Personality in Childhood and Academic Performance
Personality in Childhood and Academic Performance
Personality in Childhood and Academic Performance
Andrea Constantinou 1, Tilmann von Soest 1,2, Henrik Daae Zachrisson 3, Rosa Cheesman 1 † *
†
Joint senior authors.
Data availability: The data are available for researchers with study questions that fall within the
general aims of MoBa. Approval from a Norwegian regional committee for medical and health
and guidelines for access to data are found at [ www.fhi.no/moba-en ]. Enquiries can be sent to
[[email protected]]. Data from the Norwegian Educational Database can be accessed via
Acknowledgements & Funding: RC and EY are supported by the Research Council of Norway
(288083). This paper is part of the research project “The Choice Architecture of Admission to
2
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
Education (ArcEd)” supported by the Research Council of Norway (grant no. 325245). HDZ and
EY are supported by the ERC consolidator grant EQOP “Socioeconomic gaps in language
development and school achievement: Mechanisms of inequality and opportunity” (ERC CoG
Ethical approval: The establishment of MoBa and initial data collection was based on a licence
from the Norwegian Data Protection Agency and approval from The Regional Committees for
Medical and Health Research Ethics. The MoBa cohort is now based on regulations related to the
Norwegian Health Registry Act. The current study was approved by The Regional Committees
Abstract
This study investigates associations between personality traits at age eight and academic
performance between ages 10 to 14, controlling for family confounds. Sibling data from the
Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study (MoBa) were used (n = 9,701). First, we estimated
overall associations between Big Five personality traits and academic performance, including
literacy, numeracy, and foreign language. Second, we added sibling fixed effects to remove
unmeasured confounders shared by siblings as well as rating bias. Openness to Experience
(between-person β=0.22 [95% CI 0.21 - 0.24]) and Conscientiousness (between-person β=0.18
[95% CI 0.16 - 0.20]) were most strongly related to educational performance. Agreeableness
(between-person β=0.06 [95% CI -0.08 - 0.04]) and Extraversion (between-person β=0.02 [95%
CI 0.00 - 0.04]) showed small associations with educational performance. Neuroticism had a
moderate negative association (between-person beta=-0.14 [95% CI -0.15 - 0.11]). All
associations between personality and performance were robust to confounding: the within-family
estimates from sibling fixed effects models overlapped with the between-person effects. Finally,
childhood personality was equally predictive of educational performance across ages and
genders. Although family background is influential for academic achievement, it does not
confound associations with personality. Childhood personality traits reflect unbiased and
consistent individual differences in educational potential.
Academic performance in childhood and adolescence strongly predicts success and well-
being (Andersen et al., 2019). As such, it is important to understand why individual differences
in achievement arise. A large body of research has identified factors contributing to academic
performance. Evidence shows that cognitive resources such as general cognitive ability are
among the best predictors of academic performance (Laidra et al., 2007). However, there is also
a strong and growing interest in a range of individual characteristics that are often summarized as
“non-cognitive” skills, such as personality traits, motivation, academic mindsets, social skills,
academic perseverance, and learning strategies, that also play an essential role in shaping
students’ everyday learning behavior and their academic performance (Brandt et al., 2020;
academic performance (Cunha & Heckman, 2008; Richardson et al., 2012), to date, a limited
number of studies have evaluated the importance of primary school-age personality on later
academic outcomes and the Big Five, which is the predominant theoretical framework for
describing personality (McCrae & Costa, 1999), Mammadov (2021) synthesized independent
personality and primary school educational performance over the last 30 years. Overall, this
meta-analysis shows evidence that previous studies have mainly used correlational or cross-
sectional research designs to examine the associations between personality and academic
performance. In such designs, results may be confounded by factors that are systematically
5
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
related to personality and academic performance. Such factors include household environment
such as socio-economic status, shared school influences, systematic parental rating bias, and
genetic effects (Damian & Roberts, 2015; Paulhus et al., 1999; Serna, 2004). Importantly, even
after extensive controls for these known confounds, observational studies provide limited causal
knowledge if unmeasured confounds and reverse causality are at play. To date, we are unaware
of any studies estimating associations between the Big Five and academic performance in
primary/lower secondary school level (students aged from 10 to 14) whilst controlling for
unmeasured confounds. Thus, the main contribution of this study is the application of a sibling
fixed-effects design, which allows us to solve the limitations of previous studies by removing
influences) as well as rating bias (shared rating bias due to having the same parent).
behavior, thoughts, and feelings. Although personality traits show changes in mean levels across
the lifespan, there seems to be substantial stability even from childhood (Caspi et al., 2005;
Hampson & Goldberg, 2006). Decades of research in this field have uncovered five basic
personality factors that have repeatedly and consistently emerged upon analyses of the traits
most used in psychological instruments to describe people. The Big Five model, as proposed by
Costa and McCrae (1992), includes the following major personality dimensions: Openness to
Experience (O), Conscientiousness (C), Extraversion (E), Agreeableness (A), and Neuroticism
(N).
The Big Five is considered a robust theoretical framework for describing personality
(McCrae & Costa, 1999) and is one of the most popular and empirically supported models
6
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
(Allik et al., 2018; Feher & Vernon, 2021; Miller et al., 2003; Saucier, 2009). Even though some
studies indicated that it might be inappropriate for children since it was based on a model
initially developed for adults (De Fruyt et al., 2006; Measelle et al., 2005), a comprehensive
synthesis of 54 meta-analyses conducted by Zell & Lesick (2021), supports the notion that the
Big Five is valid for educational research with children, and has robust associations with
performance.
The Associations between the Big Five and Childhood Academic Performance (students aged
from 10 to 14)
From a theoretical perspective, several Big Five personality traits may be related to
school performance because they describe individual characteristics that may facilitate learning
and test performance. Conscientiousness may be of particular importance because students who
score high on Conscientiousness tend to be self-disciplined, effective at carrying out tasks and
organized (McCrae & John, 1992). These characteristics are expected to help students’
positive approach to learning (Vermetten et al., 2001), autonomous motivation, and critical
thinking (Bidjerano & Dai, 2007), which, in turn, may have positive effects on students’ success.
Furthermore, given that Extraversion and Agreeableness are both interpersonal traits, their
relationships with performance might be related to how student performance is measured. For
example, students who cooperate, share, and listen in the classroom, may get high-performance
evaluations in group course projects, while a human evaluator can be biased by students’
personalities (Mammadov, 2021). Finally, high levels of Neuroticism may have aversive
7
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
effects on academic performance because students with high scores on this personality
dimension tend to exhibit higher stress and anxiety levels. As a result, they tend to perform
poorer on tests, exams, or other evaluation assessments (Ackerman et al., 2011; O’Connor &
Paunonen, 2007).
The associations between the Big Five personality traits and academic performance at
cross-sectional observation studies (Borghans et al., 2016; Mammadov, 2021; Poropat, 2009;
Vedel & Poropat, 2017). The first systematic review of correlations between Big Five
personality traits and educational performance was presented by Hough et al. (1990) and has
recently been followed by more rigorous and systematic reviews and meta-analyses (Andersen et
al., 2019; Demetriou et al., 2019; Mammadov, 2021; Poropat, 2009, 2014a; Richardson et al.,
2012). Collectively, these have demonstrated that personality has reliable and substantial
associations with academic performance. More specifically, Conscientiousness was the best
2021), while Openness to Experience demonstrated a weak and positive overall association
performance in primary and lower secondary education (r = 0.15) but not at subsequent levels. In
contrast, Neuroticism was not significantly related to school performance outcomes (r = −0.02).
0.09). Overall, the mean effect size estimates reported in the meta-analysis of Mammadov (2021)
were slightly larger than those reported in previous meta-analyses (Gatzka & Hell, 2018;
McAbee & Oswald, 2013; Poropat, 2009; Trapmann et al., 2007; Vedel, 2014); however, given
8
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
that Mammadov’s meta-analytic sample was much larger and included all relevant original
studies from previous meta-analyses, his estimates are the most trustworthy.
Finally, previous research suggests that the strength of association between the Big Five
and academic performance may change depending on the level of education (Poropat, 2009). In
his meta-analysis, Mammadov (2021) found that although Conscientiousness and Neuroticism
were equally important across all educational levels, Openness to Experience, Extraversion, and
secondary and post-secondary education. One possible explanation for these findings is the
different assessment practices in post-secondary education or the fact that students’ performance
at primary school is assessed based on a standard curriculum, which tends to vary widely at
subsequent levels of education (Tatar, 1998). Previous studies indicate that the effect of early
personality on education attenuates with time; however, these studies have not consistently
measured educational performance over time. In all, it is unclear to what extent childhood
Rater Bias and Shared Familial Confounding: the Limitations of Previous Studies
To sum up, previous research provides consistent evidence that students’ personality is
related to academic performance. Nonetheless, somewhat neglected issues are rating bias and
shared familial confounding. Firstly, most research in the field is based on parental reports of
children’s personality. However, when parents rate their children’s personality, they typically
provide biased ratings since they rate them more favorably than others do (Laidra et al., 2006;
Poropat, 2013); therefore, rater bias have taken into account in these studies. Based on this, one
could argue that rating bias is a general problem in measuring childhood personality because
9
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
interpreting these ratings has implications for causal relationships between personality and
academic performance.
be considered. For example, personality traits and academic performance are heritable
characteristics, with heritability estimates for personality in adolescents and adults of ~40%
(Vukasovic & Bratko, 2015). There is a lack of studies on the heritability of Big Five personality
in children; however, Spengler et al. (2012) estimated Big Five personality to be >50% heritable
in 202 ten-year-old German twin pairs. Given the small size of this study, it is uncertain to what
personality. Some of the genes influencing achievement may also influence personality, making
it likely that associations between these variables are not only due to causal phenotypic effects
Based on the above, there is a need for studies on personality and performance that
researchers can adjust for rating bias and confounding factors shared by members of the same
family. For this reason, in this study, we use a sibling fixed-effects design, which adjusts for all
stable factors that have equal effects on siblings in the same family; this will allow us to go
beyond the limitations of previous studies. This design essentially estimates to what extent the
differences between sibling pairs in their personality can explain differences in their school
performance. There are broadly two types of familial confounding: by environments shared by
siblings or by shared genetic variants. In the case of familial environmental confounds explaining
the entire effect of personality on educational performance, we would expect the effect to go
null. Since siblings, on average only share 50% of their genes, in a scenario with only genetic
confounds explaining the effect, we would expect the association to be halved under genetic
10
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
confounding (see Figure 1). With any mixture of these two types of familial confounds, we
would expect the effect to go to a place between halved and null. In the scenario of an unbiased
effect, we would expect the effect to be close to what is found when not adjusting for
unmeasured familial confounds (i.e., “unbiased”; see Figure 1). Because childhood personality in
this study was rated by the same mother, within-pair differences cannot be due to systematic
rating bias (to the extent that it is the same for both siblings). Moreover, the use of fixed-effects
models means that all unobserved confounds shared between siblings, such as the home
environment, schools, neighborhoods, and genetics, are controlled for (again, at least to the
extent that these are experienced similarly by members of the pair). As a result, the sibling fixed
effects design provides better information about causal associations than correlational research
Figure 1. Expected between and within family associations under the assumptions of no
confounding, genetic confounding, and environmental confounding using the sibling control
design.
11
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
Hypotheses
Using a population-based sample of siblings to adjust for factors shared by members of
the same family, we aim to estimate the association between Big Five personality at age eight
and academic performance from age 10 to 14. Previous evidence suggests that confounding
systematic parental rating bias, and genetic effects, are likely to be at play. However, due to the
strong associations between personality and achievement in the literature, we hypothesize that
mid-childhood Five Factor personality still has a non-zero within-family effect on academic
performance over and beyond confounds. Given the importance of Conscientiousness in previous
studies and personality being strongly influenced by factors not shared by siblings, we
hypothesize that Conscientiousness will also be the strongest predictor of academic performance
within families.
Methods
Data were used from the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study (MoBa) conducted
pregnancy cohort study following over 100,000 pregnancies. Recruitment started in 1999, and
both mothers and fathers were invited. In 2008, the goal of more than 100,000 pregnancies was
reached (Magnus et al., 2016). In MoBa, there were 106,658 children born after August 2002,
with questionnaire data linked to register data from Statistics Norway. We present the sample
selection in Figure 2. We excluded the second and third child of multiple births (i.e., twin #2 and
triplet #2 and #3), children with lacking paternal id in the registry (to ascertain that they are full
siblings), children who died before the age of 8, children without a record in the Medical Birth
12
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
Registry of Norway, children without siblings in MoBa, and missing or invalid questionnaire
data at eight years. This resulted in an analysis sample of 9,701 children. Among these, 4,770
children were part of sibling pairs, 157 were part of sibling trios, and four children were part of
sibling quadruples. The average sibling age difference was 2.58 years with SD = 0.99. Five
hundred and sixty-six children had missing data on national test scores, but valid data on
personality to be applied in the sibling fixed effect model. The analysis sample consisted of
Dataset
n = 104,882 Lack of parental id in ssb
n = 581
Dataset
n = 104,241 Died before the age of 8
n = 275
Dataset
n = 103,966 8-year questionnaire not
submitted n = 12,602
Dataset
n = 91,364 Not in the Medical Birth
Registry of Norway n= 149
Dataset
n = 91,215 Mother participated with 1
pregnancy n = 66,953
13
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
Dataset
n = 24,262 Non-response on 8-year
questionnaire n = 11,910
Dataset
n = 12,352 Invalid HIPiC personality
data n = 48
Dataset
n = 12,304
Sibling cluster with <2
valid observations n= 2.603
Dataset
n = 9,701
Measures
At age eight, mothers rated their children’s personality using a validated Norwegian short
form of the Hierarchical Personality Inventory for Children (HiPIC; Mervielde & De Fruyt,
1999; Vollrath et al., 2016). The HiPIC short form assesses each of the Big Five Factors using
six items. We calculated standardized scores for each scale using a Graded Response Item
Academic performance was measured using standardized national test results in Grades
5, 8, and 9 for literacy, numeracy, and foreign language (i.e., English). The test in English was
not given in 9th Grade. The tests were obtained through linkage to Norway’s National Education
Database. The tests were introduced in 2007 to monitor school development. The tests are
compulsory, with 96% of all students in Norway taking them. Students with special needs and
those following introductory language courses may be exempt. At each age, we calculated a
14
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
mean score of numeracy and literacy but kept foreign language separate due to no measurement
in 9th Grade. We standardized by the complete registry population means and standard deviations
Analyses
We estimated linear mixed models by means of the mixed function in Stata 17.0. Each
child had up to three lines of data representing tests in 5th, 8th, and 9th Grade with an age variable
centered to 5th Grade. The siblings were nested within a family cluster variable. We estimated
correlated random intercepts and random slopes for age at the child (2nd) level and family (3rd)
level. The analyses were adjusted for maternal parity (i.e., sibling birth order effects). Effects of
personality estimated interacting with both age, gender, and age*gender. With the centering of
age and gender (centered to gender grand mean), any additional contributions to the model from
these interactions indexes that either the effect of personality on academic performance increases
personality over age differs by gender (i.e., age*gender*personality). For the sibling fixed effects
model, we included mean personality for the child clusters and centered the personality variables
Results
The average maternal age was 30.47 years (SD = 3.93) (min = 18; max = 45). The sibling
intraclass correlations for Five Factor personality were: Conscientiousness (.22), Openness to
Experience (.29), Extraversion (.23), Neuroticism (.23), and Agreeableness (.25). The
participants scored 0.14 SD (i.e., foreign language 5th Grade) to 0.48 SD (i.e., total academic
performance) above the population mean. The siblings were moderately correlated with each
15
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
other on national academic performance tests, ranging from intra-class correlations (ICC) = .27
(i.e., literacy 9th Grade) to .45 (i.e., foreign language 8th Grade) (see Table 1).
Table 1
We tested the overall association between all personality traits and academic performance
(a composite of literacy and numeracy) by using linear mixed models. In a first series of models,
we estimated associations between the Big Five personality traits and overall academic
performance between individuals (black), and in a next series of results, within family effects
were estimated by using sibling fixed effect analyses (gray), as shown in Table 3.
16
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
Figure 3. The associations between the Big Five and overall academic performance between
individuals and within families
As displayed in Figure 4, analyses were repeated to show associations between Big Five
personality traits and all three school subjects (symbols) separately, both between individuals
Conscientiousness
Figure 3 shows that the association between Conscientiousness and overall academic
performance was positive (between-person beta=0.18 [95% CI 0.16 - 0.20]). When controlling
found no evidence for confounding: the within-family effect (within-family beta=0.20 [95% CI
0.17 - 0.22] was overlapping with the between-person effect. We then explored differences in
associations between Conscientiousness and school subjects (i.e., literacy, numeracy, and foreign
language) and found them to be 0.14, 0.19, 0.14 (all p < .001). Associations were unattenuated
within families, estimated at 0.15, 0.21, and 0.16 for literacy, numeracy, and foreign language,
academic performance were moderated by gender or age. Analyses showed no significant gender
differences in the association and no evidence of a change in the association over time (p > .05).
Openness to Experience
Figure 3 shows that the association between Openness to Experience and overall
academic performance was positive (between-person beta=0.22 [95% CI 0.21 - 0.24]). When
controlling for confounding of the association between Openness to Experience and academic
beta=0.24 [95% CI 0.22 - 0.27] was overlapping with the between-person effect. We then
explored differences in associations between Openness to Experience and school subjects (i.e.,
literacy, numeracy, and foreign language) and found them to be 0.21, 0.20, 0.20 (all p <.001).
18
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
Associations were unattenuated within families, estimated at 0.22 (p <.001), 0.23 (p <.01), and
0.21 (p <.001) for literacy, numeracy, and foreign language, respectively (see Figure 4).
and academic performance were moderated by gender or age. Analyses showed no significant
gender differences in the association and no evidence of a change in the association over time.
Extraversion
Figure 3 shows that the association between Extraversion and overall academic
performance was positive (between-person beta=0.02 [95% CI -0.00 - 0.04]). When controlling
for confounding of the association between Extraversion and academic performance, we found
no evidence for confounding: the within-family effect (within-family beta=0.04 [95% CI 0.02 -
0.07]) overlapped with the between-person effect. We then explored differences in associations
between Extraversion and school subjects (i.e., literacy, numeracy, and foreign language) and
found them to be 0.03 (p <.001), 0.00 (p >.05), 0.03 (p <.01). Associations were unattenuated
within families, estimated at 0.05 (p <.001), 0.03 (p <.05), and 0.03 (p <.05) for literacy,
academic performance were moderated by gender or age. Analyses showed no significant gender
differences in the association and no evidence of a change in the association over time.
Neuroticism
Figure 3 shows that the association between Neuroticism and overall academic
performance was negative (between-person beta=-0.14 [95% CI -0.15 - 0.11]). When controlling
for confounding of the association between Neuroticism and academic performance, we found
19
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
no evidence for confounding: the within-family effect (within-family beta=-0.16 [95% CI -0.18
- 0.13] was overlapping with the between-person effect. We then explored differences in
associations between Neuroticism and school subjects (i.e., literacy, numeracy, and foreign
language) and found them to be -0.10, -0.14, -0.09. Associations were unattenuated within
families, estimated at -0.12, -0.17, -0.11 for literacy, numeracy, and foreign language,
academic performance were moderated by gender or age. Analyses showed no significant gender
differences in the association and no evidence of a change in the association over time.
Agreeableness
Figure 3 shows that the association between Agreeableness and overall academic
performance was positive (between-person beta=0.06 [95% CI -0.08 - 0.04]). When controlling
for confounding of the association between Agreeableness and academic performance, we found
no evidence for confounding: the within-family effect (within-family beta=0.05 [95% CI -0.09 -
0.04] was overlapping with the between-person effect. We then explored differences in
associations between Agreeableness and school subjects (i.e., literacy, numeracy, and foreign
language) and found them to be 0.06, 0.05, and 0.06. Associations were unattenuated within
families, estimated at 0.07, 0.06, and 0.06 for literacy, numeracy, and foreign language,
academic performance were moderated by gender or age. Analyses showed no significant gender
differences in the association and no evidence of a change in the association over time.
20
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
Discussion
from 10 to 14 years. Moreover, these relationships were unattenuated within families for all
personality traits and all educational outcomes. What is more, our findings indicate that
childhood personality is equally predictive for educational performance in girls and boys and
across ages 10 to 14. The strength of the association between personality and educational
performance varied across traits: Openness to Experience and Conscientiousness were the
had a modest positive association with educational performance. It is worth noting that previous
research reported that Conscientiousness is the most important personality trait in predicting
academic performance (Almlund et al., 2011; Mammadov, 2021; Poropat, 2009; Trapmann et al.,
2007; Vedel, 2014). However, in our study, Openness to Experience had a larger effect, followed
of Openness to Experience, the second best Big Five predictor of academic performance (Soto &
John, 2017), the results of this study are not expected to change the findings of previous studies
substantially.
cognitive predictor of academic performance in the mid-childhood. Our results are in line with
previous studies, showing that Conscientiousness emerges as a strong and consistent predictor
(Mammadov, 2021). This personality dimension is associated with goal setting, concentration,
effort regulation (Poropat, 2009) and self-control (MacCann et al., 2009). Conscientiousness may
as such be the expression of a greater ability to self-regulate, leading to higher levels of time on
21
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
task and consequently greater learning (Poropat, 2014). These characteristics and skills are
important for students to be successful across all academic domains and educational stages.
performance in mid-childhood. Our results are in line with previous studies where Openness to
Schouwenburg (1996), Openness to Experience seems to reflect “the ideal student” (p. 327), who
is foresighted, smart, and resourceful. In addition, other studies have reported that Openness to
Experience was found to be positively correlated with an active approach to learning (Vermetten
et al., 2001), motivation to learn (Tempelaar et al., 2007), critical thinking (Bidjerano & Dai,
2007), and it has the strongest negative correlation with absenteeism (Lounsbury et al., 2004).
These features of students high on Openness to Experience may explain why this personality
Previous studies on the Big Five and educational performance have provided mixed results
extraverted students are expected to perform better academically due to their higher energy
levels and positive attitude that leads to a desire to learn and understand. In contrast, Eysenck
(1992) suggested that extraverts would be more likely to socialize and pursue other activities
than studying, and as a result, this would lead to lower levels of performance. Consequently, it is
less clear how Extraversion affects educational performance. Empirically there is weak evidence
for its practical significance, while one widely acknowledged argument is that its influence
22
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
occurs indirectly through some mediating variables (Mammadov, 2021; Richardson et al., 2012;
association with educational performance. Indeed, our results are in line with previous studies
which found negative associations between Neuroticism and academic performance (Biderman
& Reddock, 2012; Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham, 2003; Gerbino et al., 2018). Moreover, in
the same vein, the meta-analysis of Mammadov (2021) indicated that Neuroticism does not seem
statistically significant. One possible explanation is that neurotic students tend to be more
distracted from their tasks due to their emotional state, which leads to poorer performance and
less learning. On the other hand, it is worth noting that other studies reported significant positive
correlations (Culjak & Mlacic, 2014; Lounsbury et al., 2004; Steele-Johnson & Leas, 2013).
However, the negative effect of Neuroticism seems theoretically more plausible as neurotic
students tend to demonstrate higher levels of anxiety and stress that, in turn, can result in poor
academic performance on tests or other assessments (Ackerman et al., 2011; O’Connor &
Paunonen, 2007).
performance. This result is also in line with the meta-analysis of Mammadov (2021), which
with learning processes (De Raad & Schouwenburg, 1996). This idea is consistent with another
study that found Agreeableness to be linked to compliance with teacher instructions, effort, and
staying focused on learning tasks (Vermetten et al., 2001). Prior studies have to a great extent
23
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
used educational outcomes evaluated by the teacher. In such scenarios, the interpersonal skills
In previous studies, there has been considerable concern regarding the confounding
effects of systematic rating bias (Brandt et al., 2021; Laidra et al., 2006). In our study, siblings
always had their personality evaluated by the same rater; their mother. Although siblings were
correlated with approximately .25 in their measured personality, which could partly reflect
maternal rating bias, we did not find the within-family effects of personality on educational
performance to be considerably attenuated. This does not prevent maternal rating bias from
confounding other associations with personality. However, it appears that it does not confound
By design, the sibling fixed effect approach adjusts for all stable factors that have equal
effects on siblings in the same family. This represents the socio-economic position and stable
aspects of the parents but only captures half of the shared genetic effects on personality and
to genetic confounding, we would expect a 50% reduction in the effect, something we did not
observe. This could imply that the association is not confounded by genetic variants having an
effect on both personality and academic performance, even though both traits are heritable. Full
siblings are, on average, 50% genetically different, and the within-family effects of personality
improve their learning, such as friends, libraries, engagement in homework, and interactions with
adults. This type of child-driven genetic effect mediated through the environment is termed
24
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
“active gene-environment correlation” (Plomin et al., 1977; Scarr & McCartney, 1983). Future
studies using measured genotypes such as polygenic scores representing endogenous variation
(Pingault et al., 2018) in personality and non-cognitive skills (Demange, 2021) could potentially
The results of this study show that the associations across gender, age and school
subjects, as potential moderating factors, were all consistent. Concerning gender effects, in this
and boys. As demonstrated by Spinath et al. (2014), there is some evidence to support gender
have been reported to show that the Big Five predicts academic performance differently for boys
and girls are not consistent, while only a few studies examined gender differences in the
associations between academic performance and personality traits (Freudenthaler et al., 2008;
Hicks et al., 2008; Nguyen et al., 2005; Spinath et al., 2010). Furthermore, the meta-analysis
conducted by Mammadov (2021) did not find evidence for gender as a moderator.
As far as the age effects are concerned, previous studies suggested that the strength of
associations between personality traits and academic performance change as students advance
through the education system (Poropat, 2009); therefore, the moderating effect of age is expected
to be parallel to the changes associated with education level (Poropat, 2014). Based on this
argument, the meta-analysis of Mammadov (2021) reported that the effect of age was significant
for Openness to Experience and Extraversion, while subgroup analyses indicated that the
age increased. Also, the results of the same meta-analysis showed that the associations with other
25
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
personality traits and academic performance were not affected by age. In our study, we found the
between 10 and 14 years, and this was expected since our sample consisted only of students
attending the primary level of education and the first year of lower secondary school level. As a
This study is not without limitations. First, our sibling design cannot control for
confounding factors not shared by sibling pairs (e.g., perinatal factors). Confounding factors not
shared by family members is an issue in both sibling comparisons and when comparing unrelated
individuals; it can be mitigated by adjusting for such factors. Second, our study focused on three
core subjects (i.e., literacy, numeracy, and foreign language). However, it would be interesting
for future studies to consider a broader range of academic subjects to test the associations
between personality traits and academic performance other than literacy, numeracy, and foreign
language. Future studies could also use a sibling fixed-effects design to test whether the same
results apply to upper secondary (students aged 15 to 17) and post-secondary (students after 18)
school levels. What is more, in our study, we used a nationwide sample of Norwegian children.
Future research could test whether the results found in Norway are replicable in other countries.
Openness to Experience, schools may develop and implement programs that nurture non-
cognitive skills related to these personality traits, including creative thinking, perseverance, and
not just cognitive skills. For this reason, school-based practices and policies that aim to promote
academic performance should include activities that stimulate healthy personality development.
At the same time, it is also important for teachers to understand students’ preferred approach to
26
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
learning based on their personal characteristics because this influences the quality of learning
outcomes.
Conclusion
The present study reaffirms the critical role of personality traits in explaining academic
performance. First, our results suggest that high Openness to Experience and Conscientiousness
were the personality traits most predictive of educational performance. Agreeableness and
Extraversion had a modest positive association with educational performance, while Neuroticism
for confounding of the association between all personality traits and academic performance using
a sibling fixed effects design; however, we found no evidence for confounding. Third, we found
Ackerman, P. L., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2011). Trait complexes and academic
performance: Old and new ways of examining personality in educational contexts. British
https://doi.org/10.1348/000709910X522564
Allik, J., Hřebíčková, M., & Realo, A. (2018). Unusual configurations of personality traits
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00187
Allport, W. G. (1961). Pattern and growth in personality. New York: Holt, Rienhart and
Winston.
Almlund, M., Duckworth, A. L., Heckman, J. J. & Kautz, T. (2011), Personality Psychology and
53444-6.00001-8
Andersen, S. C., Gensowski, M., Ludeke, S. G., & John, O. (2019). A stable relationship
Biderman, M.D., & Reddock, C.M. (2012). The relationship of scale reliability and validity to
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.12.012
28
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
Bidjerano, T., & Dai, D. Y. (2007). The relationship between the Big-Five model of personality
and self-regulated learning strategies. Learning and Individual Differences, 17(1), 69–81.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2007.02.001
Borghans, L., Golsteyn, B. H., Heckman, J. J., & Humphries, J. E. (2016). What grades and
achievement tests measure. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United
Brandt, N. D., Becker, M., Tetzner, J., Brunner, M., & Kuhl, P. (2021). What teachers and
parents can add to personality ratings of children: Unique associations with academic
https://doi.org/10.1177/0890207020988436
Brandt, N. D., Lechner, C. M., Tetzner, J., & Rammstedt, B. (2020). Personality, cognitive
ability, and academic performance: Differential associations across school subjects and
Briley, D. A., & Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2014). Genetic and environmental continuity in personality
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037091
Bueno D. (2019). Genetics and Learning: How the Genes Influence Educational
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141913
Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2003). Personality traits and academic examination
https://doi.org/10.1002/per.473
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Normal personality assessment in clinical practice: The
https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.4.1.5
Culjak, Z., & Mlacic, B. (2014). The Big-Five Model of Personality and the Success of High
Cunha, F. A., & Heckman, J. J. (2008). Formulating, Identifying and Estimating the Technology
738–782. https://doi.org/10.3368/jhr.43.4.738
Damian, R. I., & Roberts, B. W. (2015). The associations of birth order with personality and
De Fruyt, F., Bartels, M., Van Leeuwen, K. G., De Clercq, B., Decuyper, M., & Mervielde, I.
3514.91.3.538
De Raad, B., & Schouwenburg, H. C. (1996). Personality in learning and education: A review.
0984(199612)10:5<303::AID-PER262>3.0.CO;2-2
Demange, P. A., Malanchini, M., Mallard, T. T., Biroli, P., Cox, S. R., Grotzinger, A. D.,
Tucker-Drob, E. M., Abdellaoui, A., Arseneault, L., van Bergen, E., Boomsma, D. I., Caspi,
A., Corcoran, D. L., Domingue, B. W., Harris, K. M., Ip, H. F., Mitchell, C., Moffitt, T. E.,
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-020-00754-2
Demetriou, A., Kazi, S., Spanoudis, G., & Makris, N. (2019). Predicting school performance
from cognitive ability, self-representation, and personality from primary school to senior
Eysenck, H. J. (1992). Personality and education: The influence of extraversion, Neuroticism and
Feher, A., & Vernon, P. A. (2021). Looking beyond the Big Five: A selective review of
alternatives to the Big Five model of personality. Personality and Individual Differences,
Gatzka, T., & Hell, B. (2018). Openness and post-secondary academic performance: A meta-
Gerbino, M., Zuffianò, A., Eisenberg, N., Castellani, V., Luengo Kanacri, B. P., Pastorelli, C., &
https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12309
Hampson, S. E., & Goldberg, L. R. (2006). A first large cohort study of personality trait stability
over the 40 years between elementary school and midlife. Journal of Personality and Social
Hicks, B. M., Johnson, W., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. (2008). Moderating effects of
personality on the genetic and environmental influences of school grades helps to explain
268. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.671
Hough, L. M., Eaton, N. K., Dunnette, M. D., Kamp, J. D., & McCloy, R. A. (1990). Criterion-
related validities of personality constructs and the effect of response distortion on those
9010.75.5.581
32
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big-Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and
Laidra, K., Allik, J., Harro, M., Merenäkk, L., & Harro, J. (2006). Agreement Among
https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191106287125
Laidra, K., Pullman, H., & Allik, J. (2007). Personality and intelligence as predictors of
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.08.001
MacCann, C., Duckworth, A. L., & Roberts, R. D. (2009). Empirical identification of the major
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2009.03.007
Magnus, P., Birke, C., Vejrup, K., Haugan, A., Alsaker, E., Daltveit, A. K., Handal, M., Haugen,
M., Høiseth, G., Knudsen, G. P., Paltiel, L., Schreuder, P., Tambs, K., Vold, L., &
Stoltenberg, C. (2016) Cohort Profile Update: The Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort
https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyw029
33
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
Mammadov, S. (2021). Big five personality traits and academic performance: A meta-analysis.
Mammadov, S., Cross, T. L., & Ward, T. J. (2018). The Big Five personality predictors of
https://doi.org/10.1080/13598139.2018.1489222
McAbee, S. T., & Oswald, F. L. (2013). The criterion-related validity of personality measures for
532–544. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031748
P. John & R. W. Robins (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp.
McCrae, R., & John, O. (1992). An introduction to the five-factor model and its applications.
Measelle, J. R., John, O. P., Ablow, J. C., Cowan, P. A., & Cowan, C. P. (2005). Can children
provide coherent, stable, and valid self-reports on the Big Five dimensions? A longitudinal
study from ages 5 to 7. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 89(1), 90–106.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.1.90
Mervielde, I., & De Fruyt, F. (1999). The Construction of the Hierarchical Personality Inventory
Miller, J. D., Lynam, D., & Leukefeld, C. (2003). Examining antisocial behavior through the lens
https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.10064
Nguyen, N., Allen, L., & Fraccastoro, K.A. (2005). Personality Predicts Academic Performance:
Exploring the moderating role of gender. Journal of Higher Education Policy and
O’Connor, M. C., & Paunonen, S. V. (2007). Big Five personality predictors of post-secondary
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.03.017
Paulhus, D., Trapnell, P., & Chen, D. (1999). Birth Order Effects on Personality and
Pingault, J. B., O’Reilly, P. F., Schoeler, T., Ploubidis, G. B., Rijsdijk, F., & Dudbridge, F.
(2018). Using genetic data to strengthen causal inference in observational research. Nat Rev
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Loehlin, J. C. (1977). Genotype-environment interaction and
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.84.2.309
35
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
Poropat, A. E. (2009). A meta-analysis of the five-factor model of personality and academic
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2014.05.013
252. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12019
Richardson, M., Abraham, C., & Bond, R. (2012). Psychological correlates of university
Saucier, G. (2009). Recurrent personality dimensions in inclusive lexical studies: Indications for
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00593.x
Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: A theory of
Serna, G. S. (2004). The confounding role of personality in the relation to gender role conflict
https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000096
Spengler, M., Brunner, M., Martin, R., & Lüdtke, O. (2016). The role of personality in predicting
(change in) students’ academic success across four years of secondary school. European
https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000330
Spengler, M., Gottschling, J., & Spinath, F. M. (2012). Personality in childhood - A longitudinal
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.01.019
Spinath, B., Eckert, C., & Steinmayr, R. (2014). Gender differences in school success: What are
the roles of students’ intelligence, personality and motivation? Educational Research, 56(2),
230–243. https://doi.org/10.1080/00131881.2014.898917
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.028
Steele‐Johnson, D., & Leas, K. (2013). Importance of race, gender, and personality in predicting
https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12129
37
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
Tatar, M. (1998). Primary and secondary school teachers’ perceptions and actions regarding their
https://doi.org/10.1177/0143034398192004
Tempelaar, D. T., Gijselaers, W. H., van der Loeff, S. S., & Nijhuis, J. F. H. (2007). A structural
Trapmann, S., Hell, B., Hirn, J. O. W., & Schuler, H. (2007). Meta-analysis of the relationship
between the Big Five and academic success at university. Zeitschrift fur Psychologie,
Vedel, A. (2014). The Big Five and tertiary academic performance: A systematic review and
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.07.011
Vedel, A. & Poropat, A. E. (2017). Personality and Academic Performance. In: Zeigler-Hill V.,
Cham.
Vermetten, Y. J., Lodewijks, H. G., & Vermunt, J. D. (2001). The role of personality traits and
https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1999.1042
38
CHILDHOOD PERSONALITY AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE
Vollrath, M. E., Hampson, S. E., & Torgersen, S. (2016). Constructing a short form of the
hierarchical personality inventory for children (HiPIC): The HiPIC‐30. Personality and
https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000017
Woodfield, R., Jessop, D., & McMillan, L. (2006). Gender differences in undergraduate
https://doi.org/10.1080/03075070500340127
Zell, E. & Lesick, T. (2021). Big Five Personality Traits and Performance: A Quantitative
https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12683