FULLTEXT01
FULLTEXT01
ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
Abstract
motivational system, changing throughout time and place, fluctuating on the interest
and motivation of the individual. Early memories and attachment towards a caregiver,
lay the foundation for later attachment behavior, also known as attachment styles. In
this thesis, an exploratory approach is present. The thesis aims to introduce and
describe the neural correlates of romantic love and romantic attachment. Brain regions
concerned with reward, emotion and thought processing, such as the reward circuitry
network of the brain and the limbic system, are being investigated. So are other brain
areas involved in romantic love and romantic attachment. Research findings suggest
that brain areas responsible for affection, emotional control, learning, memory and
social judgment are all involved in the complex processes of being in love and feeling
romantically attached. These findings are represented by the involvement of the frontal
lobe, cerebral cortex, limbic system, orbitofrontal cortex, and hippocampus, anterior
cingulate cortex (ACC), ventral tegmental area (VTA), caudate tail, including the reward
vasopressin, oxytocin, dopamine, corticosterone and serotonin are all present in the
state of romantic attachment and romantic love. Overlapping evidence confirms the
involvement of the reward circuitry network, together with the limbic system as crucial
Keywords: Attachment, the attachment theory, the limbic system, the reward
Table of Contents
Abstract .................................................................................................................... 2
Introduction ............................................................................................................. 6
The hippocampus............................................................................................ 30
Vasopressin. .................................................................................................... 36
Oxytocin. ......................................................................................................... 37
Dopamine. ....................................................................................................... 40
Corticosterone. ................................................................................................ 41
Serotonin. ........................................................................................................ 41
Discussion .............................................................................................................. 42
Conclusion .............................................................................................................. 50
Appendix A ............................................................................................................. 64
Intimacy .......................................................................................................... 64
Passion ............................................................................................................ 64
Commitment ................................................................................................... 65
Appendix B ............................................................................................................. 66
Appendix C ............................................................................................................. 68
Appendix D ............................................................................................................. 69
Appendix E ............................................................................................................. 70
Introduction
a change of the state of mind implies a change in the state of the brain and vice versa”
(Friedrich, Wood, Scherer, & Neuper, 2014, p. 1). What are the neural correlates making
about and towards their significant other, or in pain or despair of lost love. When it
comes to romantic love, obsession and addiction are common experiences. Romantic
love can often result in the absence of self-control, loss of reasoning, and a constant
longing for the other individual. The abnormal behavior of people reporting being
intensely in love is linked to the same behavior present in addicted behavior, also
observed in individuals suffering from drug addiction (De Boer, Van Buel, & Ter Horst,
levels of the brains chemistry, differentiating the process of being in love from not being
motivation, enamoredness, worship, amorousness, loyalty and sexual attraction are all
scientific literature is often described mostly as feelings, science has moved away from
this assumption (Fischer H. , 2006a). Current research believes that romantic love is not
system, changing throughout time and place fluctuating on the interest and motivation
7
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
of the individual. This motivational system involves many emotions as well as thought
processes as the result of the wish or action of pursuing and maintaining a romantic
relationship (Fischer, Aron, & Brown, 2005). It is characterized both as a state common
for everyone in love, with subjective traits making the experience unique for the ones in
love.
involved. The internal motivation of carrying on one’s genes, combined with the
emotional and thought processing of reciprocating actions with one’s partner, confirms
that we prefer our partner over other possible fitting individuals (Fischer, Aron,
The bond of attachment towards a romantic partner, is not only linked to sexual
drive, which can fluctuate in a relationship (Acevedo, Aron, Fischer, & Brown, 2012).
Romantic love also involves components of intimacy towards the chosen one and the
romantic relationships.
interactions with its caretakers. This behavior is suggested to follow the individual
throughout their lives (Bowlby, 1988), influencing learned social behavior towards our
romantic partner (Shaver, Hazan, & Bradshaw, 1988). Romantic attachment, interaction
and behavior are similar to the dynamics found in close relationships with family
members, where the sexual component is naturally missing, as we are not sexually
attachment styles are suggested to follow the individual in close relationships and
respected and frequently used scientific theory. The attachment styles include; secure,
later contribution from Main and Solomon’s research from 1990. When it comes to the
neurological view on attachment, and the different attachment styles, they are suggested
This thesis aims to introduce and describe the neural correlates of romantic love
and romantic attachment. It is exploratory in the sense that a lot of attention has been
given to the topic – over many years. The discovery of neural correlates is considered
Consequently, the central question in this thesis will be; which neural correlates are
Being in love and feeling romantically attached to a partner, often demands a lot
of thought and emotional processing, together with being a highly rewarding experience.
Taking this into consideration, I want to investigate the reward, emotion and thought
processing areas of the brain, namely the reward circuitry network and the limbic
system and their involvement when it comes to romantic love and romantic attachment.
When it comes to the different attachment styles, it is also suggested that the differences
9
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
correlates. The layout of the thesis starts with a theoretical explanation of romantic love
and attachment. The attachment theory from Bowlby and Ainsworth’s (1969/1982)
relationships. After this an introduction to the neural correlates of romantic love and
attachment is presented. Brain areas such as; the frontal lobe, cerebral cortex, limbic
system (as in the amygdala, hippocampus, hypothalamus, insula) together with the
orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), ventral tegmental area
(VTA) and the caudate tail are presented separately, and sometimes together, when it
The reward circuits of the brain, together with the neurotransmitters such as
separately, and together, when found necessary in relation to romantic love and
attachment. Regarding the different attachment styles and neural correlates, they are
included when distinctive findings in neuroscience are present when considering their
impact on romantic love and attachment in relationships (Bartels & Zeki, 2000; Fischer
Although romantic love and attachment have previously been defined separately,
it becomes more complex when considering the field of neuroscience. Brain areas,
neurocircuits, hormones and other neurological compounds are often involved in both
10
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
romantic love and attachment, combining the two phenomena (Bartels & Zeki, 2004;
Fischer H., 2006; Fischer H. E., 1998; Fischer et al., 2006; Fischer et al., 2002).
how romantic love is fantasized about is not pursued in this thesis, rather included as an
attempt to explain the different experiences the individual might have of romantic love.
The basis of my interpretation will explore scientific literature and research findings on
hope and believe that scientific findings from heterosexual monogamous couples can be
I will not investigate the different perspectives on romantic love between the
genders. It is recognized that there can be more differences within genders rather than
between the genders, not to mention cultural differences and within nations. Despite
these limitations, I hope that my conclusions can also be used in a general manner.
Theoretical background
What happens in the body and the brain when being in love and attached to
romantic love, it lays within our nature (Fischer et al., 2005). This to say, that we are
also acting on instinct, to find the right partner to produce our offspring.
From an early age, children pay attention to love and partnership. Through role-
playing and mimicking parental behavior, they show similar non-sexual affection
towards the opposite sex. In puberty, romantic love and attachment are more focused on
11
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
hormone levels as well as cognitive development that has reached higher levels. The
individual then begins exploring their own sexuality together with a wish of
partner is preferred, especially when it comes to mating in order to carry on our genes
(Buss, 2015).
We might go through different phases in our lifetime, but the longing for
someone to share our life with, a partner, romantically or as a friend, is common for
many. The wish for someone to love, who loves you back, is a dream for many (Graham,
2011). We have a biological drive to reproduce combined with psychological factors such
as the need for belonging, intimacy, and affection (Gonzaga, Turner, Keltner, Campos, &
Altemus, 2006). The main question for this thesis now becomes central. What neural
correlates are involved when we experience love and romantic attachment towards a
partner?
love one of the most grounded in behavioral science is Sternberg’s triangular theory of
love (O'Leary, Acevedo, Aron, Huddy, & Mashek, 2011). Sternberg proposes intimacy,
explains intimacy as the feeling of connectedness, closeness and bondedness that one
experiences in a romantic relationship. Here, attachment might be another word for the
same behavior. Sternberg defines passion as the drive that leads to romance; the mental
and physical attraction towards the other individual, including the sexual drive
(Sternberg R. J., 1986). Lastly, decision/commitment is the component which makes the
relationship long term. In the beginning, commitment might be the pursuing and
12
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
When it comes to romantic love, it seems nearly impossible not to mention love
at first sight. Hatfield and Walster (1981) define this term as infatuation. Here they
propose the active component of being in a state of passionate arousal (Hatfield, 1988),
thought to fall in love with someone at first sight, a relationship cannot alone be based
on passion.
attachment, it is easy to understand that the universally known concept of romantic love
might be looking for someone who shares qualities of their own in addition to finding a
partner who is stable, trustworthy, caring, lovable, and who equally wants to share in a
relationship. Maybe they share a similar background, lifestyle, morals and ethical
satisfaction, relationship fulfillment, mutual love interest, and mutual investment such
This is why, it can be an emotional and irresistible debate as to whether or not the
individual should commit to their partner and how long the relationship will last.
Ambivalence can also act as a force within the individual, causing them to want to be
closer to their partner, for then at the same time push them away (Hazan & Shaver,
1987).
13
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
Fear and longing for intimacy might be present at the same time. Jealousy,
mistrust, insecurity, and delusions can all be lurking in the background. Romantic
individual often feels an intense energy, alertness, and extreme motivation to win back
their beloved. Suggested by psychiatrists, this can be linked back to our mammalian
Many people believe that their loneliness will be cured when they find a matching
when single. Often people feeling lonely are lonely within themselves, something a love
interest cannot cure (Shaver & Hazan, 1984). From Weiss (1973) perspective both
emotional loneliness and social loneliness exist. Emotional loneliness is often found in
romantic relationship, friendly love and romantic love is present, making both aspects of
loneliness possible.
Emotional loneliness is when you lack close and intimate attachment to others.
People who have recently broken up, are widowed or involved in a relationship where
Social loneliness comes from lack of family, friends or a social network. People going
through major life changes often experience this kind of loneliness as well. This is an
intrinsic feeling, depending on the individual’s perception of themselves, how they feel
and their self-esteem and is not dependent on their partner (Weiss, 1973).
qualitative research such as therapy sessions with individuals and couples. Quantitative
(Aron & Aron, 1991). The questionnaires can be Rubin’s loving and liking scales (RLLS),
the love attitudes scale (LAS), the passionate love scale (PLS) and the triangular love
scale (TLS) (Graham, 2011). For the questionnaire of the TLS, see Appendix A.
specific areas of the brain. The distribution of neurotransmitter involvement is also used
to measure love and attachment (Acevedo et al., 2012). Often the individuals are asked
to bring pictures of loved ones and friends to the lab and then fill out questionnaires for
the scientists to understand the nature of their relationship (Bartels & Zeki, 2004).
The creation of a relationship between the caregiver and toddler is the beginning
of socialization that will impact the individual for the rest of their life (Ainsworth &
Wittig, 1969; Ainsworth & Bowlby, 1991; Berger & Luckmann, 1967). During this time,
the interaction with caregivers forms a memory and learning process, establishing the
formation of socialization patterns. According to Berger and Luckmann (1967), the early
memory and learning processes lay the ground for action and reaction patterns. This
again is considered to impact the repeating patterns of behavior for the individual
throughout its lifetime. This again is affecting the child throughout its life, impacting the
chosen direction concerning education, lifestyle and partner preference (Berger &
When it comes to romantic love, the attachment theory and attachment styles
(Mikulincer & Shaver, 2016). How the individual behaves is often a repetition of the
previous memories linked to similar events (Berger & Luckmann, 1967; Bowlby, 1988).
However, studies show that memory and learning can be learned, re-learned and
new-learned (Gazzaniga, Ivry, & Mangun, 2009). The neuroscientific way of explaining
this is stimulus, response, and reinforcement, together with the plasticity of the brain
(Gazzaniga et al., 2009). Influenced by the right guidance and conscious awareness, the
individual may change their perception and how they engage in their environment,
between John Bowlby (1907-1991) and Mary Slater Ainsworth (1913- 1999). Based on
(Ainsworth M. D., 1979) it is frequently used in the behavioral sciences. The original
attachment styles are also known as secure, avoidant attachment style or avoidant-
suggested to work automatically (Verschueren, Marcoen, & Schoefs, 1996) and is the
response to the environmental influence of the individual's attachment style. The IWM
memories and learning from interactions with caregivers. Verschueren et al. (1996),
describe the IWM as being the inner representation of the child itself. It is dominated by
unconscious interpretations that are a challenge to be aware of, understand and even
change.
(1969/1982) IWM model, the attachment system gets activated by threats where the
provides a secure attachment situation (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2003). The ABS provides
an automatic response pattern over time, structuring the behavioral and reaction
Studies show that when an individual knows and feels they will master the
share a secure attachment style. However, if the person constantly feels the need to
pattern. This is an example that illustrates attachment styles (Main & Solomon, 1990;
Mikulincer & Shaver, 2003). Attachment influences the rational functioning and
situation test (SST) is most commonly used when measuring child attachment in
laboratory settings (Ainsworth & Wittig, 1969). Adult attachment is also possible to
measure in laboratory settings through couple interaction. Usually, single and couple’s
attachment style, can be investigated in forms of people discussing matters they already
many represented in the field of science. Although the attachment theory is presented as
the individual sharing one attachment style, it is not that simple. Today, scholars
include the possibility that the individual might have different traits of the different
attachment styles, formed into one unique attachment style of the individual.
measurement contains one question for each of the three attachment styles; secure,
anxious-ambivalent and insecure-avoidant, from a liking scale from one to seven, with
strongly disagree to agree strongly. Later Bartholomew and Horowitz’s (1991) made a
relationship questionnaire (RQ) where the questions were similar Hazan and Shaver’s
The relationship scale questionnaire (RSQ) (Griffin & Bartholomew, 1994) was
inspired from the previously mentioned measurement tools, together with Collins and
Read’s (1990) adult attachment scale (AAS). In the RSQ participants rate each
statement on a five-point liking scale, that fits their characteristic attachment style the
best when it comes to their feelings, thoughts, reaction patterns and behavior in
18
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
Here the scientists use the names preoccupied attachment for anxious-
attachment patterns, and four statements for fearful and preoccupied attachment styles.
survey, may serve as an indicator when calculating the results from the questionnaire.
Here the individual can see their own composite attachment ratings on a self/avoidance
Collins (1998) and later Mikulincer & Shaver (2016). This model can be found in
Appendix C1.
negative) and image of others and avoidance (positive or negative). A securely attached
individual will have both a positive view of self and others, as well as being comfortable
style has a high negative dependence, and positive avoidance resulting in a negative
interactions and relationships with others (Feeney & Noller, 1996; Sternberg & Weis,
2006).
19
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
When it comes to Hazan, Mikulincer and Shaver with colleagues, the commonly
relationships (ECR) which was first published by Brennan, Clark and Shaver in 1998.
This measure has 36 items and is quite similar to the RSQ in wording and style.
attachment. Here they have low anxiety and low avoidance patterns. Insecure
attachment style has a high avoidance and low anxiety pattern, the anxious-ambivalent
attachment has high anxiety and low avoidance pattern (Cozolino, 2014).
distribution of; 15% as disorganized, while 55% secure, 8% ambivalent and 23%
avoidant. Compared to the 1988 study when he together with Kroonenberg found
attachment patterns to be 65% secure, 14% ambivalent and 20% avoidant (van
IJzendoorn & Kroonenberg, 1988). This shows that the estimated percentages of
Ainsworth’s attachment styles are still quite precise. These statistics may also be a
of attachment styles, parenting attachment style and resulting adult attachment style,
rejecting them, being insensitive to their needs. This results in trust and dependence
issues (Ainsworth M. D., 1979). People with insecure-avoidant attachment style are
20
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
independent both emotionally and physically. When put under pressure, they choose to
handle things themselves, and are not available to help, or receive help from others.
People with a secure attachment style are easy to calm down since their caregiver
has been sensitive and trustworthy in meeting their needs. According to Bowlby, a
securely attached individual will most likely be the one who is responsive, emotionally
behavior, as a result of ambivalent caring from their earliest primary caregivers. They
will vary with clingy and attention seeking behavior and then push the caregiver away
when they eventually get a response to their needs (Ainsworth & Wittig, 1969). Since the
child does not develop any physical or emotional response of security from the
attachment figure, they will have difficulties with trusting people later in life and may
Main and Solomon (1990) added an additional attachment style to the original
behavior has previously been included in the descriptions of the two insecure
attachment styles (Ainsworth M. D., 1979; Main & Solomon, 1990). This attachment
style is a form of avoidant attachment, acting from fearful responses motivated by the
“conscious fear of anticipated rejection of others” (Bartholomew & Shaver, 1998, p. 27).
result, the individual will have trust and attachment issues, and disorganized
disorder (RAD) is included in the DSM-5 (Cumyn, 2016). This attachment is a more
social behavior in children before five years of age. An individual with the latter
disorder, has often suffered severe trauma such as neglect, maladaptive behaviors in
caregivers where the caring needs have not been met (Cumyn, 2016).
attachment style would react in the opposite manner (Hazan & Shaver, 1987).
Attachment deals with many aspects including; intimacy, fear of intimacy, loss, and
jealousy, including the ups and downs an individual might experience in a romantic
knowing that they are wanted and needed in the relationship, and often is dependent on
their partner. The fear of someone leaving and abandoning them might impact their
distant at the same time (Bowlby, 1973). An insecure-avoidant attached individual may
individual may be dependent on their partner, while causing a lot of turbulence in the
A person who is driven by emotions and fears, is often overreacting rather than
rationalizing the situation (Cozolino, 2014). Anger and avoidant patterns in behavior
are predictable. This is a result of the individual struggling with mental and sometimes
physical stability, reliability and security (Shemmings & Shemmings, 2011), also
have been found more in people with insecure attachment styles. People with an
deactivate certain brain regions, as the other two attachment styles, impacting their
to secure and insecure-avoidant individuals (Davis, Shaver, & Vernon, 2003). People
and other drugs, in an attempt to lower the negative internal processes. Anxious-
ambivalent attachment is also linked to alcohol and drug abuse when cooping with
reduction (MBSR) meditation has shown lower stress levels and might be a healthy
contributor with the aim of handling one's attachment style better. In a study on MBSR
and attachment styles, the insecure attachment styles had higher stress levels prior to
participation, the insecure group, had lower stress levels than pre-participation as
counseling have all shown different effects on romantic love, and attachment styles. The
most significant effect is found in the insecure attachment styles, revealing that self-
awareness of behavior and reaction patterns, may help the individual change their
attachment style, and attachment towards their partner or potential partner (Cozolino,
2014). The touch of a healing hand can fix a broken soul, or implied when it comes to
attachment styles; by being around a person with a secure attachment, this can make
the insecurely attached individual feel more secure about themselves and others.
report having a decline in sexual arousal, desire, and orgasm. This is often accompanied
by feeling estranged from their chosen partner (Fischer H. , 2006b). A drug designed to
lower the negative emotions and thoughts, might decrease positive emotions as well,
The ethical and moral issue here is that an attachment style is in itself not
necessarily linked to mental illnesses. When it comes to romantic love and attachment
Hilsenroth, & Vala-Stewart, 2008; Hegtvedt, 2014). Medication can however be found
Neurological research
Which are the neural correlates involved when we experience love and
brain involve cohabiting, breeding and parenting. This is in turn, motivated by lust,
attraction, and attachment (Fischer H. E., 1998). In both sexes the attraction system is
The human attraction system is often called romantic love, companionate love or
passionate love. In this thesis, romantic love is used as the common term for this
attraction system.
appetite are normal. Both sexes share an increased focus of attention, affiliated gestures,
increased empathy for the other, and intense motivation to develop a relationship with
the potential partner (Fischer et al., 2006). Nature and nurture also play an important
Specific neurochemicals influence pair bonding formation and may influence the
libido, reacting from female estrogen and male androgen hormones, is functioning as a
motor for mating. Studies on pair bonding formation are often on mammals, more
involvement, but also different behavior strategies happening at different stages of the
reproduction phase. When it comes to homo sapiens, the primary sex drive has evolved
25
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
from motivating the individual to seek sexual union with any possible mate (Fischer et
selectively choose between possible fitting individuals. This enhances the individual’s
motivation and capacity in finding a suitable mate, investing energy and mating time
units, and together, when it comes to romantic love and romantic attachment towards a
humans, as people reporting being in love with someone they have not shared sexual
behavior with, people reporting they may have sexual relations with someone they are
not in love with, and people reporting being in love with someone they have no sexual
desire to be with. This explains that lust, attraction and attachment, can act as a single
unit and together, when it comes to romantic love and romantic attachment in homo
sapiens (Fischer et al., 2002). Although the three emotion-motivation systems in this
thesis often are found acting together in symphony, as the people in the reference
literature often are reporting being in love with their partner, sexual desire towards their
addition to lowering the levels of corticosterone for both sexes. Women reports being
more attached to their partner after sexual activity than before, especially if they
orgasm, explained by oxytocin and serotonin release. The enhanced vasopressin release
26
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
in men, can be a possible explanation of why they feel more close and connected to their
Brain development is defining the human life (Gazzaniga, 2005). By the time the
infant is aged 7 to 15 months old, the myelin system is formed (Kinney, Brody, Kloman,
& Gilles, 1988). By the age of 15 months, the cerebral cortex is matured (Schore, 2005).
This is why, social experiences from infancy, are essential for the development of the
nervous system, due to the fact that they develop as reaction and reflexes (Ainsworth M.
D., 1979). Bowlby (1988) suggests that attachment behaviors can be linked to
(Schore, 2005, p. 205) influences the individual's interaction with their environment,
hence forming and maintaining attachment (Schore, 2005). Explained as “the self-
organization of the developing brain occurs in the context of a relationship with another
self, another brain” (Schore, 1996, p. 60). Schore (2005) claims the emotional
the different brain areas involved in romantic love and attachment, see Appendix E2
(Zeki, 2007).
In this thesis, as mentioned in the present study section, the neural correlates of
romantic love and romantic attachment will be investigated, introduced and described.
Following is a short introduction to the involved brain areas and their involvement in
The frontal lobe. Involved in critical judgment, decision making, planning and
higher mental processes (Gazzaniga et al., 2009), and is the largest part of the human
27
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
brain. It is located anterior to the central sulcus and dorsal to the Sylvian fissure, and
includes the motor and prefrontal cortex. The frontal lobe serves as a significant factor
The cerebral cortex. As the largest region of the cerebrum, consisting of grey
matter, the cerebral cortex is located at the most anterior part of the brain (Kandel,
Schwartz, & Jessel, 2012). The cerebral cortex involves conscious perception, awareness
thought, attention and language (Gazzaniga et al., 2009), together with romantic love,
arousal, and attachment (Fischer et al., 2002) are all linked to the involvement of
cerebral cortex.
When deactivations in the frontal lobe, temporal lobe, and precuneus occur,
moderation of critical judgment is involved. This area is also connected to romantic love
and attachment (Zeki, 2007). The forebrain, which consists of “the cerebral cortex, basal
ganglia, and diencephalon” (Gazzaniga et al., 2009, p. 73) is responsible for mentalizing
and social judgment, as well as the dynamics of understanding other people’s emotions
and intentions (Zeki, 2007) called the theory of mind (Gazzaniga et al., 2009). An
close and intimate with the chosen one, are common for people in love (Xu et al., 2012).
networks used for critical social assessment and negative emotions, while it bonds
individuals through the involvement of reward circuitry, explaining the power of love to
motivate and exhilarate” (Bartels & Zeki, 2004, p. 1155). The deactivation helps to
28
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
explain why people in love are willing to sometimes sacrifice everything, neglecting the
The limbic system. Consisting of the amygdala, basal ganglia, and papez
circuit (the hypothalamus, anterior thalamus, cingulate gyrus and hippocampus) the
limbic system serves many functions when it comes to romantic love and attachment. It
is located within the cerebrum above the brainstem, and functions as a regulatory
system essential in inhibitory control. Key functions are social cognition, memory,
learning, arousal and fear responses. When it comes to romantic love and attachment,
the entire limbic system is involved (Gazzaniga et al., 2009). Although not included in
the limbic system, but in the paralimbic system (Gazzaniga et al., 2009), the
orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) is found crucially connected to the limbic system, and is also
The amygdala. Acting as the alarm center of the brain, controlling our drives,
responses and memories in relation to others (Gazzaniga et al., 2009; Lövblad, Schaller,
& Vargas, 2014). The amygdala plays an important role when it comes to romantic love
and attachment. The activation of the left amygdala enhances positive states when
feeling in love and attached (Lanteaume et al., 2007). Deactivation of the amygdala also
causes positive feelings looking at images of an individual’s partner, when test subjects
reported being intensively in love (Zeki, 2007). The individuals fear responses of their
partner and partner's actions is lower when in love, as compared to the fear response of
friends and strangers. This makes the person in love, less apprehensive when reacting to
their partner's actions, helping us understand why a person can stay in an unhealthy
relationship.
29
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
in the brain. This is developed during infancy and continues into adulthood (Moutsiana
et al., 2015). People who have a fear of being abandoned have an enhanced volume of
the amygdala. In particular this is true for the anxious-ambivalent attachment style.
When these volumes are enhanced it suggests that their response center being in alarm
mode. The same individuals commonly share an intrinsic fear of being abandoned, hurt
or rejected by their loved one or a potential partner (Fischer H. , 2006a). Left amygdala
response is associated with negative feedback, found positively correlated with anxious-
ambivalent attachment. It is noted, people with this attachment style often suffer from
activation, when measuring love and attachment. This is with regard to both the left and
right part of the amygdala. This suggests that the individual has a more distant
& Vuilleumier, 2008), than is true for lower right activation. The insecure-avoidant
individuals do not experience the high levels of positive states as do individuals with
activation in the amygdala. Securely attached individuals, on the other hand, have
shown a mirror effect in the amygdala, but no other specific correlates were found for
People with the anxious-ambivalent attachment style are more vulnerable when
decrease in the amygdala, as compared to other attachment styles (Collins & Gillath,
2012).
People with anxious-ambivalent attachment often have problems coping with the
breakup and often experience more heightened levels of anxiety and depression. This
can be caused by their inner fear, as a result of higher amygdala activation than for the
other two attachment styles. The breakup experience might also confirm what they
already know about the risk of being in love and feeling attached, namely that they stand
a chance of being abandoned, and then will be alone once again (Fischer H. , 2006a).
The hippocampus. This brain structure is located in each temporal lobe and it
is involved in learning and memory. To be more specific, this area is responsible for
spatial memory in mammals and episodic memory in humans (Gazzaniga et al., 2009).
Moutsiana et al. (2015) did not find any results connecting hippocampal volumes to
infant attachments. However, people with a fear of being abandoned by their partner,
(Quirin, Gillath, Pruessner, & Eggert, 2010), which can be seen in the hippocampal
nuclei. Hippocampal nuclei volume in adults differs with each attachment styles. A
reduction in cell concentration in the left hippocampus (Quirin et al., 2010), is linked to
insecure attachment styles. Reduced hippocampal cell density is found in people with
post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and anxiety, which are linked to attachment
found in people with anxious-ambivalent attachment style. Quirin et al. (2010) revealed
31
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
reactions in the same areas involved in both physical and emotional pain (Sternberg &
Weis, 2006).
The hypothalamus. This is a structure located deep within the brain, and is
responsible for the release of dopamine, together with vasopressin and oxytocin (Fischer
et al., 2002). This structure is highly involved in romantic love and attachment. The
hypothalamus functions as a bridge between the endocrine and nervous system. Erotic
Nucleus accumbens and the hypothalamus become more active when introducing
certain drugs, such as cocaine which is known for inducing a state of euphoria (Breiter &
Rosen, 1999). The same brain areas that are shown to be involved in OCD (obsessive-
compulsive disorder) are also observed as activated for people in love (De Boer et al.,
2012).
mind, also involved in social interaction, memory, learning, and reward the OFC has an
important role in romantic love and attachment. It is a structure dealing with multiple
sensory information systems, such as the primary olfactory cortex, and primary
(Gazzaniga et al., 2009). Romantic love and attachment involve all the aforementioned
behaviors. The smell of a loved one, combined with visual, and auditory stimuli is highly
connected to romantic love and attachment, in particular to the aspect of affection and
satisfaction (Acevedo et al., 2012), suggesting that being in love makes the involved less
the mOFC (Xu et al., 2010). The OFC has also been called the frustration-aggression
area by Fischer (2006b). The primary rage system has pathways and neural circuits in
the OFC, controlling anger. Activations are linked to romantic love and rejection
(Panksepp, 1998). People being abandoned, rejected, ignored, or ones that have a fear of
these things have higher activations in this area as compared to others who do not share
The rage and reward pathways of the brain are closely connected, suggesting that
romantic love and abandonment can operate in tandem. The individual may experience
the rage and frustration of being abandoned, while still expressing love and attachment
towards partner leaving the relationship. When comparing rage and hate to love, the
previous partner, they will experience conflicting emotions and cravings for
reconciliation, or search for explanations as to why they are being abandoned. These
findings suggest that the opposite of romantic love is not to hate but rather indifference
(Fischer H. , 2006b).
levels of corticosterone are present. The fear of abandonment and its physical
manifestation in the body suppresses the immune system, stress the heart and increases
blood pressure. This reaction is also present when frustration is caused by someone not
33
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
unpleasant at first, it can have unhealthy implications when prolonged. The afore
mentioned neural processes are found in people who are not satisfied in their
relationships. This implies that feeling pain for a short time when being abandoned is
When it comes to attachment styles and the OFC, experiences of rejection and
attachment style share a need for maintaining a relationship with their ex-partners to
ensure that they are not disliked. The fear of being disliked or unwanted is an element
common for this attachment style. The anxious-ambivalent individual indulges in self-
Negative thoughts such as a breakup, conflict, or the death of a romantic partner are
related to activation in the anterior temporal pole, an area linked to emotion regulation
as compared to the other attachment styles. Due to the fact that they have challenges
feeling closure, combined with an intrinsic need to feel loved by their abandoning
partner. These are motivating forces for the individual to seek to reconcile (Collins &
Gillath, 2012). This is called a positive tone during a breakup. People who experience
this mechanism are more likely to take responsibility, and have reservations concerning
the breakup. The anxious-ambivalent individual is also more prone to depression post-
breakup, than the other attachment styles (Collins & Gillath, 2012).
opposite manner. This attachment style is more distant and reluctant to feel pain and
34
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
anxiety towards the breakup (Schore, 2000). Resentment and anger during a long
period often surface at the end, as mental problems and possibly health issues (Smith,
2013). Both insecure attachment styles experience pain and despair when separating
from a loved one, but they share a different set of emotions and reaction patterns.
perception of visceral sensations. The insula is known to influence the gut feeling of
(Olausson et al., 2002). The medial insula is also involved when regulating affiliative,
emotional and hormonal responses as the result of skin to skin contact (Bartels & Zeki,
2004). Ven economo neurons (VENs), found both in the ACC and anterior insula, is
responsible for processing complex social information. These neurons are known to
dominate the right hemisphere in the processing of emotional and social information.
Also found to be involved in the phenomena of reward and attachment (Cozolino, 2014).
The anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). The ACC is a connection area for the
limbic system and prefrontal cortex (Stevens, Hurley, & Taber, 2011), involved with
romantic love and attachment, associated with affect regulation, emotional and social
processing (Zeki, 2007). Activation in the dorsal ACC in people with anxious-ambivalent
attachment can be connected to physical pain and distress (Gillath, Bunge, Shaver,
Wendelken, & Mikulincer, 2005), showing that the affective-emotion regulative areas of
disorder (GAD), depression, panic disorder, PTSD and schizophrenia. Autism specter
disorder and high-functioning autism are also included (Cozolino, 2014) in this
35
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
typology. People with insecure-avoidant attachment also share abnormalities in the ACC
midbrain, responsible for sending dopaminergic projections to both the cortical regions
(mesolimbic and mesocortical circuits) and the limbic system (Hanlon, Dowdle, &
Jones, 2016). The VTA is known for regulating the involvement of dopamine, linked to
both romantic love and attachment. The region of A10 dopamine cells alter group
activations in the right medial and posterodorsal body, where the caudate nucleus
functions of sensory signals are inclined to action. Activations of the VTA is associated
with focused attention, pleasure, general arousal and motivation to pursue and gain
When it comes to the reward system of the brain, in addition to the VTA
involvement, the subcortex and striatum (caudate nucleus, putamen, globus pallidus)
have also been linked to both romantic attachments. The stratum accommodates cells
that are responsive to both drink and reward and is also found present when sexual
arousal in humans and monkeys is detected (Arnow et al., 2002; Bartels & Zeki, 2004).
Monetary reward stimuli (Schultz, 2000), and as well as stimuli associated with cocaine
avoidant attachment showed less deactivation in brain areas than securely attached
individuals in the subcallosal cingulate cortex (SCC) and the lateral prefrontal cortex
(LPFC). These areas are often involved in cognition and regulation of emotions, and
emotional withdrawal. The SCC, on the other hand, involves avoidance regulation and
thought processes.
36
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
The caudate tail. Brain areas that are associated with romantic feelings, have a
states, reward and addiction (Takahashi et al., 2015). The caudate tail is an area rich
with dopamine. People in long-term relationships are shown to have higher dopamine
activation than people who have been in a romantic relationship for a short period of
time. This result indicates that the caudate tails neural activity plays a role in
relationship satisfaction (Bartels & Zeki, 2004). The right medial and posterior-dorsal
body, are areas responsible for the release of dopamine (Fisher et al., 2002), and
together with the medial caudate nucleus which is accountable for enhancing
performance (Fisher et al., 2005) and is highly involved with romantic love and
attachment.
distinctive difference in neurotransmitters may be the reason for why the social pine
and prairie voles are being monogamous compared to the promiscuous non-social
meadow and montane voles (Curtis & Wang, 2003; Fischer et al., 2005; 2006).
influence on cognition and behavior when released within the brain (Gazzaniga et al.,
2009). The presence of vasopressin can cause the sexual dimorphism observed in rodent
species (Curtis & Wang, 2003). Sexual dimorphism can impact pair bond formation
resulting in stronger attachment in voles. The vasopressin release in the lateral septum
of the brain is found in monogamous male voles when introduced to females. This is an
37
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
area activated by recognition of faces as well as sexual dimorphism. In this study, the
centrally released vasopressin binds to the V1a receptor (Young, Nilsen, Waymire,
MacGregor, & Insel, 1999), an action not happening for promiscuous voles. The voles
share nearly the same gene sequence for the receptor but have different promoter
regions.
These findings might explain the difference between monogamy and non-
monogamy involvement (Bamshad, Novak, & De Vries, 1999), especially when it comes
to males. When in love, enhanced levels of vasopressin are found, although males share
higher alteration contra females. Induced vasopressin in males has a positive impact,
while for females too much vasopressin has a negative impact. Scientists suggest these
findings indicate a possible explanation to the problem in human males that have issues
being faithful and attached to their partner and children (Donaldson & Young, 2008).
Oxytocin. When it comes to females, oxytocin has the same impact here as
whereas too much oxytocin has a negative impact on males (Insel & Shapiro, 1992).
Oxytocin also acts as a component of bonding between the caregiver and offspring.
Enhanced pair bonding and attachment is found when inducing oxytocin in the large
only found effectual in monogamous voles (Curtis & Wang, 2003). The differences
between the social and non-social voles are in the distribution of oxytocin-containing
Curtis and Wang (2003) question whether the difference in behavior is caused by
the different distribution patterns of oxytocin receptors in the brain, similar to findings
38
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
regarding vasopressin. When inducing oxytocin in the brain from reporter genes, both
promiscuous voles when giving birth. Here the oxytocin receptors grow to be similar in
size to the social voles (Curtis & Wang, 2003). Scientists suggest the reason for voles to
show pair-bonding formations outside maternal care, is because they have copied and
activated maternal behavior towards their partners. These findings are present in both
sexes (Curtis & Wang, 2003; Fischer, Aron, & Brown, 2006).
Although vasopressin and oxytocin have been represented alone, they are
functioning together as in one pathway of the brain (Carter, 2017). The core of
attachment is found in the neurobiological systems regulating fight and flight responses,
and security and stability responses. Oxytocin might be explained as sharing a more
passive role of attachment. Vasopressin, on the other hand, has a more aggressive and
The formation of oxytocin and vasopressin systems is found affected by early life
experiences. The loss of an attachment figure, early grief, separation, and other traumas
can trigger stress and fear. Resulting in a shutdown behavior in the individual, often
regulates the anatomy’s nervous system, especially when exposed to stressful situations.
The different forms of emotion and emotional regulation can be explained under the
linked to metabolic and immune system involvement, physical health and well-being,
al., 2017; Carter, 2017). Although high oxytocin release is often linked to female
experience of romantic love and romantic attachment, it is also found present in men
p=0.04), that is, the higher the oxytocin levels the higher the score on the scale of the
ECR” (Marazziti et al., 2006, p. 1). Here the ECR is the Italian translation of the
that high oxytocin release can be linked to human anxiety (Marazziti et al., 2006).
bonding in humans. When it comes to romantic relationships, it seems healthy for the
individual to have induced oxytocin to a certain degree. Proposing that the role of
oxytocin, in this case, is to keep anxiety levels under control, so it is not harmful for the
individual (Marazziti et al., 2006). This suggests that the involvement in a romantic
relationship is resulting in stress conditions, that act differently depending on the phase
of the relationship. Very low concentration levels of oxytocin have also shown a negative
effect resulting in pain syndromes, such as abdominal pain and fibromyalgia (Marazziti
et al., 2006).
Oxytocin nasal spray is being used today in studies on females who struggle with
developing and maintaining attachment to their infant, especially when suffering from
The use of nasal spray has shown to have a significant impact on human cognition,
(2012) research paper, nasal oxytocin spray enhances the appraisal of effect from the
40
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
information of social cues from others and improves the individual's strategic levels of
processing. It is suggested that this might be a solution for women struggling with
attachment towards their romantic partner as well (Guastella & MacLeod, 2012).
behavior, especially attachment and social functioning (Curtis & Wang, 2003).
Dopamine release is a part of the reward system in the brain, starting at the substantial
niagra and ventral tegmental area to the striatum, thalamus and the cerebral cortex to
the corpus callosum, through the pons and medulla, and down the spinal cord
exercise, sex, specific food consumption, and consummation of certain drugs (Hanlon et
al., 2016), but also mating, pair-bonding, and maintenance of a relationship (Curtis &
Wang, 2003). Dopamine release is found in the nucleus accumbens during mating in
social voles.
Human disorders that can benefit from this drug are related to people struggling
with attachment, such as social phobia and autism, and involuntary movement diseases,
such as Parkinson’s disease (PD) (Takahashi et al., 2015). Introducing the antagonist
Apomorphine primarily activates the D2 receptors, but can also affect D1 receptors in
the nucleus accumbens when given at a high dosage. Activation of the D1 receptors has
population in regard to romantic love and attachment. Specific disposition of the D2,
and not D1, may induce pair bonding formation outside the breeding season. Blockage
41
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
of the D2 receptor, excluding the D1 receptor, prevent from pair bonding when mating
(Curtis & Wang, 2003). The female social voles need a higher dosage of dopamine
agonist release, in order to have the same effect as the males. Inducing the dopamine
antagonist, haloperidol, destroys the process of pair bond formation and results in
adrenal corticosteroids synthesized in the adrenal cortex” (Katsu & Iguchi, 2016, p. 527),
corticosterone is a known stress hormone. De Vries and Boyle (1998) found cohabitation
males experience sexual dysmorphia. Females often react hostile to males when levels of
corticosterone are manually induced, or with heightened stress levels, whereas males
react in the opposite manner (De Vries & Boyle, 1998). From a neurobiological
perspective, males are driven by the chase of their partner, whereas the females seek
suffering from OCD and obsessive thinking. A study with three groups; people in love,
people with OCD and controls showed the same decreased activity in blood-platelets
and central serotonin transporters for the first two groups compared to controls
(Fischer H. , 2006a).
Blood levels of serotonin also have a differential effect on the two sexes. Men in
love share lower serotonin levels in both plasma and serum serotonin, compared to
42
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
involvement and romantic love, both sexes reported obsessive thinking of their beloved
up to 65 % of the day. However, in this study women reported a more intense state and
trait of romantic love than men (Langeslang, van der Veen, & Fekkes, 2012).
When it comes to the reward pathways of the brain, people with anxious-
ambivalent attachment might have higher dopamine hence corticosterone release in the
brain, as compared to the secure and insecure-avoidant attachment styles. This to say,
romantic love, but they are also exposed to higher levels of stress and possible negative
obsessive thinking.
attachment style have lower levels of dopamine and corticosterone release in the brain
compared to the other two attachment styles. These neurological differences help to
explain the different patterns of behavior in people with a specific dominant attachment
style.
Discussion
processes of romantic love and romantic attachment. The thesis started with the
question; which neural correlates are involved when we experience love and romantic
introduce and describe the neural correlates of romantic love and romantic attachment.
The thesis approach has been exploratory for the reason that a lot of research have been
43
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
given to the topic over the years, altough the neuroscientific side of it is quite new. Since
being in love and feeling romantically attached involves a lot of reward, thought and
emotional processing I found it natural to investigate areas of the brain involved in these
cognitive processes, namely the reward circuitry network and the limbic system,
love. It was also suggested that the differences among the attachment styles were to be
neurotransmitter release.
As described in the research findings, romantic love goes deeper, and involves the
regulation of more brain areas, than the brain areas involved for mating purposes only.
This confirms that sex drive is distinctive from romantic love (Fischer et al., 2005),
although there are strong connections to it. This can be seen in the emotion-
motivational system with the three drives such as lust, attraction and attachment. For
what romantic love and romantic attachment are understood as, all these processes are
involved, in a symphony with one and another, and in cooperation with other bodily
systems as well (Fischer H. E., 1998). Most important, romantic love is a primary
motivation system, fundamental for human survival and focused on the specific reward
of mating. Nevertheless, when it comes to romantic love there is something more than
lust, sexual attraction and behavior. It is a deep connection, attachment and a wish of
committing to the chosen partner (Fischer et al., 2002). It is also a choice, an inspiration
and a motivation to continue to be together with the beloved, with investment of time,
behavioral science. Despite this, attachment as a central aspect has not always been
attachment may have been included. Sternberg (1997) directs this, mentioning Shaver et
al.’s (1988) perspectives of the attachment theory to be a natural contribution to his own
theory, and that it needs to be revised complimenting the aspect of attachment as well.
When it comes to attachment, scholars agree on the importance of the early years
of infancy (Schore, 2005). The development of the human brain suggests the first years
of life is crucial for later brain functioning, social behavior, and interaction (Belsky,
2002), including attachment formed in romantic relationships (Hazan & Shaver, 1987).
Brain areas involved in learning, memory, emotional regulation, social judgment, and
reward, are all found involved in both romantic love and attachment (Bartels & Zeki,
Bowlby and Ainsworth propose that attachment styles formed in the early years
continue to follow into adulthood, influencing social life and close relationships
environments and in close relationships. These individuals are better adjusted and more
competent, than children who share a pessimistic view of themselves and a negative
interpretation (Verschueren et al., 1996). Main and Solomon (1990) propose that it is
When it comes to romantic love, it seems that scientists agree on the different
aspects of the universal phenomena. Although there are different definitions of romantic
love, they commonly mention intimacy, lust, attraction, bondedness, attachment and
commitment as central aspects (Sternberg & Weis, 2006). When it comes to the
negative aspects of romantic love and attachment this can be experienced as loneliness,
pain, frustration and hatred (Fischer H. , 2006a). Although romantic love has often
been romanticized in the fiction literature, it is important not to forget the possible
When it comes to both attachment and romantic love brain areas linked to
emotional states, memories, thought processes, behavior, and imagination are all
involved, and sometimes they are intertwined as one (Bartels & Zeki, 2004). Concluded
by Fischer (2006b) and Cozolino (2014), childhood, adolescence and later adulthood
play a significant role when it comes to romantic love and attachment behavior.
Important to notice, that there are subjective differences in the capability to fall and
responsible for learning, memory, motivation, and reward. Brain areas that are
activated include the cerebral cortex, limbic system; amygdala, OFC, hippocampus,
hypothalamus, ACC, VTA, frontal lobe, the caudate tail and the reward circuitry
network, together with the right medial and posterior-dorsal body (Fischer et al., 2002).
related to romantic love and attachment (Zeki, 2007), such as the frontal lobe, and parts
46
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
corticosterone, together with the reward circuitry network differentiates the subjective
Women often share elevated levels of serotonin, oxytocin and dopamine when
being in love and attached to a partner. Males on the other hand shares heightened
release is found during intimacy and sexual behavior together with feeling mentally
close to one's partner, in both sexes. When it comes to being in love, men have lower
their beloved, which is a result of differing levels of serotonin (Langeslang et al., 2012).
the brain, such as in the formation of the limbic system, especially the amygdala and
hippocampus. Fear of one’s partner decreases when in love, this found to be true for all
They are also more afraid of being abandoned or rejected than other attachment styles
(Fischer H. , 2006a).
amygdala and bilateral reduction in hippocampus volume. They also do not share
deactivations in the prefrontal regions, nor decrease in blood flow (Quirin et al., 2010).
The social judgmental network of the brain is less deactivated here compared to the
other attachment styles. Here these persons might be reluctant to be and feel close, and
are not able to favor their partner as much as the other attachment styles. Stating that
47
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
love and attached, than others who do not share the same ability.
insecure attachment styles. Yet, this attachment style is difficult to categorize and detect
in neuroscience, since people with this attachment style may be suffering from mental
painful behavior. Relationship outcomes experienced painful for the individual can be
relationships and attachment behavior. By attending therapy, being aware of one's own
attachment behavior it is possible to change and modify behavior (Sternberg & Weis,
2006). Couples counseling, single therapy, CBT, psychotherapy and even meditation are
believed to be effective strategies in coping with attachment in relation to self and others
(Cozolino, 2014).
research, is based on the attachment theory, it is important for science that this theory is
being updated to fit recent scientific findings, theories and believes (Slater, 2007). Since
romantic attachment that it measures just that, and not other aspects of attachment
which is not included in the research from the beginning. It is also essential that
48
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
measurements are being adapted to the times of change, not relying on merely old
Scholars in the field of romantic love and attachment are often replicating their
own studies, or referring to colleges representing similar findings. This results in a small
group of neuroscientists using and re-using the same findings. Therefore, it would be
disconcerting if the method is later found to be incorrect (Slater, 2007). During the later
years, it has been questioned whether attachment and influence from parents from
assumes.
Most of the surveys involving romantic love and attachment are based on
students who are in college or mature adults who have been married for a long period of
time. When it comes to the different attachment styles, there is also a lack of self-reports
on the subject (Slater, 2007). Questionnaires can be criticized to be biased, because data
presented data narrow in a lack of diversity (Gazzaniga, 2005). However, with the
internet and constant presence of smart devices, it is easier for the public to be engaged
caregivers and infant. With regards to neuroscience, it is limited what EEG measures
and fMRI measures can show on the physical impact of romantic love and attachment
(Damasio, 1999). Mental processes occurring at the testing point can result in
49
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
styles from an early age following adulthood, and see if the results can be related to each
other. Including this, it would be interesting to see the individual’s ability to change
after utilizing proposed assessments, such as therapy, meditation or in some cases the
such as dopamine, serotonin, vasopressin, and oxytocin have been a topic of medical
science for years. However, inducing drugs or stimulants, to understand the neural
In “the ethical brain”, Gazzaniga (2005) explains the complex and controversial
aspects of cognitive neuroscience. There are advantages in genetics, brain training, and
drug development we need to be aware of when measuring the human brain. The law
serves as a guideline for the domain that is a person’s private life and what is not
(Gazzaniga, 2005), making the investigation on romantic love and attachment limited in
neuroscience, since this type of study is on such an intimate and subjective level
(Damasio, 1999).
answered in regard to embracing pair bonding and attachment. There is still little
knowledge regarding the cellular levels in the brain involved in both phenomena. One
thing is that the neural correlates are discovered; another aspect is the individual’s
50
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
and attachment.
Conclusion
a change of the state of mind implies a change in the state of the brain and vice versa”
interactions of their environment. The mental processes are forming the action and
reaction pattern of the individual (Gazzaniga et al., 2009). Participants who are anxious-
ambivalent may have higher activations in related brain areas, compared to secure and
attachment often have suppressed levels in brain alterations (Gillath et al., 2005).
People can, however, change their attachment styles with conscious awareness,
hence making their child attachment different from the one they have later in life
al., 2005). Gazzaniga (2005) addresses questions such as ethics and morals, the law and
the bias of focusing on specific brain activity and areas conflicting with research
findings.
As proposed in the beginning of this thesis, I aimed to introduce and describe the
approach. The complicated aspects of romantic love and the bond of attachment
51
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
towards a romantic partner as well as the discovery of neural correlates are considered
often demands a lot of thought and emotional processing, together with being a highly
reward, emotion and thought processing areas of the brain, namely the reward circuitry
network and the limbic system, together with other brain areas that might be involved
when it comes to romantic love and romantic attachment. When it comes to the
different attachment styles, it was also suggested that the differences among the
romantic love and romantic attachment, confirms the involvement of the reward
circuitry network and the limbic system as crucial, with contributing regions involved in
of what happens in our body and brain when being in love and feeling romantically
attached. Also, in addition to the basic neural correlates appearing, there are certain
neural correlates confirming that there are some differences when it comes to
The human’s ability to alter memory, and the processes of learning, re-learning
and new-learning give us the possibility to alter the effect the brain has on our actions
and decisions, hence in romantic relationships and our attachment styles. The plasticity
52
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
of the human brain shows that we also have the capability and ability to impact and
change our social behavior concerning our intrinsic and extrinsic impression of
ourselves and others and in the relationship with self and others (Cozolino, 2014).
Even though secure attachment is the most favorable, there are still highly
functioning people who share the insecure attachment styles. In a diverse world, it is
beneficial to have people with different characteristics and methods of coping (Cozolino,
2014). Although attachment styles are still being used in the science of romantic love
such complex phenomena such as romantic love and attachment (Acevedo et al., 2012).
& Weis, 2006). People, who have their individual attachment style as secure attachment,
do not always need their partner to share the same attachment type. There are well-
functioning people sharing other attachment styles and experiencing flourishing and
healthy relationships.
possible for people with insecure attachment styles to change towards a more secure
attachment style. It is known that people with insecure attachment styles might struggle
with mental disorders, and medication might be a possible addition to handling one's
As Fischer (2006b) states, fMRI studies can tell us what happens in the brain
when falling in love, continue to be in love and feeling attached to our partner. Despite
this, neuroscience still remains to explore and explain the elmental question of why we
53
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
fall in love with some people and not others. Nature and nurture play a significant role.
system than ourselves might enhance the possibility of finding the opposite sex
attractive. Possible factors can be that the other person reminds us of our caretakers
hopefully in a positive way. However, many people choose partners sharing the same
When looking at factors other than mentioned above, there are many reasons
more for falling in love and feeling attached to someone, making science leaving many
attachment, I assume science will continue to prope deeper into consciousness studies,
and investigate why people favor a possible mating partner over other equally fit mating
partners.
54
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
References
Acevedo, B., Aron, A., Fischer, H., & Brown, L. (2012). Neural correlates of marital
satisfaction and well-being: Reward, empathy and affect. Clinical
Neuropsychiatry, 9, 20-31.
Ainsworth, M. D. (1979). The Bowlby-Ainsworth attachment theory. 1(3), pp. 436-438.
Ainsworth, M. D., & Wittig, B. A. (1969). Attachment and exploratory behavior of one-
year-olds in a strange situation. (B. M. Foss, Ed.) 4, 113-136.
Ainsworth, M. S., & Bowlby, J. (1991). An ethological approach to personality
development. American Psychologist, 46(4), 333-341.
Allen, J. P., & Land, D. (1999). Attachment in adolescence. In J. Cassidy, & P. R. Shaver,
Handbook of attachment: Theory, research and clinical applications (pp. 319-
335). New York: Guilford Press.
Amini-Khoei, H., Mohammad-Asl, A., Amiri, S., Hosseini, M. J., Momeny, M., &
Hassanipour, M. e. (2017). Oxytocin mitigated the depressive-like behaviors of
maternal separation stress through modulating mitochondrial function and
neuroinflammation. Progress in Neuro-psychopharmacology and Biobehavioral
Psychiatry, pp. 169-178.
Arnow, B. A., Desmond, J. E., Banner, L. L., Glover, G. H., Solomon, A., Polan, M. L., . . .
Atlas, S. W. (2002). Brain activation and sexual arousal in healthy, heterosexual
males. Brain(5), pp. 1014-1023.
Aron, A., & Aron, E. (1991). Close relationships as including other in the self. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology(2), pp. 241-253.
Bamshad, M., Novak, M. A., & De Vries, G. J. (1999). Sex and species differences in the
vasopressin innervation of sexually naive and parental prairie voles, microtus
ochrogaster, and meadow voles, Microtus pennsylvanicus. Journal of
Neuroendocrinology, 5, 1071-1079.
Bartels, A., & Zeki, S. (2000). The neural basis of romantic love. NeuroReport(17), pp.
3829-3834.
Bartels, A., & Zeki, S. (2004). The neural correlates of maternal and romantic love.
NeuroImage, 21, 1155-1166.
55
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
Bartholomew Research Lab. (n.d). Self-report attachment measures. Retrieved July 27,
2018, from Simon Fraser University:
http://members.psyc.sfu.ca/labs/kim_bartholomew/attachment/self
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: A
test of a four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology(2),
pp. 226-244. Retrieved June 12, 2018, from http://psycnet.apa.org/buy/1991-
33075-001
Bartholomew, K., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Methods of assessing adult attachment - Do
they converge? In J. A. Simpson, & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and
close relationships (pp. 25-45). New York: Guilford Press.
Belsky, J. (2002). Developmental origins of attachment styles. Attachment and Human
Development, 4(2), 166-170.
Berger, P. L., & Luckmann, T. (1967). The social construction of reality - A treatise in
the sociology of knowledge. Garden City,, New York: Doubleday.
Bowlby, J. (1969/1982). Attachment and loss: Vol 1. Attachment. New York: Basic
Books.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and anger. New
York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Loss. New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent-child attachment and healthy human
development. New York: Basic Books.
Breiter, H. C., & Rosen, B. R. (1999). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of brain
reward circuitry in the human. Annual of the New York Acadamy of Sciences, pp.
523-547.
Buss, D. (2015). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind (Vol. 5).
Routledge Taylor & Francis Group, London & New York.
Carter, S. (2017). The role of oxytocin and vasopressin in attachment. Psychodynamic
Psychiatry(4), pp. 499-518.
Collins, N. L., & Read, S. J. (1990). Adult attachment, working models, and relationship
quality in dating couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology(4), pp.
644-663.
56
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
Collins, T. J., & Gillath, O. (2012). Attachment, breakup strategies, and associated
outcomes: The effects of security enhancement on the selection of breakup
strategies. Journal of Research in Personality(2), pp. 210-222.
Cordon, S. L., Brown, K. W., & Gibson, P. R. (2009). The role of mindfulness-based
stress reduction on perceived stress: Preliminary evidence for the moderating
role of attachment style. Journal of cognitive psychotherapy: An international
quarterly(3), pp. 258-269.
Cozolino, L. (2014). The neuroscience of human relationships: Attachment and the
developing brain. New York, NY: W.W. Norton.
Cumyn, L. (2016). Clarifying "disorder" in attachment: An overview. Annals of
psychiatry and mental health(4), pp. 1-7.
Curtis, T., & Wang, Z. (2003). The neurochemistry of pair bonding. Current Directions
in Psychological Science, 49-53.
Damasio, A. R. (1999). The feeling of what happens - Body and emotion in the making
of consciousness. New York: Harcourt Brace.
Davis, D., Shaver, P. R., & Vernon, M. L. (2003). Physical, emotional and behavioral
reactions to breaking up: The roles of gender, age, emotional involvement, and
attachment style. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin(7), pp. 871-884.
De Boer, A., Van Buel, E. M., & Ter Horst, G. J. (2012). Love is more than just a kiss: A
neurobiological perspective on love and affection. Neuroscience, 201, 114-124.
De Vries, G. J., & Boyle, P. A. (1998). Double duty for sex differences in the brain.
Behavioral Brain Research, pp. 205-213.
Donaldson, Z. R., & Young, L. J. (2008). Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the neurogenetics
of sociality. Science(5903), pp. 900-904.
Downey, G. F., & Michaelis, B. K. (1998). The self-fulfilling prophecy in close
relationships: Rejection sensitivity and rejection by romantic partners. Journal of
Personality and Personal Psychology(7), pp. 545-560.
Dozier, R. W. (2002). Why we hate: Understanding, curbing and eliminating hate in
ourselves and our world. Chicago: Contemporary Books.
Feeney, J., & Noller, P. (1996). Adult attachment (Sage series on close relationships ed.).
Thousand Oaks: SAGE Publications.
57
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
Fischer, H. (2006a). Broken hearts: The nature and risks of romantic rejection. In A. C.
Crouter, & A. Booth (Eds.), The Penn State University family issues symposia
series. Romance and sex in adolescence and emerging adulthood: Risks and
opportunities (pp. 2-38). Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Publishers.
Fischer, H. (2006b). The drive to love: The neural mechanisms for mate selection. In R.
J. Sternberg, & K. Weis (Eds.), The new psychology of love. New Haven &
London: Yale University Press.
Fischer, H. E. (1998). Lust, attraction and attachment in mammalian reproduction.
Human Nature, pp. 23-52.
Fischer, H. E., Aron, A., & Brown, L. L. (2006). Romantic love: A mammalian brain
system for mate choice. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 361,
2173-2186.
Fischer, H. E., Aron, A., Mashek, D., Li, H., & Brown, L. L. (2002). Defining the brain
systems of lust, romantic attraction and attachment. Archives of sexual behavior,
31(5), pp. 413-419.
Fischer, H., Aron, A., & Brown, L. (2005). Romantic love: An fMRI study of a neural
mechanism for mate choice. 493(1), pp. 58-62.
Friedrich, E. V., Wood, G., Scherer, R., & Neuper, C. (2014, May 27). Mind over brain,
brain over mind: Cognitive causes and consequences of the controlling brain
activity. Frontier in Human Neuroscience(348).
doi:https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00348
Gazzaniga, M. S. (2005). The ethical brain. New York: Dana Press.
Gazzaniga, M. S., Ivry, R. B., & Mangun, G. R. (2009). Cognitive Neuroscience - The
biology of the mind. New York: W.W Norton.
Gillath, O., Bunge, S. A., Shaver, P. R., Wendelken, C., & Mikulincer, M. (2005).
Attachment-style differences in the ability to suppress negative thoughts:
Exploring the neural correlates. NeuroImage(4), pp. 835-847.
Gonzaga, G. C., Turner, R. A., Keltner, D., Campos, B., & Altemus, M. (2006). Romantic
love and sexual desire in close relationships. Emotion, 6(2), 163-179.
Graham, J. M. (2011). Measuring love in romantic relationships: A meta-analysis.
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 28(6), 748-771.
58
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
Griffin, D., & Bartholomew, K. (1994). Models of the self and other: Fundamental
dimensions underlying measures of adult attachment. Journal of Personality
and Social psychology(3), pp. 430-445.
Guastella, A. J., & MacLeod, C. (2012). A critical review of the influence of oxytocin
nasal spray on social cognition in humans: Evidence and future directions.
Hormones and Behavior, pp. 410-418.
Haggerty, G., Hilsenroth, M. J., & Vala-Stewart, R. (2008). Attachment and
interpersonal distress: Examining the relationship between attachment styles
and interpersonal problems in a clinical population. Clinical psychology and
psychotherapy, pp. 1-9.
Hanlon, C. A., Dowdle, L. T., & Jones, J. L. (2016). Chapter six - biomarkers for success:
Using neuroimaging to predict relapse and develop brain stimulation treatments
for cocaine-dependent individuals. International review of neurobiology, pp.
125-156.
Hatfield, E. (1988). Passionate and companionate love. In T. p. love, R. J. Sternberg, &
M. L. Barnes (Eds.). New Haven, CT: Yale University.
Hatfield, E., & Walster, G. W. (1981). A new look at love. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(3), 511-524.
Hegtvedt, K. A. (2014). Chapter 2 - Ethics and experiments. In M. Webster, & J. Sell
(Eds.), Laboratory Experiments in the Social Sciences (2 ed., pp. 23-51). London,
United Kingdom: Academic Press.
Insel, T. R., & Shapiro, L. E. (1992). Oxytocin receptor distribution reflects social
organization in monogamous and polygamous voles. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, USA, pp. 5981-5985.
Kandel, E. R., Schwartz, J. H., & Jessel, T. M. (Eds.). (2012). Principles of Neural
Science. New York: McGraw-Hill Medical.
Karlen, L.-R. (1996). Attachment relationships among children with aggressive behavior
problems: The role of disorganized early attachment patterns. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Pscychology(1), pp. 64-73.
59
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 35, pp. 53-152). New York:
Academic Press.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics
and change. New York: Guilford Press.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2016). Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics
and change (2 ed.). New York & London: The Guilford Press.
Mikulincer, M., Birnbaum, G., Woddis, D., & Nachmias, O. (2000). Stress and
accessibility of proximity-related thoughts: Exploring the normative and
intraindividual components of attachment theory. Journal of Personal Social
Psychology, pp. 509-523.
Moutsiana, C., Johnstone, T., Murray, L., Fearon, P, Cooper, P. J., . . . L, S. (2015).
Insecure attachment during infancy predicts greater amygdala volumes in early
adulthood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry(5), pp. 540-548.
Ognibene, T. C., & Collins, N. L. (1998). Adult attachment styles, percieved social
suppoert and coping strategies. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships(3),
pp. 323-345.
Olausson, H., Lamarre, Y., Backlund, H., Morin, C., Wallin, B. G., Starck, G., . . .
Bushnell, M. C. (2002). Unmyelinated tactile afferents signal touch and project to
insular cortex. Nature Neuroscience(9), pp. 900-904.
O'Leary, K. D., Acevedo, B. P., Aron, A., Huddy, L., & Mashek, D. (2011). Is long-term
love more than a rare phenomenon: If so, what are its correlates? Social
psychological and personality science, 000(00), 1-8.
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal
emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.
Quirin, M., Gillath, O., Pruessner, J. C., & Eggert, L. C. (2010). Adult attachment
insecurity and hippocampal cell density. Social Cognitive and Affective
Neuroscience(1), pp. 39-47.
Scheele, D., Wille, A., Kendrick, K. M., Soffel-Wagner, B., Becker, B., O, G., . . .
Hurlemann, R. (2103). Oxytocin enhances brain reward system responses in men
viewing the faces of their female partner. Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences of the United States of the America(50), pp. 20308-20313.
61
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
Stevens, F. L., Hurley, R. A., & Taber, K. H. (2011). Windows to the brain - Anterior
Cingulate Cortex: Unique role in cognition and emotion. The Journal of
Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences(2), pp. 121-125.
Takahashi, K., Mizuno, K., Sasaki, A. T., Wada, Y., Tanaka, M., Ishii, A., . . . Watanabe,
Y. (2015, April 09). Imaging the passionate stage of romantic love by dopamine
dynamics. Retrieved April 06, 2018, from US National Library of Medicine
National Institutes of Health:
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4391262/
van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1995). Adult attachment representations, parental
responsiveness, and infant attachment. A meta-analysis on the predictive validity
of the Adult Attachment Interview. Psychological Bulletin, pp. 387-403.
van IJzendoorn, M., & Kroonenberg, P. (1988). Cross-cultural patterns of attachment: A
meta-analysis of the Strange Situation. Child Development, pp. 147-156.
Verschueren, K., Marcoen, A., & Schoefs, V. (1996). The internal working model of the
self, attachment, and competence in five year-year-olds. Child Development(5),
pp. 2493-511.
Vrticka, P., Andersson, F., Grandjean, D., Sander, D., & Vuilleumier, P. (2008).
Individual attachment style modulates human amygdala and striatum activation
during social appraisal. PLoS One(8), p. e2868.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0002868
Weiss, R. S. (1973). Loneliness: The experience of emotional and social isolation.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Xu, X., Aron, A., Brown, L., Cao, G., Feng, T., & Weng, X. (2010). Reward and
motivation systems: A brain mapping study of early-stage intense romantic love
in Chinese participants. Human Brain Mapping, 32, 249-258.
Xu, X., Brown, L., Aron, A., Cao, G., Feng, T., Acevedo, B., & Weng, X. (2012). Regional
brain activity during early-stage intense romantic love predicted relationship
outcomes after 40 months: An fMRI assessment. MEDLINE, 526(1), 33-38.
Young, L. J., Nilsen, R., Waymire, K. G., MacGregor, G. R., & Insel, T. R. (1999).
Increased affiliative response to vasopressin in mice expressing the v1a receptor
from a monogamous vole. Nature, pp. 766-768.
63
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
Appendix A
Sternberg’s triangular theory of love scale is commonly used when measuring romantic
love. Answers are scored on a 9-point Likert scale from “Not at all,” to “Moderately,” to
“Extremely.” The participants is advised to fill in the blank spaces with the name of one
person they care or love for deeply.
Intimacy
1. I am actively supportive of _____’s well-being.
2. I have a warm relationship with _____.
3. I am able to count on _____ in times of need.
4. _____ is able to count on me in times of need.
5. I am willing to share myself and my possessions with _____.
6. I receive considerable emotional support from _____.
7. I give considerable emotional support to _____.
8. I communicate well with _____.
9. I value _____ greatly in my life.
10. I feel close to _____.
11. I have a comfortable relationship with _____.
12. I feel that I really understand _____.
13. I feel that _____ really understands me.
14. I feel that I can really trust _____.
15. I share deeply personal information about myself with _____.
Passion
16. Just seeing _____ excites me.
17. I find myself thinking about _____ frequently during the day.
18. My relationship with _____ is very romantic.
19. I find _____ to be very personally attractive.
20. I idealize _____.
21. I cannot imagine another person making me as happy as _____ does.
22. I would rather be with _____ than with anyone else.
23. There is nothing more important to me than my relationship with _____.
24. I especially like physical contact with _____.
25. There is something almost “magical” about my relationship with _____.
26. I adore _____.
27. I cannot imagine life without _____.
28. My relationship with _____ is passionate.
65
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
29. When I see romantic movies or read romantic books I think of _____.
30. I fantasize about _____.
Commitment
31. I know that I care about _____.
32. I am committed to maintaining my relationship with _____.
33. Because of my commitment to _____, I would not let other people come between
us.
34. I have confidence in the stability of my relationship with _____.
35. I could not let anything get in the way of my commitment to _____.
36. I expect my love for _____ to last for the rest of my life.
37. I will always have a strong responsibility for _____.
38. I view my commitment to _____ as a solid one.
39. I cannot imagine ending my relationship with _____.
40. I am certain of my love for _____.
41. I view my relationship with _____ as permanent.
42. I view my relationship with _____ as a good decision.
43. I feel a sense of responsibility toward _____.
44. I plan to continue in my relationship with _____.
45. Even when _____ is hard to deal with, I remain committed to our relationship.
If the participant has high scores on all the three components this would indicate
consummate love. However, the nature of a relationship may change over time, not
saying that low scores on the three proposed scales represents a weak romantic
relationship.
Note: The questions are from “The Triangular Love Scale” from The Triangle of Love: Intimacy, Passion,
Commitment, by R. Sternberg.
66
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
Appendix B
The questionnaire originates from Griffin & Bartholomew’s (1994) original study,
Note. [Table] Found in “Self-report attachment measures” by Bartholomews Research Lab, n.d.,
from K. Bartholomew.
68
NEURAL CORRELATES OF ROMANTIC LOVE AND ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT
Appendix C
Note. [Table] Found in “Attachment styles among young adults: A test of a four-category model,”
by K. Bartholomew & L. M. Horowitz, 1991, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2, pp.
229. Copyright (1991) by “The American Psychological Association”. Retrieved June 12, 2018,
Appendix D
Attachment styles
❖ Secure attachment style: The parent has been aligned with the child, and
in tune with the child’s emotions and needs. This results in adult
The person will be emphatic and able to set boundaries for itself.
and intrusive in their communication with the child. This results in adult
abusive. The person is often untrusting while they at the same time craves
Appendix E
Figure 2. This figure shows some of the brain areas involved in romantic love and
Note. [Figure] Found in the “The neurobiology of love “ by S. Zeki, 2007, Federation of European
Biochemical Societies Letters, 581, 2575-2579. Reprinted with permission from the author.