The Social and Psychological Characteristics of No

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 26

Journal of Social and Political Psychology

jspp.psychopen.eu | 2195-3325

Original Research Reports

The Social and Psychological Characteristics of Norm Deviants: A Field


Study in a Small Cohesive University Campus
a ab
Robin Gomila* , Elizabeth Levy Paluck
[a] Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, USA. [b] Woodrow Wilson School of Public and International Affairs,
Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, USA.

Abstract
People who deviate from the established norms of their social group can clarify group boundaries, strengthen group cohesion,
and catalyze group and broader social change. Yet social psychologists have recently neglected the study of deviants. We
conducted in-depth interviews of Princeton University upperclassmen who deviated from a historical and widely known Princeton
norm: joining an “eating club,” a social group that undergraduates join at the end of their sophomore year. We explored the
themes of these interviews with two rounds of surveys during the semester when students decide whether to join an eating
club (pilot survey, N = 408; and a random subsample of the pilot survey with 90% takeup, N = 212). The surveys asked: what
are the social and psychological antecedents of deviance from norms? The data suggest that deviance is a pattern: compared
to those who conform, students who deviate by not joining clubs report a history of deviance and of feeling different from the
typical member of their social group. They also feel less social belonging and identification with Princeton and its social
environment. Students who deviate are lower in self-monitoring, but otherwise are comparable to students who conform in
terms of personality traits measured by the Big Five, and of their perception of the self as socially awkward, independent, or
rebellious. While some of these findings replicate past research, worth further exploration is the role of previous experience
with deviance and its meaning for individuals as they decide whether to deviate.

Keywords: social norms, deviance, social change, reference groups, field research

Journal of Social and Political Psychology, 2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245, https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134
Received: 2019-01-11. Accepted: 2020-01-29. Published (VoR): 2020-02-28.
Handling Editor: Hélder Alves, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
*Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, Princeton University, 424 Peretsman-Scully Hall, Princeton, NJ, 08544, USA. E-mail:
[email protected]
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License
(https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided
the original work is properly cited.

Modern social psychology emerged as a field interested in the study of the forces that lead people to conform:
forces like social norms, social roles, or authority figures. Social norms, or a person’s impression of the average
or desirable attitudes and behavior of a group (Miller & Prentice, 1996), can explain a broad range of behavioral
conformity within a group or community (Miller & Prentice, 2016; Prentice, 2012). To the question of what leads
individuals to conform to norms, social psychologists have well-established answers: people are motivated to
preserve their sense of belonging to their social groups, avoid social rejection, and feel that they have an accurate
grasp of social facts (Blanton & Christie, 2003; Cialdini & Goldstein, 2004).
Gomila & Paluck 221

Although social forces do shape human behavior, individuals often exercise agency, and their behavior is sometimes
at odds with social expectations (Swann & Jetten, 2017). Perhaps surprisingly, social psychology has focused
much less on the determinants of social norm deviance, defined as behavior that transgresses established group
norms (Marques, Yzerbyt, & Leyens, 1988). Monin and O’Connor (2011), for instance, highlighted that because
“less work documents why deviants end up in this situation in the first place, deviance remains a psychological
question mark” (p. 264).

Deviants play important roles in social groups, so much so that scholars have long argued that healthy societies
need deviants (Durkheim, 1938; Erikson, 2005). Deviants can, on the one hand, clarify the group’s boundaries,
improve the group’s decisions, and strengthen cohesion among group members; on the other hand, they can
disrupt existing norms and catalyze change (Dreu, 2002; Hornsey, 2006; Moscovici, 1976; Nemeth, Brown, &
Rogers, 2001). Sherif and Sherif (1972) highlighted: “departure from established, accustomed or expected ways
of behaving is a prominent feature of human historical change” (Sherif & Sherif, 1972, p. 4).

When social psychologists have studied deviance, they have not explored the antecedents of deviance. Instead,
their studies have examined what happens once deviance occurs—for the individuals who deviate, and for their
groups. Past research has demonstrated, for instance, that deviants are subsequently perceived as “black sheep”,
marginalized, and stigmatized (Brewer & Pickett, 1999; Crocker, Major, & Steele, 1998; Festinger, 1950; Marques,
Yzerbyt, & Leyens, 1988; Moscovici, 1994). Additionally, deviating from a norm leads to being stereotyped, iden-
tifying less with the group, and negatively impacts the deviant’s self-esteem and well-being (Jetten, Branscombe,
& Spears, 2002; Jetten, Spears, & Manstead, 1997; Link & Phelan, 2001; Major & O’Brien, 2005).

In this paper, we revisit questions about the social and psychological antecedents of deviance from peer influence
and social norms. Using a field study in a university context with a cohesive student body and an established social
norm, we ask: who deviates, and why? Specifically, we designed a series of studies that are inductive and quali-
tative examinations of real-world deviance from a century-old, widely-followed social norm at Princeton University
i
–joining an eating club . We followed qualitative interviews with repeated surveys using representative sampling
to assess the robustness of the findings from the qualitative interviews. This approach is in line with a pragmatist
philosophy of science, which “urges scientists to observe what behaviors emerge in the complexity of real life”
(Gantman et al., 2018, p. 18; James, 1907), to describe the phenomenon at play, and to formulate hypotheses
about the regularities surrounding the phenomenon (Rozin, 2001). The work is observational, and intended to be
generative of future hypothesis-testing and theory building about the causes of everyday behavioral deviance.

We designed our interviews and formulated early hypotheses about the potential drivers of deviance by drawing
on a wide literature from social psychology and sociology, which suggests that deviance is driven by both individ-
ual and contextual factors. Contextual factors that drive deviance may be very broad, as in the time period and
culture (e.g., Swidler, 1986), or more local, as in the types of identity groups that surround an individual (e.g.,
Monin & O’Connor, 2011). Individual difference factors may include a person’s level of identification with their
group (Packer & Chasteen, 2010), personality traits (e.g., Snyder, 1979), or moral beliefs (Monin & O’Connor,
2011).

Contextual Factors That May Drive Deviance


Predicting which members of a given group will deviate from an established social norm may depend on a set of
contextual factors. Social norms interact with the institutions, ideologies, symbols, and material products that to-

Journal of Social and Political Psychology


2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245
https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134
Deviance From Social Norms: Who Are the Deviants? 222

gether form a group’s culture, broadly defined as “a system of meaning linked to a set of available behavioral
practices” (Paluck & Green, 2009, p. 623; Sewell, 2005). Few perspectives in the psychology of deviance take
seriously the contextual factor of culture, although a rich literature exists in sociology. For example, Swidler (1986)
differentiates between settled and unsettled cultural periods when studying the power of social norms over behavior.

In settled times, norms are strong, injunctive, and privately endorsed by most ingroup members; few or no resources
are available for those who may consider deviating. Swidler (1986) predicted and found through case study evidence
that deviation from social norms may be nonexistent or extremely rare during settled times. Ingroup members
may conform to social norms against their will, and underestimate the probability that others hold similar attitudes
against the norm. This phenomenon, studied in psychology as pluralistic ignorance, constitutes a major obstacle
to social change in groups (Miller & Prentice, 1996; Prentice & Miller, 1996). Swidler’s (1986) research suggests
that, because contextual factors are strongly in favor of status quo norms, deviance in settled times is more ex-
traordinary.

By contrast, in unsettled times there is a greater opening for deviance, due to the emergence of structural oppor-
tunities for action against norms. In times of unsettledness or “periods of social transformation” (Swidler, 1986,
p. 278), resources emerge for those who hold attitudes against the norm. This perspective suggests that unsettled
cultural periods may shake perceptions of pluralistic ignorance and facilitate deviance. Deviance may be more
frequent during unsettled times, and may not need to involve individuals with extreme levels of moral resolve, or
defiant stances toward their social groups.

Research on who deviates in settled times is a crucial component of our understanding of deviance. However, in
order to better understand deviance as a broad and more ordinary phenomenon, it is also crucial to study deviance
in less settled periods, when change is afoot or resources are available for change. Less research has examined
deviance in these cultural periods.

Some psychological researchers have theorized about contextual drivers of deviance, including van Kleef, Wanders,
Stamkou, and Homan (2015) whose research demonstrates that high power predicts deviance, or Monin and
O’Connor (2011) in their work on defiance who categorize deviance according to the intention of the actor. In this
framework, when one deviates unintentionally, deviance constitutes a status. For example, people may uninten-
tionally deviate because they are forced by contextual factors like job loss or living in a foreign country. This research
helpfully points out that deviance is not always chosen by an individual, in contrast to deviance as a choice, which
is the subject of interest for this paper.

Individual Factors That May Drive Deviance


When individuals have the means and ability to conform and make the intentional decision to deviate, deviance
constitutes a stance (Monin & O’Connor, 2011). Social psychologists have theorized that people may consciously
decide to deviate from a norm because they consider it wrong, because they feel above it, or because they want
to be at odds with it (Monin & O’Connor, 2011; Morrison & Miller, 2012).

Psychologists studying dissent, which may or may not include dissent from social norms, have suggested other
individual differences may drive deviance—in particular, levels of identification with an ingroup. On average, ingroup
members who identify weakly with their group are more likely to voice out minority opinions (Hornsey, Majkut,
Terry, & McKimmie, 2003). Ingroup members who strongly identify with their group may publicly criticize a social

Journal of Social and Political Psychology


2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245
https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134
Gomila & Paluck 223

norm, as long as they believe that the norm may be harmful for the group. On the contrary, when low identifiers
notice that a norm may be harmful for the group, they tend to remain silent or disengage (Packer, 2012)

Psychologists have also tested whether certain personality traits predict deviance. The theory of self-monitoring
(Snyder, 1979), on the one hand, considers the existence of enduring individual differences in the extent to which
individuals regulate their behavior based on the expectations of their social context, with high self-monitors regu-
lating more than low self-monitors (Lennox & Wolfe, 1984; Snyder, 1979). On the other hand, some studies have
found associations between deviance and some of the Big Five personality traits. For instance, a study by Bodankin
and Tziner (2009) suggests a relationship between deviance in the workplace, and traits such as agreeableness,
neuroticism and conscientiousness. In their study, Bodankin and Tziner (2009) distinguish between constructive
and destructive deviance, and suggest that the direction of the relationship between these personality traits and
deviance depends on the norm at play. As described in greater length below, we study a norm that is essentially
social. Therefore, we expected negative relationships between deviance and the personality traits typically asso-
ciated with enhanced social lives: agreeableness and extraversion.

Much research remains to be done in order to understand what leads individuals to choose to deviate from social
norms. Accumulated research suggests that some individual differences matter, such as a person’s group identi-
fication or personality traits (Kirsch, Lubart, & Houssemand, 2015; Packer, 2012). Most of the research on individ-
ual differences has been conducted in the laboratory. It will be interesting to note whether these relationships hold
in a real-world context regarding a consequential decision about deviation. Further, it is impossible to know what
types of predictors we may be missing as a result of not studying these types of decisions in the world. Monroe’s
(2008) work on heroes who broke with norms to save lives during genocide is a notable exception to this rule, but
also may not inform theories of more ordinary, regular occurrences of deviation in the world.

Finally, while we are interested in the characteristics of individuals who choose to deviate, theory proposes that
many individuals are marked as deviants even though they did not choose to deviate in any meaningful behavioral
manner (Monin & O’Connor, 2011). These individuals may perceive themselves as different from others because
of the words or actions of other people, rather than a choice they initiated. The relationship between feeling different
from other people and deviating by choice is of interest. For example, individuals who choose to deviate may end
up feeling different from others and as a result, be more likely to deviate again. Or more simply, deviance may
become a pattern composed of a history of feeling different and acts of deviance by choice.

The Present Research


The Eating Club Norm at Princeton
For the past century, the great majority of Princeton undergraduate students have joined Princeton’s eating clubs,
which are independent and private institutions where students have meals and parties. To quote an undergraduate
student, becoming a member of one of the eleven eating clubs has been considered a “rite of passage.” In short,
belonging to an eating club at Princeton is a social norm.

Technically, any Princeton student who wants to conform to this social norm by joining an eating club has the
means and ability to become a member. Although the clubs are private institutions, Princeton increases students’
financial aid packages to assist students to join, in recognition of the eating club’s importance to undergraduate
life. Furthermore, while some clubs are selective (“bicker clubs”), others (“sign-in clubs”) will accept any under-

Journal of Social and Political Psychology


2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245
https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134
Deviance From Social Norms: Who Are the Deviants? 224

graduates who decide to join. In the spring of their sophomore year, all Princeton students make a choice to
conform to the eating club norm by becoming a member, or to deviate by becoming an independent.

Historically the eating club norm could be characterized as part of a settled culture at Princeton (Swidler, 1986),
because almost all students belonged and projected the idea that one should belong, as a Princeton student
(characterizing a strong descriptive and injunctive norm, respectively). Also, few or no resources were available
for those who would consider deviating, such as alternative cafeterias or kitchens in dorm rooms.

Today, however, the eating club norm might be characterized as part of a more unsettled culture at Princeton.
Fewer students are joining eating clubs, in part because the University is providing more alternatives for eating
and socializing on the campus including co-ops, alternative eating groups where students cook and eat together.
As a consequence, the norm is perceived “on the decline” by Princeton undergraduates, which is reflected in recent
news articles about Princeton eating clubs, stating that eating clubs Presidents “are combatting declining mem-
bership” (Shashkini, 2018). This is also illustrated in the survey data that we collected in Spring 2017 from 408
Princeton students: the eating club norm is perceived by the students, on average, as weaker than it actually is.
We asked the participants to estimate the percentage of eating club members on campus, and although the real-
ity is that 73% of the upperclassmen were members in 2016 (L. Schmucki, personal communication, September
7, 2019), participants’ median and the mean response were approximately 10 percentage points lower (median
= 65%, mean = 63%). Three quarters of all participants estimated that less than 70% of all students were eating
club members (Figure 1).

Figure 1. Density of the students’ estimate of the proportion of eating club members. The vertical dashed line represents the
actual proportion of eating club members on campus.

Journal of Social and Political Psychology


2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245
https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134
Gomila & Paluck 225

Additionally, the norm of joining clubs is explicitly and publicly debated, as demonstrated by a recent referendum
students held on the topic of whether students should have to “bicker” to gain entry to the club. The referendum
did not pass, but it was notable as the first time in the history of Princeton that the tradition was up for debate.
Statistics released by the eating clubs show that the proportion of sophomores who participated in the admission
process between 2013 and 2016 nonetheless remained stable: between 79% and 81% of sophomores took part
in the process each year.

Thus, the current climate at Princeton provides an excellent opportunity to study deviants, because they are
growing in number even though the norm remains relatively strong, clear, and tied to the Princeton student iden-
tity. However, while the choice to become a deviant has grown more common, the experience could not be char-
acterized as an easy one. While some independent students join an alternative eating group, for the most part
students who deviate are not automatically part of an alternative social group for eating or socializing.

To conclude, Princeton has recently become an unsettled environment in relation to Eating Club, which provides
us with the unique opportunity to study a larger sample of deviants at a time where the norm is clear, but debated.
In this context, we take the opportunity to study: who deviates? What guides their choice to not join an eating
club? Although some of the reasons why students decide not to join eating clubs may be specific to eating clubs
themselves, this context constitutes a unique opportunity to study a reasonable sample of deviants (over 30% of
the sophomore class), and provides an opportunity to derive more general lessons about the types of contexts
that give rise to a decision to stray from the norm and the types of people who make those decisions.

Overview of the Studies


In this paper, we integrate qualitative and quantitative research to understand who deviates from a well-established
social norm in a real-world setting. We ask: who deviates? How do they perceive their choice to deviate, how do
they identify with their group, and how do they describe themselves and their history of deviation or conformity?

We first conducted in-depth interviews with all Princeton upperclassmen who responded to an advertisement to
talk about their decision to join or not join an eating club at Princeton. The first author used a semi-structured in-
terview approach, in which general questions guided the conversation but did not attempt to test hypotheses. The
purpose of the interview was to elicit students’ open-ended thoughts about their decision, their views of Princeton,
their identities and previous experiences. We analyzed the general themes of these interviews, and used them
to construct a pilot survey that targeted all Princeton sophomores as they considered whether to join an eating
club. The objective of the pilot survey was twofold: first, identify the population of sophomore students who fore-
casted that they would deviate or conform to the eating club norm, and second, test the survey questions and
scales that we designed after conducting the interviews on a larger sample of Princeton students. Following this,
we conducted the main survey, for which we sampled a representative subset of the original sample, balanced
by students who forecast that they would deviate vs. conform to the eating club norm. The main survey took place
at the end of the students’ semester, after they had been living with their decision to deviate or conform.

Journal of Social and Political Psychology


2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245
https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134
Deviance From Social Norms: Who Are the Deviants? 226

Study 1: Qualitative and Quantitative Data Collections


on Students Who Decided to Deviate vs. Conform

Study 1a: Interviews of Deviant and Conformist Students


Method
Semi-structured interview approach — The objective of this set of interviews was to gain a better understanding
of the population of interest and learn about Princeton students’ social life. Specifically, interviews with students
who did or did not deviate from the eating club norm allowed us to gather crucial insights from the students about
their perception of the clubs, the context of their decision to join or not join, and their lives more generally.

Recruitment — Since the objective of the interviews was to understand the reasons why a minority of students
on campus decide to deviate from the eating club norm, we primarily recruited students who decided not to join
an eating club. We also interviewed some eating club members in order to get a sense of the common reasons
why students join the clubs. We recruited independents and eating club members separately, by advertising the
study to different Princeton undergraduate mailing lists (see recruitment materials in Appendix A in the Supple-
mentary Online Materials [SOM]). The interviews lasted for 30 minutes on average, and participants were paid at
a rate of $12 per hour for their time.

We interviewed a total of 26 Princeton upperclassmen (8 juniors and 18 seniors). Overall, the students that we
interviewed covered 16 different departments and majors (e.g., History of Science, Engineering, Psychology &
Neuroscience, Journalism, French, Economics, and Computer Science). Interviewees were White (46%), Asian
(42%), Black (7%) and Hispanic (3%). The great majority of the interviewees were women; only 1 interviewee
identified as a man.

Procedure — We set up meetings in the psychology department with the juniors and seniors who agreed to
participate. Interviews were semi-structured and conducted by the first author. The interviewer had a pre-determined
set of open questions to ask to the participants, but allowed himself to explore some particular themes or responses
whenever something relevant came up in the conversation. The interviews were not recorded; the interviewer
took written notes during the session that he later typed.

We asked open questions to the independent interviewees about what it feels like to be an independent at
Princeton, the reasons why they decided to go independent, their perception of eating clubs and the eating club
norm at Princeton, and their identification with Princeton. In addition, we asked a set of questions about their
background and experiences with past social groups. In particular, we asked whether they deviated from their
group norms in the past, if their past social groups were different from Princeton, and how different they felt from
the typical member of their past groups (see Appendix B, SOM, for interview question prompts).

The purpose of Study 1a was not to identify systematic differences between the students who deviated vs. con-
formed to the eating club norm. To do so, we would have needed a much larger sample of Princeton upperclassmen.
Instead, we conducted an inductive thematic analysis to identify recurring themes in the qualitative data (Braun
& Clarke, 2006). We then used these themes to understand better the context of the eating club norm at Princeton
on the one hand, and formulate hypotheses about the antecedents of norm deviance on the other hand.

Journal of Social and Political Psychology


2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245
https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134
Gomila & Paluck 227

Our thematic analysis strategy closely followed the guidelines introduced by Braun and Clarke (2006). During
data collection, we went through a phase of iterative reading of the interviews, during which we familiarized ourselves
with the data. Every time we had conducted and transcribed two to four interviews, the authors would meet to
read the interview transcripts together and discuss initial ideas about the content. By the end of this familiarization
phase, our meeting notes included a list of themes that we had identified in the data. Once data collection was
over, both authors reviewed the interview transcripts and meeting notes again to build together a list of the primary
patterns of responses about: i) the reasons why participants decided to join or not join an eating club (e.g., the
different costs associated with deviating from the norm), ii) their past and current social life, and iii) their tendency
to deviate from other social norms. Then, we reviewed and named the themes (Braun & Clarke, 2006) by going
through the interview transcripts again, and confirmed the prevalence of these themes in the data. Finally, we
selected vivid examples that illustrate each of the themes that we identified (Braun & Clarke, 2006), which we
report in this article.

Interview Findings
We organize this findings section into three main themes that we identified from an analysis of interview notes.
The first theme is about the social and logistical costs of deviating from the eating club norm at Princeton. The
second is about the deviants’ relationship with their past groups, and the third about their feeling of belongingness
and identification to a current group, Princeton University.

ii
Theme One: The costs of deviance from the eating club norm — Participants highlighted two types of costs
associated with deviating from the eating club norm at Princeton: social and logistical costs.

The social costs associated with deviating from social norms. In sixteen of eighteen interviews, participants who
were independents talked about feeling isolated, ignored, or left out by friends who belong to clubs. We quote
three of these participants as examples of what most of them told us about how it feels to not be an eating club
member on campus:

“People who go independent are kind of marginalized and are perceived as not fitting. There’s also no
cohesion between independents. The listserv was created last year, but in any case, you never know
who else is going independent.”

“There’s a huge culture of eating meals with others here, and when you’re an independent you have to
plan a lot more to see your friends. It’s kind of hard, but I manage to see them once a week.”

“Independents are ignored. It’s just a general vibe, perceived as less social or doing their own thing.”

The participants also expressed feeling judged, even ashamed for being independent. One of them said that since
she dropped out of her eating club, she feels that her social status is different, “in particular with underclassmen
who perceive independents as not super cool.” Another student explicitly mentioned that “people segregate because
of eating clubs; they are judging you, judging if you are socially apt.” On this note, a different interviewee shared
the following story:

“Recently at a Shabbat dinner, the eating club question came up. I had to say that I was an independent.
I could see that they were judging me from the face they made. It makes me uncomfortable and angry,
but I understand them in a way, because I used to be like that too.”

Journal of Social and Political Psychology


2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245
https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134
Deviance From Social Norms: Who Are the Deviants? 228

The logistical costs associated with deviating from social norms. Going independent not only involves this type
of social cost, but also has negative logistical implications. One of the interviewees thought that undergraduates
“have no freedom of choice, because it is really hard to go to grocery stores, there’s not a lot of options and most
of us don’t have a car.” She added, “the university is forcing the students to take the most conventional route.”

Another participant shared thoughts about how the current structure makes it difficult to be an independent: “Inde-
pendents are a little off the grid, even housing is on one side of campus. Freshmen and sophomores interact with
a lot of people, then there’s no more community. Also, there’s no support on campus, I have to take a bus once
a week to go get groceries, and that’s not easy.” Finally, a senior confessed: “I just want to graduate, leave this
place, and be part of a world where you can eat with whoever you wanna eat with anytime."

Theme Two: Independents’ past relationship with membership groups — We explored how participants
described their relationship with their social groups in the past. In contrast to eating club members, independent
students emphasized having a history of both deviating from social norms and feeling different from others.

Past instances of deviance. The interviewer asked the students if they felt that, before becoming a Princeton
student, they were the type of person who would tend to not follow mainstream behavior. The majority of the inde-
pendent interviewees responded to this question with ease, in contrast with the reaction of most of the eating club
members. The content of the independents’ responses also appeared qualitatively different: three of the seven
eating club members said that they mostly conformed or always tried to fit in. Another one just said that she didn’t
party too much in high school. On the contrary, independent students expressed much more interest in the
question and were eager to share stories. One of them responded: “my whole life!” Another student explained:

“Yes. I was not a typical student in high school. I worked at a grocery store and I really liked the people
at the grocery store. But in school, I never wanted to follow the crowd. Also, most people in my hometown
get married very young, don’t go to college, and don’t even understand why I am going to Princeton.”

A third participant responded that she had not followed the mainstream much and told the interviewer:
iii
“Here at Princeton, I created and tailored my major in Visual and Performing Arts and I am the only one
who is doing that.” A final example comes from a student who voiced out: “I don’t take into account what
other people think and do. I do what I want to do, and I’m not prone to social pressure. People in my band
do a lot of stuff that I don’t really understand, such as dating. I think that people go on date because of
social pressure.”

Feeling different from the typical ingroup member. Interview data also suggested that the independents, on top
of reporting deviating from norms in the past, also mentioned feeling different from the typical member of their
past groups. One student explained that she never felt like a “typical person because she is a twin, gender non-
conforming, and not the kind of typical Muslim person who fits at the typical religious event.” Another interviewee
mentioned that she was never part of the main crowd. She explained: “kids were looking at me differently. I per-
formed better, because I was more intelligent.” A senior said that she “always felt different”, and added: “I had a
different childhood, mostly because I lived in developing countries and that makes you grow up maybe too fast.
I saw stuff that are hard to process for a 12-year old kid. I feel 5 years older than the students here.” As a final
example, one of the independent interviewees shared:

“I always felt different. I am Malaysian, but I was born in New Zealand, from Chinese parents. My parents
met in New Zealand, but then they moved to Malaysia. I don't really feel part of any of any of these cultures

Journal of Social and Political Psychology


2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245
https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134
Gomila & Paluck 229

really. I also went to a British school and never really felt part of that either. So I was always aware that
I was not like everyone. But being at Princeton made it even more salient how unique my life is.”

Theme Three: Identification and belonging — One of our objectives during the interviews was to explore how
the independents would respond to questions related to their identification to Princeton. Although a few students
responded negatively, the answers were overall positive.

We noticed that the independents tended to emphasize their institutional more than their social identification with
Princeton. For instance, an interviewee specified, “I do identify with my department, anthropology, and the pursuit
of knowledge at Princeton.” Another student expressed her love for architecture and said that she really threw
herself into the department. A third independent responded, “I try to identify with the best parts of what Princeton
is. And a part of identifying with a group is to be able to criticize it. I won't be back for each Reunions, but I identify,
although I'm wary and critical.”

In the surveys we explored further the idea that independent students may separate out the social and institutional
dimensions of their identification to resolve the psychological conflict triggered by deviating from a well-established
social norm.

Who Are the Deviants? Summary of the Interview Findings


Our analysis of the exploratory interviews suggests that deviating from the eating club norm at Princeton may
come with substantive costs of two types: social and logistical. Socially, most junior and senior independents re-
ported feeling left out, ignored, and isolated from others. Some of them also expressed feeling ashamed or judged
by others, which adds a prescriptive component to the eating club norm at Princeton. These social costs often
coexist with logistical costs. For instance, most independents do not have access to a kitchen, and when they do,
they need to make extra efforts to find groceries. Also, it is difficult logistically for independent students to have
meals with friends who belong to eating clubs.

The interviews also allowed us to explore factors in the deviants’ past and present social lives that may be respon-
sible for their decision to deviate from the eating club norm at Princeton. Interviews with deviants, as opposed to
eating club members, featured stories in which they deviated from norms in the past, and felt different from the
typical member of their past social groups on dimensions such as intelligence, gender identity, and other charac-
teristics. Finally, the interviews suggest that the deviants identified with Princeton as an institution, but described
belonging less on a social level, particularly in comparison to conformists.

Overall, we concluded from the interviews that the setting at Princeton would be interesting for a more systematic
investigation of the social and psychological characteristics of people who deviate from an ingroup norm. The
setting features both costs of deviation (social and logistical), but also a relatively numerous group of people who
do choose to deviate, which would make generalization about deviants more reliable. Using our descriptive findings
from the interviews, we designed a survey for Princeton sophomores who were currently deciding whether or not
to join an eating club.

Study 1b: Students Deciding to Deviate or Conform


We used ideas generated from the interviews to create a pilot survey, which had two goals. First, we used the
pilot survey to identify a large sample of prospective deviants and conformists among students on campus,

Journal of Social and Political Psychology


2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245
https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134
Deviance From Social Norms: Who Are the Deviants? 230

meaning second year students facing their mid-year decision about whether to join an eating club or not. We
planned to invite these students to participate in a subsequent survey at the end of the semester (Study 2) after
they had been living with their decision to deviate or conform from the eating club norm. Second, we used the pilot
survey to test the relevance of survey questions and scales for assessing some of the themes that arose in our
interviews, with a broader population of students.

Method
We fielded the pilot survey in the first week of the Spring semester, when students have their first opportunity to
either conform to the eating club norm (“prospective conformists”), or deviate from the norm and become an inde-
pendent (“prospective deviants”). To recruit participants, we sent an email to all Princeton sophomores from an
undergraduate research assistant inviting them to participate in the survey in exchange for a $5 Amazon gift card
(Appendix A, SOM). The participants completed the survey online by clicking the link to the Qualtrics survey from
the email. In order to encourage participation, we mentioned in the recruitment email that we would possibly
contact them back at the end of the spring semester with the opportunity to take part in a $20 follow-up survey
(Appendix A, SOM). We collected as many respondents as possible, in an attempt to identify a relatively large
proportion of students who planned to deviate. The survey remained open for six days: out of the 1,308 sophomores
of this class year, 408 participated (i.e., 31% of the entire population of sophomores).

Although identifying systematic differences between the students who deviated vs. conformed to the eating club
norm was not the primary objective of the pilot survey, we briefly describe the main correlates of deviance that
appeared in the data. The full text of the pilot survey can be found in Appendix C (SOM). We include a table of
item means in Appendix D (Table S1, SOM) and a correlation matrix (Table S2, SOM) in Appendix E (SOM). Fi-
nally, the data are available at https://osf.io/gxy6v/.

In addition to asking the students if they were about to join an eating club, the survey asked about their past and
present experiences with norms and social groups (e.g., past norm deviance, perception of norms, belongingness
and identification, expectations about Princeton), and mental health and demographic characteristics. We pre-
registered groups of explanatory variables (see SOM), but we treated all predictors but belonging and identification
as exploratory, given the nature of this survey as a pilot and as a tool for identifying the population of “new” deviants
and conformists on campus.

Main Results
Feeling different from others in the past and present — Prospective deviants reported feeling more different
from the typical ingroup member than prospective conformists by 10.42 percentage points (0 = feel very different,
100 = feel very similar; Mdeviants = 47.08; Mconformists = 57.51; SD = 18.97; p < .001; Appendix D, Table S1, SOM).
This analysis suggests a significant positive relationship between feeling different from others and choosing to
deviate, which supports qualitative data collected in the context of study 1a.

Deviance as a choice in the past and present — Prospective deviants reported deviating in the past from social
norms at a greater rate than prospective conformists (1 = never chose to deviate, 5 = always chose to deviate;
Mdeviants = 2.91; Mconformists = 2.54; SD = 1.04; p = .002; Appendix D, Table S1, SOM). This finding, in line with the
interview results, suggests the existence of a significant positive relationship between choosing to deviate in the
past and in the present.

Journal of Social and Political Psychology


2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245
https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134
Gomila & Paluck 231

Belonging and identification — Prospective deviants reported lower levels of identification with and belonging
to Princeton University than prospective conformists by 6 percentage points (0 = I do not belong / identify at all,
100 = I belong / identify a lot; Mbel.deviant = 61.95; Mbel.conformists = 68.05; SD = 22.01; p = .02; Mident.deviants = 71.82;
Mident.conformists = 77.91; SD = ; 21.83; p = .02).

Study 2: Main Survey of Deviant and Conformist Students

Using results from our pilot survey and combining those insights with our qualitative data, we launched our main
survey toward the end of the spring semester after students had officially made their decisions about whether or
not to join an eating club (i.e., to be a conformist or deviant, respectively). The objectives of the survey were de-
scriptive, but we hypothesized several relationships between characteristics and experiences of the students on
the one hand, and their decision to deviate on the other. The survey’s pre-registration can be found at
https://osf.io/gxy6v/

Theory- and Data-Driven Hypotheses


Based on both theory and our data from Studies 1a and 1b, we formulated the following hypotheses.

• Hypothesis 1: Feeling Different From Others in the Past and Present. We expected deviants to report
feeling more different from the typical members of their group in the past and in the present. This hypothesis
emerged from the findings of both the interviews and survey data. This relationship has been suggested by
theoretical work (e.g., Monin & O’Connor, 2011), but was never empirically tested.

• Hypothesis 2: Deviance as a Choice in the Past and Present. We expected deviants to report deviating
from social norms in the past and in the present at greater rates than conformists.

• Hypothesis 3: Belonging and Identification. We expected deviants to have lower levels of identification
with Princeton and to feel that they belong to the Princeton community less than conformists. Additionally,
based on some hints from the interviews, we added items exploring possible differences between how the
deviants’ and conformists’ identification and belongingness relate to the academic vs. social aspect of Princeton.

• Hypothesis 4: Self Monitoring. We expected deviants to have lower levels of self-monitoring than conformists.

• Hypothesis 5: Big Five Personality Traits. We expected extraversion and agreeableness to negatively
correlate with deviance, and neuroticism to be negatively associated with deviance.

Additional Measures: Individual Differences and Mental Health


In order to explore further how deviating from the eating club norm may be related to one’s self-perceived willingness
and ability to socialize, we asked the participants about the extent to which they see themselves as: socially
awkward, a rebel, an independent thinker, and a maverick. In addition, we measured participants’ mental health,
which is a typical correlate of belongingness (Cialdini & Goldstein, 2004), hypothesized to be a predictor of deviance
from the eating club norm in this study.

Journal of Social and Political Psychology


2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245
https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134
Deviance From Social Norms: Who Are the Deviants? 232

Method
Recruitment Strategy
At the end of spring 2017, we contacted all prospective deviants (n = 116) and a representative subsample of the
prospective conformists (n = 120) from Study 1b, and invited them to participate in a follow-up survey (i.e., Study
2). We offered a $20 Amazon gift card for their participation in the 30-minute follow-up survey. We deliberately
made this compensation large in order to limit attrition. In the end, a total of 212 “deviating” and “conforming”
sophomores took the survey, that is, 90% of those who were recruited. Although attrition is rather small (i.e., 10%),
we conducted robustness checks using inverse probability weighting, in which we attribute greater weight to the
participants who are most similar to those who are missing in our final follow-up survey sample. Our results are
the same with this procedure and so below we present only the unweighted results.

Materials and Procedure


At the beginning of the survey, the participants were asked if they joined an eating club. This binary variable returns
1 for deviants, 0 for conformists, and constitutes the norm deviation indicator that we use in subsequent analyses.
Out of the116 prospective deviants that we recontacted, 109 participants (i.e., 94%) reported having actually de-
viated from the norm. Deviants (vs. conformists) constituted 50.4% of our main survey sample.

Subsequently, the participants responded questions exploring different characteristics and experiences we hypoth-
esized would correlate with their decision to deviate from vs. conform to the eating club norm. At the end of the
survey, the participants were asked to answer demographic questions (gender, ethnicity, family income and reli-
giosity), which we use as covariates in all regressions. We now turn into describing the survey in more detail. For
the full list of items, please refer to Appendix F (SOM).

Measures
Feeling different from others in the past and present — The objective of this series of questions was to explore
the idea that feeling different from the typical ingroup member in the past may increase one’s probability of devi-
ating from norms. We first asked the participants to name their most important past membership groups and asked
them: i) “did you usually feel like you fit in?” (0 = not at all; 100 = completely); ii) “how often did people from these
groups intentionally make you feel different?” (0 = never; 100 = all the time); iii) “Before becoming a Princeton
student, in your social life, how often did you feel different from other people?” (scales: 1= never; 4 = always); iv)
“At what age do you think you started to feel different from other people?”; v) “Do you feel that you have a lot in
common with the average Princeton student?” (0 = I have nothing in common; 100 = I have a lot in common).

We also explored the different ways in which respondents have felt different from other members of their social
groups. We asked the participants to select all the characteristics that made them feel different from others in the
past from the following list: religion, race, values, gender identity, sexual orientation, ideas, personality, skills,
habits, and interests.

Deviance as a choice in the past and present — We asked participants the following three questions: i) “How
often do you not behave in accordance with the mainstream or socially expected behavior at Princeton?” (0 =
never; 100 = all the time); ii) “Prior to arriving at Princeton, how often would you behave in opposition to mainstream
social behaviors?” (0 = never; 100 = all the time); iii) After asking participants to think about the first time they
deliberately deviated from a norm, we asked: “how old do you think you were”.

Journal of Social and Political Psychology


2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245
https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134
Gomila & Paluck 233

Belonging and identification — The series of questions about belonging and identification were composed of
eight items: four items dedicated to belongingness, and the other four items dedicated to identification. Both
subseries of four items were formulated in the exact same way, using the same scale from 0 (not at all) to 100 (a
lot). The series of questions about belongingness came first, in the following order: i) “To what extent do you feel
that you belong to the Princeton community, in general?”; ii) “To what extent do you feel that you belong to the
Princeton community, socially speaking [iii) academically speaking]?”; iv) “Is it more important to you to belong
to Princeton socially or academically?”

Following this series of questions about belonging, the participants responded to the same series of question
about identification (e.g., “To what extent do you feel that you identify with the Princeton community, in general?”,
etc.)

Personality — We used the Ten Item Personality Measure (Gosling, Rentfrow, & Swann, 2003) to assess the
Big five personality traits of the participants, who were asked to rate the extent to which they perceived themselves
as, for instance: extroverted and enthusiastic; critical and quarrelsome; or open to experience and complex. To
assess the participants’ level of self-monitoring, we used items from the Self-Monitoring Scale (Lennox & Wolfe,
1984; α = .65).

Mental health — We assessed the participants’ mental health through self-report of sadness and depression on
the one hand (e.g., “Please select the options that most closely describe the level of depression or sadness that
you have been feeling in the past 2 weeks: none, mild, moderate, moderately severe, severe”), and happiness
on the other hand (i.e., “How happy are you”, scale from 1= not at all happy to 100 = extremely happy). We created
an index of negative mental health (α = .81) by averaging three items: the participants’ self-reports of sadness,
depression, and happiness (reverse coded).

Demographics — We asked the participants for their gender, race, family income, and religiosity. We controlled
for these variables in each of the regression analyses presented below.

Unreported survey items — We also measured other potential social and psychological correlates of norm de-
viation, such as levels of anticipatory socialization, items from the Self-Determination Scale (Sheldon, 1995), and
emotions about the eating club norm, which were for exploratory purposes and not reported here (but see Appendix
F, SOM, for the full survey).

Results
Our objective was to understand and describe the relationships between deviance decisions and variables de-
scribing students’ social memberships, past instances of deviance, and their personality, rather than to build a
iv
predictive model. As such, we run multiple linear regressions of the binary deviance indicator (Hellevik, 2009)
on each of these variables separately. Bivariate models also recognized the fact that many variables correlated
v
with one another, creating problems of multicollinearity . For each regression, we used robust standard errors
and controlled for demographic variables (i.e., gender, ethnicity, family income, and religiosity).

Ydeviance = β0 + β1predictori + β2demographic1 + ... + β5demographic4 + ε

Journal of Social and Political Psychology


2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245
https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134
Deviance From Social Norms: Who Are the Deviants? 234

For each outcome variable of interest, we first conduct a multiple regression using the aforementioned model
(Hellevik, 2009). When we run separate multiple regressions on variables that are part of the same theme (e.g.,
“deviance as a status in the past and present”), we use the Bonferroni correction to compute adjusted p-values.
All the significance levels of the predictors are robust to this correction. Significance stars in the Results section
reflect adjusted p-values. Finally, we illustrate the relationship between deviance and the independent variable
under consideration by computing and displaying the probability of an individual’s deviation for different values of
the independent variable, maintaining the pre-specified demographic covariates at their mean.

Deviance as a Pattern
In line with Hypothesis 1, the set of regressions examining the difference between deviants’ and conformists’ past
and present experiences with their group suggests that deviants tend to feel less similar from the “typical Princeton
2
student” than conformists (b1 = -.006, SErobust = .001, padj = .002; R adj = .17; Figure 2).

Figure 2. Predicted probability of deviance from the eating club norm, based on separate regressions of the binary eating club
deviance item on each self-reported item of deviance as status in the past and present, controlling for demographic variables
set at their mean.

Journal of Social and Political Psychology


2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245
https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134
Gomila & Paluck 235

The data also support the idea that the deviants have a history of feeling more different from other people in their
groups compared to the conformists (Hypothesis 1). Self-reports of feeling different from the typical group member
2
are recalled at a younger age by deviants (b1 = -.02, SErobust = .009, padj = .035; R adj = .14; Figure 2). Also predicting
deviance are self-reports of perceiving oneself as not fitting well in their past social groups (b1 = -.005, SErobust =
2
.002, padj = .035; R adj = .14; Figure 2) and that former group members made one feel different (b1 = .003, SErobust
2
= .001, padj = .035; R adj = .14; Figure 2).

Although deviants are more likely to report feeling different from others than are conformists, and that they started
to feel this way at a younger age compared to conformists, Figure 3 reveals that deviants do not have significantly
different reasons for feeling different. When asked for the reasons they sometimes felt different from others, Figure
3 reveals that deviants and conformist report at similar rates feeling different from other members of their social
groups because of their personality, values, gender identity, and religion. Descriptively, those who deviated from
the eating club norm picked more dimensions along which they felt different than those who conformed, but the
difference is not statistically significant (Mdeviants = 4.01; Mconformists = 3.63; 1.55; p = .12)

Figure 3. Proportion of deviants and conformists who reported that the items displayed on the y-axis (e.g., personality, values,
race, etc.) have made them feel different from other members of their social groups, and 95% confidence interval bars.

Journal of Social and Political Psychology


2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245
https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134
Deviance From Social Norms: Who Are the Deviants? 236

In line with Hypothesis 2, deviants reported more active choices to deviate from social norms in the present and
past, compared to conformists (see Figure 4). The results revealed that deviants were significantly more likely to
2
report that they chose to deviate earlier in their life (b1 = -0.03, SErobust = .008, padj < .001; R adj = .18). Specifically,
deviants reported starting to deviate at a significantly younger age, on average 11.3 years old, than conformists,
who reported starting to deviate at 13.4 years old on average. In addition, deviants reported deviating at higher
rates than conformists in the past, although this difference does not reach conventional levels of statistical signif-
2
icance (b1 = 0.04, SErobust = .03, p = .12; R adj = .14). Finally, deviant students also reported that they currently
choose to deviate from social norms other than eating club norms compared to students who conform to the eating
2
club norm (b1 = 0.006, SErobust = .001, padj < .001; R adj = .19).

Figure 4. Predicted probability of deviance from the eating club norm, based on separate regressions of the binary eating club
deviance item on self-reported deviance from other norms in the past and present, controlling for demographic variables set
at their mean.

Journal of Social and Political Psychology


2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245
https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134
Gomila & Paluck 237

Belonging and Identification


The results also support Hypothesis 3, demonstrating that deviants report lower levels of belongingness and
2
identification than conformists (b1.belonging = -0.005, SErobust = .001, padj = .001; R adj = .16; Figure 5; b1.identification =
2
-0.004, SErobust = .001, padj = .005; R adj = .15). For instance, the regression model predicts that, compared to those
who feel that they belong “a lot” to the Princeton community, the students who feel that they belong “a little” to
the Princeton community are more likely to deviate from the eating club norm by around 35 percentage points.
The magnitude, direction and significance of the relationship between deviance and identification items mirrors
those of belonging, and are displayed in Appendix I (Figure S2, SOM).

Figure 5. Predicted probability of deviance from the eating club norm based on separate regressions of the binary deviance
item on self-reported belongingness items, controlling for demographic variables set at their mean.

Although we did not find significant differences between the deviants and the conformists in terms of academic
2
belonging (b1 = 0.002, SErobust = .002, p = .2; R adj = .11) and identification (b1 = -0.0009, SErobust = .001, p = .55;
2
R adj = .11), deviants reported prioritizing their academic belonging over their social belonging (b1 = 0.006, SErobust
2
= .002, padj = .001; R adj = .20), and their academic identification over their social identification (b1 = 0.007, SErobust
2
= .001, padj < .001; R adj = .17).

Journal of Social and Political Psychology


2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245
https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134
Deviance From Social Norms: Who Are the Deviants? 238

Self-Monitoring and Personality Traits


The results supported Hypothesis 4, with deviant students scoring significantly lower in self-monitoring than con-
2
formist students (b1 = 0.18, SErobust = .056, p = .002; R adj = .15, Figure 6). However, contrary to Hypothesis 5,
there were no differences between conformists and deviants for any of the items of the Big five, that is, extraversion
2 2
(b1 = -0.027, SErobust = .02, p = .15; R adj = .12), neuroticism (b1 = 0.00, SErobust = .02, p = .99; R adj = .11), openness
2
to experience (b1 = 0.02, SErobust = .03, p = .48; R adj = .11), conscientiousness (b1 = 0.002, SErobust = .02, p = .92;
2 2
R adj = .11) and agreeableness (b1 = -0.02, SErobust = .03, p = .44; R adj = .12). Similarly, we found no difference
2
between deviants and conformists in terms of how much of a maverick (b1 = 0.01; SErobust = .02, p = .75; R adj =
2
.11), a rebel (b1 = 0.02, SErobust = .02, p = .25; R adj = .12), an independent thinker (b1 = 0.04, SErobust = .02, p =
2 2
.14; R adj = .12), or how social awkward they feel (b1 = 0.03, SErobust = .02, p = .09; R adj = .13).

Figure 6. Predicted probability of deviance based on separate regressions of the binary deviance indicator on the self-monitoring
index, controlling for demographic variables set at their mean.

Mental Health
2
Deviants and conformists did not differ in terms of their mental health (b1 = -0.04, SErobust = .03, p = .18; R adj =
.12; Appendix J, Table S6, SOM).

Journal of Social and Political Psychology


2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245
https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134
Gomila & Paluck 239

General Discussion

We used an established Princeton University norm (i.e., joining an Eating Club) to which 73% of the undergraduate
students adhere, to examine the social and psychological correlates of deviance from social norms. Exploratory
interviews of junior and senior undergraduates revealed that deviating from the eating club norm at Princeton by
refusing to join an eating club comes with substantive social costs. This suggests that the norm is prescriptive
(students should join eating clubs), and not just descriptive (most students do join clubs).

However, deviance from the eating club norm may have become relatively easier than it used to be because of
recent developments. The university offers financial subsidies to students who do not join eating clubs, and in-
creasing numbers of students are choosing to remain independent from clubs. This may make the eating club
norm seem less “settled” (Swidler, 1986). In addition, our data suggest this to be the case: students perceive the
norm to be weaker (fewer students joining eating clubs) than it actually is. As a result, our survey findings, which
highlight the personality and social experiences that differentiate students who deviate from the students who
conform, paint a picture of deviance from a social norm that is undergoing change. As a result, while deviance
from the eating club norm still comes with costs, the present research focuses on more “ordinary” deviance. This
feature of this research addresses a gap in the literature on more “ordinary” deviance, as opposed to previous
scholarship on deviation from strong and highly punitive norms (e.g., rescuing Jews during the Holocaust; Monroe,
2008). During “unsettled” times, the norm seems to be on the decline and structural opportunities exist for those
who decide to deviate.

Our qualitative and quantitative data consistently suggest three themes regarding those who deviate. First, deviance
is a pattern. Individuals who decide to deviate from social norms deviated more in the past, started to deviate
earlier, and deviate more in the present. These results are true for two different experiences of deviance: being
made to feel different by members of previous social groups, and intentionally deviating from norms of previous
and present social groups. Second, individuals who choose to deviate tend to be low identifiers who feel that they
belong to their social group less than those who decide to conform. Third, we found that deviants tend to be lower
self-monitors (Snyder, 1979) than the conformists, but we found no differences in personality traits as measured
by the Big Five, or in levels of mental health between deviants and conformists.

On the one hand, the finding that people deviating in the present also report deviating in the past seems intuitive
and in need of no explanation: individuals strive to maintain a coherent and consistent self (Fiske, 1993; Ross,
Lepper, & Ward, 2010; Swann & Bosson, 2010), which may explain that past behavior is a good predictor of future
behavior. On the other hand, patterns of deviance seem to need more explanation. Most research on social norms
focuses on the rewards of following social norms and the strong costs of violating them. Previous findings, as well
as our own, show that deviance comes with high social and psychological costs (respectively, social sanctions,
and feeling odd or guilty for deviating; Almenberg, Dreber, Apicella, & Rand, 2010; Asch, 1955; Cialdini & Goldstein,
2004). Related research suggests that social sanctions effectively promotes more conformity and fewer instances
of deviance on a collective level (Fehr & Fischbacher, 2004).

On an individual level, there is less research, and a possible question for future experimental research is why in-
dividuals choose deviance repeatedly over the course of time. For example, do some people who repeatedly de-
viate from norms over time have a different way of predicting or dealing with the social and psychological costs?
Individuals in general may overestimate social sanctions incurred by deviance. For those who actually deviate,

Journal of Social and Political Psychology


2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245
https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134
Deviance From Social Norms: Who Are the Deviants? 240

they may discover that they experience lower social costs than expected. Additionally, acts of deviance may lead
individuals to learn new strategies for minimizing the social consequences of deviating, and as a result, may feel
more capable about deviating again. Strategies may include the ability to justify their deviant behavior to others
or seeking other forms of belonging to the same context (see below).

Another way deviance may become part of one’s identity is through the development of narratives around the
deviant self. An experience of deviation as a choice (or as something imposed on them, as we observed in our
survey), may shape the individual’s own perception of the self as deviant. Individuals may present themselves as
deviant by status (e.g., “I’ve always been different from others”), or by choice (e.g., “I’m the type of person who
likes to do different things”). These themes have been previously explored by Monin and O’Connor (2011), who
theorized about deviation by choice vs. status; and by Blanton and Christie (2003), who theorized and showed
how individuals prefer to define the self as deviants in positive ways from others (“sticking out in good ways”).
These narratives and identities may influence one’s own and others’ expectations for and actual instances of deviant
behavior.

Those who deviated from the eating club norm at Princeton feel that they socially belong and identify less to the
typical Princeton student. Deviants and conformists both feel that they belong and identify with Princeton academ-
ically, but deviants were more likely than conformists to say that they prioritized academics over social life. One
possibility is that those who deviate may compensate their decreased level of social belonging by increasing their
feelings of academic and institutional belonging. This may be one strategy to minimize the costs of deviation for
future investigation.

Limitations
One limitation of this study is that we study deviance from one specific type of norm, which is highly social—mem-
bership in a club. Thus, our description of deviants from this norm may only apply to the type of people who do
not wish to be as social as others, or who do not get along as well with others. This is a possibility, and is one
more reason why we need more research on the topic of deviance. However, we also point out that we did not
find personality differences that one might expect if deviance in this specific case had everything to do with degree
of sociality—in particular, we do not find differences in terms of personality traits as measured by the Big Five
(Bodankin & Tziner, 2009). Furthermore, those who deviated from the eating club norms did not differ from others
in their perception of the self as socially awkward, as independent thinker, or as rebellious. Finally, deviants and
conformists did not differ either in the way they feel different from other people, and we did not find differences in
terms of in mental health or academic identification or belonging. Still, we hope that this research inspires similar
descriptive and experimental research exploring other types of norms in other settings.

A limitation to causal inference is that the series of studies described in this paper are not experimental. The ob-
jective of the set of studies presented in this article was to explore the social and psychological correlates of de-
viance, not to make causal or directional claims about the causes of deviance. In this sense, we did not manipulate
any treatments and our results rely only on participants’ self-reports about their past and present, which implies
that they might have erroneously retrieved and reported information from their past. More research, especially
experimental research, is needed to understand whether individuals who deviated from a norm build narratives
around their past and present self that are coherent with their deviant behavior, such as reporting more acts of
deviance in the past, or whether deviance is part of a causal loop in which feeling different from others and devi-
ating begets deviance.

Journal of Social and Political Psychology


2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245
https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134
Gomila & Paluck 241

Although we cannot rule out the possibility that demand characteristics may have driven the differences between
deviants and conformists, it is unlikely. During the recruitment period, we never mentioned to the students that
this study was about deviance from norms or eating clubs. We deliberately called the surveys “the sophomore
survey”, and made sure that the question asking the participants about their decision to join an eating club was
part of a broader set of general questions. As a result, we believe that the participants were not actively seeking
to respond to the object of the study.

These descriptive results point the way toward more research on social norm deviance. First, researchers should
seek to understand how and why the social and psychological costs that one endures after deviating correlate
with their probability to deviate again. Experimental designs in various settings and that involve deviance from
different kinds of social norms are necessary to understand what kinds of characteristics and experiences are
general drivers of deviance, and which are specific to context. Second, future research should explore if and how
feeling different from other members of one’s group may increase one’s probability to deviate, and why. Third,
because acts of deviance often come with negative consequences, individuals may update their perception of the
self, or of the costs, after deviating from a social norm. What types of narratives do deviants develop about the
self or about the act of deviating, that shape their own and others’ expectations about future behavior? Investigations
into these and other questions will greatly contribute to our understanding of patterns of deviance.

Notes
i) We describe the eating club norm and its prescriptive component later on. To provide some context to the reader, eating
clubs are student-led private institutions that upperclassmen may join. This is where most Princeton upperclassmen have
meals and party. In 2016, 73% of Princeton upperclassmen were members of an eating club, which by definition makes it a
descriptive norm.

ii) In addition to costs, the participants brought up barriers to conforming to the eating club norm during the interviews, which
stem from economic and religious constraints. In particular, students mentioned that they would not join eating clubs for
materialistic (expensive membership) or religious reasons (concerns related to religious laws about diet and alcohol consumption).

iii) The real title of this participant’s major was modified to protect their privacy; details from other quotes have also been
changed for this reason.

iv) The findings described in the results section are robust to logistic regression analyses. Because the objective of these
analyses is to estimate the average marginal effects of our predictors of interest in terms of probabilities, we report the results
of linear regression analyses (see Woolridge, 2002, pp. 455-456; e.g., Hoffman, Trawalter, Axt, & Oliver, 2016).

v) We provide a correlation matrix (Table S3) in Appendix G (SOM). In addition, we used Factor Analysis (Appendix H, Tables
S4, S5, and Figure S1, SOM) to confirm that the items that we used to test each hypothesis contribute together to separate
components of the PCA. This analysis revealed 4 principal components that together, explain 75% of the variance (Appendix
H, SOM). PC1 can be defined as social belonging and identification, PC2 as academic belonging and identification, PC3 as
feeling different in the past and present, and PC4 as deviating in the past and present.

Funding
The authors have no funding to report.

Competing Interests
The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Journal of Social and Political Psychology


2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245
https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134
Deviance From Social Norms: Who Are the Deviants? 242

Acknowledgments
Thanks to Eldar Shafir, Alin Coman, Chris Crandall, Daniela Urbina Julio, Elsa Voytas, Hannah Korevaar, Herrissa Lamothe,
Ian Lundberg, Ana Gantman, Rebecca Littman, Sherry Wu, Joel Martinez, Jason Chin, John-Henry Pezzuto, and all the
members of Betsy Levy Paluck’s lab for helpful comments.

Data Availability
A dataset and supplementary materials for this study are freely available (for access, see Index of Supplementary Materials
below)

Supplementary Materials

The OSF project folder includes: i) the entire dataset along with its codebook, ii) the R code, which includes all of the analyses
reported in this article, iii) an online appendix document with all of the appendices mentioned in the main body of this article,
and iv) the pre-registration of the pilot and main surveys (for access see Index of Supplementary Materials below).

Index of Supplementary Materials

Gomila, R., & Paluck, E. L. (2020). Deviance from social norms: Who are the deviants [Code, data, online appendix, &
pre-registration information]. OSF. https://osf.io/gxy6v/

References

Almenberg, J., Dreber, A., Apicella, C., & Rand, D. G. (2010). Third party reward and punishment: Group size, efficiency and
public goods (SSRN Scholarly Paper No. ID 1715305). Retrieved from Social Science Research Network website:
https://papers.ssrn.com/abstract=1715305

Asch, S. E. (1955). Opinions and social pressure. Scientific American, 193(5), 31-35.
https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican1155-31

Blanton, H., & Christie, C. O. (2003). Deviance regulation: A theory of action and identity. Review of General Psychology, 7(2),
115-149. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.7.2.115

Bodankin, M., & Tziner, A. (2009). Constructive deviance, destructive deviance and personality: How do they interrelate? The
Amfiteatru Economic Journal, 11(26), 549-564.

Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3(2), 77-101.
https://doi.org/10.1191/1478088706qp063oa

Brewer, M. B., & Pickett, C. L. (1999). Distinctiveness motives as a source of the social self. In T. R. Tyler, R. M. Kramer, &
O. P. John (Eds.), The psychology of the social self (Applied Social Research series, pp. 71–87). Mahwah, NJ, USA:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Cialdini, R. B., & Goldstein, N. J. (2004). Social influence: Compliance and conformity. Annual Review of Psychology, 55(1),
591-621. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142015

Crocker, J., Major, B., & Steele, C. (1998). Social stigma. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of
social psychology (4th ed., pp. 504–553). New York, NY, USA: McGraw-Hill.

Journal of Social and Political Psychology


2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245
https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134
Gomila & Paluck 243

Dreu, C. K. W. D. (2002). Team innovation and team effectiveness: The importance of minority dissent and reflexivity. European
Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 11(3), 285-298. https://doi.org/10.1080/13594320244000175

Durkheim, E. (1938). The rules of sociological method (8th ed.). Chicago, IL, USA: University of Chicago Press.

Erikson, K. (2005). Wayward puritans: A study in the sociology of deviance. Boston, MA, USA: Pearson/Allyn and Bacon.

Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2004). Third-party punishment and social norms. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25(2), 63-87.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(04)00005-4

Festinger, L. (1950). Informal social communication. Psychological Review, 57(5), 271-282. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0056932

Fiske, S. T. (1993). Social cognition and social perception. Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 155-194.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.44.020193.001103

Gantman, A., Gomila, R., Martinez, J. E., Matias, J. N., Paluck, E. L., Starck, J., . . . Yaffe, N. (2018). A pragmatist philosophy
of psychological science and its implications for replication. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 41, Article e127.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X18000626

Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., & Swann, W. B. (2003). A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. Journal of
Research in Personality, 37(6), 504-528. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00046-1

Hellevik, O. (2009). Linear versus logistic regression when the dependent variable is a dichotomy. Quality & Quantity, 43(1),
59-74. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11135-007-9077-3

Hoffman, K. M., Trawalter, S., Axt, J. R., & Oliver, M. N. (2016). Racial bias in pain assessment and treatment recommendations,
and false beliefs about biological differences between blacks and whites. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
of the United States of America, 113(16), 4296-4301. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1516047113

Hornsey, M. J. (2006). Ingroup critics and their influence on groups. In T. Postmes & J. Jetten (Eds.), Individuality and the
group: Advances in social identity (pp. 74–92). https://doi.org/10.4135/9781446211946.n5

Hornsey, M. J., Majkut, L., Terry, D. J., & McKimmie, B. M. (2003). On being loud and proud: Non-conformity and
counter-conformity to group norms. British Journal of Social Psychology, 42(3), 319-335.
https://doi.org/10.1348/014466603322438189

James, W. (1907). Pragmatism’s conception of truth. The Journal of Philosophy, Psychology, and Scientific Methods, 4(6),
141-155. https://doi.org/10.2307/2012189

Jetten, J., Branscombe, N. R., & Spears, R. (2002). On being peripheral: Effects of identity insecurity on personal and collective
self-esteem. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32(1), 105-123. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.64

Jetten, J., Spears, R., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1997). Distinctiveness threat and prototypicality: Combined effects on intergroup
discrimination and collective self-esteem. European Journal of Social Psychology, 27(6), 635-657.
https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(199711/12)27:6<635::AID-EJSP835>3.0.CO;2-#

Kirsch, C., Lubart, T., & Houssemand, C. (2015). Creative personality profile in social sciences: The leading role of autonomy.
Creativity: Theories – Research – Applications, 2(2), 180-211. https://doi.org/10.1515/ctra-2015-0020

Lennox, R. D., & Wolfe, R. N. (1984). Revision of the Self-Monitoring Scale. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
46(6), 1349-1364. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.46.6.1349

Link, B. G., & Phelan, J. C. (2001). Conceptualizing stigma. Annual Review of Sociology, 27(1), 363-385.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.soc.27.1.363

Journal of Social and Political Psychology


2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245
https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134
Deviance From Social Norms: Who Are the Deviants? 244

Major, B., & O’Brien, L. T. (2005). The social psychology of stigma. Annual Review of Psychology, 56(1), 393-421.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070137

Marques, J. M., Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Leyens, J.-P. (1988). The “Black Sheep Effect”: Extremity of judgments towards ingroup
members as a function of group identification. European Journal of Social Psychology, 18(1), 1-16.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420180102

Miller, D. T., & Prentice, D. A. (1996). The construction of social norms and standards. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski
(Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 799–829). New York, NY, USA: Guilford Press.

Miller, D. T., & Prentice, D. A. (2016). Changing norms to change behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 67(1), 339-361.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015013

Monin, B., & O’Connor, K. (2011). Reactions to defiant deviants: Deliverance or defensiveness? In J. Jetten & M. J. Hornsey
(Eds.), Rebels in groups: Dissent, deviance, difference, and defiance (pp. 261–280). Chichester, United Kingdom:
Wiley-Blackwell.

Monroe, K. R. (2008). Cracking the code of genocide: The moral psychology of rescuers, bystanders, and Nazis during the
Holocaust. Political Psychology, 29(5), 699-736. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2008.00661.x

Morrison, K. R., & Miller, D. T. (2012). Explaining differences in opinion expression. In J. Jetten & M. J. Hornsey (Eds.), Rebels
in groups: Dissent, deviance, difference and defiance (pp. 219–237). https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444390841.ch12

Moscovici, S. (1976). Social influence and social change (Vol. 10). London, United Kingdom: Academic Press [published in
cooperation with European Association of Experimental Social Psychology].

Moscovici, S. (1994). Three concepts: Minority, conflict, and behavioral style. In S. Moscovici, A. Mucchi-Faina, & A. Maass
(Eds.), Nelson-Hall series in psychology: Minority influence (pp. 233–251). Chicago, IL, USA: Nelson-Hall Publishers.

Nemeth, C., Brown, K., & Rogers, J. (2001). Devil’s advocate versus authentic dissent: Stimulating quantity and quality.
European Journal of Social Psychology, 31(6), 707-720. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.58

Packer, D. J. (2012). The dissenter’s dilemma, and a social identity solution. In J. Jetten & M. J. Hornsey (Eds.), Rebels in
groups: Dissent, deviance, difference and defiance (pp. 281–301). https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444390841.ch15

Packer, D. J., & Chasteen, A. L. (2010). Loyal deviance: Testing the normative conflict model of dissent in social groups.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 5-18. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167209350628

Paluck, E. L., & Green, D. P. (2009). Deference, dissent, and dispute resolution: An experimental intervention using mass
media to change norms and behavior in Rwanda. The American Political Science Review, 103(4), 622-644.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055409990128

Prentice, D. A. (2012). The psychology of social norms and the promotion of human rights. In R. Goodman, D. Jinks, & A. K.
Woods (Eds.), Understanding social action, promoting human rights (pp. 23–43).
https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195371895.003.0002

Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (1996). Pluralistic ignorance and the perpetuation of social norms by unwitting actors. In M. P.
Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 28, pp. 161–209).
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60238-5

Ross, L., Lepper, M., & Ward, A. (2010). History of social psychology: Insights, challenges, and contributions to theory and
application. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. 3-50).
https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470561119.socpsy001001

Journal of Social and Political Psychology


2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245
https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134
Gomila & Paluck 245

Rozin, P. (2001). Social psychology and science: Some lessons from Solomon Asch. Personality and Social Psychology
Review, 5(1), 2-14. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327957PSPR0501_1

Sewell, W. H. (2005). Logics of history: Social theory and social transformation. London, United Kingdom: University of Chicago
Press.

Shashkini, N. (2018, May 10). ICC presidents are combating declining membership at sign-in eating clubs. The Princetonian.
Retrieved from
https://www.dailyprincetonian.com/article/2018/05/icc-presidents-are-combating-declining-membership-at-sign-in-eating-clubs

Sheldon, K. M. (1995). Creativity and self-determination in personality. Creativity Research Journal, 8(1), 25-36.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj0801_3

Sherif, M., & Sherif, C. W. (1972). Reference groups: Exploration into conformity and deviation of adolescents. New York, NY,
USA: Harper and Row.

Snyder, M. (1979). Self-monitoring processes. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 12,
pp. 85–128). https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60260-9

Swann, W. B., Jr., & Bosson, J. K. (2010). Self and identity. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of
social psychology (5th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 589–628). https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470561119.socpsy001016

Swann, W. B., & Jetten, J. (2017). Restoring agency to the human actor. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(3),
382-399. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616679464

Swidler, A. (1986). Culture in action: Symbols and strategies. American Sociological Review, 51(2), 273-286.
https://doi.org/10.2307/2095521

van Kleef, G. A., Wanders, F., Stamkou, E., & Homan, A. C. (2015). The social dynamics of breaking the rules: Antecedents
and consequences of norm-violating behavior. Current Opinion in Psychology, 6, 25-31.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.03.013

Woolridge, J. M. (2002). Econometric analysis of cross section and panel data. Cambridge, MA, USA: MIT Press.

Journal of Social and Political Psychology PsychOpen GOLD is a publishing service by


2020, Vol. 8(1), 220–245 Leibniz Institute for Psychology Information (ZPID),
https://doi.org/10.5964/jspp.v8i1.1134 Trier, Germany. www.leibniz-psychology.org

You might also like