Self-Compassion Levels in Music
Self-Compassion Levels in Music
Self-Compassion Levels in Music
Jamey Kelley
University of North Texas
Alison Farley
University of Georgia
Introduction
Among music education practitioners, there is a widespread belief that music
study can help build healthy levels of self-esteem (Duerksen & Darrow, 1991;
Fisher, 1988). Self-esteem can be described as an individual’s sense of self-worth,
perceived value, or how much one likes oneself (Neff, 2003a). Among the many
findings on self-esteem since the early 1980s, researchers have identified a strong
relationship between self-esteem levels and mental health outcomes. For instance,
individuals who reported having low self-esteem were significantly more likely to
experience suicidal ideations, a lack of motivation, or depression (see Harter, 1999,
167
Contributions to Music Education
for a review), while individuals identified as having high levels of self-esteem were
more likely to report higher satisfaction in life or general happiness (Baumeister,
Campbell, Krueger, & Vohs, 2003). Many programs, including educational and
arts interventions, have been developed to promote healthy levels of self-esteem
within young people.
Consequently, researchers in music education have investigated if music study
promotes healthy self-esteem in students, to mixed results. Early studies that first
explored the relationship between self-esteem and music study reported signifi-
cant relationships, although most of these results are qualified in some way. Nolin
and Vander Ark (1977) reported significant differences in self-esteem between
ninth-grade choral and band students and those who did not participate in mu-
sic; however, the researchers found multiple differences between the two observed
groups in addition to self-esteem, including socio-economic status and attitudes
toward music. Additionally, Duke, Flowers, and Wolfe (1997) surveyed a large
sample of piano students from various parts of the United States inquiring about
the participants’ parents, teachers, and experience as a piano student. Students,
parents, and teachers reported that piano study had a significant positive impact
on the piano students, including higher self-esteem and feelings of happiness. De-
spite these results, the relationship between music tuition and high self-esteem is
unclear due to many confounding variables, such as supportive parents or dispos-
able income, that may account for the relationship observed between self-esteem
and positive mental outcomes. More recently, Rickard et al. (2013) reported an in-
crease of self-esteem in elementary students when incorporating additional music
classes to their curriculum when compared to control groups; however, the same
increase in self-esteem was detected, as compared to control groups, in a group of
students that learned juggling instead of music classes, suggesting that collabora-
tive experiences may be the catalyst for changes in self-esteem. Thus, while there
is evidence of a relationship between self-esteem and musical study, the empirical
record is incomplete on the nature of that relationship and could be a result of
other important factors, like socio-economic status.
While some studies have reported a positive relationship of music study on
self-esteem, other researchers have found limited effects (Knox-Anderson &
Rickard, 2007; Lomen, 1970; Wamhoff, 1972) or no effects at all (Michel, 1971;
Michel & Farrell, 1973; Rickard, Bambrick, & Gill, 2012). In an effort to examine
the influence of piano study on self-esteem while controlling for socio-economic
factors, Costa-Giomi (2004) tracked the self-esteem of young people who were
randomly assigned to either a piano instruction group or a group that did not
receive formal music instruction; socio-economic status was controlled in the ran-
168
Jamey Kelley and Alison Farley
169
Contributions to Music Education
170
Jamey Kelley and Alison Farley
that have examined mindfulness, but researchers have noted that Neff ’s theoreti-
cal conceptualization of self-compassion is more robust in predicting depression,
anxiety, and quality of life than other instruments that examine mindfulness (Van
Dam, Sheppard, Forsyth, & Earleywine, 2011).
In the emerging literature about self-compassion in social psychology, there
are reported differences in self-compassion based on group characteristics. Ado-
lescents generally exhibit lower levels of self-compassion than older people (Neff,
2003a). While the relationship between gender and self-compassion levels is still
unclear, slight differences have been reported between gender groups (with wom-
en reporting lower self-compassion than their male counterparts); however, the
differences in self-compassion between gender groups dissipates as participant
samples increase in age (Yarnell, Stafford, Neff, Reilly, Knox, & Mullarkey, 2015).
Cultural distinctions also seem to inform an individual’s experience with self-
compassion. For instance, college students in cultures where Buddhism is preva-
lent were found to have significantly higher levels of self-compassion as compared
to college students in more individualistic societies (Neff, 2003b), although dif-
ferences in self-compassion levels among other types of cultures has not been
detected (Birkett, 2014).
To date, there is no existing study examining self-compassion in relation
to music study. While there are no reported differences between musicians and
non-musicians in relation to self-compassion, several researchers have provided
evidence that musicians have significantly different personality traits than indi-
viduals from the general population. Kemp (1996) reported several differences
between the personalities of professional musicians and the general population.
Musicians, on average, are more introverted than the general population. This
inward-looking trait can be characterized as a disconnection from social groups
and a reticence to share feelings or emotions.
Neuroticism is another personality trait that has been identified as being over-
represented among musicians in comparison to the general population (Kemp,
1996). This trait of emotional instability can be typified by regular experiences
of heightened emotions as well as volatility in personal relationships and social
groups. In a study on popular musicians, Cooper and Wills (1989) found that
popular musicians also reported higher levels of neuroticism than the general pub-
lic. Kemp (1996) has also reported that musicians had higher identification with
independence when compared to control groups, a trait which is characterized by
dominance, competitiveness, and decisiveness.
In addition to personality differences, researchers have reported higher in-
cidence of anxiety in musicians when compared to general populations. Many
171
Contributions to Music Education
Study One
Method
Participants
Adult students were surveyed at a large university in the Pacific Northwest
of the United States. “Music students” were operationally defined as undergradu-
ate or graduate students whose studies focused on music study (i.e., majoring in
music); this definition was agreed upon by the researchers and a consulting team
from a university research group that included researchers in music education,
nursing, and neurobiology, as well as students. The rationale for this definition is
172
Jamey Kelley and Alison Farley
that students who pursue a music degree have a qualitatively different experience
in music study than students who pursue music as a hobby (e.g., participation in
marching band).
Participants included 49 music majors and 52 non-music majors (N = 101;
56% female). Music majors were recruited from classes found in the core curricu-
lum of a music degree (e.g., music theory, performance ensembles) while non-ma-
jors were recruited from music-focused classes that were populated with students
not pursuing a music degree (i.e., music appreciation, history of rock ’n roll, etc.).
Participants volunteered to stay after class to complete a questionnaire provided
by the researchers. Prior to collecting data, the researchers acquired Institutional
Review Board approval.
Instruments
Participants completed the Self-Compassion Scale (Neff, 2003b), a 26-item
questionnaire developed to measure levels of self-compassion. The questionnaire
was presented in-person and completed in a paper/pencil format. Participants
were presented with statements reflecting the three positive and three negative (six
total) components of self-compassion: Self-judgment (e.g., “I’m disapproving and
judgmental about my own flaws and inadequacies.”), Self-kindness (e.g., “I try to
be loving towards myself when I’m feeling emotional pain.”), Over-identification
(e.g., “When I’m feeling down I tend to obsess and fixate on everything that’s
wrong.”), Mindfulness (e.g., “When something upsets me I try to keep my emo-
tions in balance.”), Isolation (e.g., “When I think about my inadequacies, it tends
to make me feel more separate and cut off from the rest of the world.”) and Com-
mon Humanity (e.g., “When things are going badly for me, I see the difficulties as
part of life that everyone goes through.”).
Participants were asked to respond to each of these statements on a five-point
Likert-type scale (1 – almost never; 5 – almost always). Cumulative scores for
participants were calculated with positive components (self-kindness, mindfulness
and common humanity) scored according to the response provided. Alternatively,
negative responses (self-judgment, over-identification and isolation) were reverse
scored. Additionally, demographic information was collected (age, gender, major,
and country of birth).
Results
Following the summation of individual scores, group responses were analyzed
and examined. Overall, the questionnaire was found to be reliable (Cronbach’s a =
.92) with each subscale revealing a similar acceptable level of reliability. As a group,
music majors’ mean self-compassion score was 72.76 (SD = 16.22) with non-music
173
Non-Music
Majors
14
12
Contributions to Music Education
10
majors’ mean self-compassion score averaging higher at 78.82 (SD = 17.56). For
8
analysis, a two-group independent t-test was performed. Although the mean self-
compassion
6
score for music students was lower than non-music students, when
comparing the two groups, the results failed to reject the null hypothesis, t(99) =
4
1.80, p = .074. In other words, the results indicate that there was no difference in
self-compassion
2
levels between the music majors and non-music majors that could
not be explained by random error.
0 Participants’ scores were then broken down into the Mindfulness
six component parts of
Self-Kindness Self-Judgment Common Humanity Isolation Over-Identification
the self-compassion (e.g., self-kindness, mindfulness) survey to determine if any
one component
Figure would reveal
1. Self-Compassion a difference
Levels. This figurebetween thereported
displays the groups.components
No significant
of self-dif-
compassion levels between music majors and non-music majors.
ferences in reported self-compassion components were detected by group. Music
and non-music students reported self-compassion components are displayed in
Figure 1.
18 Music
Majors
16
Non-Music
Majors
14
12
10
0
Self-Kindness Self-Judgment Common Humanity Isolation Mindfulness Over-Identification
Figure 1. Self-Compassion Levels. This figure displays the reported components of self-compassion
Figure 1. Self-Compassion Levels. This figure displays the reported components of self-
between music majors and non-music
compassion majors.
between music majors and non-music majors.
Discussion
An evaluation of previous research led the authors to hypothesize that music
majors may report lower self-compassion levels than participants majoring in pro-
grams outside of music. While music majors reported self-compassion levels lower
than their non-music peers, this result was not significant and failed to reject the
null hypothesis. Because the p value of the difference between groups (p = .074)
was nearing the critical value (a = .05) in addition to music students reporting
lower self-compassion levels for each of the six subcomponents of the construct,
174
Jamey Kelley and Alison Farley
the researchers re-examined the data to ascertain if there was a Type-2 error pres-
ent, where there is a significant difference between groups but it is undetected
due to a lack of power. The researchers initiated a post-hoc power analysis using
G*Power software (Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, 2007). After inputting the
number of participants for each group, the estimated power (1 – b) to detect a
medium effect size (Cohen’s d = .5) was .70, below the standard of .80 of research
in the social sciences (Overland, 2014). Due to the deficient power in this analysis
along with the general trend of musicians’ lower scores in self-compassion as well
as the individual components, the research team decided to pursue a replication
to substantiate findings.
Study Two
Method
Participants
Data were gathered from music and non-music majors from two universi-
ties in the United States. One institution was a large public university in the
Southeastern part of the country; the other university was a small, private reli-
gious school located in the Midwestern part of the United States. This second
data collection comprised 69 music majors and 49 non-music majors (N = 118,
58% female). Participants were recruited from music and non-music classes (e.g.,
Introduction to Speech) via email from the authors in which prospective partici-
pants were provided a link to complete the questionnaire at their leisure.
Instrument
Participants completed the same 26-item Self-Compassion Scale (Neff,
2003b) and demographic items as the first data collection. Music majors were also
asked to respond to one additional item (“How often do you experience perfor-
mance anxiety?”) on a 5-point, Likert-type scale. This scale was anchored with
a response of “1” being “almost never” and “5” being “almost always.” Similar to
Study 1, composite self-compassion scores were tabulated for each participant.
Positive components were scored exactly as the participant responded and nega-
tive components were reverse scored, as in the first study. Responses from the
performance anxiety inquiry did not require any adjustment by the scorers and
were analyzed as they were reported.
175
Contributions to Music Education
Results
Two different types of analyses were run with this data set. First, mean group
scores were compared to detect differences in self-compassion between music ma-
jor and non-music major groups. Second, responses from the performance anxiety
inquiry were compared to self-compassion scores of the music major group to
examine a potential relationship. In comparing the two groups’ summed self-com-
passion scores, no significant difference was found between groups, which again
failed to reject the null hypothesis, t(118) = 0.13, p > .05. In this study, music ma-
jors scored an average of 76.96 (SD = 15.21) and non-music majors scored 76.57
(SD = 16.56) in self-compassion.
Reports of performance anxiety and self-compassion scores by music majors
were compared to identify a potential relationship. As seen in Table 1, there was
no significant difference between composite self-compassion scores and reported
experiences of performance anxiety, r(67) = -.18, p > .05. Components of self-
compassion were also examined in relation to performance anxiety. Five of the six
subcomponents revealed no significant relationship to performance anxiety (self-
kindness, r(118) = -.09, p > .05; self-judgment, r(67) = -.20, p > .05; common
humanity, r(67) = .05, p >.05; isolation, r(67) = -.13, p > .05; mindfulness, r(67) =
-.08, p > .05); however, over-identification was identified as having a significant,
moderate correlation with reported incidence of performance anxiety, r(67) = -.31,
p < .05.
Discussion
Results from the second data collection revealed no significant difference in
self-compassion between the music and non-music groups, supporting the initial
finding of the prior data collection. The researchers added one additional item to
explore the relationship of reported performance anxiety in music majors with
self-compassion. While there was not a strong relationship detected between self-
compassion and reported performance anxiety, there was one significant correla-
tion detected between performance anxiety and one component of self-compas-
sion: over-identification.
A low-to-moderate significant negative correlation, r = -.31, p <.05, was found
between self-compassion scores and the over-identification component of self-
compassion. In other words, participants who might “fixate” or “overly obsess”
(Neff, 2003b) on negative aspects of their performance reported more frequent
experiences of performance anxiety. While the researchers’ hypothesis related to
self-compassion was not fully supported, the singular relationship between over-
identification and performance anxiety exhibited by these participants merits fur-
ther exploration.
176
Jamey Kelley and Alison Farley
General Discussion
The purpose of this study was to explore self-compassion levels of music stu-
dents and their non-music peers. Despite previous evidence that musicians differ
from non-musical peers in certain personality traits (e.g, neuroticism, introver-
sion, independence), the current studies revealed no significant difference in self-
compassion between groups based on field of study, music or otherwise. While
results did not align with our original hypothesis, it seems logical that a great deal
of variability in self-compassion is present in both music students and students
of other disciplines. In other words, there are individuals with high or low self-
compassion levels in both the music and non-music groups. It is reasonable to be-
lieve that many musicians are likely to be compassionate towards themselves when
encountering obstacles, an important trait to become successful in a career where
high-pressured performances are expected. In contrast, it is just as likely that other
musicians may be less compassionate toward themselves if they do not meet their
high expectations.
While the focus of this project examined the reported self-compassion lev-
els of music and non-music students, a limitation of the study is the creation of
the groups by an arbitrary designation (e.g., their focus of study). It can be as-
sumed that many in the non-music group had significant musical experiences and
training, but decided to pursue other disciplines. Further study should explore
self-compassion using a more quantitative metric for musical experiences, like the
Goldsmith Music Sophistication Index (Müllensiefen, Gingras, Stewart, & Mu-
sil, 2011), to robustly examine the relationships between music experience and
self-compassion.
While our compared groups were similar in age and nation of origin, other
factors such as socioeconomic status, ethnicity, and religious tradition were not
considered; these factors could have an unknown influence on our results. The
lack of any distinction in self-compassion between music students and non-music
students may have been due to competing factors within the self. Stratification
or sample matching may be useful in future studies to fully understand if music
tuition can impact self-compassion. Alternatively, our samples may have been less
homogenous as compared to samples in previous research where personality dif-
ferences were detected between musicians and the general population. Samples
from both private and public institutions were more diverse than prior studies (i.e.,
recruiting participants who were members of a professional orchestra or conserva-
tories) as well as student bodies being more diverse than when much of the prior
research was undertaken. Differences in self-compassion may emerge between
more specific groups within music students; for instance, students who concen-
177
Contributions to Music Education
178
Jamey Kelley and Allison Farley
179
Contributions to Music Education
References
Alemán, X., Duryea, S., Guerra, N. G., McEwan, P. J., Muñoz, R., Stampini, M., &
Williamson, A. A. (2017). The effects of musical training on child development:
A randomized trial of El Sistema in Venezuela. Prevention Science, 18(7),
865-878. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-016-0727-3.
Asmus, E. P. (1985). Sixth graders’ achievement motivation: Their views of
success and failure in music. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music
Education, 85, 1-13. Retrieved from https://www-jstor-org.proxy-remote.galib.
uga.edu/stable/40317938.
Baumeister, R. F., Campbell, J. D., Krueger, J. I., & Vohs, K. D. (2003). Does high
self-esteem cause better performance, interpersonal success, happiness, or
healthier lifestyles? Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 4(1), 1-44.
Retrieved from https://www-jstor-org.proxy-remote.galib.uga.edu/sta-
ble/40062291.
Birkett, M. (2014). Self-compassion and empathy across cultures: Comparison of
young adults in China and the United States. International Journal of Research
Studies in Psychology, 3(3), 25-34. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.5861/
ijrsp.2013.551.
Cooper, C. L., & Wills, G. I. (1989). Popular musicians under pressure.
Psychology of Music, 17(1), 22-36. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1177%
2F0305735689171003.
Costa-Giomi, E. (2004). Effects of three years of piano instruction on children’s
academic achievement, school performance and self-esteem. Psychology of Music,
32(2), 139-152. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0305735604041491.
Crocker, J., & Park, L. E. (2004). The costly pursuit of self-esteem.
Psychological Bulletin, 130(3), 392-414. Retrieved from https://psycnet.apa.org/
doi/10.1037/0033-2909.130.3.392.
Demorest, S. M., Kelley, J., & Pfordresher, P. Q. (2017). Singing ability, musical self-
concept, and future music participation. Journal of Research in Music Education,
64(4), 405-420. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0022429416680096.
Duerksen, G. L., & Darrow, A. (1991). Music class for the at-risk: A music
therapist’s perspective. Music Educators Journal, 78(3), 46-49.
Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.2307%2F3398288.
180
Jamey Kelley and Allison Farley
Duke, R. A., Flowers, P. J., & Wolfe, D. E. (1997). Children who study piano with
excellent teachers in the United States. Bulletin of the Council for Research in
Music Education, 132, 51-84. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/
stable/40375333.
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible
statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical
sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175-191. Retrieved from https://doi.
org/10.3758/BF03193146.
Fisher, R. E. (1988). Reaching beyond the intrinsic value of music. Music
Educators Journal, 74(9), 39-41. Retrieved from https://journals.sagepub.com/
doi/pdf/10.2307/3400501?casa_token=01OxidQuMOoAAAAA:arWEUNajD
cBgPWgJUo7jOm9SBm9VwEZMOLvQjA2i_Y7GYuOXlcjQXFxg_XUIejT-
vjShMfGw_Q0w.
Harter, S. (1999). The construction of the self: A developmental perspective. New York:
Guilford Press.
Kemp, A. E. (1994). Aspects of upbringing as revealed through the personalities of
musicians. The Quarterly Journal of Music Teaching and Learning, 5(4), 34-41.
Retrieved from http://www-usr.rider.edu/~vrme/v16n1/volume5/visions/winter3.
Kemp, A. E. (1996). The musical temperament: Psychology and personality of musicians.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Kenny, D. T., Davis, P., & Oates, J. (2004). Music performance anxiety and
occupational stress amongst opera chorus artists and their relationship with state
and trait anxiety and perfectionism. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 18, 757-777.
Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2003.09.004.
Knox-Anderson, H., & Rickard, N. S. (2007). A preliminary examination of
short-term effects of an active class-based music program on young adolescents’
self-esteem and anger expression. Australian Journal of Music Education, (1), 4.
Retrieved from http://proxy-remote.galib.uga.edu/login?url=http://search.
ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=ehh&AN=32540299&site=eds-live.
Kobori, O., Yoshie, M., Kudo, K., & Ohtsuki, T. (2011). Traits and cognitions of
perfectionism and their relation with coping style, effort, achievement, and
performance anxiety in Japanese musicians. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 52(4),
663-676. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.03.001.
Lehmann, A. C., Sloboda, J. A., & Woody, R. H. (2007). Psychology for musicians:
Understanding and acquiring the skills. New York: Oxford University Press.
181
Contributions to Music Education
182
Jamey Kelley and Allison Farley
183
Contributions to Music Education
Van Kemenade, J. F., Van Son, M. J., & Van Heesch, N. C. (1995). Performance
anxiety among professional musicians in symphonic orchestras: A self-report
study. Psychological Reports, 77(2), 555-562. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.24
66%2Fpr0.1995.77.2.555.
Wamhoff, M. J., (1972). A comparison of self-concept of fourth grade students enrolled
and not enrolled in instrumental music in selected schools in the Barstow, California
School District. Music Index. (Accession No. MAH0001778739).
Yarnell, L. M., Stafford, R. E., Neff, K. D., Reilly, E. D., Knox, M. C., &
Mullarkey, M. (2015). Meta-analysis of gender differences in self-
compassion. Self and Identity, 14(5), 499-520. Retrieved from https://doi.org/
10.1080/15298868.2015.1029966.
184