Pone.0239612 Actividad Inducida

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 17

PLOS ONE

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Individual test-retest reliability of evoked and


induced alpha activity in human EEG data
Manuel Vázquez-Marrufo ID1*, Rocı́o Caballero-Dı́az1, Rubén Martı́n-Clemente2,
Alejandro Galvao-Carmona ID3, Javier J. González-Rosa4,5
1 Experimental Psychology Department, Faculty of Psychology, University of Seville, Sevilla, Spain, 2 Signal
Processing and Communications Department, Higher Technical School of Engineering, University of Seville,
Sevilla, Spain, 3 Department of Psychology, Universidad Loyola Andalucı́a, Seville, Spain, 4 Department of
Psychology, Universidad de Cádiz, Cádiz, Spain, 5 Institute of Biomedical Research and Innovation of Cádiz
(INiBICA), Cádiz, Spain

a1111111111 * [email protected]
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
Abstract
Diverse psychological mechanisms have been associated with modulations of different
EEG frequencies. To the extent of our knowledge, there are few studies of the test-retest
reliability of these modulations in the human brain. To assess evoked and induced alpha reli-
OPEN ACCESS abilities related to cognitive processing, EEG data from twenty subjects were recorded in 58
Citation: Vázquez-Marrufo M, Caballero-Dı́az R, derivations in two different sessions separated by 49.5 ± 48.9 (mean ± standard deviation)
Martı́n-Clemente R, Galvao-Carmona A, González- days. A visual oddball was selected as the cognitive task, and three main parameters were
Rosa JJ (2020) Individual test-retest reliability of analyzed for evoked and induced alpha modulations (latency, amplitude and topography).
evoked and induced alpha activity in human EEG
data. PLoS ONE 15(9): e0239612. https://doi.org/
Latency and amplitude for evoked and induced modulations showed stable behavior
10.1371/journal.pone.0239612 between the two sessions. The correlation between sessions for alpha evoked and induced
Editor: Tomoyoshi Komiyama, Tokai University
topographies in the grand average (group level) was r = 0.923, p<0.001; r = 0.962, p<0.001,
School of Medicine, JAPAN respectively. The within-subject correlation values for evoked modulation ranged from 0.472
Received: May 26, 2020
to 0.974 (mean: 0.766), whereas induced activity showed a different range, 0.193 to 0.892
(mean: 0.655). Individual analysis of the test-retest reliability showed a higher heterogeneity
Accepted: September 10, 2020
in the induced modulation, probably due to the heterogeneous phases found in the second
Published: September 23, 2020 case. However, despite this heterogeneity in phase values for induced activity relative to the
Copyright: © 2020 Vázquez-Marrufo et al. This is onset of the stimuli, an excellent correlation score was obtained for group topography, with
an open access article distributed under the terms values that were better than those of the grand average evoked topography. As a main con-
of the Creative Commons Attribution License,
which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
clusion, induced alpha activity can be observed as a stable and reproducible response in
reproduction in any medium, provided the original the cognitive processing of the human brain.
author and source are credited.

Data Availability Statement: All relevant data are


within the manuscript and its Supporting
Information files.

Funding: This study was supported by Plan Introduction


Nacional de Excelencia (Ministerio de Economı́a y One of the challenges in neuroscience is finding potential neurophysiological parameters of
Competitividad, Government of Spain, PSI2016-
cognitive processing. In human cognitive psychophysiology, electroencephalography (EEG)
78133-P). The funders had no role in study design,
data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or
has contributed diverse techniques, such as ERPs (event-related potentials) or frequency analy-
preparation of the manuscript. JJGR was funded sis, to study diverse cognitive mechanisms. Regarding spectral analysis, considerable evolution
through a Ramón y Cajal research fellowship (RYC- has occurred from the application of the Fourier theorem to the more recent time-frequency

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612 September 23, 2020 1 / 17


PLOS ONE Reliability of evoked and induced alpha activities

2015-18467) awarded by the Spanish Ministry of analysis [1]. An advantage of time-frequency techniques is that they allow the calculation of
Economy and Competitiveness. modulations in the spectral domain on a millisecond scale. One of the first approaches was
Competing interests: The authors have declared developed in the last century by Pfurstcheller [2], showing that desynchronization in the alpha
that no competing interests exist. band could be interpreted as the activation of a neural area related to the task demand. This
phenomenon was termed event-related desynchronization (ERD).
From this study, many experiments using ERD calculations have been performed to under-
stand the potential link of frequency bands with cognitive mechanisms (sensorial, cognitive
and motor) in the human brain [3–5]. A similar technique (defined as temporal spectral evolu-
tion (TSE)) was proposed by Hari et al. [6], in which a subtle change was included in the proto-
col to calculate spectral modulations. Instead of the squaring step applied in the ERD, the
authors used a rectification function to change the negative voltages to positive voltages in the
desired frequency band. A benefit of this approach is that the amplitude of the signal is not dis-
torted; therefore, values are represented in microvolts. From then to now, some studies have
been conducted using this technique in the study of information processing [7–9].
However, the result of the TSE calculation (as the ERD) mixes activities that are evoked or
induced with the onset of the stimuli. Spectral evoked modulations can be easily obtained
directly from the time-domain averages. However, induced modulations cannot be calculated
with the same procedure. A solution that has been proposed is to subtract evoked activity from
temporal spectral evolution modulation in the experimental conditions during the study [10].
The result is sometimes termed induced activity [11, 12]. This activity is not in phase with the
onset of the stimuli, but it is “induced” by the onset of the stimuli.
In recent years, induced activity has received more attention as the basis of some cognitive
mechanisms that are hidden by conventional analysis of evoked activity [10, 13, 14]. Moreover,
to the extent of our knowledge, no studies have investigated the reliability of induced modula-
tions in human EEG signals.
In regard to the test-retest reliability of the frequency bands with different spectral tech-
niques, some studies have revealed that these measurements range from moderate to high lev-
els in between-sessions comparisons. For instance, Burgess and Gruzelier [15] measured alpha
power in the baseline period and found an acceptable level of consistency for this band (>0.7)
using the classification defined by other authors [16]. However, in another study, other authors
described poor reliability between measures in an auditory memory task for the alpha band
[17]). Due to this variability of the reliability scores, other studies have focused on what factors
could be determinant. Neuper et al. [18] pointed out that the frequency band and the brain
region were critical for the reliability of the alpha band. In a similar line of research, Friedrich
et al. [19] confirmed the importance of these factors but also indicated that the cognitive task
performed is determinant in the test-retest reliability of the EEG frequency band considered.
This last result obligates us to specifically analyze the reliability of any band in any cognitive
task that is intended to be used in longitudinal studies.
However, test-retest reliability studies are usually focused only on group comparisons
(grand averages) and not on individual comparisons that are relevant for the potential applica-
tion of these techniques in the clinical field. Previous studies in our lab have analyzed this issue
in other EEG measures, such as P3 or alpha-TSE [20, 21]. The results showed that the correla-
tion for group comparisons was excellent for both parameters (P3 and alpha-desynchroniza-
tion). However, correlation scores were not as reliable in all the individual comparisons
performed for these measures.
The main goal of the present experiment is to analyze the reliability of the evoked and
induced alpha activity in a visual oddball task. The benefits of the study are envisioned in two
ways. First, a highly reliable score for these measures would suggest their potential use in longi-
tudinal follow-up studies for each subject in the event that a therapeutic strategy is utilized

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612 September 23, 2020 2 / 17


PLOS ONE Reliability of evoked and induced alpha activities

(pharmacological, neuropsychological rehabilitation, etc.). Moreover, a specific individual


analysis of all participants would allow better assessment of the potential clinical application of
these activities (i.e., case reports).
As a second benefit, a reproducible response for induced activity would suggest that this
activity, in spite of its heterogeneous phase values in each trial, might represent a highly stable
correlate of the cognitive processing.

Methods
Ethics statement
All participants enrolled in the present study signed informed consent before their inclusion.
The experiment was carried out in compliance with the Helsinki Declaration.
The study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Seville (proj-
ect code: PSI2016-78133-P).

Participants
Twenty-five adults (university students and faculty staff) were included in the experiment. All
participants reported no history of neurological conditions or drug consumption. Five subjects
were rejected due to artifacts that were impossible to remove in at least one electrode of the
6x7 matrix analyzed (see Fig 1 for detailed locations of recording derivations and the 6x7
matrix). The final sample was composed of 20 adults (8 males, 12 females) ranging in age from
21 to 45 years (mean 28.6 ± 8.1) (mean ± standard deviation) years (all but one were right-
handed) who participated in two EEG recordings separated by a mean of 49.5 ± 48.9 days.
This interval of time was defined because one of the main aims in the present study is to assess

Fig 1. Electrode positions recorded in the scalp. Fifty-eight scalp electrodes are depicted. The red electrodes were
used to analyze the amplitude differences between sessions (6x7 matrix). Abbreviations: F (frontal), FC (frontocentral),
C (central), CP (central), P (parietal), PO (parietooccipital), L (line), z (zero or midline).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612.g001

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612 September 23, 2020 3 / 17


PLOS ONE Reliability of evoked and induced alpha activities

the reliability of the evoked and induced measures in time windows used commonly in the
clinical field (from 1 week as a double confirmation study to 3 months in more distanced fol-
low-ups). A majority of the participants of this sample participated in our previous study [10];
however, because not all of them were available for a follow-up study, some of them were
replaced by other participants.

Experimental paradigm
The cognitive task employed in the present study was a “visual oddball”. The task the subjects
were to perform consisted of the discrimination of uncommon visual stimuli in a sequence of
frequent stimuli. The target stimulus (appearance probability: 25%) was a rectangle with a
checkerboard pattern comprising red and white squares, whereas the standard stimulus was
equivalent in size with the same pattern but with black and white squares. Both stimuli were
displayed in the center of the screen, and in their absence a fixation point was present to pre-
vent changes in eye position. The screen was located 70 centimeters from the participant’s
eyes, and the size of both stimuli was 7.98% of the visual angle on the X-axis and 9.42% of the
visual angle on the Y-axis. Both stimuli lasted for 500 milliseconds (ms), and the stimulus
onset asynchrony (SOA) interval was one second. One block with 200 trials was used in a pseu-
dorandom presentation (total recording duration: 3 minutes and 20 seconds). Participants
indicated the target presentation pressing the serial mouse button with the right index finger
and ignoring the standard stimulus. Behavioral variables (reaction time, accuracy percentage
for the target and overall (including no responses for the standard stimuli)) were calculated at
the end of the experiment.

EEG acquisition and analysis


EEG signals were recorded from 58 scalp electrodes (Ag/AgCl) mounted in an Electro-Cap
and located in standard positions of the 10–10 system [22] (see Fig 1). The electrode signals
were amplified using BrainAmp amplifiers (Brain Products GmbH, Germany) and digitally
stored using Brain Vision Recorder 1.03 (Brain Products GmbH, Germany). All derivations
were referenced during the recording to the linked earlobe channel and later re-referenced to
an averaged reference. The ground electrode was placed at the center of the forehead.
To monitor vertical and horizontal electrooculograms (VEOG and HEOG, respectively),
bipolar recordings from electrodes situated in the inferior and superior positions of the left
orbit and in the external canthi of the ocular orbits were registered. Considering that P1/N1
components are retinotopic and they contribute to the evoked activity, we applied this rejec-
tion procedure to preclude any trial with eyes not centered on the stimuli. A digitization rate
of 500 Hz, a bandpass of 0.01–100 Hz and an impedance below 5 kOhm were set during the
recording.
The following protocol was applied to calculate evoked and induced modulations with the
Brainvision Analyzer 1.05 (Brain Products GmbH, Germany). A common preprocessing pro-
cedure consisted of 1) ocular correction of the blinking artifact in the scalp electrodes using
the algorithm developed by Gratton et al. [23], 2) segmentation of the continuous EEG record-
ing (-200 to 1000 ms, zero being the onset of the target stimulus), 3) baseline correction based
on the previous interval to the stimulus (-100 to 0 ms), and 4) trials in which the HEOG signal
was outside the ±50 μV range were rejected. At this point, two possible protocols were created
for the EEG signal. To obtain evoked alpha activity, the steps of averaging, filtering (8–13 Hz,
48 dB/octave, Butterworth) and rectifying were applied to the EEG signal [10] (see Fig 2 for a
schematic procedure of evoked and induced calculations).

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612 September 23, 2020 4 / 17


PLOS ONE Reliability of evoked and induced alpha activities

Fig 2. Schematics of EEG analyses to obtain evoked and induced modulations. See the main text for a detailed
description of the procedure. Abbreviations: TSE: Temporal spectral evolution.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612.g002

In cases of induced alpha activity, temporal spectral evolution (TSE) was calculated first
with the following procedure: bandpass filtering (8–13 Hz, 48 dB/octave, Butterworth) per-
formed over EEG epochs, signal rectification and finally averaging for the target condition.
After this processing, a subtraction of evoked activity from TSE modulation was subsequently
performed to calculate the induced response [10]. All the individual averages comprised at
least 45 artifact-free trials (session 1 (evoked and induced): 48.4 ± 2.7; (session 2 (evoked and
induced): 48.5 ± 3.3). No significant differences in the number of trials were found between
sessions.
The latency values of the evoked and induced alpha activity were calculated for each subject
in the electrode that showed the maximum amplitude in the grand average (Oz). The evoked
alpha peak was identified as the maximum positivity in the interval between 50 and 250 milli-
seconds (ms). For an induced alpha “valley”, the latency value was calculated as the maximum
negativity in the same range. To better determine the peak, a low pass filter (5 Hz (48 dB/

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612 September 23, 2020 5 / 17


PLOS ONE Reliability of evoked and induced alpha activities

octave)) was used to eliminate small high-frequency fluctuations, as has been described in pre-
vious studies [7, 24]. After the latency was determined for each subject, amplitude values for
the rest of the electrodes were documented in that latency for topographical study. All ampli-
tude values were measured relative to the -100 to 0 ms prestimulus baseline period. No inter-
polation procedures were applied to preclude modifications of the data that could affect
further topographical analysis. Last, to calculate the potential differences in absolute terms for
the amplitude between evoked and induced activity, the latter was rectified to positive values
after calculation.

Phase analysis for evoked and induced activity


A potential contribution of evoked rather than induced activity could occur; therefore, the
phase content of both modulations was checked to ensure that induced modulation was truly
nonphase locked activity. To do so, the evoked response was estimated through averaging over
trials and then subtracted from each of the individual trials [11, 12]. Later, the trials were fil-
tered in the desired band (8–13 Hz), and the Hilbert transform was applied to calculate the
instantaneous phase. The phases of induced activity were measured at the time instant t = 129
ms in single trials, corresponding to the estimated grand average peak interval of induced
modulation. In addition, the phases of the evoked responses were calculated by following an
equivalent protocol at t = 140 ms. Moreover, Nelli et al. [25] have recently shown that the
amplitude of the alpha oscillations, as well as the behavioral performance in visual processing
tasks, are linked to the instantaneous frequency of the EEG in the alpha band (where ‘instanta-
neous frequency’ is defined as the mathematical derivative of the phase of the EEG signal, i.e.,
the instantaneous rate of change of the phase). According to experimental evidence, the fluctu-
ations of the instantaneous frequency regulate the efficiency in the processing of the visual
information, as these fluctuations predict the desynchronization of the alpha rhythms. This
suggests that the instantaneous frequency plays an important role in understanding the mech-
anisms that regulate alpha oscillations and visual processing. Consequently, it is appropriate
for the purposes of our research to study the test-retest reliability of the instantaneous fre-
quency of both evoked and induced alpha activities. To this end, as described in Nelli et al
[25], we use the Hilbert transform to represent the alpha band-filtered EEG signal as a complex
waveform: x(t) = C(t) exp(i w(t)), where w(t) is the phase of the EEG in radians. We then
approximate the derivative of the unwrapped phase angles by the slope of the local regression
line. This derivative divided by 2 pi gives a smooth estimate of the instantaneous frequency
in Hz.

Statistical analyses
All variables were checked for normality using the Shapiro-Wilk test. Parametric or nonpara-
metric statistical methods were used depending on the normality results. Reaction time was
compared between sessions using a paired t-test for dependent variables. Accuracy differences
among sessions were analyzed by the Wilcoxon test. Latency of evoked and induced activity
was analyzed with ANOVA with two factors: factor 1: “type of activity” (levels (2): evoked and
induced; factor 2: “session” (levels (2): S1 and S2). To examine topographical differences in the
amplitude of evoked and induced activities between the two sessions, an analysis of variance
(ANOVA) was applied with the following factors: factor 1: “type of activity” (levels (2): evoked
and induced; factor 2: “session” (levels (2): S1 and S2); factor 3: “anterior-posterior position”
of the electrode (levels (6): frontal; frontocentral; central; centroparietal; parietal; parietoocci-
pital); factor 4: “lateral-medial position” (levels (7): from lateral left to lateral right, example:
line 5, line 3, line 1, midline or line zero (z), line 2, line 4, line 6) (i.e. 6). F5, F3, F1, Fz, F2, F4,

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612 September 23, 2020 6 / 17


PLOS ONE Reliability of evoked and induced alpha activities

F6) (see Fig 1 for the 6x7 matrix resulting from these factors). A Greenhouse-Geisser correc-
tion for sphericity was applied. A Bonferroni correction was carried out in multiple compari-
son post hoc analysis.
To analyze the correlations between maps (individual vs. individual for each session; and
grand average S1 vs grand average S2), we performed individual averages for each subject,
obtaining a mean value of amplitude for each electrode of the 6x7 matrix in both sessions and
then proceeded with the correlation analysis. After that, Pearson’s product-moment r was
employed for all comparisons. As suggested by Kileny and Kripal [26], the 0.05 significance
level was divided by the number of contrasts made for all correlation analyses (42 (2: grand
average maps; 40: individual maps)) (new p<0.001 for significant results). Finally, the coeffi-
cient of variation (CV) was analyzed to define the suitability of the behavioral (reaction time
and accuracy) and latency measures of evoked and induced activities for longitudinal studies.
CV value for all parameters was calculated using the formula described elsewhere [27] (coeffi-
cient of variation = (standard deviation/mean) x 100).

Results
Behavioral data
The reaction times showed no significant difference between the two sessions (S1: 317 ± 35.2;
S2: 310 ± 38 ms) (t(1,18) = 1.96, p = 0.065) (see Table 1 for the mean values of all the behavioral
and evoked/induced alpha latencies). No significant differences were found in the percentage
of accuracy for global performance (target and standards) (session 1: 97.1 ± 4.9; session 2:
97.3 ± 5.1) or the specific accuracy percentage for the target stimulus (session 1: 99.3 ± 1.3; ses-
sion 2: 99.2 ± 1.3).

Evoked and induced alpha reliability


Regarding the latency parameter, both activities collapsed (evoked and induced) (S1: 137 ms;
S2: 132 ms) showed no significant differences, which was due to the “session” factor (F(1,19) =
0.544, p = 0.469) (Fig 3) or “type of activity” factor (evoked: 140 ms; induced: 129 ms) (F(1,19)
= 3.62, p = 0.072). No statistically significant values were found for the interaction of both fac-
tors (session and type of activity) (see S1 Text in S1 File for all negative results).
For the topographical analysis of the amplitude, ANOVA showed that the triple interaction
of “type of activity” x “anterior-posterior position” and “medial-lateral position” was signifi-
cant (F(30,570) = 1.78, p = 0.007) (ƞ2: 0.086) (see S1 Table in S1 File for amplitude values of
each electrode). Post hoc analysis showed that this interaction was based on higher amplitude
values for evoked activity compared to induced activity in all derivations of the 6x7 matrix
(averaged value for evoked activity: 0.88 microvolts, induced activity: 0.43 microvolts) (Fig 3).
No effects were found for the session factor or in its interaction with other location factors.

Table 1. Latency values for behavior and frequency for each session.
S1 S2
RT ACC T ACC G LAT E LAT I RT ACC T ACC G LAT E LAT I
MEAN 317 99,3 97,1 143 131 310 99,2 97,3 137 128
STD DEV 35,2 1,3 4,9 37,8 36,6 38 1,3 5,1 41 39,9
CV 11,1 1,3 5,1 26,4 28 12,2 1,3 5,2 30 31,1

Abbreviations: RT: Reaction time (in milliseconds). S1: session 1. S2: session 2. ACC: accuracy. T: target. G. global (target and standard). Lat. latency (in milliseconds).
E: evoked. I: induced. STD: standard deviation. CV: coefficient of variation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612.t001

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612 September 23, 2020 7 / 17


PLOS ONE Reliability of evoked and induced alpha activities

Fig 3. A) Grand average evoked and induced traces for both sessions in an Oz derivation. B) 2D head maps in the
latencies of maximum amplitude for evoked and induced modulations. The X-axis represents “time” expressed in
milliseconds (ms), and the Y-axis represents the “amplitude” of the evoked and induced activity in microvolts (μV).
The vertical dashed line indicates the onset of the stimulus. Abbreviations: S1: session 1, S2: session 2.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612.g003

Regarding the correlation analyses, a high score was found for the grand average maps of
evoked (r: 0.923) and induced activities (r: 0.962) (Fig 3). For individual correlation analyses, a
considerable range of values was found for both activities (evoked and induced) (see Table 2
and Figs 4 and 5). For evoked activity, and considering the classification proposed by Klein
[16], 18 of 20 subjects exhibited moderate to high reliability scores on their individual maps
(r > 0.500). With regard to induced activity, the number of subjects that obtained a moderate
to high correlation score between session maps was 15. On average, evoked and induced activi-
ties exhibited r values of 0.766 and 0.655, respectively.
Finally, coefficient of variation analyses showed that there were differences in all parameters
measured. The lower value was obtained by the accuracy variables (target and global) (from 1.3
to 5.2), followed by another behavioral variable (reaction time) (11.1–12) (see Table 1). A higher
value (from 26.4 to 31.1) was found for the latencies of both activities (evoked and induced).
Regarding the potential contribution of evoked over induced activity, phase analysis of both
modulations revealed that a disperse collection of phases was present in induced activity that
was not particularly focused on the phase values of evoked activity in each subject. In Fig 6,
plots of the phase values are presented for evoked and induced alpha modulations in both ses-
sions (S1 and S2) (see S2 Text in S1 File for amplitude values of each electrode).
On the other hand, using the Hilbert transform as in Nelli et al [25], we calculate the evolu-
tion over time (zero being the onset of the stimulus) of the estimated instantaneous frequen-
cies, averaged across subjects. Welch’s test shows that we cannot reject the hypothesis that the
evoked average instantaneous frequencies in sessions 1 and 2 are the same, as there are p values
larger than the 5% threshold over the entire time interval. An equivalent result is found when
comparing induced average instantaneous frequencies from different sessions (Fig 7).

Discussion
Behavioral data
Behavioral analyses showed that there was no significant difference between the sessions. In
previous studies, some authors have reported a reliable score for reaction time in test-retest

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612 September 23, 2020 8 / 17


PLOS ONE Reliability of evoked and induced alpha activities

Table 2. Individual and group correlation values for the topographical maps in evoked and induced activities.
SUBJECT EVOKED (S1 vs S2) INDUCED (S1 vs S2)
1 0.915� 0.817�
2 0.729� 0.721�
3 0.895� 0.749�
4 0.472 0.433
5 0.900� 0.655�

6 0.562 0.432
7 0.778� 0.193
8 0.854� 0.866�

9 0.589 0.761�
10 0.864� 0.894�
11 0.958� 0.819�

12 0.792 0.746�

13 0.612 0.658�

14 0.857 0.426
15 0.711� 0.655�

16 0.596 0.801�

17 0.974 0.892�
18 0.866� 0.546�
19 0.488 0.202
20 0.917� 0.836�

GROUP 0.923 0.962�

Correlation values for the topographical maps between subjects and for both sessions are calculated by Pearson’s
product-moment r. All values marked with an asterisk (� ) were significant after Bonferroni correction (p<0.002).
Abbreviations: S1: session 1. S2: session 2.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612.t002

reliability analysis for oddball tasks [20, 21, 28]. Moreover, the lack of differences in accuracy
variables (target and global) ensured that the reliability observed in the reaction time was not
affected by a speed-accuracy tradeoff. However, a statistical trend was observed with a faster
reaction time in the second session (a difference of 7 ms). A parsimonious interpretation is
that practicing task execution could improve the behavioral response. However, an individual-
ized observation of behavioral data showed that some participants were faster in the second
session, but some were faster in the first session, suggesting that the practice effect is not always
present and that other variables, such as motivation, could also play a critical role. Regarding
the coefficient of variation, reaction time and accuracy showed that both variables exhibited
good values and could be considered for the assessment of cognitive processing with values
well under 30, as suggested by some authors [27]. However, the possibility of assessment for
reaction time would be limited to the group analysis in which variations between subjects
would be compensated in the mean and standard deviation values. For individual measures, as
mentioned above, increases and decreases in the reaction time between sessions suggest that
this behavioral variable may be unsuitable for use in longitudinal studies.

Evoked and induced alpha reliability


Regarding reliability, latencies of evoked and induced activities were not significantly different
due to the session factor. A second factor (type of activity) showed that no differences were
found either in the latency parameter between evoked and induced modulations. When

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612 September 23, 2020 9 / 17


PLOS ONE Reliability of evoked and induced alpha activities

Fig 4. 2D head maps for each subject in both sessions for evoked activity. Pairs of 2D head maps are displayed for
each of the 20 subjects (labeled from 1 to 20) participating in the experiment. The left side of the pair is the evoked
topography in session 1, and the right side is the evoked topography for session 2. Note that the scale (in microvolts)
has been adjusted for each subject to allow an optimized representation of the individual topography.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612.g004

compared to the evoked activity described in a previous study from our lab, this result does
not support the suggested shortening of induced latency [10]. Future studies manipulating
other cognitive variables (time expectation, cognitive load, etc.,) may provide more substantial
results about the potential change in the onset of induced modulation with regard to evoked
activity.

Fig 5. 2D head maps for each subject in both sessions for induced activity. Pairs of 2D head maps are displayed for
each of the 20 subjects (labeled from 1 to 20) participating in the experiment and the grand average (GA). The left side
of the pair is the induced topography in session 1, and the right side is the induced topography for session 2. Note that
the scale (in microvolts) has been adjusted for each subject to allow an optimized representation of the individual
topography.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612.g005

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612 September 23, 2020 10 / 17


PLOS ONE Reliability of evoked and induced alpha activities

Fig 6. Polar plot of phase analysis results for evoked and induced (Sessions 1 and 2) activity for the alpha band (8–
13 Hz) in each subject (labeled from 1 to 20). Inner circle represents values for session 1 (red dot (evoked) and green
crosses (induced)) and outer circle values for session 2 (orange dot (evoked) and blue crosses (induced)).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612.g006

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612 September 23, 2020 11 / 17


PLOS ONE Reliability of evoked and induced alpha activities

Fig 7. Instantaneous phase analyses. Upper left corner: average instantaneous frequency (IF) of the evoked responses in session 1 (red) and session 2
(orange). Colored area represents the 95% confidence interval of the IF in session 1, meaning that the true value of the IF is inside these limits with a
probability of 95%. It is noteworthy that the orange curve also lies in this confidence interval. The remaining illustrations can be interpreted in a similar
way. The figure in the upper right corner shows the 95% confidence interval of the evoked IF in session 2. The figures in the bottom row show the IFs of
the induced responses and their respective confidence intervals, all of them calculated by an equivalent procedure and in the same time interval.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612.g007

In regard to the coefficient of variation in the latency variable for evoked and induced laten-
cies, scores ranged from 26.4 to 31.1, indicating that values of this parameter in our study are
around the limit suggested by Polich and Herbst [27] for its use in cognitive assessment. Con-
sidering the averaged standard deviation of latency values (between 30 and 40), it can be stated
that the variability of this parameter is significant in this sample, and therefore, it does not
appear to be suitable for use in individual follow-up. However, group comparisons are more
reliable between measures and look appropriate for the group hypothesis.
In regards to the topography, the maximum values for evoked activity were located in pos-
terior electrodes, as has been described in other studies [7, 29], and for induced activity [9, 10,
13]. It is necessary to note that, despite the absence of statistically significant results, grand
average maps for both sessions show a subtle displacement of alpha activity (both evoked and
induced) to the right hemisphere, perhaps as a result of the well-known lateralization of atten-
tional systems [30, 31].
In terms of the session factor, no differences were found for either modulation between mea-
sures. In previous test-retest studies, other components of the time and frequency domain (P3

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612 September 23, 2020 12 / 17


PLOS ONE Reliability of evoked and induced alpha activities

and alpha-ERD) exhibited an increase in the amplitude for session 2 and were highly consistent
in all experimental subjects [20, 21]. The increment for the amplitude in these variables was
interpreted as a result of the increase in synchrony (reaching an optimal performance for the
neural network) because of the task repetition. However, if the resulting evoked activity in the
present experiment represents the spectral content of P1/N1, these ERP components have been
described as highly stable in intersession measures of their amplitudes [28, 32]. Therefore, it
seems that evoked (and induced) responses in sensory neural networks do not have a great mar-
gin to improve synchrony, and their amplitudes remain steady after the repetition of the task.
However, the fact that the EEG signal may not be sensitive enough for these modulations
(evoked and induced) to show the increase in amplitude by repetition cannot be overlooked.
Another remarkable result is that evoked activity had higher amplitudes (in absolute terms)
compared to the amplitudes of induced activity. This result was described by our group in a
previous study [10]. It has been suggested by other authors that the amplitude decrement of
induced alpha activity would be modulated by the characteristics of the task (i.e., requiring
more top-down or bottom-up processes) [33]. In the present study, a top-down process could
not represent a highly demanding top-down mechanism of control, as it is a temporal expecta-
tion (based on the fixed SOA of our task); consequently, a lower amplitude was observed for
induced activity compared to that of the evoked response caused by the presentation of the sti-
muli. However, and as a limitation of the current study, it is necessary to highlight that current
amplitude results have to be taken cautiously, as we described in a previous publication from
our lab [10]. One of the reasons is that the subtraction of the evoked activity from TSE activity
to obtain the induced activity is assuming that evoked activities are stable in time across trials.
Therefore, future studies have to be performed to confirm the present results with different
procedures described by other authors [34] to control for the possibility of jitter effects biasing
induced activity.
In any case, topographies of evoked and induced modulations are highly similar, and conse-
quently, common neural areas could be involved in both activities. As far as we know, there
are no previous studies about the neural generators of alpha-induced activity in the literature.
However, Flaveris et al. [35] compared alpha activity elicited by moving images in which per-
ception alternates between dissociated fragments to a single unified object and vice versa.
Time-frequency analysis of EEG data revealed that when shifts in perception occurred, a
greater decrease in the alpha band was found, and its neural generator was located in the lateral
occipital cortex. Neural source modeling in future studies could disentangle potential differ-
ences in neural generators of induced and evoked activity and improve the present under-
standing of the psychophysiological role of both activities.
Regarding correlation analyses, grand average maps showed excellent scores for both activi-
ties (evoked and induced) (r > 0.9). These values suggest that the follow-up of the topographi-
cal changes at the group level and in both activities could be adequate. It is important to
emphasize that the correlation scores were excellent even when the calculation of the ampli-
tude was performed in different latencies.
In the case of the individual topographical analyses, correlation scores ranged from moder-
ate to excellent in both activities. However, in some subjects, r values under 0.5 were found (2
for evoked and 5 for induced). A potential cause for the difference between both activities
regarding their individual reliability is that evoked modulation is probably more related with
bottom-up mechanisms. Therefore, the potential change between sessions is limited because
less contribution comes from experience or subject strategies to perform the task. In the
induced activity, more possible functional assemblies could be found involving top-down pro-
cesses (strategies and practicing) that are more diverse in individual human subjects and conse-
quently less reliability in the topographical distributions can be found. In any case, considering

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612 September 23, 2020 13 / 17


PLOS ONE Reliability of evoked and induced alpha activities

all these scores, it seems necessary to consider, with caution, the potential use of the individual
maps in the longitudinal study of the cognitive mechanisms indexed by these modulations. In
the particular case of the clinical application of these measures, it has to be noted that the het-
erogeneity between sessions for the evoked and induced activities are independent from the
number of days between measures (see S1 and S2 Figs in S1 File). Good correlation scores were
found for both low (one week between measures) to a higher number of days (more than 3
months). Therefore, assessment in short- to mid-range follow-ups is possible for these EEG
activities but cautiously as stated above. In any case, future studies are required to properly
assess this effect in larger samples of patients to confirm clinical suitability.
In the case of induced activity, phase analyses have corroborated, as in previous studies [10,
36], that there is not a significant contribution of evoked activity in the induced modulation
for this cognitive task. Moreover, regarding the interdependence between amplitude and
instantaneous frequency values, a previous study has demonstrated that both parameters
reflect a common change in the visual information processing [25]. The results from the pres-
ent study have added that these measures are highly reliable for both activities (evoked and
induced) and between sessions.
In regards to the physiological role of alpha band, evoked activity could represent the spec-
tral alpha content of early ERP components as P1/N1 and could consequently be related to the
active processing of the visual stimuli. With respect to induced activity, the valley represents
the desynchronization of the alpha band, probably related to the inhibition of neural noise in
that band that could interfere with stimulus processing. This balance of processing and inhibi-
tion for the alpha band has been debated in various studies [10, 13, 33], and no clear consensus
has been reached yet.
Previous studies have shown that induced modulation is associated not only with the
target stimuli analyzed in the present study but also with the standard stimuli [10]. Moreover,
induced modulations can also be observed in longer latencies (approximately 300 ms) with
observable changes in topography, suggesting different roles for this activity during informa-
tion processing [10]. Future studies are required to understand the psychological variables that
can modulate this activity in cognitive processing.
In summary, evoked and induced activities related to the cognitive task used in this study
are highly reliable and include two main parameters in the group analyses: latency and topog-
raphy. It is also relevant to point out that the identification of the peak (for evoked activity) or
valley (for induced activity) to calculate the latency does not represent a difficult procedure as
peaks and valleys (evoked and induced activity, respectively) are clearly formed. Moreover,
high correlation scores for grand average maps in both activities even when latencies are differ-
ent indicate the robustness of these activities representing cognitive mechanisms.
Regarding individual analyses, test-retest results and correlation scores for topographical
maps suggest that the application of the main parameters (latency, amplitude and topography)
in longitudinal follow-up studies has to be considered cautiously because in some cases, they
are not highly reliable (r scores under 0.500) and changes between sessions for latency can be
in both directions (increasing or decreasing). Finally, it is remarkable that the grand averages
of induced alpha by the presentation of the stimuli exhibits such a high value of correlation for
between-session maps (r: 0.962), suggesting that this activity represents a highly stable and
reproducible response of the human brain during cognitive processing.

Supporting information
S1 File.
(DOCX)

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612 September 23, 2020 14 / 17


PLOS ONE Reliability of evoked and induced alpha activities

Author Contributions
Conceptualization: Manuel Vázquez-Marrufo, Rocı́o Caballero-Dı́az, Rubén Martı́n-Cle-
mente, Javier J. González-Rosa.
Data curation: Rocı́o Caballero-Dı́az, Rubén Martı́n-Clemente, Alejandro Galvao-Carmona.
Formal analysis: Manuel Vázquez-Marrufo, Rocı́o Caballero-Dı́az, Rubén Martı́n-Clemente,
Alejandro Galvao-Carmona.
Funding acquisition: Manuel Vázquez-Marrufo, Javier J. González-Rosa.
Investigation: Manuel Vázquez-Marrufo, Rubén Martı́n-Clemente, Alejandro Galvao-Car-
mona, Javier J. González-Rosa.
Methodology: Manuel Vázquez-Marrufo, Rocı́o Caballero-Dı́az, Rubén Martı́n-Clemente.
Project administration: Manuel Vázquez-Marrufo, Javier J. González-Rosa.
Resources: Manuel Vázquez-Marrufo, Rubén Martı́n-Clemente, Alejandro Galvao-Carmona.
Software: Manuel Vázquez-Marrufo, Rocı́o Caballero-Dı́az, Rubén Martı́n-Clemente, Alejan-
dro Galvao-Carmona.
Supervision: Manuel Vázquez-Marrufo.
Validation: Manuel Vázquez-Marrufo, Rubén Martı́n-Clemente, Alejandro Galvao-Carmona,
Javier J. González-Rosa.
Visualization: Manuel Vázquez-Marrufo, Rocı́o Caballero-Dı́az.
Writing – original draft: Manuel Vázquez-Marrufo, Rocı́o Caballero-Dı́az.
Writing – review & editing: Manuel Vázquez-Marrufo.

References
1. Cohen MX. Analyzing Neural Time Series Data: Theory and Practice. The MIT Press. Cambridge.
2014.
2. Pfurtscheller G. Graphical display and statistical evaluation of event-related desynchronization (ERD).
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1977, Nov; 43(5):757–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694
(77)90092-x PMID: 72657
3. Babiloni C, Babiloni F, Carducci F, Cappa SF, Cincotti F, Del Percio C, et al. Human cortical rhythms
during visual delayed choice reaction time tasks. A high-resolution EEG study on normal aging. Behav
Brain Res. 2004, Aug 12; 153(1):261–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2003.12.012 PMID: 15219728
4. Hidalgo-Muñoz AR, López MM, Galvao-Carmona A, Pereira AT, Santos IM, Vázquez-Marrufo M, et al.
EEG study on affective valence elicited by novel and familiar pictures using ERD/ERS and SVM-RFE.
Med Biol Eng Comput. 2014, Feb; 52(2):149–58. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11517-013-1126-6 PMID:
24257836
5. Arakaki X, Lee R, King KS, Fonteh AN, Harrington MG. Alpha desynchronization during simple working
memory unmasks pathological aging in cognitively healthy individuals. PLoS One. 2019, Jan 2; 14(1):
e0208517. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208517 PMID: 30601822
6. Hari R, Salmelin R, Mäkelä JP, Salenius S, Helle M. Magnetoencephalographic cortical rhythms. Int J
Psychophysiol. 1997, Jun; 26(1–3):51–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0167-8760(97)00755-1 PMID:
9202994
7. Vázquez Marrufo M, Vaquero E, Cardoso MJ, Gómez CM. Temporal evolution of alpha and beta bands
during visual spatial attention. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2001, Oct; 12(2):315–20. https://doi.org/10.
1016/s0926-6410(01)00025-8 PMID: 11587900
8. Capilla A, Schoffelen JM, Paterson G, Thut G, Gross J. Dissociated α-band modulations in the dorsal
and ventral visual pathways in visuospatial attention and perception. Cereb Cortex. 2014, Feb; 24
(2):550–61. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs343 PMID: 23118197

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612 September 23, 2020 15 / 17


PLOS ONE Reliability of evoked and induced alpha activities

9. Ellmore TM, Ng K, Reichert CP. Early and late components of EEG delay activity correlate differently
with scene working memory performance. PLoS One. 2017, Oct 10; 12(10):e0186072. https://doi.org/
10.1371/journal.pone.0186072 PMID: 29016657
10. Vázquez-Marrufo M, Garcı́a-Valdecasas M, Caballero-Diaz R, Martin-Clemente R, Galvao-Carmona A.
Multiple evoked and induced alpha modulations in a visual attention task: Latency, amplitude and topo-
graphical profiles. PLoS One. 2019, Sep 26; 14(9):e0223055. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.
0223055 PMID: 31557253
11. Truccolo WA, Ding M, Knuth KH, Nakamura R, Bressler SL. Trial-to-trial variability of cortical evoked
responses: implications for the analysis of functional connectivity. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2002, vol.
113, pp. 206–226. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1388-2457(01)00739-8 PMID: 11856626.
12. David O, Kilner JM, Friston KJ. Mechanisms of evoked and induced responses in MEG/EEG. Neuro-
image. 2006, Jul 15; 31(4):1580–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.034 PMID: 16632378
13. Sabate M, Llanos C, Enriquez E, Gonzalez B, Rodriguez M. Fast modulation of alpha activity during
visual processing and motor control. Neuroscience. 2011, 189:236–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
neuroscience.2011.05.011 PMID: 21619912.
14. Munneke GJ, Nap TS, Schippers EE, Cohen MX. A statistical comparison of EEG time- and time-fre-
quency domain representations of error processing. Brain Res. 2015 Aug 27; 1618:222–30. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.brainres.2015.05.030 Epub 2015 May 30. PMID: 26032741
15. Burgess AP, Gruzelier JH. The reliability of event-related desynchronisation: a generalisability study
analysis. Int J Psychophysiol. 1996, Oct; 23(3):163–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0167-8760(96)00046-3
PMID: 8947782
16. Kline P. The handbook of psychological testing. 2nd ed. London: Routledge. 2000.
17. Krause CM, Sillanmäki L, Häggqvist A, Heino R. Test-retest consistency of the event-related desyn-
chronization/event-related synchronization of the 4–6, 6–8, 8–10 and 10–12 Hz frequency bands during
a memory task. Clin Neurophysiol. 2001, May; 112(5):750–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1388-2457(01)
00501-6 PMID: 11336889
18. Neuper C, Grabner RH, Fink A, Neubauer AC. Long-term stability and consistency of EEG event-
related (de-)synchronization across different cognitive tasks. Clin Neurophysiol. 2005, Jul; 116
(7):1681–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2005.03.013 PMID: 15922658
19. Friedrich EV, Scherer R, Neuper C. Stability of event-related (de-) synchronization during brain-com-
puter interface-relevant mental tasks. Clin Neurophysiol. 2013, Jan; 124(1):61–9. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.clinph.2012.05.020 PMID: 22749465
20. Vázquez-Marrufo M, González-Rosa JJ, Galvao-Carmona A, Hidalgo-Muñoz A, Borges M, Peña JL,
et al. Retest reliability of individual p3 topography assessed by high density electroencephalography.
PLoS One. 2013, May 1; 8(5):e62523. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0062523 PMID: 23658739
21. Vázquez-Marrufo M, Galvao-Carmona A, Benı́tez Lugo ML, Ruı́z-Peña JL, Borges Guerra M, Izquierdo
Ayuso G. Retest reliability of individual alpha ERD topography assessed by human electroencephalog-
raphy. PLoS One. 2017, Oct 31; 12(10):e0187244. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0187244
PMID: 29088307
22. American Electroencephalographic Society. Guideline thirteen: guidelines for standard electrode posi-
tion nomenclature. American Electroencephalographic Society. J Clin Neurophysiol. 1994, Jan; 11
(1):111–3. PMID: 8195414
23. Gratton G, Coles MG, Donchin E. A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencepha-
logr Clin Neurophysiol. 1983, 55(4):468–484. https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(83)90135-9 PMID:
6187540
24. Enatsu R., Nagamine T., Matsubayashi J., Maezawa H., Kikuchi T., Fukuyama H. et al. The modulation
of rolandic oscillation induced by digital nerve stimulation and self-paced movement of the finger: A
MEG study. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 2014, 337, 201–211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.
2013.12.011 PMID: 24368012
25. Nelli S., Itthipuripat S., Srinivasan R. and Serences J.T., Fluctuations in instantaneous frequency pre-
dict alpha amplitude during visual perception. 2017, Nature communications. 8 (1), 2071:1–12. https://
doi.org/10.1038/s41467-017-02176-x PMID: 29234068
26. Kileny PR, Kripal JP. Test-retest variability of auditory event-related potentials. Ear Hear. 1987, 8
(2):110–114. https://doi.org/10.1097/00003446-198704000-00008 PMID: 3582802
27. Polich J, Herbst KL. P300 as a clinical assay: rationale, evaluation, and findings. Int J Psychophysiol.
2000, 38(1):3–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0167-8760(00)00127-6 PMID: 11027791
28. Cassidy SM, Robertson IH, O’Connell RG. Retest reliability of event-related potentials: Evidence from a
variety of paradigms. Psychophysiology, 2012, 49(5):659–664. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.
2011.01349.x PMID: 22335452

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612 September 23, 2020 16 / 17


PLOS ONE Reliability of evoked and induced alpha activities

29. Worden MS, Foxe JJ, Wang N, Simpson GV. Anticipatory biasing of visuospatial attention indexed by
retinotopically specific alpha-band electroencephalography increases over occipital cortex. J Neurosci.
2000, 15; 20(6):RC63. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-06-j0002.2000 PMID: 10704517
30. Di Russo F., Martı́nez A., Sereno M.I., Pitzalis S., Hillyard S.A. Cortical sources of the early components
of the visual evoked potential. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002, Feb; 15(2):95–111. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.
10010 PMID: 11835601
31. Gonzalez Andino SL, Michel CM, Thut G, Landis T, Grave de Peralta R. Prediction of response speed
by anticipatory high-frequency (gamma band) oscillations in the human brain. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005,
24(1):50–8. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20056 PMID: 15593272
32. Kompatsiari K, Candrian G, Mueller A. Test-retest reliability of ERP components: A short-term replica-
tion of a visual Go/NoGo task in ADHD subjects. Neurosci Lett. 2016 Mar 23; 617:166–72. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.neulet.2016.02.012 Epub 2016 Feb 6. PMID: 26861197
33. Klimesch W, Sauseng P, Hanslmayr S. EEG alpha oscillations: the inhibition-timing hypothesis. Brain
Res Rev. 2007, 53(1):63–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.06.003 PMID: 16887192.
34. McKewen M, Cooper PS, Wong ASW, Michie PT, Sauseng P, Karayanidis F. Task-switching costs
have distinct phase-locked and nonphase-locked EEG power effects. Psychophysiology. 2020; 57(5):
e13533. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13533 PMID: 31994736
35. Flevaris AV, Martı́nez A, Hillyard SA. Neural substrates of perceptual integration during bistable object
perception. J Vis. 2013, Nov 18; 13(13):17. https://doi.org/10.1167/13.13.17 PMID: 24246467
36. Riecanský I, Katina S. Induced EEG alpha oscillations are related to mental rotation ability: the evidence
for neural efficiency and serial processing. Neurosci Lett. 2010, Sep 27; 482(2):133–6. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.neulet.2010.07.017 PMID: 20637833

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239612 September 23, 2020 17 / 17

You might also like