Multidimensional Sense of Humor Scale
Multidimensional Sense of Humor Scale
Multidimensional Sense of Humor Scale
Archive
University of Zurich
University Library
Strickhofstrasse 39
CH-8057 Zurich
www.zora.uzh.ch
Year: 2018
Psychometric evaluation of the revised Sense of Humor Scale and the construction
of a parallel form
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/humor-2016-0085
Abstract: McGhee (1996, Health, healing and the amuse system: Humor as survi-
val training. Dubuque: Kendall/Hunt; 1999, Health, healing and the amuse sys-
tem: Humor as survival training (3rd edition). Dubuque: Kendall/Hunt) proposed
a model of the sense of humor including the six “humor skills” of enjoyment of
humor, laughter, verbal humor, finding humor in everyday life, laughing at
yourself, and humor under stress, measured with the Sense of Humor Scale
(SHS). The purpose of the present study is to evaluate the psychometric proper-
ties of the SHS (revised version from 1999) and to develop a parallel form of the
SHS to double the amount of items for each humor skill. Combing these two
forms should yield reliable and factorially valid scales of the six humor skills.
Participants in two online studies (n = 315 and 542) completed the SHS and its
parallel form, along with measures of various outcomes. The psychometric
properties of the SHS were of mixed quality, and those of the parallel form
were uniformly good. The parallel-test reliability was sufficiently high to regard
the two scales as parallel versions. Combining the two measures resulted in
reliable and distinguishable scales of the six humor skills. All humor skills
correlated positively with humor-related attitude and mood, cheerfulness, and
life satisfaction. Importantly, they spanned different dimensions of the sense of
humor, underscoring the usefulness of each humor skill.
1 Introduction
In 1996, McGhee proposed a multi-faceted concept of the sense of humor, an
assessment tool for its measurement, and a training program for enhancing the
sense of humor. McGhee (1979) stated that humor is a form of play – the play
with ideas. Without a playful frame of mind, the same event may be seen as
interesting, puzzling, annoying, frightening, etc., but not as funny. Playfulness
and seriousness were assigned core roles in the sense of humor model. While
people in general might be capable of spotting incongruities, absurdities, and
ironies of life, only the mentally playful will find humor in them. A habitually
serious attitude or frame of mind will hinder treating incongruities humorously.
McGhee (1996) postulated that playfulness is inherited and that all children
play, but that socialization (entering school, the workplace) may counteract
this tendency to play and may cause individuals to lose their ability to be
playful. The humor-training program for the “terminally serious” aims at redis-
covering a playful attitude or outlook on life with the claim that rediscovery is a
key element for positive changes (McGhee 1996; Ruch and McGhee 2014).
While playfulness is the foundation of the sense of humor, it is not a
quality specific to humor. McGhee (1996) proposed six genuine humor skills,
namely: enjoyment of humor, laughter, verbal humor, finding humor in every-
day life, laughing at yourself, and humor under stress. In the first version of
the model, these six elements were hierarchically organized from enjoyment of
humor to finding humor under stress, inasmuch as the ones higher in the
hierarchy were assumed to be more difficult to develop. To measure these
skills, McGhee (1996) introduced the sense of humor scale (SHS), which later
(McGhee 1999) underwent a revision. [This revised version is also printed in
McGhee (2010).] Both versions of the SHS were deductively developed. They
differ with respect to the number of items (five vs. four per humor skill) and the
answer format of the response scale (four-point vs. seven-point, 1 = strongly
disagree, 4/7 = strongly agree). The six humor skills are the same in both
versions and may be added together to form a “total sense of humor” (SoH)
score or a “humor quotient”. In the first version of the SHS, this score was also
weighted (i. e., laughing at yourself weighted by a factor of 1.5 and humor
under stress weighted by 2).
A first psychometric evaluation (Ruch and Carrell 1998), which investigated
the SHS along with McGhee’s scales of the foundation of humor (i. e., mental
frame of mind and mood quality) yielded high internal consistencies for an
overall score (SHS and two scales of “seriousness and negative mood” and
“playfulness and positive mood”) in both an American and a German sample
(0.90 to 0.92). However, the five-item skills laughter and enjoyment of humor
yielded Cronbach’s alphas lower than 0.70 (see also Wrench and McCroskey
2001). Furthermore, it was more fruitful to build bipolar scales of positive vs.
negative mood and playful vs. serious attitude. The revision of the scale was
partly successful, as investigated mostly with the German version of the SHS.
The number of items for the SHS humor skills was dropped from five to four
without a substantial loss of reliability for four of the scales; however, enjoy-
ment of humor and laughter again yielded coefficients below 0.70 (Proyer et al.
2010; only for enjoyment of humor in Proyer 2014). Positive vs. negative mood
and playfulness vs. seriousness yielded satisfactory internal consistencies
(Müller and Ruch 2011; Proyer et al. 2010, 2012; Proyer 2014). Also the SoH
score and laughing at yourself proved to be internally consistent in several
studies (Beermann and Ruch 2011; Proyer et al. 2012; Proyer 2014; Ruch and
Heintz 2016).
Several studies investigated the validity of the SHS, but some essential
information is still missing. A principal component analysis of the eight scales
(positive and negative mood/attitude and the SHS) in German and English
samples yielded separate but highly correlated factors for the six humor skills
and the playful vs. serious dimensions (Ruch and Carrell 1998). However, no
factor analysis at the item level has been undertaken yet to see whether the six
concepts can be distinguished. Wrench and McCroskey (2001) conducted a factor
analysis of 48 items (24 SHS items and 24 inverted items) and it “[…] revealed a
strong primary factor” (p. 148), suggesting the use of the total SoH score only.
Thus, it yet remains to be demonstrated that the six humor skills can be
recovered as separate factors.
There is ample evidence that the SHS correlated very highly (0.60– 0.74)
with other humor scales. For example, the SoH score correlated with the
VIA-IS humor scale, the self- and peer rated socially warm (vs. socially cold)
style of humorous conduct, and trait cheerfulness (Müller and Ruch 2011;
Ruch and Carrell 1998; Ruch et al. 2011; Wrench and McCroskey 2001). In
fact, all facets of trait cheerfulness and the six humor skills belonged to the
same factor, suggesting these might be almost interchangeable concepts
(Ruch and Carrell 1998). This was also found for the revised SHS, in which
all scales and the SoH score loaded strongly on a cheerful-engaged playful-
ness factor (Proyer 2014).
Soury and Devillers (2014) found that the SoH score correlated positively
with the frequency and length of recorded laughter in response to four passive
and active laughter-inducing tasks (such as watching funny videos or a tongue-
twister game). Beermann and Ruch (2011) supported the convergent validity of
laughing at yourself with a moderate convergence between self-reports, peer-
reports, and the frequency of observed smiling and laughter in response to
distorted portraits of oneself (see Hofmann, this issue).
Only indirect evidence exists for the idea that there is a hierarchy in the
humor skills. Humor under stress had the lowest mean and enjoyment of humor
had the highest mean, but the proposed rank order was not found for all scales
(Ruch and Carrell 1998). However, Müller and Ruch (2011) found that both
competent (vs. inept) and reflective (vs. boorish) humor styles tended to corre-
late most highly with the components of the sense of humor that are most
difficult to acquire and low to negative with the ones that are easiest to develop.
The ultimate test would be the latency needed to develop these humor skills in a
training setting; this would clarify the best order among the concepts. Similarly,
it is not known whether playfulness (and low seriousness) are “motors” for the
humor skills.
The combination of the six humor skills into the SoH score in both theory
and empirical research is reflective of the early notion that sense of humor is
unidimensional. The SHS was developed when multi-dimensional measure-
ments and models were as yet unknown, as, for instance, the State-Trait-
Cheerfulness-Inventory (STCI; Ruch et al. 1996) and the Humorous Behavior Q-
Sort Deck (Craik et al. 1996). Thus, locating the individual humor skills in a
multidimensional humor space is of interest. A recent multidimensional model
of the sense of humor (Ruch 2012a) was derived from factor analyses of self- and
peer-reports of the Humorous Behavior Q-Sort Deck (Craik et al. 1996). This
model of four sense of humor dimensions entails social fun (being a funny
entertainer and showing prosocial humor in groups), mockery (entailing sar-
casm, cynicism, laughing at others, making fun of others, and criticizing others
humorously), humor ineptness (not being able to tell jokes or to laugh at
oneself, and reacting inappropriately to humor and jokes), and cognitive/reflec-
tive humor (making witty comments and word plays, liking intellectually and
sophisticated humor).
The present study has five aims. First, we test the psychometric properties of
the revised SHS (McGhee 1999): specifically, the reliability (internal consis-
tency and unidimensionality) of the six scales and the SoH score and the
corrected-item total correlations (CITCs) of the 24 items. Second, we develop
and psychometrically test a parallel form of the SHS (SHS-P), a form that also
entails four items per humor skill. We expect the SHS-P to be at least as
psychometrically sound as the SHS. Additionally, the parallel-test reliability
of the humor skills of the SHS and the SHS-P is expected to be sufficiently
high. Although the SHS-P could potentially be used as a stand-alone instru-
ment to measure the six humor skills, we did not intend this usage in the
present study. Instead, our third aim is to combine the items of the SHS and
the SHS-P (resulting in the combined SHS) to allow for a longer and
consequently more reliable and factorially valid measurement of the six humor
skills. Fourth, the overlap of the six humor skills with humor-related measures
(playful vs. serious attitude, positive vs. negative mood, the temperamental
basis of the sense of humor, and four dimensions of the sense of humor) and
life satisfaction is investigated. As attitude and mood were considered the
“motor” of the sense of humor (McGhee 1996, 1999), each humor skill should
positively correlate with playful vs. serious attitude and positive vs. negative
mood. Next, it is tested whether the combined SHS and cheerfulness could be
regarded as more or less interchangeable constructs (as was found by Ruch
and Carrell 1998). Also, we examine whether the humor skills differentially
relate to the other humor-related concepts. Finally, each humor skill and the
SoH score are expected to correlate positively with life satisfaction, in accor-
dance with McGhee’s claims that they would foster positive emotions and
resilience, which should eventually result in enhanced evaluations of one’s
life. Life satisfaction was also deemed relevant because it complements posi-
tive and negative mood (as affective components of subjective well-being) as
the cognitive component of subjective well-being.
2 Methods
2.1 Participants
2.1.1 Sample 1
Overall, 324 participants completed all items of the SHS and the SHS-P. A total of
315 participants (42.9 % men) with a mean age of 36.67 (SD = 14.66) ranging from
18 to 85 years provided valid responses in this study. Participants were primarily
Swiss (67.6 %) or German (18.4 %), but several other nationalities were also
represented (13.9 %). Most participants were well educated, with 53.3 % having
passed tertiary education, 19.0 % having A-levels, 23.4 % having an apprentice-
ship, and 4.4 % having up to nine years of education. Two-thirds (65.7 %) of the
participants were employed.
2.1.2 Sample 2
Overall, 544 participants completed all items of the SHS and the SHS-P. A total
of 542 participants (28.8 % men) with a mean age of 40.45 (SD = 12.86) ranging
2.2 Instruments
The SHS comprises 24 items, four each for the six humor skills (enjoyment of
humor, laughter, verbal humor, finding humor in everyday life, laughing at
oneself, and humor under stress). The scale comprises a seven-point Likert-
like answer format from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (7). The scale
midpoint (4) is labeled “neutral”.
The SHS-P was developed by having graduate psychology students familiar with
test construction write 52 items for the six skills based on McGhee’s descriptions,
taking into account the psychometric properties (i. e., CITCs and item intercor-
relations) and wording of the SHS items. Experts on test construction and humor
supervised the students during this process. The items were adapted and fina-
lized in discussions between the experts and the first author, resulting in 24 new
items (four per humor skill). While the item contents should differ from the SHS
items, each humor skill should be parallel to the SHS (i. e., the SHS-P should
measure the same humor skills as the SHS). The scale comprises the same
1 Results were similar when the two samples were analyzed separately.
answer format as the SHS. The SHS-P items can be obtained by contacting the
authors.
In addition to the SHS, McGhee (1999) also proposed two scales that measure
playful vs. serious attitude and positive vs. negative mood with eight items each.
The scales employ a seven-point Likert-like answer format, with varying answer
options (e. g., from very sad [1] to very happy [7]). The scale midpoint (4) is
always labeled “neutral”. Internal consistencies in the present study (N = 857)
were 0.73 for attitude and 0.86 for mood.
The STCI-T < 60 > is a 60-item questionnaire measuring trait cheerfulness, ser-
iousness, and bad mood using a four-point Likert scale from strongly disagree (1)
to strongly agree (4). Internal consistencies in the present study (n = 177) were
0.94 (cheerfulness), 0.86 (seriousness), and 0.96 (bad mood).
The 4DHS is a short scale containing markers of the four sense of humor
dimensions (Ruch 2012a) with six items each. Sample items are “I have a
reputation of being a funny entertainer” (social fun), “I have an earthy, salty
humor” (mockery), “I cannot laugh about my own weaknesses and failures”
(humor ineptness), and “I like hearing and creating witty wordplays” (cognitive/
reflective humor). The instrument employs a seven-point Likert-scale from
strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (7). Internal consistencies in the present
study (n = 140) were 0.79 (social fun), 0.82 (mockery), 0.54 (humor ineptness),
and 0.73 (cognitive/reflective humor).
2.3 Procedure
3 Results
Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics of the SHS and SHS-P scales to evaluate
their psychometric properties.
As can been seen in Table 1, Cronbach’s alphas of the SHS scales ranged
from 0.56 (enjoyment of humor) to 0.84 (humor under stress) with a median
Table 1: Descriptive statistics and internal consistencies of the scales of the Sense of Humor
Scale (SHS) and the Parallel Form of the SHS (SHS-P).
Enjoyment of humor
SHS . . . −. . . . −.***
SHS-P . . −. . . . −. −.***
Laughter
SHS . . −. . . . . −.**
SHS-P . . −. . . . .*** .
Verbal humor
SHS . . −. −. . . −.*** −.*
SHS-P . . −. . . . −. −.***
Everyday life
SHS . . −. . . . .* −.*
SHS-P . . −. . . . . −.
Laughing at yourself
SHS . . −. . . . . −.
SHS-P . . −. . . . . .
Humor under stress
SHS . . −. −. . . . −.
SHS-P . . −. . . . −. .
Total sense of humor
SHS . . −. . . . . −.**
SHS-P . . −. . . . . −.
Note: N = 857 (n = 856 for age). Everyday life = finding humor in everyday life, Sk = skewness,
K = excess kurtosis, Alpha = Cronbach’s alpha, MIIC = mean inter-item correlation,
rgender = correlation with gender (male = 1, female = 2), rage = correlation with age.*p < 0.05.
**p < 0.01. ***p < 0.001.atheoretical minimum = 1 and maximum = 7.
Table 2: Fit indices of the unidimensionality models of the Sense of Humor Scale (SHS) and the
Parallel Form of the SHS (SHS-P) and range of the standardized loadings.
Enjoyment of humor
SHS .* . . . . .–.
SHS-P .** . . . . .–.
Laughter
SHS . . . . . .–.
SHS-P . . . . . .–.
Verbal humor
SHS .*** . . . . .–.
SHS-P .* . . . . .–.
Everyday life
SHS . . . . . .–.
SHS-P .** . . . . .–.
Laughing at yourself
SHS .*** . . . . .–.
SHS-P . . . . . .–.
Humor under stress
SHS .* . . . . .–.
SHS-P . . . . . .–.
Sense of humor score
SHS .*** . . . . .–.
SHS-P .*** . . . . .–.
Note: N = 857. Everyday life = Finding humor in everyday life.*p < 0.05. **p < 0.01. ***p < 0.001.
Table 3 shows the psychometric properties of the 24 SHS and SHS-P items.
As shown in Table 3, the CITCs were higher than the median of all remaining
correlations with the SHS and SHS-P scales, except for Item 19 of the SHS. This
Table 3: Psychometric properties of the 24 items of the Sense of Humor Scale (SHS) and the
Parallel Form of the SHS (SHS-P).
Enjoyment of humor
. . . . . . . .
. . . . . . . .
. . . . . . . .
. . . . . . . .
Laughter
. . . . . . . .
. . . . . . . .
. . . . . . . .
. . . . . . . .
Verbal humor
. . . . . . . .
. . . . . . . .
. . . . . . . .
. . . . . . . .
Everyday life
. . . . . . . .
. . . . . . . .
. . . . . . . .
. . . . . . . .
Laughing at yourself
. . . . . . . .
. . . . . . . .
. . . . . . . .
. . . . . . . .
Humor under stress
. . . . . . . .
. . . . . . . .
. . . . . . . .
. . . . . . . .
Note: N = 857. Everyday life = Finding humor in everyday life, CITC = corrected item-total corre-
lations, Mdn r = median correlation with the five remaining scales of the SHS or SHS-P,
respectively.
item referred to the importance of having a large amount of humor in one’s life
and reflected SoH in general rather than its specific humor skill (enjoyment of
humor). CITCs ranged from 0.25–0.72 for the SHS items and from 0.43–0.80 for
the SHS-P items, underscoring that they were related to their corresponding
scale, but were not redundant. The median of the CITCs was numerically higher
for the SHS-P (0.66) than for the SHS items (0.58), and the discrimination to the
other scales was slightly better for the SHS-P (median correlation = 0.43) than for
the SHS items (0.48). All items showed sufficient variability (SD > 1) and were
approximately normally distributed. The means across the 24 SHS-P items were
numerically higher (M = 4.96, SD = 1.39) than those of the SHS (M = 4.54,
SD = 1.50). Both means were above the neutral answer option, indicating that
participants – on average – tended to agree with the items.
Given the sufficient psychometric properties of the SHS-P and its high conver-
gence with the SHS, we aimed at combining their items to create more internally
consistent and factorially valid scales of the six humor skills. For this purpose,
Item 19 (SHS enjoyment of humor) was excluded, as it would have undermined
the separation of the six skills (see Table 3). Table 5 shows the descriptive
Scales () () () () () () () () () () () () ()
Enjoyment of humor
() SHS
() SHS-P .
Laughter
() SHS . .
() SHS-P . . .
Verbal humor
() SHS . . . .
() SHS-P . . . . .
Everyday life
() SHS . . . . . .
() SHS-P . . . . . . .
Laughing at yourself
() SHS . . . . . . . .
() SHS-P . . . . . . . . .
Humor under stress
() SHS . . . . . . . . . .
() SHS-P . . . . . . . . . . .
Total sense of humor
() SHS . . . . . . . . . . . .
Sense of Humor Scale
SHS-P . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Note: N = 857. Everyday life = finding humor in everyday life. Parallel-test reliabilities marked in italics.All correlations p < 0.001.
Table 5: Descriptive statistics, CITC ranges, internal consistencies, and scale intercorrelations
of the combined sense of humor scale.
Combined Sense M SD CITC range () () () () () () ()
of Humor
Scales
Note: N = 857. CITC = corrected item-total correlation. Cronbach’s alpha in italics.All correlations
p < 0.001.
I 01
Enjoyment
…
of humor
I 19
I 02
Laughter
…
Item 01 I 20
Item 02 I 03
Verbal
…
Item 03 humor
Sense of I 21 Sense of
…
humor I 04 humor
Everyday
…
Item 45
I 22 life
Item 46
I 05
Item 47 Laughing
… at yourself
I 23
I 06
Humor un-
…
I 24 der stress
I 01 I 01
Enjoyment Enjoyment
…
…
of humor of humor
I 19 I 19
I 02 I 02
Laughter Laughter
…
I 20 I 20
I 03 I 03
Verbal Verbal
…
humor humor
I 21 Sense of I 21
I 04 humor I 04
Everyday Everyday
…
I 22 life life
I 22
I 05 I 05
Laughing Laughing
…
I 23 at yourself at yourself
I 23
I 06 I 06
Humor un- Humor un-
…
Figure 1: Depiction of the four structural models of the combined Sense of Humor Scale tested
in the CFA. Everyday life = finding humor in everyday life.
Table 6: Fit indices of the CFA models of the humor skills of the combined Sense of Humor
Scale.
humor and verbal humor) and 0.62 (finding humor in everyday life and humor
under stress; median = 0.41). Despite measurement error being accounted for,
this median is numerically lower than the median of the observed intercorrela-
tions of the six humor skills, indicating that the general SoH factor contributed
to the relationships between the humor skills.
Playful vs. serious attitude .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** . .***
Positive vs. negative mood .* .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** . .***
STCI-T
Cheerfulness .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** . .***
Seriousness −.** −.*** −.** −.** −.*** −.*** .* . −.***
Bad mood −. −.** −.*** −.*** −.*** −.*** .*** . −.***
R .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .***
R . . . . . . .
DHS
Social fun .** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** . .***
Mockery . . .*** .* . . .* . .*
Humor ineptness .* −.* −. −.* −.*** −.** .*** . −.*
Cognitive/reflective .** .** .** .** .** .** .*** . .**
R .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .***
R . . . . . . .
SWLS .* .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** .*** . .***
Sense of Humor Scale
Note: N = 857 (attitude and mood), n = 177 (STCI-T), n = 140 (4DHS), and n = 513 (SWLS). Enjoy = enjoyment of humor, Verbal = verbal humor,
Everyday = finding humor in everyday life, LAY = laughing at yourself, Stress = humor under stress.*p < 0.05. **p < 0.01. ***p < 0.001.
similar). Overlaps with bad mood were also large, while they were somewhat
smaller with seriousness.
The 4DHS explained 13 % (enjoyment of humor) to 62 % (verbal humor) of
the variance in the combined SHS scales (median = 32 %). All combined SHS
scales correlated significantly and positively with social fun and cognitive/
reflective humor. Verbal humor, finding humor in everyday life, and the SoH
score also correlated positively with mockery, although these relationships were
numerically smaller than the correlations with social fun and cognitive/reflec-
tive humor. Most combined SHS scales also showed significant negative correla-
tions to humor ineptness, while verbal humor was uncorrelated with it and
enjoyment of humor correlated positively with it. The six combined SHS scales
overlapped most strongly with social fun, cognitive/reflective humor, and
humor ineptness, and to a lesser extent with mockery.
Lastly and as expected, the relationships with life satisfaction were positive
and significant for all combined SHS scales (median = 0.26). Still the size of
relationships varied across the humor skills, ranging from 0.10 (enjoyment of
humor) to 0.32 (laughter). Comparing the correlation of the SoH score and the
multiple correlations across the six humor skills across all outcomes, it can be
seen that the overlap was always numerically larger for the six skills than the
SoH score. This further supports the importance of taking the individual skills
into account.
4 Discussion
The first aim of the present study was to examine the psychometric properties of
the revised SHS (McGhee 1999). The internal consistencies of all of the skills and
the total score were > 0.70 with the exception of enjoyment of humor (0.56) and
laughter (0.65). This is in accord with previous findings (Proyer et al. 2010).
Unidimensionality (or homogeneity), as tested in the CFA, was supported for
four humor skills (enjoyment of humor, laughter, finding humor in everyday life,
and humor under stress), but not the SoH score. This implies that it is worth-
while to study the SHS humor skills in more detail, as they cannot be mapped
onto a single SoH factor. At the item level, the CITCs were sufficient and larger
than the correlations with the other humor skills. The only exception was Item
19 (“It is important for me to have a lot of humor in my life”), which was less
representative of the humor skill it was assigned to (enjoyment of humor), but
was related to SoH in general. Overall, the SHS scales had mixed psychometric
properties, with the SoH score being internally consistent (though not
six-factor model, indicating that once general sense of humor was accounted for,
the two humor skills were essentially unrelated to one another. Passively seeking
out humorous stimuli and actively producing humor can thus be regarded as
independent skills. By contrast, the correlation between finding humor in every-
day life and humor under stress dropped only slightly from 0.75 to 0.62. Thus
these two humor skills were similar beyond general sense of humor, and they also
make use of similar skills (e. g., humor under stress could be conceived as finding
humor in stressful situations, representing a more elaborate and specific humor
skill similar to finding humor in everyday life).
The fourth aim was to investigate the overlap of the six humor skills with
humor-related measures and life satisfaction. Each humor skill and the SoH
score correlated significantly and positively with playful vs. serious attitude and
positive vs. negative mood, supporting McGhee’s (1999) idea that they represent
the “motor” of the sense of humor. The six combined SHS humor skills showed
an overlap of 74 % with cheerfulness, which supports the notion that they are
highly related and almost interchangeable concepts (as proposed by Ruch and
Carrell 1998). In addition, each SHS humor skill could be differentially located
within other humor-related concepts: They correlated positively and signifi-
cantly with social fun and cognitive/reflective humor, and negatively with
seriousness. All humor skills except enjoyment of humor correlated negatively
with bad mood. Verbal humor and finding humor in everyday life correlated
positively with mockery. Finally, enjoyment of humor correlated positively with
humor ineptness, verbal humor was uncorrelated with it, and the other four
humor skills correlated negatively with it. Thus, each humor skills covered
slightly different aspects of the sense of humor dimensions, and also the sizes
of their relationships varied. For example, laughing at yourself correlated more
strongly with humor ineptness than humor under stress did (−0.40 and −0.22,
respectively). Lastly, every humor skill and the SoH score were found to corre-
late positively with life satisfaction, with the smallest effects emerging for
enjoyment of humor and verbal humor and the strongest ones occurring for
laughter and laughing at yourself.
Overall, both the pattern of correlations across the six humor skills and the
size of the relationships varied. Similar effects can be expected for other mea-
sures of humor and well-being, which should be investigated in future research.
Importantly, assessing the individual humor skills allows evaluating the effec-
tiveness of practicing each humor skill in McGhee’s humor training program. For
example, it could show which of them (and their improvements by the humor
training) are more or less relevant for obtaining well-being outcomes. The
current study merely describes the existence of a relationship; causality needs
to be established in an experimental design.
4.2 Conclusions
Sample 1. We would also like to thank Frank Appletree Rodden for his helpful
comments on an earlier version of this manuscript and Moritz Meyer for drawing
Figure 1.
References
Beermann, Ursula & Willibald Ruch. 2011. How virtuous is humor? What we can learn from
current instruments. The Journal of Positive Psychology 4. 528–539.
Craik, Kenneth, Martin D. Lampert & Arvalea Nelson 1996. Sense of humor and styles of
everyday humorous conduct. Humor: International Journal of Humor Research 9. 273–302.
Diener, Ed, Robert A. Emmons, Randy J. Larsen & Sharon Griffin 1985. The satisfaction with life
scale. Journal of Personality Assessment 49(1). 71–75.
McGhee, Paul E. 1979. Humor: Its origin and development. San Francisco: WH Freeman and
Company.
McGhee, Paul E. 1996. Health, healing and the amuse system: Humor as survival training.
Dubuque: Kendall/Hunt.
McGhee, Paul E. 1999. Health, healing and the amuse system: Humor as survival training, 3rd
edn. Dubuque: Kendall/Hunt.
McGhee, Paul E. 2010. Humor as survival training for a stressed-out world: The 7 humor habits
program. Bloomington: AuthorHouse.
Müller, Liliane & Willibald Ruch. 2011. Humor and strengths of character. The Journal of Positive
Psychology 6(5). 368–376.
Proyer, Rene T. 2014. A psycho-linguistic approach for studying adult playfulness: A replication
and extension toward relations with humor. The Journal of Psychology 148. 717–735.
Proyer, Rene T., Rahel Flisch, Stefanie Tschupp, Tracey Platt & Willibald Ruch. 2012. How does
psychopathy relate to humor and laughter? Dispositions toward ridicule and being
laughed at, the sense of humor, and psychopathic personality traits. International Journal
of Law and Psychiatry 35. 263–268.
Proyer, Rene T., Willibald Ruch & Liliane MüLler. 2010. Sense of humor among the elderly:
Findings with the German version of the SHS. Zeitschrift für Gerontologie und Geriatrie 43.
19–24.
R Core Team. 2015. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna:
R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
Rosseel, Yves. 2012. lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of
Statistical Software 48(2). 1–36.
Ruch, Willibald. 2012a. Towards a new structural model of the sense of humor: Preliminary
findings. AAAI Fall Symposium: Artificial Intelligence of Humor 2. 68–75.
Ruch, Willibald. 2012b. Four dimensions of humor scale. University of Zurich Unpublished
manual.
Ruch, Willibald & Amy Carrell. 1998. Trait cheerfulness and the sense of humor. Personality and
Individual Differences 24. 551–558.
Ruch, Willibald, Gabriele Köhler & Christoph van Thriel. 1996. Assessing the “humorous
temperament”: Construction of the facet and standard trait forms of the State-Trait-
Cheerfulness-Inventory – STCI. Humor: International Journal of Humor Research 9.
303–339.
Ruch, Willibald & Sonja Heintz. 2016. The virtue gap in humor: Exploring benevolent and
corrective humor. Translational Issues in Psychological Science 2. 35–45.
Ruch, Willibald & Paul E. McGhee. 2014. Humor intervention programs. In Acacia C. Parks &
Stephen M. Schueller (eds.), The Wiley Blackwell handbook of positive psychological
interventions, 179–193. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.
Ruch, Willibald, Rene T. Proyer, Claudia Esser & Otilia Mitrache. 2011. Cheerfulness and
everyday humorous conduct. In Romanian Academy (eds.), Yearbook of the ‘George Baritiu’
Institute of History in Cluj-Napoca, 67–87. Bucharest: Romanian Academy Publishing
House.
Schermelleh-Engel, Karin, Helfried Moosbrugger & Hans Müller. 2003. Evaluating the fit of
structural equation models: Tests of significance and descriptive goodness-of-fit mea-
sures. Methods of Psychological Research Online 8(2). 23–74.
Soury, Mariette & Laurence Devillers. 2014. Smile and laughter in human-machine interaction:
A study of engagement. In Nicoletta Calzolari, Khalid Choukri, Thierry Declerck, Hrafn
Loftsson, Bente Maegaard, Joseph Mariani, Asuncion Moreno, Jan Odijk & Stelios Piperidis
(eds.), Proceedings of the ninth international conference on Language Resources and
Evaluation (LREC’14), 3633–3637. Paris: European Language Resources Association.
Wrench, Jason S. & James C. McCroskey 2001. A temperamental understanding of humor
communication and exhilaratability. Communication Quarterly 49(2). 142–159.
Bionotes
Willibald Ruch
Willibald Ruch is a Full Professor of Psychology at the University of Zurich, Switzerland. His
research interests are in the field of personality and assessment, with a special focus on humor
and laughter, cheerfulness, and smiling. In his doctoral dissertation at the University of Graz
(Austria) in 1980, he developed a taxonomy of jokes and cartoons and studied their relation to
personality. His more recent work, together with his research team at the University of Zurich,
includes humor from a positive psychology perspective, the effectiveness of humor training
programs and clown interventions, the ability to laugh at oneself, the fear of being laughed at
(gelotophobia), and the measurement of humor.
Sonja Heintz