Speed of Processing Effects On Spoken Idiom Comprehension: Cristina Cacciari (Cacciari - Cristina@unimore - It)
Speed of Processing Effects On Spoken Idiom Comprehension: Cristina Cacciari (Cacciari - Cristina@unimore - It)
372
word (Experiment 1). The irrelevant meaning of the target and others finding it already at idiom offset (Titone &
interfered less for skilled readers with high general Connine, 1994; Tabossi et al., in press).
knowledge than for both skilled and less skilled participants
with low general knowledge (Experiment 3). The present study
We investigated the comprehension of idioms with an early
Idiomatic expressions: how are they vs. late point of identification (early identification vs. late
represented and understood? identification idioms) with a cross-modal lexical decision
Idiomatic expressions are phrasal units whose meaning paradigm. An idiom-related visual word was presented
generally cannot be derived from the meaning of the before the idiom verb (control position) or at idiom offset
constituent parts. Few studies investigated the role of (Tabossi & Zardon, 1993). A facilitation for the target at
individual differences in idiomatic meaning comprehension offset with respect to the control position indicates whether
in language-unimpaired adults (e.g., Johnson, 1991; Nippold is the idiom meaning activated. Most of the evidence
& Martin, 1989; Nippold, Moran & Schwarz, 2001). These collected so far concerns non ambiguous idioms. This study
results showed that idiom understanding iss associated with instead used strings having both a literal and an idiomatic
measures of intelligence, academic achievement in reading interpretation (ambiguous idioms, e.g., “break the ice”)
and listening comprehension, and with general non-verbal embedded in contexts either biasing the literal or the
measures of mental capacity. None of these studies directly idiomatic meaning of the string. Ambiguous idioms are well
addressed the role of individual SOP in on-line spoken suited for testing alternative predictions on both idiomatic
idiom comprehension. meaning retrieval processes and sentential ambiguity effects.
Idioms are very widespread in language, but the processes Specifically, we aimed at testing: a. whether the dominant
underlying their on-line comprehension are still meaning of an idiom (in our case, the figurative one) is
controversial. For “Lexical look-up” models (Bobrow & activated irrespective from contextual information or only
Bell, 1973; Swinney & Cutler, 1979; Gibbs, 1980), idioms when contextually appropriate (as in Colombo, 1995, 1998),
are multiword lexical units accessed as such from the mental b. the interaction between meaning dominance and idiom
lexicon. Their meaning is apprehended by direct memory identification.
retrieval and not elaborated via linguistic processing. The As recently argued by Hannon and Daneman (2001), the
fast nature of idiomatic meaning retrieval, often faster than presence of individual differences obscured by group data
corresponding literal sentences, is explained postulating an can be one of the causes of inconsistent findings in the
“horse race” between a full compositional analysis of the literature. Therefore, this study tested the extent to which
string, that requires longer reaction times, and the faster individual SOP differences modulated ambiguous idioms
retrieval of its global figurative meaning. This class of comprehension in language unimpaired undergraduates.
models differs in terms of representational assumptions: for This factor might account for some of the incongruencies
Bobrow and Bell (1973), idioms are stored in a separate found in the literature insofar as none of the existing on-line
idiom list in the mental lexicon whereas for Swinney and studies tested, to our knowledge, the effect of SOP.
Cutler (1979) they are represented as non-compositional Evidence exists that non-aged participants with faster SOP
units together with the other items. For “Non-Lexical” (henceforth, fast participants) process sentential information
models (Cacciari & Tabossi, 1988; Tabossi & Zardon, 1993, provided more actively than those with slower SOP
1995; Titone & Connine, 1994; Tabossi, Fanari & Wolf, in (henceforth, slow participants) (e.g., McNamara, 1997;
press), idioms are represented as configurations of lexical Long, Oppy & Seely, 1994; Long, Seely & Oppy, 1999).
items, with no duplication of lexical information and no Hence, we expect fast and slow participants to differ in
separate representation for idioms. The Configuration spoken idiom comprehension as well.
Hypothesis (Cacciari & Tabossi 1988) belongs to this
second class and posits that idiom meanings are associated Experiment 1
with configurations formed by the same words activated We divided our participants into fast and slow using the
during the comprehension of literal sentences. The distribution of the individual mean speed of response in the
idiomatic string is processed word-by-word, as any other cross-modal lexical decision task (see below). It is
piece of language (Peterson et al., 2001), until enough reasonable to assume that both fast and slow participants
information accumulates to render the string identifiable as a would recognize early identification idioms before offset.
memorized figurative string. Only at this moment is the Hence, a facilitation for the visual target with respect to
idiomatic meaning retrieved. Usually, the recognition of the control condition should be observed for early identification
idiomatic nature of the string depends on a specific part of idioms indicating an idiomatic meaning activation at offset.
the idiom that can be located early in the string (early If meaning dominance effects extend to idioms, we might
identification idioms), or later on (late identification idioms), expect to find activation in both literal and idiomatic
for instance at offset. Hence, the point in which the string is contexts. The picture is less clear for late identification
identified as idiomatic should determine how early is idioms for which the existing findings are contrasting. If
activated the idiomatic meaning. However, the evidence on their idiomatic meaning indeed requires time to be identified
idiom meaning activation especially for late identification and emerge (as in Cacciari & Tabossi, 1988; Tabossi &
idioms is still inconclusive with some studies showing that Zardon, 1993, 1995), then no activation should be observed
activation requires time occurring only after idiom offset at the string offset especially in literal contexts. The
(Cacciari & Tabossi, 1988; Tabossi & Zardon, 1993, 1995) activation of late identification idiom meaning might in fact
require an idiomatic context that increases the expectation
373
toward a figurative interpretation of the string (as in Tabossi non-word by pressing the appropriately labeled response
& Cacciari, 1988; Colombo, 1993, 1998). However, Titone keys.
and Connine (1994) and Tabossi et al. (in press), found that
the meaning of late identification non-ambiguous idioms Results and Discussion
was already available at offset in neutral contexts. If these 35 participants whose mean lexical decision times were
findings extend to ambiguous idioms, then we might find below the 45° percentile were considered as fast participants
activation of late identification idiom irrespective from the and 35 participants whose times exceeded the 55° percentile
contextual bias. If indeed fast participants outperform slow as slow. The statistical analyses were conducted on the log10
participants in processing sentential information (as in transformation of the raw lexical decision times. Table 1
McNamara, 1997; Long et al, 1994, 1999), then differential reports untransformed means. In order to reduce variability,
idiomatic meaning activation patterns should emerge for late data points ±2 SDs from the mean response time of each
identification idioms. participant were excluded from the analyses (1.9%).
374
late identification idioms, no difference emerged for fast employed. At the beginning of the experiment, participants
participants [SOP X Identification: Fp(1,68)=4.17, p= .04; were advised that it was composed by two sessions and that
Fi(1,22)=5.53, p = .03]. Both fast and slow participants were in the second a response deadline was used. The instructions
faster when the targets were presented at offset than at were repeated at the beginning of each session. In Session
control position [SOP X Target: Fp(1,68)=3.78, p=.05]. The 2, a warning message appeared on the screen when the
idiomatic meaning of early identification idioms was deadline was over. Each new trial begun 900 ms after the
activated by both fast and slow participants. Despite what response (or the deadline end). The distribution of the
we expected, slow participants showed a full-fledged response times obtained in Session 1 was used to divide the
activation of the idiomatic meaning of late identification participants into fast vs. slow in both sessions.
idioms, whereas for fast participants the result only
approached significance (p=.07) [SOP X Target X Results and Discussion
Identification: Fp(1,68)=5.33, p=.02]. Why? One possibility Based on the results of Session 1, we split the participants
has to do with the slow response pace of slow participants: into fast and slow ones (with 37 participants per group) with
early identification and late identification idioms in fact the same procedure of Experiment 1. We present the results
differ in the amount of perceptual input required for the of the two sessions separately (Table 2). Session 1: A high
recognition of their idiomatic nature, with the first accuracy level was again observed with no fast vs. slow
recognized as idioms well before their offset (early difference (2.2% vs. 1.3% of errors respectively, t<1). The
identification point), and the second only when the last word idiomatic meaning of early identification idioms was already
has occurred (late identification point). The response pace of activated at offset, as in Experiment 1 [Target:
slow participants was so slow (see the 544 ms delay with Fp(1,72)=114.23, p<.001; Fi(1,11)=124.63, p<.001], by both
respect to fast ones) that the difference between early and fast and slow participants [SOP X Target: Fp(1,72)=10.02,
late identification idioms presumably was annulled: when p=.002; Fi(1,11)=25.64, p<.001]. The mean decision times
slow participants responded to the target associated with a of fast vs. slow participants again significantly differed
late identification idiom, enough perceptual input had [Fp(1,72)=166.75, p<.001; Fi(1,11)=403.79, p<.001].
already arrived to transform a late identification idiom into
an early identification one. Thereby Experiment 2 was Table 2: Mean decision times in literal and idiomatic
designed in which a response deadline was introduced. contexts, for P1/P2 (i.e., control vs. experimental positions),
fast (F) and slow (S) participants in Session 1 (early
Experiment 2 identification idioms) and Session 2 (late identification
Experiment 2 was divided into two sessions: in Session 1 idioms and response deadline).
only early identification idioms were used with the same Session 1 Session 2
experimental procedure of Experiment 1. This was Literal Idiomatic Literal Idiomatic
motivated by the need of clear cut time measures to be used Context Context Context Context
to divide participants into fast vs. slow (see below. In P1/P2 P1/P2 P1/P2 P1/P2
Session 2 only late identification idioms were employed and F 877/719 847/729 671/628 683/594
a response deadline introduced (see below). The presence of S 1416/1080 1467/1020 749/661 754/664
a response deadline should clarify the extent to which slow
participants indeed activated the idiomatic meaning of late
identification idioms, or whether they simply annulled the Session 2: The idiomatic meaning of late identification
difference between early vs. late identification due to their idioms was already activated at offset, as in Experiment 1
slowed response rate. Breznitz and Berman (2003) recently [Target: Fp(1,72)=80.306, p<.001; Fi(1,11)=82.170, p<.001]
documented a beneficial effect on sentence comprehension in both types of context [Context X Target: Fp(1,72)=2.87,
of a reading rate acceleration for slow readers, normal p=.094; Fi(1,11)=5.65, p=.03]. Despite the deadline, fast and
readers and dyslexic. Hence, we might expecta general slow participants overall significantly differed [105 ms,
speeding up of idiomatic meaning activation. Fp(1,72)=19.77, p<.001; Fi(1,11)=51.4, p<.001], although to
a lesser extent (105 ms) than in Session 1 (452 ms) and in
Method Experiment 1 (544 ms). No specific effect of SOP emerged.
Participants. Eighty-five undergraduates of the University However, if the time pressure speeded up the lexical
of Modena participated in Experiment 2. They were native decision on idiom-related targets fostering idiomatic
speakers of Italian and had not participate in any phase of meaning activation, it also had an accuracy cost as shown by
Experiment 1. the higher mean error rate of Session 2 (Session 2: 5%;
Materials and Procedure. The experimental materials were Session 1: 1.8%), with more errors in literal than in
those used in Experiment 1 but presented in two sessions idiomatic contexts [Fp(1,72)=3.9, p=.05; Fi(1,11)=4.14,
divided by a short rest. In Session 1 only early identification p=.06]. Slow participants were as much accurate as fast ones
idioms (plus appropriate fillers) were used with the same (5.4% vs. 4.7%, respectively). Unsurprisingly, slow
procedure of Experiment 1. In Session 2 only late participants gave more post-deadline responses than fast
identification idioms (plus fillers) were employed and a ones [3.7% vs. 2.9%, respectively; Fp(1,72)=3.59, p=.06].
response deadline introduced whose length (1007 ms) More post-deadline responses were provided by both slow
resulted from summing up the mean decision time of fast and fast participants when the targets were presented at
participants in Experiment 1 plus 1.5 SD. The same cross- control position than at idiom offset [Fp(1,72)=17.03,
modal lexical decision paradigm of Session 1 was
375
p<.0001; Fi(1,11)=17.74, p=.001], and more so in idiomatic meaning. In contrast, fast participants processed sentential
than in literal contexts [Context X Target: Fp(1,72)=5.07, information more actively, consistently with the literature.
p=.03]. The response deadline introduced in Experiment 2
Under time pressure, a recruitment of attentional and affected both groups of participants with a more striking
processing resources speeded up the response times of both effect on slow ones. With a deadline, the magnitude of the
fast and slow participants. The response deadline accelerated facilitation for the idiomatic target was in fact quite similar
slow participants more than fast ones, as suggested by the for fast and slow participants (66 ms vs. 89 ms,
583 ms mean between-session time difference for slow respectively). That imposing a response deadline speeded up
participants (i.e., a decrease of 46.8%) as compared with the mental processing and changed the allocation of time and
149 ms difference for fast participants (i.e., 19%). processing resources among different task components had
already been documented in the literature on judgment and
Conclusion decision-making (for a review, see Svenson & Maule, 1993)
The idiomatic meaning of ambiguous spoken idioms was and on reading comprehension (Breznitz & Berman, 2003).
activated at offset for both early and late identification According to Breznitz and Bearman (2003), who
idioms, as in Titone and Connine (1994) and Tabossi et al. extensively investigated this “acceleration phenomenon”,
(in press) but not in Cacciari and Tabossi (1988) and reading acceleration extends attention span, reduces
Colombo (1993, 1998). Then the effects of context of the distractibility, helps to overcome some capacity limitations
present study are certainly less clear-cut than those reported of short term memory and of working memory and increases
in Tabossi and Cacciari (1988) and Colombo (1993, 1998). word retrieval from the mental lexicon. Identifying the
This can at least in part be due to a meaning dominance factors underlying the differences between fast and slow
effect analogous to that reported in studies on semantic participants is the major problem we are currently
ambiguities: we in fact selected idiom strings with a investigating in a set of experiments designed to assess the
dominant figurative meaning. specific role of sensorimotor speed components, cognitive
How to reconcile these different findings? We can discard speed components and personality-based components in
an idiom familiarity effect since the expressions employed accounting for SOP differences in spoken idiom
in these studies were all familiar. More sound candidates for comprehension.
explaining these discrepancies lie in methodological Overall, the results of this study are more compatible with
differences. Titone and Connine, Cacciari and Tabossi, Non-Lexical models of idiom comprehension than with
Tabossi et al. used a cross-modal lexical decision paradigm Lexical-look up ones. In fact we concurred with other
and Colombo a self-paced reading times paradigm. Then, studies in showing that idioms do not behave as long words,
the cross-modal studies employed different control since their meaning activation requires time and can start
conditions: here we compared the lexical decision time for a only after the string is identified as idiomatic. The SOP
same target word presented either before the idiom, where effects we found on idiomatic meaning activation are best
no facilitation at all is expected, or at the idiom offset. Such accommodated by Non-Lexical models that posit that the
a modality can be more sensitive toward smaller amount of idiomatic sentence is fully processed up to recognition of its
activation than when an idiom-related target is compared figurative nature (and syntactically, even later on). Only
with an idiom-unrelated word, as in Cacciari and Tabossi, then can the idiomatic meaning be retrieved. In contrast,
for instance. Idioms also vary in the extent to which the Lexical-look up models assume direct retrieval of an idiom
strings also have a literal meaning: when an idiom string has meaning, a cognition operation that might prove less
two possible interpretations they rarely are equally frequent sensitive toward SOP differences being less demanding even
with the dominance of the idiomatic meaning affecting for slow participants. In sum, idiom comprehension cannot
meaning activation (Popiel & McRae, 1988). A word should be reduced to mere meaning retrieval from the mental
then be said on the notion of identification point (or lexicon.
predictability) that has been differently operationalized.
Furthermore the decision on an idiom identification point Acknowledgments
relies on off-line, paper-and-pencil measures whose The present study was funded by a grant COFIN 2003
predictive effect on on-line measures should be further (2003119330_005) to the first author.
assessed.
Last, and not at all least, we took individual differences References
into serious account. Did SOP differences modulate spoken Breznitz, Z. & Berman, L. (2003). The underlying factors
idiom comprehension? Although further investigations are of word reading rate. Educational Psychology Review, 15,
needed (and are in progress), we observed a clear-cut SOP 3, 247-265.
effect. Specifically, in Experiment 1, slow participants were Bobrow, S. & Bell, B. (1973). On catching on to idiomatic
more sensitive to the difference between early identification expressions. Memory & Cognition, 1, 343-346.
and late identification idioms than fast ones, and processed Cacciari, C. & Tabossi, P. (1988). The comprehension of
the first faster than the second. Spoken idiom identification idioms. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 668-683.
occurs word-by-word and is successfully achieved when Cacciari, C. & Tabossi, P. (in prep.). Factors affecting
enough information is build up to render an idiom idiomatic meaning time course: the role of idiom length
recognizable: only then can its meaning be retrieved. Slow and context.
participants needed more perceptual input than fast Colombo, L. (1993). The comprehension of ambiguous
participants to identify a string as idiomatic and activate its idioms in context. In Cacciari, C., Tabossi, P. (Eds.),
376
Idioms. Processing, Structure and Interpretation (pp. 3- Myerson, J., Hale, S., Zheng, Y., Jenkins, L. & Widaman,
26). Hillsdale, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum. K. F. (2003). The difference engine: a model of diversity
Colombo, L. (1998). Role of context in the comprehension in speeded cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10,
of ambiguous Italian idioms. In D. Hillert (Ed.), Sentence 2, 262-288.
processing: a cross-linguistic perspective. Syntax and Nippold, M. A., & Martin, S. T. (1989). Idiom interpretation
Semantics, vol. 31. (pp. 405-425). New York: Academic in isolation versus context. A developmental study with
Press. adolescents. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 32,
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R. & Ziegler, 59-66.
J. (2001). DRC: a dual-route cascaded model of visual Nippold, M. A., Moran, C., & Schwarz, I. E. (2001). Idiom
word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological understanding in preadolescents: Synergy in action.
Review, 108, 204-256. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 10, 2,
Faust, M. E., Balota, D. A., Spieler, D. H. & Ferraro, F. R. 169-180.
(1999). Individual differences in information-processing Peterson, R. R., Burgess, C., Dell, G. S., & Eberhard, K.L.
rate and amount: implications for group differences in (2001). Dissociation between syntactic and semantic
response latency. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 6, 777-799. processing during idiom comprehension. Journal of
Gernsbacher, M.A. & Faust, M. (1991). The mechanism of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and
suppression: a component of general comprehension skill. Cognition, 90, 227-234.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory Popiel, S. J., & McRae, K. (1988). The figurative and literal
and Cognition, 17, 245-262. senses of idioms, or all idioms are not used equally.
Gibbs, R.B. (1980). Spilling the beans on understanding and Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 17, 475-487.
memory for idioms. Memory & Cognition, 8, 449-456. Salthouse, T. A. (1994). The nature of the influence of speed
Hale, S. & Jensen, J. (1994). Global processing-time on adult age differences in cognition. Developmental
coefficients characterize individual differences in Psychology, 30, 2, 240-259.
cognitive speed. Psychological Science, 5, 384-389. Seidenberg, M. S. & McClelland (1989). A distributed
Hannon, B. & Daneman, M. (2001). A new tool for developmental model of word recognition and naming.
measuring and understanding individual differences in the Psychological Review, 96, 523-568.
component processes of reading comprehension. Journal Svenson, O. & Maule, A. J. (1993). (Eds.), Time pressure
of Educational Psychology, 93, 1, 103–128. and stress in human judgment and decision making. New
Johnson, J. (1991). Developmental versus language-based York: Plenum Press.
factors in metaphor interpretation. Journal of Educational Swinney, D. & Cutler, A. (1979). The access and processing
Psychology, 83, 4, 470–483. of idiomatic expression. Journal of Verbal Learning and
Just, M. A. & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of Verbal Behaviour, 18, 523-534.
comprehension: individual differences in working memory. Tabossi, P. & Cacciari, C. (1988). Context effects in the
Psychological Review, 99, 122-149. comprehension of idioms. Proceedings of the Tenth
Kello, C. T. & Plaut, D. C. (2003). Strategic control over rate Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society
of processing in word reading: a computational (pp.90-96). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
investigation. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 207- Tabossi, P., Fanari, R. & and Wolf, K. (in press). Spoken
232. idiom recognition: meaning retrieval and word
Long, D. L., Oppy, B. J. & Seely, M. R. (1994). Individual expectancy. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research.
differences in the time course of inferential processing. Tabossi, P., Zardon, F. (1993). The activation of idiomatic
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, meaning in spoken language. In C. Cacciari & P. Tabossi
and Cognition, 20, 1456-1470. (Eds.). Idioms. Processing, Structure and Interpretation
Long, D. L., Seely, M. R. & Oppy, B. J. (1999). The (pp. 145-162). Hillsdale, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum.
strategic nature of less skilled readers’ suppression Tabossi, P., & Zardon, F. (1995). The activation of
problem. Discourse Processes, 27, 281-302. idiomatic meaning. In M. Everaert, E. van den Linden,
McCabe, J., & Hartman, M. (2003). Examining the locus of A. Schenk, & R. Schreuder (Eds.), Idioms: structural and
age effects on complex span tasks. Psychology and Aging, psychological perspectives (pp. 273-282). Hillsdale, NJ:
18, 3, 562-572. Erlbaum.
McNamara, D. (1997). Comprehension skills: a knowledge- Titone, D.A. & Connine, C.N. (1994). Comprehension of
based account. Proceedings of the Nineteenth Annual idiomatic expressions: Effect of predictability and
Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 508- literality. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
513). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Memory, and Cognition, 20, 5, 1126-1138.
McNamara, D & McDaniel, M.A. (2004). Suppressing Vernon, P. A. & Jensen, A. R. (1984). Individual and group
irrelevant information: knowledge activation or differences in intelligence and speed of information
inhibition? Journal of Experimental Psychology: processing. Personality and Individual Differences, 5,
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 2, 465-482. 411-423.
377