Johnson - A Measure of Cooperative, Competitive, and Individualistic Attitudes
Johnson - A Measure of Cooperative, Competitive, and Individualistic Attitudes
Johnson - A Measure of Cooperative, Competitive, and Individualistic Attitudes
SUMMARY
A. INTRODUCTION
There are several hundred studies comparing the relative effects of
cooperative, competitive, and individualistic situations (5, 6). These three
types of social interdependence may be defined asfollows (3, 5): Coopera-
tive interdependence involves a positive correlation among goalattain-
mentsof the people involved; competitive interdependence involves a nega-
tive correlation among the goal attainments of the people involved; and
when the goal attainments of people are independent, an individualistic
situation exists. Cooperative, competitive, and individualistic conditions
have been found to be related to performance in achievementsituations,
self-esteem, achievement motivation, attitudes toward other students, at-
titudes toward school and subject areas, attitudes toward teachers and
other school personnel, locus of control, perspective-taking ability, psycho-
logical health, and many other important aspects of cognitive and social
development. Despite the proliferation of studies of cooperative, competi-
B. METHOD
1. Samples
The samples were, respectively, the entire student population (over 6000
students from grades 1 through 12) of a suburban school district, 143
12th-graders, 241 7th-graders, 153 12th-graders, 270 7th-graders and 152
college undergraduates. All samples were from a metropolitan area in the
Midwest. With the exception of the first sample, each population was a
nonprobability sample involving voluntary participation of all students
within collaborating teachers’ classes. Anonymity of each student was
assured.
2. Scale Development
The original development of the scales was part of the development of a
larger instrumentto be used for evaluation of affective outcomes of schools.
As part of the Minnesota School Affect Assessment (MSAA), attitude items
containing a four-point false-true range were generated to measure such
factors as cooperation, competition, and individualistic efforts, importance
of school, attitudes toward the teacher, and locus of control. The items
were administered to all students in grades 1 through 12 in a large subur-
ban school district in the Midwest (4, 7). The results were subjected to
principal component factor analyses with varimax rotations; among the
scales resulting were measures of cooperative, competitive, and individ-
ualistic interdependence with three items perscale.
D. W. JOHNSON AND A. A. NOREM-HEBEISEN 255
one group of Ss, the 153 high school seniors, to determine generalizability
of the factor analysis across factor methodology. Other analyses made to
test for generalizability included separate Alpha factor analyses across sex
and age groups, Cronbach's alpha measure of internal reliability, and
correlations among the three scales.
C. RESULTS
Given below, in the following order, are the descriptions of the results of
the factor analyses, the examination of factor structures separately for
males and females, a test of factor generalizability, a final factor analysis
on the items designated for use in the refined scales, Cronbach alpha
indices, and interscale correlations.
To heighten the sensitivity of the analysis to dimensions within the item
set, initial factor analyses on the responses of each sample were conducted
without specifying the number of factors to be generated; the only criterion
was that all factors have eigen values= 1. Among the two 12th-grade
samples, analysis conducted under these specifications produced three fac-
tors with three or more items loading= .5 on each factor. High loading
items for these factors were used to designate the factors’ contents; the
logical components were consistently cooperative, competitive, and indi-
vidualistic interdependence between oneself and others. By contrast, factor
analyses without specification of the number of factors in the 7th-grade
samples produced up to four or five factors with three or four items with
factor loadings = .5 on each factor. Whenthe logical contents of these item
sets were examined, it appeared that logical subsets of cooperative, compet-
itive, and individualistic social interdependence had emerged: liking to
cooperate and valuing cooperative learning, liking to compete and valuing
competitive learning, and liking to study alone and valuing individualistic
learning.
In order to determine whether single measures of cooperative, competi-
tive, and individualistic interdependence could be derived from the items
or whether the liking and valuing components of the factors needed to be
separated into distinct scales, the factor structures for males and females
were examined and a contrast of Alpha and Rao procedures of factor
analysis was conducted. Examination of the factor structures for males and
females within the 7th-grade and 12th-grade samples provides a clearer
picture of the emergence of item subsets. Subsets of cooperation occurred
only for 7th-grade females, subsets of competition occurred for 7th-grade
males and females, and subsets for individualistic efforts occurred for
7th-grade males and females and for 12th-grade males.
D. W. JOHNSON AND A. A. NOREM-HEBEISEN 257
Contrast of Alpha and Rao factor analyses were made for the responses
of the 12th-graders in Study 4. Patterns of factor loadings were markedly
similar across all analyses when the number of factors was specified at
three. When the numberoffactors wasleft unspecified, the factor loadings
for competitive and individualistic interdependence were markedly similar
across the two methods, but the item subsets in the cooperative inter-
dependence factor found in the Alpha analysis for the 7th-grade females
were apparent in the Rao analysis for this 12th-grade sample. The oth-
erwise generally high comparability of factor structure across these analyses
indicates that the three attitude scales are factorially robust.
The subdivisions of the major constructs into subsets among 7th-graders
raised the issue of developing six measures of social interdependence rather
than three. The choice to use three attitude scales was made on the basis of
several considerations: (a) item subsets did not occur consistently across
7th-grade and 12th-grade samples, (b) there was high continuity between
responses of the 7th- and 12th-graders when the subsets of items within
each of the three major constructs were combined, (c) the stability of the
three primary factors was evident in the comparison of Alpha and Rao
factor analyses, (d) indices of internal reliability were more robust when the
subset of items found among 7th-graders were combined, and (e) the
purpose of the studies was to develop single measures of cooperative,
competitive, and individualistic attitudes. The three scales (with the sub-
sets of items indicated) appear in the Appendix.
A final check on the factorial structure of the scales was conducted by
submitting the restricted item pool of 22 items to factor analysis. This
analysis was conducted for Studies 4 and 5, as the Ss in these studies had
answeredthe full range of 32 items. As a test of the relative independence
of the three constructs, the items were forced into three factors. The
patterns of factor loading for these analyses reflect a robust and consistent
factor structure with item loadings consistent with the logical contentof the
items. In the 12th-grade analysis, however, three of the individualistic
items show notable strong negative loadings on the cooperation scale
(—.50, —.48, —.45), while in the 7th-grade analysis three of the competitive
items show a positive loading on the individualistic factor (.38, .32, .33)
and one competitive item shows a moderate loading on the cooperative
factor (.37). These loadings are another reflection of the tendency toward
item subsets on the three scales among younger students.
Cronbach alphaindices of internal reliability were computed on the three
factors reflecting attitudes toward cooperative, competitive, and individ-
ualistic social interdependence. From Tables 1 and 2 it may be seen that
TABLE 1
CRONBACH ALPHA INDICES FOR EVOLVING MEASURES OF SOCIAL INTERDEPENDENCE
D. Discussion
Three relatively independent self-report scales with substantial internal
reliability were developed to measure attitudes toward cooperative, com-
petitive, and individualistic interdependence between oneself and others in
an educationalsetting. It is hoped that these scales will be of assistance in
TABLE 2
INTERCORRELATIONS AMONG SCALES OF SOCIAL INTERDEPENDENCE
Cooperative
Entire school district -02 —.32
12th grade, 1975 68 -.21
7th grade, 1975 wll -.21
12th grade, 1976 .00 —.48
7th grade, 1976 —.03 —.28
College, 1977 —.01 —.60
Competitive
Entire school district 24
12th grade, 1975 .00
7th grade, 1975 22
12th grade, 1976 .04
7th grade, 1976 32
College, 1977 .04
D. W. JOHNSON AND A. A. NOREM-HEBEISEN 259
a. Cooperative Interdependence.
(1). Liking to cooperate.
T like to help other students learn.
b. Competitive Interdependence.
(1). Liking to compete.
I like to do better work than other students.
I work to get better grades than other students do.
I like to be the best student in the class.
I don’t like to be second.
c. Individualistic Independence.
(1). Liking to study alone.
I don’t like working with other students in school.
I like to work with other students. (reverse)
It bothers me when I have to work with other students.
REFERENCES
1. AHLGREN, A., & JoHNSON, D. W. Sex differences in cooperative and competitive
attitudes from the second through twelfth grades. Devel. Psychol., in press.
2. DeEuTscH, M. A theory of cooperation and competition. Hum. Relat., 1949, 2, 129-352.
D. W. JOHNSON AND A. A. NOREM-HEBEISEN 261
3. ——-. Cooperation and trust: Some theoretical notes. In M. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska
Symposium on Motivation. Lincoln, Neb.: Univ. Nebraska Press, 1962. Pp. 275-320.
4. Jounson, D. W. Evaluating affective outcomes of schools. In H. Walberg (Ed.), Evalua-
tion School Performance. Berkeley, Calif.: McCutchan, 1974.
5. Jonnson, D. W. & JoHNSON, R. Learning Together and Alone: Cooperation, Competi-
tion, and Individualization. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1975.
6. ~~, Social interdependence in the classroom: Cooperation, competition, and indi-
vidualism. J. Res. Devel. in Educ., 1978. 12 (Fall).
7. MINNESOTA HIGH SCHOOL TESTING PROGRAM. Minnesota School Affect Assessment:
Manual. Minneapolis: Univ. Minnesota, 1977.