.
Des Brisay, M. (1995). Practical considerations in the construction of program specific
ESL tests: The CanTEST story. In R. Courchêne, S. Burger, C. Cornaire, R.
LeBlanc, S. Paribakht, & H. Séguin (Eds.), Twenty-five years of second language
teaching at the University of Ottawa (pp. 260–268). Ottawa, Canada: Second
Language Institute.
Dörnyei, Z. (2005). The psychology of the language learner: Individual differences in
second language acquisition. New York: Routledge.
Dufva, M., & Voeten, M. J. (1999). Native language literacy and phonological memory
as prerequisites for learning English as a foreign language. Applied
psycholinguistics, 20, 329–348.
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (2007). Peabody picture vocabulary test (4th ed.).
Minneapolis, MN: Pearson.
Dunn, L. M., Theriault-Whalen, C. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1993). Adaptation française
du Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-revised. Paris: Éditions du Centre de
Psychologie Appliquée.
Field, J. (2003). Promoting perception: Lexical segmentation in second language
listening. ELT Journal, 57, 325–334.
Goh, C. (2002). Exploring listening comprehension tactics and their interaction
patterns. System, 30, 185–206.
Vandergrift, L., & Tafaghodtari, M. H. (2010). Teaching learners how to listen does
make a difference: An empirical study. Language Learning, 65, 470–497.
Wesche, M., Peters, M., & MacFarlane, A. (1994). The “Bain linguistique”: A core
French experiment. Ottawa, Canada: Curriculum Services Department, Ottawa
Board of Education.
Wilson, I., Kaneko, E., Lyddon, P., Okamoto, K., & Ginsburg, J. (2011).
Nonsense-syllable sound discrimination ability correlates with second language
(L2) proficiency. In W.-S. Lee & E. Zee (Eds.), Proceedings of the 17th
International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 2133–2136). Hong Kong: City
University of Hong Kong.