Essay Dreams
Essay Dreams
Contents
Preserved writings from early Mediterranean civilizations indicate a relatively abrupt change in
subjective dream experience between Bronze Age antiquity and the beginnings of the classical era.
[18]
In visitation dreams reported in ancient writings, dreamers were largely passive in their dreams, and
visual content served primarily to frame authoritative auditory messaging.[19][10][20] Gudea, the king of
the Sumerian city-state of Lagash (reigned c. 2144–2124 BCE), rebuilt the temple of Ningirsu as the
result of a dream in which he was told to do so.[6] After antiquity, the passive hearing of visitation
dreams largely gave way to visualized narratives in which the dreamer becomes a character who
actively participates.
From the 1940s to 1985, Calvin S. Hall collected more than 50,000 dream reports at Western
Reserve University. In 1966, Hall and Robert Van de Castle published The Content Analysis of
Dreams, in which they outlined a coding system to study 1,000 dream reports from college students.
[21]
Results indicated that participants from varying parts of the world demonstrated similarity in their
dream content. The only residue of antiquity's authoritative dream figure in the Hall and Van de
Castle listing of dream characters is the inclusion of God in the category of prominent persons.
[22]
Hall's complete dream reports were made publicly available in the mid-1990s by his
protégé William Domhoff. More recent studies of dream reports, while providing more detail,
continue to cite the Hall study favorably.[23]
In the Hall study, the most common emotion experienced in dreams was anxiety. Other emotions
included abandonment, anger, fear, joy, and happiness. Negative emotions were much more
common than positive ones.[21] The Hall data analysis showed that sexual dreams occur no more
than 10% of the time and are more prevalent in young to mid-teens.[21] Another study showed that 8%
of both men's and women's dreams have sexual content.[24] In some cases, sexual dreams may
result in orgasms or nocturnal emissions. These are colloquially known as "wet dreams".[25]
The visual nature of dreams is generally highly phantasmagoric; that is, different locations and
objects continuously blend into each other. The visuals (including locations, people, and objects) are
generally reflective of a person's memories and experiences, but conversation can take on highly
exaggerated and bizarre forms. Some dreams may even tell elaborate stories wherein the dreamer
enters entirely new, complex worlds and awakes with ideas, thoughts and feelings never
experienced prior to the dream.
People who are blind from birth do not have visual dreams. Their dream contents are related to other
senses like hearing, touch, smell and taste, whichever are present since birth.[26]
Neurophysiology
Main article: Cognitive neuroscience of dreams
Further information: Neuroscience of sleep
Dream study is popular with scientists exploring the mind–brain problem. Some "propose to reduce
aspects of dream phenomenology to neurobiology."[27] But current science cannot specify dream
physiology in detail. Protocols in most nations restrict human brain research to non-invasive
procedures. In the United States, invasive brain procedures with a human subject are allowed only
when these are deemed necessary in surgical treatment to address medical needs of the same
human subject.[28] Non-invasive measures of brain activity like electroencephalogram (EEG) voltage
averaging or cerebral blood flow cannot identify small but influential neuronal populations.
[29]
Also, fMRI signals are too slow to explain how brains compute in real time.[30]
Scientists researching some brain functions can work around current restrictions by examining
animal subjects. As stated by the Society for Neuroscience, "Because no adequate alternatives
exist, much of this research must be done on animal subjects."[31] However, since animal dreaming
can be only inferred, not confirmed, animal studies yield no hard facts to illuminate the
neurophysiology of dreams. Examining human subjects with brain lesions can provide clues, but the
lesion method cannot discriminate between the effects of destruction and disconnection and cannot
target specific neuronal groups in heterogeneous regions like the brain stem.[29]
Generation
Denied precision tools, obliged to depend on imaging, much dream research has succumbed to
the law of the instrument. Studies detect an increase of blood flow in a specific brain region and then
credit that region with a role in generating dreams. But pooling study results has led to the newer
conclusion that dreaming involves large numbers of regions and pathways, which likely are different
for different dream events.[32]
Image creation in the brain involves significant neural activity downstream from eye intake, and it is
theorized that "the visual imagery of dreams is produced by activation during sleep of the same
structures that generate complex visual imagery in waking perception."[33]
Dreams present a running narrative rather than exclusively visual imagery. Following their work
with split-brain subjects, Gazzaniga and LeDoux postulated, without attempting to specify the neural
mechanisms, a "left-brain interpreter" that seeks to create a plausible narrative from whatever
electro-chemical signals reach the brain's left hemisphere. Sleep research has determined that
some brain regions fully active during waking are, during REM sleep, activated only in a partial or
fragmentary way.[34] Drawing on this knowledge, textbook author James W. Kalat explains, "[A]
dream represents the brain's effort to make sense of sparse and distorted information.... The cortex
combines this haphazard input with whatever other activity was already occurring and does its best
to synthesize a story that makes sense of the information."[35] Neuroscientist Indre Viskontas is even
more blunt, calling often bizarre dream content "just the result of your interpreter trying to create a
story out of random neural signaling."[36]
Theories on function
Main article: Oneirology
Further information: Rapid eye movement sleep
For humans in the pre-classical era, and continuing for some non-literate populations into modern
times, dreams are believed to have functioned as revealers of truths sourced during sleep from gods
or other external entities.[37][11] Ancient Egyptians believed that dreams were the best way to receive
divine revelation, and thus they would induce (or "incubate") dreams. They went to sanctuaries and
slept on special "dream beds" in hope of receiving advice, comfort, or healing from the gods.[14] From
a Darwinian perspective dreams would have to fulfill some kind of biological requirement, provide
some benefit for natural selection to take place, or at least have no negative impact on fitness.
Robert (1886),[38] a physician from Hamburg, was the first who suggested that dreams are a need
and that they have the function to erase (a) sensory impressions that were not fully worked up, and
(b) ideas that were not fully developed during the day. In dreams, incomplete material is either
removed (suppressed) or deepened and included into memory. Freud, whose dream studies
focused on interpreting dreams, not explaining how or why humans dream, disputed Robert's
hypothesis[39] and proposed that dreams preserve sleep by representing as fulfilled those wishes that
otherwise would awaken the dreamer.[40] Freud wrote that dreams "serve the purpose of prolonging
sleep instead of waking up. Dreams are the GUARDIANS of sleep and not its disturbers."[41]
Hindu
In the Mandukya Upanishad, part of the Veda scriptures of Indian Hinduism, a dream is one of three
states that the soul experiences during its lifetime, the other two states being the waking state and
the sleep state.[55] The earliest Upanishads, written before 300 BCE, emphasize two meanings of
dreams. The first says that dreams are merely expressions of inner desires. The second is the belief
of the soul leaving the body and being guided until awakened.
Abrahamic
In Judaism, dreams are considered part of the experience of the world that can be interpreted and
from which lessons can be garnered. It is discussed in the Talmud, Tractate Berachot 55–60.
The ancient Hebrews connected their dreams heavily with their religion, though the Hebrews
were monotheistic and believed that dreams were the voice of one God alone. Hebrews also
differentiated between good dreams (from God) and bad dreams (from evil spirits). The Hebrews,
like many other ancient cultures, incubated dreams in order to receive a divine revelation. For
example, the Hebrew prophet Samuel would "lie down and sleep in the temple at Shiloh before the
Ark and receive the word of the Lord". Most of the dreams in the Bible are in the Book of Genesis.[56]
Christians mostly shared the beliefs of the Hebrews and thought that dreams were of a supernatural
character because the Old Testament includes frequent stories of dreams with divine inspiration.
The most famous of these dream stories was Jacob's dream of a ladder that stretches from Earth
to Heaven. Many Christians preach that God can speak to people through their dreams. The famous
glossary, the Somniale Danielis, written in the name of Daniel, attempted to teach Christian
populations to interpret their dreams.
Iain R. Edgar has researched the role of dreams in Islam.[57] He has argued that dreams play an
important role in the history of Islam and the lives of Muslims, since dream interpretation is the only
way that Muslims can receive revelations from God since the death of the last prophet, Muhammad.
According to Edgar, Islam classifies three types of dreams. Firstly, there is the true dream (al-
[58]
ru’ya), then the false dream, which may come from the devil (shaytan), and finally, the meaningless
everyday dream (hulm). This last dream could be brought forth by the dreamer's ego or base
appetite based on what they experienced in the real world. The true dream is often indicated by
Islam's hadith tradition.[58] In one narration by Aisha, the wife of the Prophet, it is said that the
Prophet's dreams would come true like the ocean's waves.[58] Just as in its predecessors,
the Quran also recounts the story of Joseph and his unique ability to interpret dreams.[58]
Buddhist
In Buddhism, ideas about dreams are similar to the classical and folk traditions in South Asia. The
same dream is sometimes experienced by multiple people, as in the case of the Buddha-to-be,
before he is leaving his home. It is described in the Mahāvastu that several of the Buddha's relatives
had premonitory dreams preceding this. Some dreams are also seen to transcend time: the Buddha-
to-be has certain dreams that are the same as those of previous Buddhas, the Lalitavistara states. In
Buddhist literature, dreams often function as a "signpost" motif to mark certain stages in the life of
the main character.[59]
Buddhist views about dreams are expressed in the Pāli Commentaries and the Milinda Pañhā.[59]
Other
Dreaming of the Tiger Spring (虎跑夢泉) Statue at Hupao Spring (Hupaomengquan) in Hangzhou, Zhejiang,
China.
In Chinese history, people wrote of two vital aspects of the soul of which one is freed from the body
during slumber to journey in a dream realm, while the other remained in the body.[60] This belief and
dream interpretation had been questioned since early times, such as by the philosopher Wang
Chong (27–97 CE).[60]
The Babylonians and Assyrians divided dreams into "good," which were sent by the gods, and "bad,"
sent by demons.[61] A surviving collection of dream omens entitled Iškar Zaqīqu records various
dream scenarios as well as prognostications of what will happen to the person who experiences
each dream, apparently based on previous cases.[6][62] Some list different possible outcomes, based
on occasions in which people experienced similar dreams with different results.[6] The Greeks shared
their beliefs with the Egyptians on how to interpret good and bad dreams, and the idea of incubating
dreams. Morpheus, the Greek god of dreams, also sent warnings and prophecies to those who slept
at shrines and temples. The earliest Greek beliefs about dreams were that their gods physically
visited the dreamers, where they entered through a keyhole, exiting the same way after the divine
message was given.
Antiphon wrote the first known Greek book on dreams in the 5th century BCE. In that century, other
cultures influenced Greeks to develop the belief that souls left the sleeping body.
[63]
Hippocrates (469–399 BCE) had a simple dream theory: during the day, the soul receives images;
during the night, it produces images. Greek philosopher Aristotle (384–322 BCE) believed dreams
caused physiological activity. He thought dreams could analyze illness and predict diseases. Marcus
Tullius Cicero, for his part, believed that all dreams are produced by thoughts and conversations a
dreamer had during the preceding days.[64] Cicero's Somnium Scipionis described a lengthy dream
vision, which in turn was commented on by Macrobius in his Commentarii in Somnium Scipionis.
Herodotus in his The Histories, writes "The visions that occur to us in dreams are, more often than
not, the things we have been concerned about during the day."[65]
The Dreaming is a common term within the animist creation narrative of indigenous Australians for a
personal, or group, creation and for what may be understood as the "timeless time" of formative
creation and perpetual creating.[66]
Some Indigenous American tribes and Mexican populations believe that dreams are a way of visiting
and having contact with their ancestors.[67] Some Native American tribes have used vision quests as
a rite of passage, fasting and praying until an anticipated guiding dream was received, to be shared
with the rest of the tribe upon their return.[68][69]
Interpretation
Main article: Dream interpretation
Further information: Psychoanalysis and Precognition
Beginning in the late 19th century, Austrian neurologist Sigmund Freud, founder of psychoanalysis,
theorized that dreams reflect the dreamer's unconscious mind and specifically that dream content is
shaped by unconscious wish fulfillment. He argued that important unconscious desires often relate
to early childhood memories and experiences.[7] Carl Jung and others expanded on Freud's idea that
dream content reflects the dreamer's unconscious desires.
Dream interpretation can be a result of subjective ideas and experiences. One study found that most
people believe that "their dreams reveal meaningful hidden truths".[70] The researchers surveyed
students in the United States, South Korea, and India, and found that 74% of Indians, 65% of South
Koreans and 56% of Americans believed their dream content provided them with meaningful insight
into their unconscious beliefs and desires. This Freudian view of dreaming was believed significantly
more than theories of dreaming that attribute dream content to memory consolidation, problem-
solving, or as a byproduct of unrelated brain activity. The same study found that people attribute
more importance to dream content than to similar thought content that occurs while they are awake.
Americans were more likely to report that they would intentionally miss their flight if they dreamt of
their plane crashing than if they thought of their plane crashing the night before flying (while awake),
and that they would be as likely to miss their flight if they dreamt of their plane crashing the night
before their flight as if there was an actual plane crash on the route they intended to take.
Participants in the study were more likely to perceive dreams to be meaningful when the content of
dreams was in accordance with their beliefs and desires while awake. They were more likely to view
a positive dream about a friend to be meaningful than a positive dream about someone they disliked,
for example, and were more likely to view a negative dream about a person they disliked as
meaningful than a negative dream about a person they liked.
According to surveys, it is common for people to feel their dreams are predicting subsequent life
events.[71] Psychologists have explained these experiences in terms of memory biases, namely a
selective memory for accurate predictions and distorted memory so that dreams are retrospectively
fitted onto life experiences.[71] The multi-faceted nature of dreams makes it easy to find connections
between dream content and real events.[72] The term "veridical dream" has been used to indicate
dreams that reveal or contain truths not yet known to the dreamer, whether future events or secrets.
[73]
In one experiment, subjects were asked to write down their dreams in a diary. This prevented the
selective memory effect, and the dreams no longer seemed accurate about the future.[74] Another
experiment gave subjects a fake diary of a student with apparently precognitive dreams. This diary
described events from the person's life, as well as some predictive dreams and some non-predictive
dreams. When subjects were asked to recall the dreams they had read, they remembered more of
the successful predictions than unsuccessful ones.[75]
The cheshire cat, John Tenniel (1820–1914), illustration in Alice's Adventures in Wonderland, 1866 edition.
Dreams have also featured in fantasy and speculative fiction since the 19th century. One of the best-
known dream worlds is Wonderland from Lewis Carroll's Alice's Adventures in Wonderland, as well
as Looking-Glass Land from its sequel, Through the Looking-Glass. Unlike many dream worlds,
Carroll's logic is like that of actual dreams, with transitions and flexible causality.
Other fictional dream worlds include the Dreamlands of H. P. Lovecraft's Dream Cycle[78] and The
Neverending Story's[79] world of Fantastica, which includes places like the Desert of Lost Dreams, the
Sea of Possibilities and the Swamps of Sadness. Dreamworlds, shared hallucinations and other
alternate realities feature in a number of works by Philip K. Dick, such as The Three Stigmata of
Palmer Eldritch and Ubik. Similar themes were explored by Jorge Luis Borges, for instance in The
Circular Ruins.
Modern popular culture often conceives of dreams, as did Freud, as expressions of the dreamer's
deepest fears and desires.[80] In speculative fiction, the line between dreams and reality may be
blurred even more in service to the story.[81] Dreams may be psychically invaded or manipulated
(Dreamscape, 1984; the Nightmare on Elm Street films, 1984–2010; Inception, 2010) or even come
literally true (as in The Lathe of Heaven, 1971).[80]
Lucidity
Main article: Lucid dream
Lucid dreaming is the conscious perception of one's state while dreaming. In this state the dreamer
may often have some degree of control over their own actions within the dream or even the
characters and the environment of the dream. Dream control has been reported to improve with
practiced deliberate lucid dreaming, but the ability to control aspects of the dream is not necessary
for a dream to qualify as "lucid"—a lucid dream is any dream during which the dreamer knows they
are dreaming.[82] The occurrence of lucid dreaming has been scientifically verified.[83]
"Oneironaut" is a term sometimes used for those who lucidly dream.
In 1975, psychologist Keith Hearne successfully recorded a communication from a dreamer
experiencing a lucid dream. On April 12, 1975, after agreeing to move his eyes left and right upon
becoming lucid, the subject and Hearne's co-author on the resulting article, Alan Worsley,
successfully carried out this task.[84] Years later, psychophysiologist Stephen LaBerge conducted
similar work including:
Recollection
Further information: Dream diary
The recollection of dreams is extremely unreliable, though it is a skill that can be trained. Dreams
can usually be recalled if a person is awakened while dreaming.[87] Women tend to have more
frequent dream recall than men.[87] Dreams that are difficult to recall may be characterized by
relatively little affect, and factors such as salience, arousal, and interference play a role in dream
recall. Often, a dream may be recalled upon viewing or hearing a random trigger or stimulus.
The salience hypothesis proposes that dream content that is salient, that is, novel, intense, or
unusual, is more easily remembered. There is considerable evidence that vivid, intense, or unusual
dream content is more frequently recalled.[88] A dream journal can be used to assist dream recall, for
personal interest or psychotherapy purposes.
Adults report remembering around two dreams per week, on average.[89][90] Unless a dream is
particularly vivid and if one wakes during or immediately after it, the content of the dream is typically
not remembered.[91] Recording or reconstructing dreams may one day assist with dream recall. Using
the permitted non-invasive technologies, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)
and electromyography (EMG), researchers have been able to identify basic dream imagery,[92] dream
speech activity[93] and dream motor behavior (such as walking and hand movements).[94][95]
In line with the salience hypothesis, there is considerable evidence that people who have more vivid,
intense or unusual dreams show better recall. There is evidence that continuity of consciousness is
related to recall. Specifically, people who have vivid and unusual experiences during the day tend to
have more memorable dream content and hence better dream recall. People who score high on
measures of personality traits associated with creativity, imagination, and fantasy, such as openness
to experience, daydreaming, fantasy proneness, absorption, and hypnotic susceptibility, tend to
show more frequent dream recall.[88] There is also evidence for continuity between the bizarre
aspects of dreaming and waking experience. That is, people who report more bizarre experiences
during the day, such as people high in schizotypy (psychosis proneness), have more frequent dream
recall and also report more frequent nightmares.[88]
Miscellany
Illusion of reality
Main article: Dream argument
Some philosophers have proposed that what we think of as the "real world" could be or is an illusion
(an idea known as the skeptical hypothesis about ontology). The first recorded mention of the idea
was in the 4th century BCE by Zhuangzi, and in Eastern philosophy, the problem has been named
the "Zhuangzi Paradox."
He who dreams of drinking wine may weep when morning comes; he who dreams of weeping may
in the morning go off to hunt. While he is dreaming he does not know it is a dream, and in his dream
he may even try to interpret a dream. Only after he wakes does he know it was a dream. And
someday there will be a great awakening when we know that this is all a great dream. Yet the stupid
believe they are awake, busily and brightly assuming they understand things, calling this man ruler,
that one herdsman—how dense! Confucius and you are both dreaming! And when I say you are
dreaming, I am dreaming, too. Words like these will be labeled the Supreme Swindle. Yet, after ten
thousand generations, a great sage may appear who will know their meaning, and it will still be as
though he appeared with astonishing speed.[96]
The idea also is discussed in Hindu and Buddhist writings.[97] It was formally introduced to Western
philosophy by Descartes in the 17th century in his Meditations on First Philosophy.
Absent-minded transgression
Dreams of absent-minded transgression (DAMT) are dreams wherein the dreamer absent-mindedly
performs an action that he or she has been trying to stop (one classic example is of a quitting
smoker having dreams of lighting a cigarette). Subjects who have had DAMT have reported waking
with intense feelings of guilt. One study found a positive association between having these dreams
and successfully stopping the behavior.[98]
Non-REM dreams
Hypnogogic and hypnopompic dreams, dreamlike states shortly after falling asleep and shortly
before awakening, and dreams during stage 2 of NREM-sleep, also occur, but are shorter than
REM-dreams.[99][100]
Daydreams
Main article: Daydream
Hallucination
Main article: Hallucination
A hallucination, in the broadest sense of the word, is a perception in the absence of a stimulus. In a
stricter sense, hallucinations are perceptions in a conscious and awake state, in the absence of
external stimuli, and have qualities of real perception, in that they are vivid, substantial, and located
in external objective space. The latter definition distinguishes hallucinations from the related
phenomena of dreaming, which does not involve wakefulness.
Nightmare
Main article: Nightmare
A nightmare is an unpleasant dream that can cause a strong negative emotional response from the
mind, typically fear or horror, but also despair, anxiety and great sadness. The dream may contain
situations of danger, discomfort, psychological or physical terror. Sufferers usually awaken in a state
of distress and may be unable to return to sleep for a prolonged period of time.[105]
Night terror
Main article: Night terror
A night terror, also known as a sleep terror or pavor nocturnus, is a parasomnia disorder that
predominantly affects children, causing feelings of terror or dread. Night terrors should not be
confused with nightmares, which are bad dreams that cause the feeling of horror or fear.[106]
Déjà vu
Main article: Déjà vu
One theory of déjà vu attributes the feeling of having previously seen or experienced something to
having dreamed about a similar situation or place, and forgetting about it until one seems to be
mysteriously reminded of the situation or the place while awake.[107]