Chapter 6 - Urban Psychology of Smart Sustainable Cities - SVD Ham
Chapter 6 - Urban Psychology of Smart Sustainable Cities - SVD Ham
Chapter 6 - Urban Psychology of Smart Sustainable Cities - SVD Ham
Urban Psychology
of Smart
Sustainable Cities
Learning outcomes
• Describe the contribution of urban psychology to the creation of
pleasant, human-scaled urban environments for living, working and
recreating.
• Apply a variety of tools, such as observations, mapping, self-reporting
and Virtual Reality to investigate human behaviour in urban settings.
• Analyse the urban environment by applying the conceptual model of
six basic human needs and use this for designing interventions in the
public space.
74
6
6.1 Introduction
Up until 2005, urban psychology was not mentioned separately in the field
of psychology. With cities growing denser and bigger, more and more
psychologists started paying specific attention to the urban environment.
Urban living influences all aspects of life, including health, employment and
income, social status and social interaction, and personal development.
Size, density, and configuration of the urban physical environment affect
the psychological and social experiences and behaviours of people living
in urban areas (APA, 2005). Yet, that only scratches the surface. How do we
deal with diversity, poverty, inequality, crime, violence, and gentrification?
In addition, on a much smaller scale, how do buildings, street design, and
even façade gardens affect our sense of privacy, possibilities of social inter-
action, and our sense of home?
In recent decades mental well-being has become an important part
of city-making. It has led to the desire to build cities where people feel
comfortable and where people’s needs are met. In order to build these
human-scaled cities, planners, architects, designers, policymakers, and
others involved in city-making, require a better understanding of the basic
conditions needed for people to live happily and feel at home in their urban
environments. It also requires an understanding of environmental aspects,
such as personal space, territoriality, privacy, and crowding (Gifford, 1997).
Urban psychology provides the tools and methods to create this under-
standing of people’s needs. This chapter describes the contribution of
urban psychology to creating pleasant, human-scaled urban environments
for living, working and visiting. A conceptual model is presented, based on
six basic human needs, that functions as a framework to understand any
urban environment from a human perspective.
76
between urbanization on the one hand and mental health and social
6
deviancy on the other (Marsella, 1998).
The tide is turning, and social scientists argue that urban residents (even
those of less economic means) are survivors and thrivers, instead of victims.
They cope with the urban environment and transcend the stressors. Life in
urban settings can even generate resilience and other positive outcomes
and urbanization does not automatically result in disorganization (Saegert
& Winkel, 1996). Living in cities can be just as fulfilling and rewarding as
living in rural areas. Residents in urban environments, for example strongly
identify with their neighourhoods. To many people it is even a source of
informal and formal social relationships. Even dense, urban neighbour-
hoods can have a high degree of social organization, such as community
groups, social clubs, and political organizations.
Of course, living in cities can be stressful and sometimes even dangerous,
but for the majority of people living, working and visiting cities is an exhila-
rating experience. So much so, that by 2018, 55% of the world’s population
have made cities their daily habitat. Besides dealing with the negative
effects of urban living, the question becomes more and more how the
positive effects can be increased. A new perspective is needed on living in
cities. Urban psychologist, alongside other social scientists, play a pivotal
role in creating this perspective.
An important part of this new perspective are the Social Development Goals
(SDG’s). SDG 11 (Sustainable cities and communities) specifically focuses on
creating urban environments that are socially inclusive, equal and resilient,
economically viable and sustainable. Other SDG’s apply as well, such as
SDG 3 (Good health and well-being), SDG 8 (Decent work and economic
growth) and SDG 10 (Reduced inequalities). Urban psychologist can con-
tribute to this agenda, by helping understand the environment-behaviour
relationship and creating psychologically pleasant and healthy urban
environments. In the next part, the urban psychology is more thoroughly
discussed.
78
their living environments, based on their ideas and desires. We mainly
6
achieved this through co-creation. The projects have produced a model
based on six psychological needs, which helps to observe and analyse
the environment-behaviour interdependency, but it also helps to ask the
right questions during interviews and shape co-creative processes. Below
follows a description of each need in the model.
80
6
The need for proximity
The third need is to create personal distances to others. The distance
depends on the social relation we have towards others (Hall, 1966). The
better you know someone, the shorter the distance. There are four distances
(or circles). The first distance is between 0-45 cm and is for intimate contacts
only. Think of a partner, family or very close friends. The second distance
is between 30-120 cm and is considered personal space that is only ac-
cessible to people we know well to moderately well. The third distance is
named social space and is between 120-350 cm. We keep this distance in
more formal contacts (such as business contacts and with people we do
not know, such as in urban environments). The fourth and final distance is
from 350 cm and further. This is the distance people keep at public events,
such as a speaker and the audience.
These distances are flexible, also because the environment sometimes
does not allow to keep more distance. Crowded shopping streets usually
do not allow to keep at least 120 cm of distance to other. This might lead to
a loss of control and therefore a stress reaction. As described in the need
for control, there are tactics to overcome this loss of control, for example
avoiding a busy street or cognitively accepting that the crowdedness is an
inherent part of a busy shopping street.
In urban environments the need for social distance is visible. As described in
the first need, people seek out the edges of public places to sit. In residential
areas people claim parts of the space in front of their homes, usually picking
places where they can sit with their backs to the wall. Research on the use
of the private sidewalk (stoop) also shows that people feel most satisfied
with a zone at least 120 cm deep, measured from the façade (Ulden et al.,
2015). This corresponds with the third distance, the social distance.
82
aware of the urban environment. Being more aware increases the chance
6
of social connection and affiliative behaviour.
In recent years the discussion about the modernist city design has been
more important than ever. The dominance of cars in public space has
become an issue in ever bigger and denser cities. Not only is the devotion
of large parts of public space to cars, not sustainable and smart, it is also
unhealthy. Other modes of transport (strong public transport infrastructure,
cycle paths and pedestrian walkways) are more sustainable towards the
urban future and are healthier to urbanites as well. Less space for cars
means an increased amount of space for pedestrians and cyclists. Besides,
it also makes urban spaces more interesting to be in.
This relates to the need for (environmental) distraction. Urban life can be
anonymous and this has consequences for affiliative behaviour. Research
shows that in urban areas we are less likely to help people we do not know
(Bell et al., 2001). This is especially true in cases where we feel rushed. In
an experiment, Darley & Batson (1973) showed that students that were told
they were late for a presentation, were much less likely to help a person
lying on the ground asking for help. In some cases, students stepped over
the person to proceed their way to the classroom. Interestingly, these
theology students had just prepared a short presentation about the good
Samaritan. Even being primed by the story of someone helping a person in
need, was not enough to suppress the feeling of being rushed.
Observations
Understanding a place usually starts with observations on both the char-
acteristics of the environment. This helps to identify possible challenges
and opportunities in the environment and gain knowledge of the way
people behave in that setting. Where are benches placed? How high are
the buildings? How is the ground floor (plinth) of the building designed?
How much space is there for walking, cycling and cars? How much
green is there? How is it placed? The next step in observation is people’s
behaviour. Where do people walk, sit, talk, sleep or stand? What activities
are happening? How many people walk in certain places? How many
cars and bikes pass by? All these behaviours can be written down on a
coding form, in order to standardize the observations as much as possible.
It is important to determine, beforehand, which observations should be
84
measured. Alongside, it helps to visualize the observations by drawing the
6
situation and marking the behaviour on the map.
This mapping has been done numerous times, also by architects and urban
planners. Architect Jan Gehl, for example, drew maps of streets he observed
to visualize how people used the sidewalk (Gehl & Svarre, 2013). Donald
Appleyard made elaborate observations on the social interaction on
streets, trying to determine the influence of car traffic on the amount, type
and location of social interaction between people living in the streets. He
concluded that the busier the traffic, the less social interaction and meeting
points were observed in streets (Appleyard, 1981). Observations can also be
done in different ways. Urban planner William Whyte recorded time-lapse
videos of public streets and squares to understand how design of public
spaces can influence people’s behaviour. This study led to the re-evalu-
ation of public space and its design worldwide. More recent methods of
observational mapping are through GPS-trackers and even through Wi-
Fi-points or Google Maps Timeline. These can provide great insights into
people’s behaviours and choices in public space. Recently it has become
more challenging to gather such data due to privacy regulations.
Self-report measurement
Observations are a useful method to start understanding the environ-
ment-behaviour relationship. Self-report measurements are a logical
addition to better understand decision making, cognitive processes,
attitudes, and moods. Self-report can be done through questionnaires
or interviews and are a great combination with on-site observations.
Self-report can also be done outside of the urban setting. A special form of
self-report, for example, is Kevin Lynch’s method of cognitive mapping. The
method is based on his research on people’s perception of cities and how
they mentally map information (1960). This technique is a visual form of
self-report, because it heavily relies on people’s ability to remember infor-
mation and situations and could be coloured by biases. Nevertheless, these
techniques are useful to gain a better understanding of specific spaces and
people’s perception of it in relation to design.
Psychogeography
Urban psychologists have been using new technologies as well to measure
people’s responses to urban environments. Urban psychologist Collin Ellard
(2015) also has been interested in cognitive mapping, asking people to
walk around specific urban areas with their phones and wearing tools to
measure physical responses, such as heart rate. Ellard calls this psychoge-
ography. At specific locations he asked people to fill in questions about their
experiences and make pictures of what they were looking at. This provided
lots of data, sometimes showing that people say they experience one thing,
while the physical measurements tell something different (Battersby, 2015).
86
buildings, and enjoy the sense of community in the many community
6
homes, at their children’s schools and in self-organized clubs and networks.
Over time all this changed. Slowly at first, but in the past two decades these
neighbourhoods have taken a turn for the worst. Homes were noisy and
poorly insulated and became less sought after. Nowadays, most of the
homes are amongst the cheapest social housing in the city, attracting a wide
variety of people with low income, (mental) health issues, poor education,
and from a diversity of cultural backgrounds. Many people only speak
their native language, causing a gap between the original residents in the
neighbourhood and the many newcomers. On top of that, in recent years,
many small, family-owned and local neighbourhood stores have disap-
peared and have been replaced by either corporate stores (supermarkets)
or family-owned stores from different cultures (many Turkish bakeries and
supermarkets). These changes reflected the socio-demographic transition
in the neighbourhood.
At the same time, this transition also reflected the changes in the social
fabric. New networks flourished, which also created a large group of dissat-
isfied and worried current residents, who saw their trusted social networks
disappear. Together with the dwindling social capital in the neighbourhood,
criminality rose. This varied from burglaries and break-ins to incidents on
the streets involving knives and guns. With the reputation of the neighbour-
hoods worsening, the social housing corporation Havensteder wanted
to figure out what was needed to improve the social and psychological
attachment to home and neighbourhood. They received many complaints
from residents (mostly noise complaints), saw an increase of people only
living there for a short period of time, and received signals that people
were becoming more and more detached from their living environment.
The process
The case study consisted of two parts: on the one hand working with
residents to come up with a plan to improve living conditions, according
to the model of the six needs, and on the other hand, to perform research
to understand the impact of the actions taken and the chosen process. As
a team we chose to split the project into two phases: an analysis phase
(including the set-up of a baseline for the research) and a co-creation
phase.
For the analysis phase we took two steps. Since there were more apartment
buildings than we could involve in the research, we first had to make a
selection. For this we gathered information to select the apartment buildings
where we would perform the research. We spatially assessed the spaces
in and around the apartment buildings and the shared entrances. We
created a booklet to systemically observe the entrances. For each space
(public space, building, and shared entrance) we identified aspects to
survey and observe, such as the cleanliness, the quality of the lighting, the
maintenance, broken windows, doors, or mailboxes. We also focused on
the ownership issues, such as personal items placed in the shared spaces
and signs of ownership (and control) on the streets. The survey led to spider
graphs of each building and its surroundings, allowing us to assess the
places most in need of improvement. Based on the outcomes, we selected
one apartment building where we actively worked with residents.
On top of that we wanted to gather data to better understand the compo-
sition of the neighbourhood, the strength of the social capital and peoples’
attitudes towards the neighbourhood. In practice it turned out to this data
was not available at the apartment building level. The housing corpora-
88
tion hardly had any usable data. The municipality did have data, but only
6
on district (and sometimes) neighbourhood level. This data showed, for
example, a decrease in social capital over the past years, an increase in
feelings of unsafety and criminality in the neighbourhood, and a lack of trust
between people. To translate these data to the apartment building level,
we interviewed professionals working with residents in these building. This
gave good insights and allowed us to choose the apartment buildings that
were fit for the research. In this case we choose those apartment buildings
that, according to the observations and in the opinion of the interviewees,
did have structural issues, but could be improved by working with residents.
Apartment buildings with bigger social challenges, such as criminal activi-
ties, were not selected for two reasons. First, because it would become hard
or even impossible to interpret the research data. Second, because we did
not believe such challenges can be alleviated by social projects such as this
case study. Other types of interventions, such as an eviction of a terrorizing
neighbour or the closing of a drug home, were to be executed first to regain
a basic level of living quality.
After selecting the 20 apartment buildings for this case study, the second
step was to interview participants and ask them to fill in a questionnaire.
We constructed a semi-structured interview based on the six needs model.
The interview revolved around the concept of feeling at home, because
we wanted to better understand residents’ emotional relationship with
their living environment. Therefore, the interview started with each resi-
dent’s personal story of why they came to live in the neighbourhood and
their experiences in the neighbourhood. From there on we assessed the
extent to which residents felt the six types of needs were met in and around
their apartment building. For example, we extensively asked about their
need for social interaction, who they met on a daily basis, where they met
people and if all this satisfied their need for belonging. We did the same
for the other needs, such as control, where we asked about their expe-
rience of privacy and the need to claim parts of the shared entrance for
personal expression. In total, we interviewed 25 of the 32 residents living in
the apartment building. The response to the questionnaire was 25 in the
experimental group and 36 in the control group. The control group only
received a questionnaire and no interview.
The results from the analysis phase were used in the co-creation process.
Quickly it became clear that, to improve sense of place and feelings of
home, the shared entrance had to be improved drastically. This matched
with the results of the spatial survey that most attention should be paid to the
shared entrance spaces. They were not well maintained, socially uninviting
and did not allow for any personal ownership. As a result, we designed a
process that revolved around the residents’ wishes, consisting of four steps:
(1) a design workshop in the shared entrances, (2) discussing the first design
draft of the shared entrance (3) implementing the design with residents in
their shared entrance space, and (4) celebrating the design intervention.
The results
The co-creation process delivered a very visible outcome: a completely
renovated shared entrance. Working with residents put considerable
pressure on the housing corporation to fix this shared space. The housing
corporation managed to push the renovation of the entrances two years
forward. Residents decided (within certain financial boundaries) in which
colour the entrance would be painted and which materials would be used
(i.e. for the floor). Also, the mailboxes were renewed.
On top of these basic renovations, we asked residents to consider if and
how they wanted to take ownership of the shared space, how they wanted
to socially interact with their neighbours and how the shared space could
contribute to their sense of home. In the end, residents chose to improve
the shared space by adding plants and to add stickers with residents’
favourite quotes about feeling at home, see Figure 6.2. Because the texts
were placed on the wall between two homes, they were designed in such a
way that residents could collaborate with their neighbours to come up with
a text they were both content with.
Analyzing the research results, comparing the after measurement with
the baseline measurement, we noticed big changes. In general, by ren-
ovating the shared entrance, the connection between the homes and the
neighbourhood had been restored. Before, people only used the shared
entrance functionally, and therefore did not feel at home there. It was not a
social space or a place to form any emotional attachment with. By deciding
on the physical appearance of the shared entrance, and by organizing
the living room in the shared space and setting up social interaction, a
new emotional bond had formed. This bond became the foundation to feel
more at home in the shared space and in the neighbourhood. The results
show that in the experimental group residents feel considerably more
at home in the shared space afterwards than the control group did (see
Figure 6.3). Restoring this feeling also led to a different attitude towards the
neighbourhood. This shows in the results that residents in the experimental
group are less inclined to move than the control group (see Figure 6.4).
A relationship between shared entrance spaces and the neighbourhood
seems to exist on a psychological and social level.
Looking at the six needs, the following conclusions can be drawn. Concern-
ing the first need, that of overview, the results clearly showed that working
on the shared entrance improved the sense of safety (see Figure 6.5). During
the interviews and in the questionnaires, people mentioned feeling unsafe
beforehand. This was due to the lighting condition and the fact that groups
90
Figure 6.3: Feeling at home in Figure 6.4: Desire to relocate.
6
the shared space.
of local youth sometimes held themselves up at the front door of the shared
entrance. This invoked fear by many residents, even though nothing had
ever happened. Residents experienced a lack of overview (lighting) and a
lack of refuge (could not get inside without fear). By improving the lighting
inside the shared entrance and outside at the door, this improved. Also, by
redesigning the mailboxes the entrance felt cleaner, which influenced the
experience of safety.
When it comes to the need for control, residents clearly mentioned at the
start of the project they felt no control at all. Some residents wanted to
take control, by placing personal items in front of their doors (doormat,
flowers, etc.). Yet, they were convinced these items would be stolen. Six
months after the project, there were clear signs of residents taking control
and ownership. People placed personal items together with the plants in
the shared entrance and maintained plants together. One resident even
used the shared entrance to store clothes she collected to give out to poor
residents in the neighbourhood. Something she would never have done
before, since she was afraid things might get stolen.
On the need for proximity and belonging, we noticed a strong self-reported
effect on social interaction (see Figure 6.6). Residents in the experimental
group clearly state that they have built up social ties in the past year, more
so than the control group did. This does not necessarily mean that residents
Figure 6.6: I have established contacts Figure 6.7: Residents help each other if
with my neighbours. needed.
92
This case study shows the need for proper analysis to understand how
6
residents perceive and use space. Doing that helps to identify the spaces
that need improvement and have the most effect on people’s psychological
needs. In this case study it led to a greater sense of safety, more ownership,
a bigger sense of belonging and, most importantly, to a different emotional
attachment to the shared entrance. As said earlier, we form affective at-
tachments with a multitude of places. A shared entrance, such as this one,
is particularly important because residents use this space on a daily basis.
Exercise 2
Observing public behaviour
Observe peoples’ behaviour in a public place, such as a square, a park or a
busy shopping street. Use an observation form to collect your observations,
based on the psychological needs. After the analysis think of design inter-
ventions that could make the place feel more pleasant.
To create an observation form or map take the following steps. First, define
your research question. For example, the effect a well-designed ground
floor on the behaviour of people passing-by. Second, define the specific
behaviour you wish to observe (walking speed, number of times people stop,
number of times people interact, the number of people listening, number
of people donating money, etc.). Put these in a matrix, with the behaviour
observations in the columns and for each participant a new row. Third, add
things such as day of the week, time of day, weather conditions, etc. to the
matrix. Fourth, define your measuring tools. Number of people could be
counted, while walking speed can be measured by stopwatch. Additionally,
you can observe situations with multiple observers, each having a separate
task. One observer could be gathering quantitative data, while the other
could be gathering qualitative data. The fifth, and final step, is to put the
data together and make an analysis.
Exercise 3
Mapping social behaviour: belonging in an urban context
Urban environments are said to be anonymous. Yet, many people start
friendships in cities and become part of social networks. Observe in public
space how social groups use space, and how individuals engage in social
contact. Follow the same steps as in Exercise 2, but now use a map instead
of a form. If possible, also interview people about your observations to
determine how if they recognize those and how they might affect their
sense of belonging in public space.
94
6
Exercise 4
Emotional attachment to urban space
Interview people about their emotional attachment to urban spaces. Start
with their behaviour and thoughts in certain spaces (i.e. the street they live
in compared to a square they often visit), and try to identify the anchors
in space that people have emotional associations with. Anchors can be a
variety of things, such as a tree or a building that brings up memories.
6.8 Assignment
Analyse, compare and define interventions for public spaces
The assignment is to observe and analyse two urban environments (a
public place and a residential place), and to compare the differences.
• Define two places. This could be a public place such as a square, a
park or a shopping street, or a residential place such as a street, a cul-
de-sac or a residential square.
• Form two groups and assign each group to one of the places.
• Create an observation form based on the six needs model. Observe
for both places where people sit, for how long, how people walk, how
often they socialize and where, how often they turn their heads, what
activities they employ, etc. Use at least two different techniques, such
as mapping, counting, performing street-interviews, etc.
• Each group visits their site for at least one day, during different times,
to make observations and to interview people. If there is time to gather
more data, visit the site multiple times during a longer period.
• Visualize the observations for each place on a map and draw conclu-
sions about peoples’ behaviour.
• Compare the findings with the findings of the other group. Determine
the similarities and differences, and discuss those.
• Collectively think of design interventions that could improve the place.
References
APA (2015). Towards an urban psychology: Research, Action, and Policy.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Appleyard, D. (1981). Livable Streets. Berkeley, CA: University of California
Press.
Barker, R.G. (1964). Big school, small school: High school size and student
behavior. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
96
Lynch, K. A. (1960). The Image of the City. Cambridge: MIT Press.
6
Marsella, A.J. (1998). Urbanization, mental health and social deviancy: A
review of issues and research. American Psychologist, 53, pp. 621-623.
Mineka, S., R.W. Hendersen (1985). Controllability and predictability in
acquired motivation. Annual Review of Psychology, 36, pp. 495-529.
Morgan, P. (2010). Towards a developmental theory of place attachment,
Journal of Environmental Psychology, 30(1), pp. 11-22.
Reed, L. (1989). Is feeling ‘in control’ related to happiness in daily life? Psy-
chological Reports, 64 (3), pp. 775-784.
Rosnow, R., R. Rosenthal (1989). Statistical procedures and the justification
of knowledge in psychological science. American Psychologist, 44,
pp. 1276-1284.
Saegert, S., G. Winkel (1996). Paths to community empowerment: Organiz-
ing at home. American Journal of Community Psychology, 24(4), pp
517-550.
Saunders, D. (2010). De trek naar de stad. Amsterdam: De Bezige Bij.
Seligman, M. (1965). Learned Helplessness. Annual Review of Medicine, 23,
pp 407-412.
Tiger, L., M. McGuire (2010). God’s Brain. Amherst, NY: Prometheus Books.
Ulrich, R.S. (1979). Visual landscapes and psychological well-being.
Landscape research, 4, pp. 17-23.
United Nations (2018). https://www.un.org/development/desa/en/news/
population/2018-revision-of-world-urbanization-prospects.html.
Van Ulden, E., D. Heussen, S. Van der Ham (2015). De Stoep: Ontmoetingen
tussen huis en straat. Rotterdam: NAi Booksellers.
West, G. (2017). Scale: The universal laws of life, growth, and death in
organisms, cities and companies. New York, NY: Penguin Book.
Whyte, W. H. (2007). Introduction, ‘The life of plazas’, ‘Sitting space’, and
‘Sun, wind, trees, and water’. The urban design reader, 348-63.