Sibling Relationship and Friendship in Adolescents With Autism

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Journal of Child and Family Studies (2020) 29:583–591

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-019-01570-x

ORIGINAL PAPER

Sibling Relationship and Friendship in Adolescents with Autism


Spectrum Disorder
1
Fiorenzo Laghi ●
Antonia Lonigro2 Susanna Pallini3 Graziana Marziano1 Roberto Baiocco1
● ● ●

Published online: 19 September 2019


© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Abstract
Objectives The study was designed to compare sibling and best friend relationships engaged by adolescents with autism
spectrum disorder (ASD).
Methods Twenty adolescents with ASD, aged 15–18 years ((M = 16.80, SD = 0.95), and their typically developing (TD)
siblings (n = 20), aged 18 to 24 years (M = 20.05, SD = 1.67), were recruited for the current study. Behaviors underpinning
attachment, caregiving and affiliative systems were evaluated as well as negative interaction behaviors in sibling relation-
ships and friendships. Non-parametric statistical analyses (the Wilcoxon signed-ranks test and the Mann–Whitney U-test)
1234567890();,:
1234567890();,:

were carried out to verify the differences in sibling and best friend relationships.
Results Adolescents with ASD referred a higher presence of negative interaction behaviors in sibling relationships com-
pared to friendships. However, when mixed and non-mixed friendships were distinguished, the descriptions of typically
developing (TD) siblings and (TD) best friends overlapped for caregiving behaviors, revealing as both of them were
depicted as a source of support and comfort by adolescents with ASD. A lower presence of behaviors underpinning
attachment, caregiving and affiliations characterized relationships with best friends with disability compared to TD best
friends.
Conclusion This study highlighted that TD siblings provided a strong support for their siblings with ASD. The same role
was also played by TD best friends.
Keywords Autism spectrum disorder Sibling relationships Friendship Behavioral system theory
● ● ●

Deficits in social communication, social interaction and (Bauminger and Kasari 2000; Lasgaard et al. 2009). The
difficulties in functioning levels characterizing Autism key features of ASD, such as the presence of few shared
Spectrum Disorder (ASD) strongly concern parents for interests with others, the occurrence of highly routinized
negative implications that these impairments may have on and stereotypical behaviors, and the inability to adequately
social life for their daughters and sons with ASD at all ages. initiate and respond to both verbal and non-verbal social
Several studies have broadly demonstrated how students cues (for details, see American Psychiatric Association
with ASD are more often neglected or rejected by their 2013) make very infrequent to view a student with ASD as
classmates (Laghi et al. 2018a; Ochs et al. 2001), experi- a central or popular figure in the classroom (Chamberlain
encing more frequently feelings of loneliness at school et al. 2007; Leslie and Billingsey 2010; Di Maggio et al.
compared to their typically developing (TD) peers 2016; Laghi et al. 2016a, 2016b, 2018c). Nevertheless,
students with ASD have major difficulties to meet their
friends outside the school context (Kuo et al. 2013; Ors-
mond et al. 2004) and maintain a close friendship over time
* Fiorenzo Laghi
[email protected]
(Rowley et al. 2012). Even when students with ASD make
same-aged friends, the self-perceived quality of their
1
Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, Sapienza friendships is often lower than those referred by their TD
University of Rome, via dei Marsi 78, 00185 Rome, Italy counterparts (Calder et al. 2013).
2
Department of Human Sciences, Università Europea di Roma, Researchers agree that among children and adolescents
Rome, Italy with ASD high levels of cognitive skills, in particular a
3
Department of Education, University of Rome Tre, Rome, Italy good intellectual functioning and well-developed
584 Journal of Child and Family Studies (2020) 29:583–591

language abilities, constitute a protective factor which 2009; Tomeny et al. 2012), highlighting how the natural
may compensate a part of deficits in social cognition shift between intimacy, fun and emotion sharing on the
(e.g., theory of mind skills), thus allowing people with one side, and conflict, treat and tease on the other side is
ASD to spontaneously interact with other people in a common to all sibling relationships, with or without a
more socially complex and co-regulated way (Bauminger person with ASD (Brody 2004). As argued by McHale
et al. 2010; Lonigro et al. 2014; Sigman and Ruskin et al. (2012), positive sibling relationship behaviors (e.g.,
1999). Moreover, when students with ASD interact with helping, teaching, caring) and negative sibling relation-
TD friends (e.g., mixed dyads) they engage more fre- ship ones (e.g., teasing, arguing, undermining) are not
quently in more advanced social behaviors (e.g., eye two opposite poles of the same continuum, but they can
contact with smile) than they do when they interact with co-exist. In the same way, opposite emotions may be felt
other disabled students (e.g., non-mixed dyads). Overall, by TD siblings. They often refer a great sense of
in mixed friendships, both partners, including children admiration for their disabled siblings for challenges that
and adolescents with ASD, are more responsive to one they must cope with every day (Kaminsky and Dewey
another and exhibit a greater positive social orientation 2001; Laghi et al. 2018b; Petalas et al. 2009) as well as
than in non-mixed dyads (Bauminger et al. 2008). In a feelings of embarrassment for problem behaviors (e.g.,
previous study, Bauminger-Zviely et al. (2014) found physical aggression, self-injury, tantrums) displayed by
that during recess time, which occurred between struc- siblings with ASD in public (Mascha and Boucher 2006).
tured activities, preschoolers with ASD who held good Recently, Tomeny et al. (2017) have argued that less-
cognitive abilities conversed spontaneously with their advanced social abilities held by individuals with ASD
friends, displayed higher levels of social complexity, often encourage TD siblings to provide care for them.
fewer pragmatic pitfalls, and more adequate interactions The Behavioral System Theory (Furman and Wehner
than they did when they interacted with non-friends. 1994), which is premised on Attachment Theory (Ains-
In the family context, the core symptoms of ASD may worth 1989; Bowlby 1969; Pace et al. 2016) and Sulli-
sometimes make parenting and siblinghood challenging vanian Theory (Sullivan 1953), constitutes a good lens to
(Benderix and Sivberg 2007; Higgins et al. 2005; Ludlow observe and understanding the quality of interactions
et al. 2012; Sikora et al. 2013). Several studies have among siblings and friends, respectively. A behavioral
broadly documented how raising children with ASD may system is a goal-corrected system which is finalized to
hamper everyday activities due to the core symptoms of maintain a relatively steady state between the individual
ASD, such as the difficulty in managing unexpected and the environment, thus warranting survival. In close
changes (e.g., using a different street to go home). relationships, three behavioral systems are activated,
Among parents of children with ASD minor levels of whose degrees may vary by the type of relationship (e.g.,
parental self-efficacy and elevated levels of parenting friends, siblings). The Attachment System aims at
stress have been often observed compared to families maintaining some degree of proximity to an attachment
with TD daughters and sons (Karst and Van Hecke 2012). figure in order to gain comfort and security (Bowlby
It must be considered that the lifelong burden places not 1969). Two different kinds of behavior characterize the
only on parents but also on siblings of children with attachment system: Seeking the other out as a safe haven
ASD. While several studies dealing with the impact of when upset or distressed, and using the other person as a
having a child with ASD on parents have been carried out secure base to engage in exploratory behaviors. Con-
(Martins et al. 2015; Thomas et al. 2018), the same ceptualized as reciprocal to the attachment system, the
cannot be said for the impact on TD siblings who have a Caregiving System refers to behaviors finalized to pro-
sibling with ASD (Cicirelli 1995; Petalas et al. 2015; vide comfort and security to the other person by either
Rivers and Stoneman 2003; Stoneman 2005). Never- providing a safe haven or a secure base (George and
theless, the findings provided by recent studies have not Solomon 2008). Finally, the Affiliative System is based
led to a shared agreement among researchers (McHale on humans’ biological predisposition to interact with
et al. 2012, 2016; Walton and Ingersoll 2015). If on the others for protection and co-operative food-sharing
one hand, a poorer quality of interactions has been opportunities (e.g., companionship).
observed in sibling relationships of individuals with ASD The purpose of the current study was to compare sibling
compared to TD siblings or sibling pairs where one has and best friend relationships as described by adolescents
another disability (Hodapp and Urbano 2007; Mascha with ASD. We hypothesized that among sibling relation-
and Boucher 2006; Orsmond and Seltzer 2007), on the ships, behaviors underpinning the attachment system
other hand, no differences have been found between would prevail among siblings with ASD whilst those
different kinds of sibling pairs (Angell et al. 2012; relating to the caregiving system would be more engaged
Dempsey et al. 2012; Diener et al. 2015; Petalas et al. by TD siblings, without excluding the presence of negative
Journal of Child and Family Studies (2020) 29:583–591 585

interactions. When friendships are considered, we hypo- to TD siblings if they were interested in participating at a
thesized that behaviors underlying the attachment system support group for sibling of adolescents or young adults
would be less present among non-mixed friendships ado- with ASD.
lescents (e.g., Autism Spectrum Disorder, Intellectual
Disabilities) compared to mixed ones. We were also Measures
interested in verifying the role of gender in friendships,
hypothesizing to confirm in adolescence the same trend The Italian adaptation of network of relationships
observed in past studies on childhood (for details, see Dean inventory: behavioral systems version
et al. 2014).
The Italian adaptation of network of relationships
inventory: behavioral systems version (NRI-BSV; Fur-
Method man and Buhrmester 2009; Dellagiulia et al. 2011) is a
pencil-and-paper questionnaire comprising 25 items. It is
Participants finalized to evaluate 8 features of close relationships. In
details, 2 scales evaluate attachment behaviors: (a) par-
The sample included 40 participants: 20 adolescents with ticipant seeks safe haven (e.g., “How do you seek out this
ASD and 20 TD siblings. Participants with ASD (16M and person when you’re upset?”), and (b) participant seeks
4F) ranged in age from 15 to 18 years (M = 16.80, SD = secure base (e.g., “How much does this person
0.95), with average intelligence quotient (IQ total score, M = encourages you to try new things that you’d like to do but
104,15, SD = 5.34; range: 90–110). The diagnosis of each are nervous about?”). Two scales assess caregiving
participant with ASD had been assured, prior to the inclu- behaviors: (a) participant provides safe haven “e.g.,
sion in the study, by experienced psychiatrists and psy- “How much does this person turn to you for comfort and
chologists using gold standard diagnostic instruments for support when s/he is troubled about something?”), and
ASD. TD siblings (13F and 7M) ranged in age from 18 to (b) participant provides secure base (e.g., “How much do
24 years (M = 20.05, SD = 1.67). All of the TD siblings in you encourage this person to try new things that s/he
the research were older than their siblings with ASD, and would like to do but is nervous about?”). The compa-
they were biological siblings to the children with ASD. Two nionship scale evaluates affiliative behaviors (e.g., “How
point three years (SD = 1.65) was the average absolute much do you and this person spend free time together?”).
value of sibling age differences (range equaled 1–8 years). Moreover, this instrument allows us to assess negative
interaction scales, namely conflict (e.g., “How much do
Procedure you and this person get upset with or mad at each
other?”), antagonism (e.g., “How much do you and this
We recruited participants in this study by distributing person hassle or nag one another), and criticism (e.g.,
information sheet about the study to local organizations and “How much do you and this person say mean or harsh
centers that provided services to adolescents and young things to each other?). Items measure the extent to which
adults with ASD. The eligibility requirements were (1) to features occur in the relationship across a five-point
have a certified diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder, (2) Likert format, from 1—Little or None—to 5—The most.
the absence of intellectual disability, and (3) the participa- Eight scores are computed, one for each scale, by aver-
tion of one of their TD siblings. All of the participants lived aging the corresponding items. Moreover, 2 second-order
in the family home. factors may be computed. Support factor is obtained by
Approval from the University Institutional Review Board averaging affiliation, attachment and caregiving scales,
was obtained before data were collected for the current whereas Negative Interaction is derived by averaging
study. Participants with ASD and their TD siblings were conflict, antagonism, and criticism scales. The NRI-BSV
independently evaluated at a research laboratory at the has been successfully used in both clinical and non-
Faculty of Medicine and Psychology (Sapienza University clinical studies, in the Italian context too, providing good
of Rome) by a team of clinical psychologists with the evidence dealing with its validity (for details, see Della-
supervision of the first author and took approximately giulia et al. 2011).
35 min to complete. In the current study, we administered the NRI-BSV to
Application consisted of the participants fulfilling the both adolescents with ASD and their TD siblings, respec-
questionnaires in a quiet research laboratory, after informed tively. For friendship, the NRI-BSV was only administered
consent was acquired from both participants and their to adolescents with ASD. Thus, we had information on
parents if they were under eighteen-year-old. After friendship only through the point of view provided by
administering the questionnaires, the experimenters asked adolescents with ASD.
586 Journal of Child and Family Studies (2020) 29:583–591

Friendship with the best friend the best friend. All statistical analyses were performed with
SPSS 24 for Windows.
The most shared definition of best friend was adopted to
formulate the questions. As described in previous studies
(for details, see Baiocco et al. 2014), best friend is defined Results
as someone with whom one shares the closest kind of
friendship, excluding intimate or sexual activities. The Convergence among Reporters: Differences
questions deal with the existence of a best friend (e.g., “Do between Adolescents with ASD and Their Siblings
you have a best friend?”), the best friend’s noun and gender
(e.g., “What is her/his name?”), the duration of best The Wilcoxon signed-ranks test showed statistically sig-
friendship (e.g., “How long has she/he been your best nificant differences for Seeks secure base, and Conflict,
friend?”), and the characteristics of best friend (e.g., “Can where adolescents with ASD reported higher scores than
you tell me about some of your best friend?). The last their siblings; TD siblings reported higher scores for Pro-
question was structured to explore the possibility that best vides safe haven, and Provides secure base than their ASD
friend had a diagnosis of ASD. A confirmation was asked to sibling, as reported in Table 1.
siblings of adolescents with ASD because they knew their
best friends, thus constituting a reliable source. Best Friend Relationship

Data Analyses All participants with ASD nominated a best friend who
were 16 to 22 years of age (M = 18.40, SD = 1.57); for
Due to the small number of sibling pairs, a continuous female adolescents with ASD (n = 4) the majority of their
normal distribution could not be assumed. Thus, non- best friends were same sex (3F and 1M); for male adoles-
parametric statistical analyses were used. The Wilcoxon cents with ASD (n = 16) 13 was same sex friend and 3 were
signed-ranks test was carried out to analyze the mean-level different sex. Fisher’s exact test was used because the
differences between participants with ASD and their sib- number of observations in the cells of the contingency table
lings in the evaluation of their relationship. For adolescents was less than 5, pointing out no cross-gender best friendship
with ASD, pair-wise comparisons with Wilcoxon signed- differences (p = 0.06). Of these, 11 relationships involved
rank were used to verify whether there were differences in best friends with an ASD diagnosis, and 9 relationships
scale and subscale scores among different types of rela- involved best friends without ASD diagnosis. Fisher’s exact
tionships (sibling vs. best friend). Mann–Whitney U-tests test pointed out no gender differences (p = 0.28). The
were computed to examine group differences (presence or mean duration of friendship, expressed in months, was
absence of a cross gender friendship) in relationship with 62.60 (SD = 44.72; range: 4–192). Sixty-point one year

Table 1 Sibling relationship differences between adolescents with ASD and their TD siblings: descriptive statistics and
significant effects
Sibling relationship quality Report of adolescents with ASD TD siblings Significant value
N = 20 N = 20
M SD Alpha M SD Alpha Z p

Scales
Seeks safe haven 2.72 1.11 0.90 2.10 0.89 0.88 −1.86 0.06
Seeks secure base 2.95 1.02 0.88 2.05 1.16 0.90 −2.56 0.01
Provides safe haven 2.18 1.07 0.89 2.85 1.30 0.88 −1.95 0.05
Provides secure base 2.68 1.17 0.90 3.40 1.05 0.87 −2.05 0.04
Companionship 2.98 1.08 0.88 2.95 0.88 0.88 −0.53 0.60
Conflict 3.02 0.72 0.89 2.43 0.58 0.85 −2.61 0.01
Criticism 2.47 1.07 0.83 2.05 0.80 0.86 −1.21 0.23
Antagonism 2.82 0.83 0.82 2.60 0.94 0.88 −0.83 0.41
Factors
Support 2.70 0.86 0.90 2.67 0.79 0.88 −0.46 0.65
Negative Interaction 2.77 0.82 0.86 2.36 0.61 0.87 −1.42 0.16
Journal of Child and Family Studies (2020) 29:583–591 587

Table 2 Sibling and best friend relationship differences as described by adolescents with ASD: descriptive statistics and
significant effects
Relationship quality by adolescents with ASD TD siblings Friends Significant value
M SD Alpha M SD Alpha Z p

Scales
Seeks safe haven 2.72 1.11 0.90 2.62 1.26 0.90 −0.20 0.84
Seeks secure base 2.95 1.02 0.88 2.95 1.14 0.88 −0.10 0.92
Provides safe haven 2.18 1.07 0.89 2.57 1.24 0.89 −0.97 0.33
Provides secure base 2.68 1.17 0.90 2.90 1.29 0.88 −0.95 0.34
Companionship 2.98 1.08 0.88 3.47 1.18 0.87 −1.43 0.15
Conflict 3.02 0.72 0.89 1.60 0.68 0.88 −3.85 0.00
Criticism 2.47 1.07 0.83 1.48 0.61 0.86 −3.50 0.00
Antagonism 2.82 0.83 0.82 1.37 0.51 0.82 −3.88 0.00
Factors
Support 2.70 0.86 0.90 2.90 1.07 0.88 −1.06 0.29
Negative Interaction 2.77 0.82 0.86 1.48 0.54 0.86 −3.89 0.00

(SD = 1.50) was the average absolute value of age differ- Companionship, and Criticism than adolescents who
ences (range equaled 0–5 years). reported a best friend with an ASD diagnosis, as reported in
Table 3. Similar results were obtained for second order
Differences in Relationship: Sibling and Best Friend factor Support.

To investigate differences in relationship (sibling vs. best


friend), we conducted the Wilcoxon signed–ranks test on Discussion
NRI-BSV dimensions, comparing sibling and best friend
evaluations. The analysis revealed significant differences The present study contributes to the research literature
only for Conflict, Criticism, and Antagonism, where ado- dealing with social relationships in ASD. The use of
lescents with ASD reported higher scores for sibling rela- behavioral systems to compare sibling relationships and
tionship than best friend relationship. Similar results were friendships represents the novel aspect of our investigation.
obtained for second order dimension, Negative interaction, The first aim of the present study was to analyze similarities
as reported in Table 2. and differences in sibling relationships, considering the
perspective of both siblings, with ASD and with a typical
Differences in Best Friend Relationship: Same and development. It is very interesting to notice that com-
Cross Gender plementary behavioral systems were activated. Whether
behaviors relating to caregiving system prevailed among
We performed Mann–Whitney U-tests to verify the differ- TD siblings, behaviors dealing with attachment system were
ences for evaluating best friend relationship, considering more reported by siblings with ASD as well as conflict. TD
two different groups (same or different sex of best friend). siblings described their sibling relationships in terms of a
No significant differences were found for cross-gender reliable base from which their own siblings with ASD
friendship. depart to explore the environment and engage in new
activities. In details, they referred to display support and
Differences in Best Friend Relationship: Best Friend encourage their siblings with ASD to try new plays or
with or without an ASD Diagnosis activities and pursue their goals. Moreover, TD siblings
described their role of a safe haven where their siblings with
Additionally, we performed Mann–Whitney U-tests to ASD may return in case of danger or when they are trou-
verify the differences for evaluating best friend relationship bled, worried or upset to receive support and comfort.
(with or without an ASD diagnosis). Significant differences In a reciprocal way, adolescents with ASD confirmed the
were found for type of friendship. Adolescents with ASD role of support provided by their TD sisters and brothers,
who reported a TD best friend obtained higher scores for particularly for seeking secure base. Thus, TD siblings seem
Seeks safe haven, Seeks secure base, Provides secure base, to play a role in the improvement of autonomy and
588 Journal of Child and Family Studies (2020) 29:583–591

Table 3 Best friend with or without an ASD diagnosis: descriptive statistics and significant effects
Relationship quality with Best friend with a TD best friend Significant value
best friend diagnosis of ASD (N = 9)
(N = 11)
M SD M SD U p

Scales
Seeks safe haven 1.91 0.76 3.48 1.24 13.5 0.01
Seeks secure base 2.36 0.77 3.67 1.14 14.5 0.01
Provides safe haven 2.09 0.94 3.15 1.37 26.5 0.08
Provides secure base 2.24 0.87 3.70 1.30 17 0.01
Companionship 3.03 1.01 4.00 1.21 19 0.02
Conflict 1.55 0.70 1.67 0.69 43 0.60
Criticism 1.27 0.65 1.74 0.46 24 0.03
Antagonism 1.30 0.55 1.44 0.47 39.5 0.40
Factors
Support 2.33 0.67 3.60 1.08 13 0.01
Negative Interaction 1.37 0.59 1.62 0.49 32 0.17

independence for their siblings with ASD. However, this documented by past investigations with school-aged chil-
issue leads us to ascertain whether the role assumed by TD dren (Dean et al. 2014). When adolescents with ASD
sibling may have a cost for them because they might engage described their social relationships, they referred higher
in a caregiver role that usually reserves to parents. This risk levels of negative interactions—including conflict, criticism
should not be neglected but it needs to be further considered and antagonism—in sibling relationships compared to
in research. Tomeny et al. (2017) have recently argued on friendships. It is reasonably to think that the great number of
the risk of parentification among TD siblings of brothers hours that siblings spend together may expose them a higher
and sisters with ASD, distinguishing sibling-focused par- likelihood of conflicts and quarrels compared to time spent
entification from parent-focused parentification. According with best friends. Interestingly, when a fine-grained analysis
their results, the latter form appeared to be more likely was carried out distinguishing among two types of friend-
predictive of anxiety and depression among TD siblings. ships, the descriptions of sibling relationships and mixed
Future research should further examine the impact of par- friendships by adolescents with ASD overlapped, high-
entification by TD siblings on their mental health and social lighting how TD siblings and TD best friends represent a
conduct, extending the interest towards the relation between secure base or a safe haven for adolescents with ASD in
different forms of parentification and the activation of order to carry out their daily activities. Furthermore, the
behavioral systems by TD siblings. interactions with TD best friends were depicted by adoles-
Interestingly, negative aspects of sibling relationships cents with ASD using behaviors underpinning affiliation
were highlighted only by siblings with ASD, who described and attachment behavioral systems, revealing how in mixed
higher levels of conflict compared to those reported by TD interactions both partners—TD best friends and adolescents
siblings. It is well documented that TD siblings, particularly with ASD—offered and received support more than in non-
if they are older than disabled siblings, are prone to develop mixed interactions. This finding is consistent with previous
a sense of duty, responsibility and commitment towards investigations examining the differences between social
their siblings with ASD because of the challenges and dif- interactions in mixed and non-mixed friendships (Bau-
ficulties that they must face every day (Cridland et al. 2016; minger et al. 2003, 2008; Bauminger-Zviely et al. 2014). It
Tozer et al. 2013). Perhaps, this support role displayed by is well documented that mixed friendships are more similar
TD siblings and overall difficulties that they have to face in to typical friendships compared to non-mixed ones. In
quality of siblings of children with ASD might obscure mixed friendships, children and adolescents with ASD are
minor conflicts that occur daily (e.g., how much they and involved in co-regulated social exchanges, displaying
their siblings with ASD disagree). However, it must be said greater positive social orientation, more advanced receptive
that this is only our hypothesis and future studies need to language abilities and responsiveness than in non-mixed
test this possibility. In regard to friendship, our results friendships (Bauminger et al. 2008). However, in mixed
showed that adolescents with ASD socialized with same- friendships higher levels of criticism were described by
gender peers, thus confirming the trend which was adolescents with ASD compared to non-mixed friendships.
Journal of Child and Family Studies (2020) 29:583–591 589

As for sibling relationships, the bonds with TD friends Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
appeared to confirm that positive and negative aspects may
co-exist in the same relationship. As argued by McHale and
colleagues (2012), behaviors regarding helping, caring,
comforting on the one side and those referring to teasing, References
blaming, arguing on the other side are not placed on
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American
opposite poles, but their engagement depends on contextual Psychologist, 44(4), 709–716. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.
situation. Moreover, episodes of criticism may constitute for 44.4.709.
both adolescents with ASD and their best friends as an American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical
manual of mental disorders. 5th Edn Washington, DC: American
opportunity to learn how functionally behave when a dis-
Psychiatric Association.
agree about situations or events occurs. Angell, M. E., Meadan, H., & Stoner, J. B. (2012). Experiences of
siblings of individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Autism
Limitations Research and Treatment, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1155/2012/
949586.
Baiocco, R., Pallini, S., & Santamaria, F. (2014). The development
The study presents several limits that need to be mentioned. and validation of an Italian short form of the adolescent friend-
Firstly, the low number of adolescents involved in our study ship attachment scale. Measurement and Evaluation in Coun-
does not allow us to infer general patterns relating to sibling seling and Development, 47(4), 247–255. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0748175614538060.
relationships and friendships in autism spectrum condition.
Bauminger-Zviely, N., Karin, E., Kimhi, Y., & Agam-Ben-Artzi, G.
Nevertheless, the descriptions of friendships were provided (2014). Spontaneous peer conversation in preschoolers with high-
by only adolescents with ASD. It would be very interesting functioning autism spectrum disorder versus typical development.
to compare the perspective of adolescents with ASD Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(4), 363–373.
https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12158.
with that provided by their best friends to verify whether
Bauminger, N., Solomon, M., & Rogers, S. J. (2010). Predicting
similarities or differences occur. Secondly, attention was friendship quality in autism spectrum disorders and typical
limited to adolescents with ASD and an average intelligence development. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
quotient. Future research efforts should be devoted to 40(6), 751–761. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-009-0928-8.
Bauminger, N., Solomon, M., Aviezer, A., Heung, K., Brown, J., &
investigate sibling relationships and friendships among
Rogers, S. (2008). Friendship in high-functioning children with
adolescents with ASD and intellectual disability. Thirdly, ASD: mixed and non-mixed dyads. Journal of Autism and
TD siblings were older than their sisters and brothers with Developmental Disorders, 38(7), 1211–1229. https://doi.org/10.
ASD. Our results may be influenced by this aspect, thus 1007/s10803-007-0501-2.
Bauminger, N., Shulman, C., & Agam, G. (2003). Peer interaction and
further investigation needs to exclude this possibility.
loneliness in high-functioning children with autism. Journal of
Finally, family functioning, that may impact on sibling Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(5), 489–507. https://
relationships, were not evaluated. Future research may doi.org/10.1023/A:1025827427901.
include this aspect to shed light on mechanisms that may Bauminger, N., & Kasari, C. (2000). Loneliness and friendships in
high functioning children with autism. Child Development, 71(2),
hinder or promote satisfactory sibling relationships.
447–456. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00156.
Benderix, Y., & Sivberg, B. (2007). Siblings’ experiences of having a
Author Contributions F.L. designed and executed the study, analyzed brother or sister with autism and mental retardation: a case study
the data, and wrote the paper. A.L. collaborated with the design and of 14 siblings from five families. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 22
writing the manuscript. S.P. and R.B. collaborated in the writing and (5), 410–418. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pedn.2007.08.013.
editing the final manuscript. Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Volume 1, Attachment. New
York, NY: Basic Books.
Compliance with Ethical Standards Brody, J. (2004). Siblings’ direct and indirect contributions to child
development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13
(4), 124–126. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00289.x.
Conflict of Interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of
Calder, L., Hill, V., & Pellicano, E. (2013). “Sometimes I want to play
interest.
by myself”: understanding what friendship means to children
with autism in mainstream primary schools. Autism: The Inter-
Ethical Approval All procedures performed in this study were in national Journal of Research and Practice, 17(3), 296–316.
accordance with the ethical standards of institutional research com- https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361312467866.
mittee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments Chamberlain, B., Kasari, C., & Rotheram-Fuller, E. (2007). Involve-
or comparable ethical standards. The Department of Social and ment or isolation? The social networks of children with autism in
Developmental Psychology, Faculty of Medicine and Psychology— regular classrooms. Journal of Autism and Developmental Dis-
Sapienza University of Rome—provided approval for the study. orders, 37(2), 230–242. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-
0164-4.
Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual Cicirelli, V. G. (1995). Sibling relationships across the lifespan. New
participants included in the study. As required by the Italian system York, NY: Plenum Publishers.
law, parental consent is always required for children up to 18 Cridland, E. K., Jones, S. C., Stoyles, G., Caputi, P., & Magee, C. A.
years old. (2016). Families living with autism spectrum disorder: roles and
590 Journal of Child and Family Studies (2020) 29:583–591

responsibilities of adolescent sisters. Focus on Autism and Other selected peer buddies for adolescents with autism spectrum dis-
Developmental Disabilities, 31(3), 196–207. https://doi.org/10. orders: the role of social, emotional, and mentalizing abilities.
1177/1088357615583466. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 150
Dean, M., Kasari, C., Shih, W., Frankel, F., Whitney, R., Landa, R., … (4), 469–484. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2015.1087375.
Harwood, R. (2014). The peer relationships of girls with ASD at Laghi, F., Lonigro, A., Levanto, S., Ferraro, M., Baumgartner, E., &
school: Comparison to boys and girls with and without ASD. Baiocco, R. (2016b). The role of nice and nasty theory of mind
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(11), 1218–1225. in teacher–selected peer models for adolescents with autism
https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12242. spectrum disorders. Measurement and Evaluation in Counsel-
Dellagiulia, A., Palummieri, A., & Mecenate, C. (2011). Le relazioni ing and Development, 49(3), 207–219. https://doi.org/10.1177/
interpersonali in adolescenza alla luce della teoria dell’attacca- 0748175615596784.
mento. Un primo utilizzo in Italia del questionario “Network of Laghi, F., Lonigro, A., Pallini, S., & Baiocco, R. (2018a). Peer buddies
Relationships Inventory”. Orientamenti Pedagogici, 3(58), in the classroom: the effects on spontaneous conversations in
619–640. students with autism spectrum disorder. Child and Youth Care
Dempsey, A., Llorens, A., Brewton, C., Mulchandani, S., & Goin- Forum, 47, 517–536. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10566-018-9449-y.
Kochel, R. (2012). Emotional and behavioral adjustment in Laghi, F., Lonigro, A., Pallini, S., Gradilone, A., Bechini, A.,
typically developing siblings of children with autism spectrum Marziano, G., & Baiocco, R. (2018b). Sibling relationships and
disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder, 42(7), family functioning in siblings of early adolescents, adolescents
1393–1402. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1268-9. and young adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of
Diener, M. L., Anderson, L., Wright, C. A., & Dunn, M. L. (2015). Child and Family Studies, 27, 793–801. https://doi.org/10.
Sibling relationships of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder 1007/s10826-017-0921-3.
in the context of everyday life and a strength-based program. Laghi, F., Lonigro, A., Baumgartner, E., & Baiocco, R. (2018c). What
Journal of Child and Family Studies, 24(4), 1060–1072. https:// does the intention to be a volunteer for a student with autism
doi.org/10.1007/s10826-014-9915-6. predict? The role of cognitive brain types and emotional and
Di Maggio, R., Zappulla, C., & Pace, U. (2016). The relationship behavior characteristics. Psicologia Educativa, 24(1), 26–30.
between emotion knowledge, emotion regulation and adjustment https://doi.org/10.5093/psed2018a7.
in preschoolers: a mediation model. Journal of Child and Family Lasgaard, M., Nielsen, A., Eriksen, M. E., & Goossens, L. (2009).
Studies, 25(8), 2626–2635. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-016- Loneliness and social support in adolescent boys with autism
0409-6. spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Dis-
Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (2009). The network of relationships orders, 40(2), 218–226. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-009-0851-z.
inventory: behavioral system version. International Journal of Leslie, S. D., & Billingsey, B. S. (2010). What boys with autism spectrum
Behavioral Development, 33(5), 470–478. https://doi.org/10. disorder say about establishing and maintaining friendships. Focus
1177/0165025409342634. on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 25(4), 220–230.
Furman, W., & Wehner, E. A. (1994). Romantic views: toward a https://doi.org/10.1177/1088357610378290.
theory of adolescent romantic relationships. In R. Montemayor, Lonigro, A., Laghi, F., Baiocco, R., & Baumgartner, E. (2014).
G. R. Adams, G. P. Gullotta (Eds), Advances in adolescent Mind reading skills and empathy: evidence for nice and nasty
development, Vol. 6: Relationships during adolescence. Thou- ToM behaviours in school-aged children. Journal of Child
sand Oaks, CA: Sage. and Family Studies, 23(3), 581–590. https://doi.org/10.1007/
George, C., & Solomon, J. (2008). The caregiving system: a beha- s10826-013-9722-5.
vioral systems approach to parenting. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Ludlow, A., Skelly, C., & Rohleder, P. (2012). Challenges faced by
Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and parents of children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder.
clinical implications. 2nd Edn New York, NY: Guilford Press. Journal of Health Psychology, 17(5), 702–711. https://doi.org/10.
Higgins, D. J., Bailey, S. R., & Pearce, J. C. (2005). Factors associated 1177/1359105311422955.
with functioning style and coping strategies of families with a Martins, R., Bonito, I., Andrade, A., Albuquerque, C., & Chaves, C.
child with an autism spectrum disorder. Autism: The International (2015). The impact of the diagnosis of autism in parents of
Journal of Research and Practice, 9(2), 125–137. https://doi.org/ children. Procedia—Social and Behavioral Sciences, 171(16),
10.1177/1362361305051403. 121–125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2015.01.097.
Hodapp, R. M., & Urbano, R. C. (2007). Adult siblings of individuals Mascha, K., & Boucher, J. (2006). Preliminary investigation
with Down syndrome versus with autism: findings from a lar- of a qualitative method of examining siblings’ experiences
gescale US survey. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, of living with a child with ASD. British Journal of Develop-
51(12), 1018–1029. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2788.2007. mental Disabilities, 52(1), 19–28. https://doi.org/10.1179/
00994.x. 096979506799103659.
Kaminsky, L., & Dewey, D. (2001). Siblings relationships of children McHale, S. M., Updegraff, K. A., & Feinberg, M. E. (2016). Siblings
with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31 of youth with autism spectrum disorders: theoretical perspectives
(4), 399–410. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010664603039. on sibling relationships and individual adjustment. Journal of
Karst, J. S., & Van Hecke, A. V. (2012). Parent and family impact of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(2), 589–602. https://
autism spectrum disorders: a review and proposed model for doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2611-6.
intervention evaluation. Clinical Child and Family Psychology McHale, S. M., Updegraff, K. A., & Whiteman, S. D. (2012). Sibling
Review, 15(3), 247–277. https://doi.org/10.1007/s.10567-012- relationships and influences in childhood and adolescence.
0119-6. Journal of Marriage and Family, 74(5), 913–930. https://doi.org/
Kuo, M. H., Orsmond, G. I., Cohn, E. S., & Coster, W. J. (2013). 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2012.01011.x.
Friendship characteristics and activity patterns of adolescents Ochs, E., Kremer-Sadlik, T., Solomon, O., & Sirota, K. G. (2001).
with an autism spectrum disorder. Autism: The International Inclusion as a social practice: views of children with autism.
Journal of Research and Practice, 17(4), 481–500. https://doi. Social Development, 10(3), 399–419. https://doi.org/10.1111/
org/10.1177/1362361311416380. 1467-9507.00172.
Laghi, F., Federico, F., Lonigro, A., Levanto, S., Ferraro, M., Orsmond, G. I., & Seltzer, M. M. (2007). Siblings of individual with
Baumgartner, E., & Baiocco, R. (2016a). Peer and teacher- autism spectrum disorders across the life course. Mental
Journal of Child and Family Studies (2020) 29:583–591 591

Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, Sikora, D., Moran, E., Orlich, F., Hall, T. A., Kovacs, E. A., &
13(4), 313–320. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrdd.20171. Delahaye, J., et al. (2013). The relationship between family
Orsmond, G. I., Krauss, M. W., & Seltzer, M. M. (2004). Peer rela- functioning and behavior problems in children with autism
tionships and social and recreational activities among adolescents spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7
and adults with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental (2), 307–315. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2012.09.006.
Disorders, 34(3), 245–256. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JADD. Stoneman, Z. (2005). Siblings of children with disabilities: research
0000029547.96610.df. themes. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities
Pace, U., Zappulla, C., & Di Maggio, R. (2016). The mediating role of Research Reviews, 43(5), 339–350. https://doi.org/10.1111/jftr.
perceived peer support in the relation between quality of attach- 12021.
ment and internalizing problems in adolescence: a longitudinal Sullivan, H. S. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry. New
perspective. Attachment and Human Development, 18(5), York, NY: Norton & Co.
508–524. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2016.1198919. Thomas, P. A., King, J. S., Mendelson, J. L., & Nelson-Gray, R. O.
Petalas, M. A., Hastings, R. P., Nash, S., & Duff, S. (2015). Typicality (2018). Parental psychopathology and expectations for the futures
and subtle differences in sibling relationships: experiences of of children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Applied
adolescents with autism. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 24 Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 31(1), 98–105. https://doi.
(1), 38–49. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-013-9811-5. org/10.1111/jar.12337.
Petalas, M. A., Hastings, R. P., Nash, S., Dowey, A., & Reilly, D. Tomeny, T. S., Barry, T. D., & Bader, S. H. (2012). Are typically-
(2009). “I like that he always shows who he is”: the perceptions developing siblings of children with an Autism Spectrum Dis-
and experiences of siblings with a brother with autism order at risk for behavioural, emotional, and social maladjust-
spectrum disorder. International Journal of Disability Devel- ment? Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6(1), 508–518.
opment and Education, 56(4), 381–399. https://doi.org/10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2011.07.012.
1080/10349120903306715. Tomeny, T. S., Ellis, B. M., Rankin, J. A., & Barry, T. D. (2017).
Rivers, J. W., & Stoneman, Z. (2003). Sibling relationships when a Sibling relationships quality and psychosocial outcomes among
child has autism: marital stress and support coping. Journal of adult siblings of individuals with autism spectrum disorder and
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(4), 383–394. https:// individuals with intellectual disability without autism. Research
doi.org/10.1023/A:1025006727395. in Developmental Disabilities, 62, 104–114. https://doi.org/10.
Rowley, E., Chandler, S., Baird, G., Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Loucas, 1016/j.ridd.2017.01.008.
T., & Charman, T. (2012). The experience of friendship, victi- Tozer, A., Atkin, K., & Wenham, A. (2013). Continuity, commitment
mization and bullying in children with an autism spectrum dis- and context: adult siblings of people with autism plus learning
order: associations with child characteristics and school disability. Health & Social Care in the Community, 21(5),
placement. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6(3), 480–488. https://doi.org/10.1111/hsc.12034.
1126–1134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2012.03.004. Walton, K. M., & Ingersoll, B. R. (2015). Psychosocial adjustment and
Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and change in the social sibling relationships in siblings of children with autism spectrum
competence of children with autism, Downs syndrome, and disorder: risk and protective factors. Journal of Autism and
developmental delays. Monographs of the Society for Research in Developmental Disorders, 45(9), 2764–2778. https://doi.org/10.
Child Development, 64, 1–108 (1, Serial No. 256). 1007/s10803-015-2440-7.
Journal of Child & Family Studies is a copyright of Springer, 2020. All Rights Reserved.

You might also like