Frison 2017
Frison 2017
Abstract
Although studies have shown that Instagram use and young adults’ mental health are cross-sectionally asso-
ciated, longitudinal evidence is lacking. In addition, no study thus far examined this association, or the reverse,
among adolescents. To address these gaps, we set up a longitudinal panel study among 12- to 19-year-old
Flemish adolescents to investigate the reciprocal relationships between different types of Instagram use and
depressed mood. Self-report data from 671 adolescent Instagram users (61% girls; MAge = 14.96; SD = 1.29)
were used to examine our research question and test our hypotheses. Structural equation modeling showed that
Instagram browsing at Time 1 was related to increases in adolescents’ depressed mood at Time 2. In addition,
adolescents’ depressed mood at Time 1 was related to increases in Instagram posting at Time 2. These
relationships were similar among boys and girls. Potential explanations for the study findings and suggestions
for future research are discussed.
Leuven School for Mass Communication Research, Faculty of Social Sciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium.
603
604 FRISON AND EGGERMONT
important to examine the impact of Instagram liking for at we hypothesize that Instagram browsing (* passive con-
least two reasons. First, Instagram users can easily express a sumption) at Time 1 will relate to increases in adolescents’
positive attitude toward any content by simply clicking the depressed mood at Time 2 (H1), whereas Instagram posting
Like button. As a result, liking is a popular SNS activity.14 In (* broadcasting) and liking (* directed communication) at
addition, Lee et al.15 recently revealed that participants who Time 1 will relate to decreases in adolescents’ depressed
more frequently use the Like button reported greater bonding mood at Time 2 (H2).
social capital, suggesting that liking may be beneficial for
users’ mental health. Adolescents’ depressed mood and different types
To sum up, the present study aims to enhance our under- of Instagram use
standing of the reciprocal relationships between different
types of Instagram use (i.e., browsing, posting, and liking) Various studies have already investigated loneliness and
Downloaded by Gothenburg University Library from online.liebertpub.com at 10/18/17. For personal use only.
peers, they were instructed to place their questionnaire in a complete the survey at both time points, (1) to increase sta-
closed envelope after completing. Strict confidentiality of the tistical power37 and (2) to reduce the risk of problems en-
answers was assured. The institutional review board of the countered in SEM when sample sizes are small (e.g.,
host university approved the study procedures. nonconvergent solutions, incorrect standard errors, and so
on).38–40 The full information maximum likelihood proce-
Participants dure was used to estimate missing data. The chi-squared to
degrees of freedom ratio (v2/df), the root-mean-squared error
In total, 1,840 participants filled out the questionnaire at of approximation (RMSEA), and the comparative fit index
Time 1; 1,577 filled out the questionnaire at Time 2; and (CFI) were used to determine the goodness-of-fit of the
1,235 filled out the questionnaire at both time points (i.e., models.41
67% of total). More importantly, a total of 671 Instagram To investigate the reciprocal relationships between dif-
Downloaded by Gothenburg University Library from online.liebertpub.com at 10/18/17. For personal use only.
members filled out the questionnaire at Time 1 and 622 at ferent types of Instagram use and adolescents’ depressed
Time 2; 440 Instagram members filled out the questionnaire mood, we computed an autoregressive cross-lagged panel
at both time points (i.e., 66% of total). The mean age of those model.
participants with an Instagram account at Time 1 was 14.96 All variables at Time 2 were predicted by their preceding
(SD = 1.29) and 39% of this sample were boys. As this study values at Time 1 and by the value of the respective inde-
aims to investigate the impact of Instagram use, we only pendent variables at Time 1. We further allowed covariances
included those participants who had an Instagram account at between our control variables and the study variables at Time
Time 1 or Time 2 in our analyses. 1 and by estimating paths from these control variables to
Differences were explored between those who filled out each of the study variables at Time 2. In addition, we mod-
one questionnaire and those who filled out both question- eled covariances between study variables measured at the
naires with regard to all relevant Time 1 variables. Using same time point and allowed covariances between the error
Pillai’s trace, a multivariate analysis of variance showed terms of the same indicators.42
significant differences (V = 0.02, F(4, 628) = 2.57, p < 0.05, To test whether the relationships differed between boys
hp2 = 0.02). Followup univariate analyses revealed that ado- and girls, we performed a multiple group comparison test.
lescents who participated in both waves scored lower on This test compared a model in which all structural paths were
Instagram posting Time 1 (M = 3.65; SD = 0.07 vs. M = 3.92; allowed to vary across groups with a model in which all
SD = 0.10, F(1, 653) = 4.58, p < .05) and depressed mood structural paths were fixed to be equal across groups. If D v2
Time 1 (M = 1.80; SD = 0.04 vs. M = 1.94; SD = 0.05, F(1, was significant ( p < 0.05), we conducted a path-by-path
650) = 3.30, p = 0.01). analysis to examine whether the hypothesized relationships
differed significantly between the two groups.
Measures
Control variables. Participants responded to questions Results
about gender and age.
Preliminary analyses
Different types of Instagram use. Using a seven-point The descriptive statistics for and zero-order correlations
Likert scale (1 = never to 7 = several times per day), partici- between all relevant variables is presented in Table 1.
pants were asked (1) ‘‘How often do you look at photos
posted by other Instagram users?’’ (i.e., browsing), (2) ‘‘How Cross-lagged model
often do you post a photo on Instagram?’’ (i.e., posting), and
(3) ‘‘How often do you ‘like’ a photo on Instagram?’’ (i.e., The cross-lagged model, presented in Figure 1, showed a
liking). good fit of the data and yielded a chi-square value of 391.87
with 131 degrees of freedom, p < 0.001, RMSEA = 0.06;
Depressed mood. To assess participants’ depressive CFI = 0.96; v2/df = 2.99. Results revealed that Instagram
symptoms, we used The Center for Epidemiological Studies browsing at Time 1 positively predicted adolescents’ de-
Depression Scale for Children (CES-DC). Using a four-point pressed mood at Time 2, b = 0.16, B = 0.04, SE = 0.02,
Likert scale (1 = not at all to 4 = a lot), participants rated the p < 0.05. Instagram posting and liking at Time 1, however,
frequency of 20 statements for the past 2 weeks. Olsson and were not related to depressed mood at Time 2. Thus, results
von Knorring investigated the psychometric properties of support H1, but could not confirm H2.
this measure in a Swedish adolescent sample.36 They found In addition, our research question examined whether ad-
support for one strong underlying factor, that is, depressed olescents’ depressed mood was related to Instagram brows-
mood (e.g., ‘‘During the past week, I wasn’t able to feel ing, posting, or liking. Results showed that depressed mood
happy, even when my family or friends tried to help me feel at Time 1 positively predicted Instagram posting at Time 2,
better’’) (aTime1 = 0.87; aTime 2 = 0.88). Based on the average b = 0.08, B = 0.10, SE = 0.05, p = 0.05. Depressed mood at
of these six items, an estimate of adolescents’ depressed Time 1, however, did not predict Instagram browsing and
mood was created. liking at Time 2.
Furthermore, the predictors in the model collectively ex-
plained 28% of the variance in adolescents’ depressed mood
Data analysis
(R2 = 0.28), 35% of the variance in Instagram posting
The hypothesized relationships were tested using struc- (R2 = 0.35), 20% of the variance in Instagram browsing
tural equation modeling (SEM) in AMOS (SPSS, IBM). We (R2 = 0.20), and 24% of the variance in Instagram liking
decided to include all participants, also those that did not (R2 = 0.24).
606 FRISON AND EGGERMONT
stimulate negative comparison behaviors, which in turn may depressed mood were similar for boys and girls. This lack of
increase young people’s depressed mood. Future research is moderation contrasts previous cross-sectional studies that
thus needed (1) to test the moderating role of strangers fol- found support for a moderating role of gender in the asso-
lowed through Instagram and (2) to examine the mediating ciation between specific types of Facebook use and young
role of negative comparison behaviors on Instagram in the people’s depressive symptoms.44,45 This lack of gender dif-
relationship between Instagram browsing and adolescents’ ference, however, may be partly due to the fact that female
depressed mood. Instagram users (61%) were overrepresented in our sample.
Contrary to our expectations (i.e., H2), which were based Perhaps gender differences did not emerge because there
on previous Facebook research,6 no support was found for an were not enough boys to test for them. Nonetheless, this
association between Instagram posting/liking and adoles- study is the first to examine gender differences in the re-
cents’ depressed mood. This lack of significant relationships ciprocal associations between Instagram use and adoles-
Downloaded by Gothenburg University Library from online.liebertpub.com at 10/18/17. For personal use only.
may be because Instagram is much more image driven than cents’ depressed mood. More research is thus needed to
Facebook. More specifically, because Instagram’s prime further test the role of gender in these relationships.
focus is on sharing images and videos, Instagram posting and
liking exclusively refers to posting and liking of images and Limitations
videos. Facebook posting and liking, however, can refer to
images and videos, but also status updates. Instagram may Although the present study is the first to explore important
therefore provide users less possibilities to express their reciprocal associations between different types of Instagram
emotions and seek social support and thus to decrease their use and adolescents’ depressed mood, it is not without lim-
depressed mood, than Facebook, which may potentially ex- itations. A first limitation refers to the fact that we relied on
plain why Instagram liking and posting are not capable to single-item measures to assess different types of Instagram
decrease adolescents’ depressed mood. use. Although we believe these single-item measures are
more meaningful than general measures such as time spent
Adolescents’ depressed mood and different types on Instagram, we recommend that future research should rely
of Instagram use on multi-item measures when assessing specific types of
Instagram use. Second, this study is limited by the important
Regarding the opposite relationships, our results revealed dropout of participants between Times 1 and 2. Although this
that depressed mood is related to increases in Instagram post- dropout limits the study findings, our current associations
ing. This finding is in line with prior studies that revealed between different types of Instagram use and adolescents’
positive associations between depressive symptoms and Fa- depressed mood would likely be even stronger when attrition
cebook status updating,30 but extend prior research by showing would be absent because the participants who dropped out
that depressed mood may also be predictive of broadcasting posted more on Instagram and felt more depressed. To pro-
Instagram content. In addition, this finding is also in line with vide a more correct understanding of the actual strengths of
Mood Management Theory,8,9 which argues that media are these associations, future research should try to minimize
purposively used for coping with moods. However, by re- dropout of participants.
vealing that a specific type of media use (i.e., Instagram post-
ing), instead of the choice of a certain medium, may be used to
Conclusion
manage moods, our study extends Mood Management Theory.
Based on these insights, we therefore recommend that future Despite these limitations, the present study provides in-
research that aims to test this theory should not only address the sight into the reciprocal relationships between different types
choice of media to manage moods but also how media are used of Instagram use (i.e., browsing, posting, and liking) and
for reducing negative and enhancing positive moods. adolescents’ depressed mood. Overall, we can conclude (1)
Furthermore, adolescents with greater depressed mood may that adolescents have a higher chance to develop a greater
be more likely to post images or videos on Instagram, perhaps, depressed mood when they browse more often through In-
to enhance their image in the eyes of other social network stagram and (2) that adolescents have a higher chance to post
members. Instagram is a visually centered platform that allows more on Instagram when they have higher levels of de-
their users to easily edit and filter their photos and videos; it is pressed mood.
thus an ideal medium to present oneself in their best possible These results have important implications that warrant
way, even a false way. This strategic self-presentation on In- consideration. More specifically, adolescents, but also par-
stagram may especially attract adolescents with high levels of ents and clinicians should be made aware of the harmful
depressed mood, because it not only allows them to enhance impact of passive consumption of SNS content, as looking at
their image in the eye of others but also to find encouragement photo’s on Instagram (i.e., Instagram browsing) may in-
(e.g., through likes and positive comments). In line with this crease teens’ depressed mood 6 months later. Future pre-
suggestion, Mehdizadeh found that college students lower in vention and intervention programs aimed at reducing passive
self-esteem engaged more often in self-promotional behaviors consumption on SNSs are therefore particularly needed.
on Facebook.43 Future research is however needed to further Thus, our findings not only offer important insights for teens
enhance our understanding of how depressed mood relates to and practitioners but also useful guidance for future pre-
increases in adolescents’ posting behavior on Instagram. vention and intervention programs.
Author Disclosure Statement 18. Stapleton P, Luiz G, Chatwin H. Generation validation: the
role of social comparison in use of Instagram among
No competing financial interests exist.
emerging adults. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social
Networking 2017;20:142–149.
References
19. Li X, Chen W, Popiel P. What happens on Facebook stays
1. Lenhart A. (2015) Teens, social media, and technology on Facebook? The implications for perceived, receiving,
overview 2015. Washington, DC: Pew Internet & American and giving social support. Computers in Human Behavior
Life Project. 2015;51:106–113.
2. Anderson BYM. (2015) Technology device ownership: 20. Vitak J. (2012) Keeping connected in the Facebook age.
2015. Washington, DC: Pew Internet & American Life Doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University. https://
Project. vitak.files.wordpress.com/2009/02/vitak-dissertation-2012.
Downloaded by Gothenburg University Library from online.liebertpub.com at 10/18/17. For personal use only.
3. Van Waeg S, Van Hoecke L, Demeulenaere A, et al. (2016) pdf (accessed July 25, 2017.)
Onderzoeksrapport Apenstaartjaren 5. Gent, Belgium: 21. Aubrey JS, Rill L. Investigating relations between Face-
Mediaraven & Linc. book use and social capital among college undergraduates.
4. Steinberg L. (2005) Adolescence. New York: McGraw-Hill. Communication Quarterly 2013;61:479–496.
5. Skrove M, Romundstad P, Indredavik MS. Resilience, 22. Fardouly J, Vartanian LR. Negative comparisons about
lifestyle and symptoms of anxiety and depression in ado- one’s appearance mediate the relationship between Face-
lescence: the Young-HUNT study. Social Psychiatry and book usage and body image concerns. Body Image 2015;
Psychiatric Epidemiology 2013;48:407–416. 12:82–88.
6. Frison E, Eggermont S. Toward an integrated and differential 23. Frison E, Eggermont S. ‘‘Harder, Better, Faster, Stronger’’:
approach to the relationships between loneliness, different types negative comparison on Facebook and adolescents’ life
of Facebook use and adolescents’ depressed mood. Commu- satisfaction are reciprocally related. Cyberpsychology,
nication Research 2015;1–28 [Epub ahead of print] DOI: Behavior, and Social Networking 2016;19:158–164.
10.1177/0093650215617506. 24. Brown Z, Tiggeman M. Attractive celebrity and peer im-
7. Lup K, Trub L, Rosenthal L. Instagram #Instasad?: explor- ages on Instagram: effect on women’s mood and body
ing associations among Instagram use, depressive symptoms, image. Body Image 2016;19:37–43.
negative social comparison, and strangers. Cyberpsychology, 25. Yang C. Instagram use, loneliness, and social comparison
Behavior, and Social Networking 2015;18:247–252. orientation: interact and browse on social media, but don’t
8. Zillmann D. Mood management through communication compare. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Net-
choices. American Behavioral Scientist 1988;31:327–340. working 2016;19:703–708.
9. Zillmann D. (2000) Mood management in the context of se- 26. Clayton RB, Osborne RE, Miller BK, et al. Loneliness,
lective exposure theory. In Roloff ME, ed. Communication anxiousness, and substance use as predictors of Facebook
yearbook 23. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, pp. 103–123. use. Computers in Human Behavior 2013;29:687–693.
10. Tsitsika AK, Tzavela EC, Janikian M, et al. Online social 27. Lou LL, Yan Z. An examination of the reciprocal re-
networking in adolescence: patterns of use in six European laionship of loneliness and Facebook use among first-year
countries and links with psychosocial functioning. Journal college students. Journal of Educational Computing Re-
of Adolescent Health 2014;55:141–147. search 2012;46:105–117.
11. Burke M, Kraut R, Marlow C. (2011) Social capital on 28. Ryan T, Xenos S. Who uses Facebook? An investigation
Facebook: differentiating uses and users. In Proceedings of into the relationship between the Big Five, shyness, nar-
the 2011 ACM Conference on Human Factors in Comput- cissism, loneliness, and Facebook usage. Computers in
ing Systems. Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada: Asso- Human Behavior 2011;27:1658–1664.
ciation for Computing Machinery (ACM) SIGCHI, 29. Sheldon KM, Abad N, Hinsch C. A two-process view
pp. 571–580. of Facebook use and relatedness need-satisfaction:
12. Verduyn P, Lee DS, Park J, et al. Passive Facebook usage disconnection drives use, and connection rewards it.
undermines affective well-being: experimental and longi- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 2011;100:
tudinal evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology 766–775.
2015;144:480–488. 30. Scherr S, Brunet, A. Differential influences of depression
13. Deters FG, Mehl MR. Does posting Facebook status up- and personality traits on the use of Facebook. Social Media
dates increase or decrease loneliness? An online social + Society 2017;3:1–14.
networking experiment. Social Psychological and Person- 31. Knobloch S, Zillmann D. Mood management via the digi-
ality Science 2013;4:1–8. tal jukebox. Journal of Communication 2002;52:351–366.
14. Hampton KN, Goulet LS, Rainie L, et al. (2011) Social 32. Knobloch S. Mood adjustment via mass communication.
networking sites and our lives. Washington, DC: Pew In- Journal of Communication, 2003;53:233–250.
ternet & American Life Project. 33. Dillman Carpentier FR, Brown JD, Bertocci M, et al. Sad
15. Lee E, Kim YJ, Ahn J. How do people use Facebook fea- kids, sad media? Applying mood management theory to
tures to manage social capital? Computers in Human Be- depressed adolescents’ use of media. Media Psychology
havior 2014;36:440–445. 2008;11:143–166.
16. Rae JR, Lonborg SD. Do motivations for using Facebook 34. Johnson BK, Knobloch-Westerwick S. Glancing up or
moderate the association between Facebook use and psy- down: mood management and selective comparison on
chological well-being? Frontiers in Psychology 2015;6:1–9. social networking sites. Computers in Human Behavior
17. Steers MLN, Wickham RE, Acitelli LK. Seeing everyone 2014;41:33–39.
else’s highlight reels: how Facebook usage is linked to 35. Costello EJ, Copeland W, Angold A. Trends in psychopa-
depressive symptoms. Journal of Social and Clinical Psy- thology across the adolescent years: what changes when
chology 2014;33:701–731. children become adolescents, and when adolescents be-
INSTAGRAM USE AND ADOLESCENTS’ DEPRESSED MOOD 609
come adults? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry equation modeling. Journal of Abnormal Psychology
2011;52:1015–1025. 2003;112:558–577.
36. Olsson G, von Knorring A-L. Depression among Swedish 43. Mehdizadeh S. Self-presentation 2.0: narcissism and self-
adolescents measured by the self-rating scale Center for esteem on Facebook. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and
Epidemiology Studies-Depression Child (CES-DC). Euro- Social Networking 2010;13:357–364.
pean Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 1997;6:81–87. 44. Frison E, Eggermont S. Exploring the relationships be-
37. Raaijmakers QA. Effectiveness of different missing data tween different types of Facebook use, perceived online
treatments in surveys with Likert-type data: introducing the social support, and adolescents’ depressed mood. Social
relative mean substitution approach. Educational and Psy- Science Computer Review 2015;45:1–19.
chological Measurement 1999;59:725–748. 45. Simoncic TE, Kuhlman KR, Vargas I, et al. Facebook use
38. Anderson JC, Gerbing DW. The effect of sampling error on and depressive symptomatology: investigating the role of
Downloaded by Gothenburg University Library from online.liebertpub.com at 10/18/17. For personal use only.
convergence, improper solutions, and goodness-of-fit indi- neuroticism and extraversion in youth. Computers in Hu-
ces for maximum likelihood confirmatory factor analysis. man Behaviour 2014;40:1–5.
Psychometrika 1984;49:155–173.
39. Marsh HW, Balla JR. Goodness-of-fit indices in confir-
matory factor analysis: the effect of sample size and model Address correspondence to:
complexity. Quality and Quantity 1994;28:185–217. Dr. Eline Frison
40. Marsh HW, Balla JR, McDonald RP. Goodness-of-fit in- Leuven School for Mass Communication Research
dexes in confirmatory factor analysis: the effect of sample Faculty of Social Sciences
size. Psychological Bulletin 1988;103:391–410. KU Leuven
41. Byrne BM. (2010) Structural Equation Modeling with Parkstraat 45 (PO box 3603)
AMOS: Basic Concepts, Applications and Programming. Leuven B-3000
Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum. Belgium
42. Cole DA, Maxwell SE. Testing mediational models with
longitudinal data: questions and tips in the use of structural E-mail: [email protected]