Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 13 April 2018
Sec. Neurogenomics

Adar3 Is Involved in Learning and Memory in Mice

\r\nDessislava Mladenova,Dessislava Mladenova1,2Guy Barry,&#x;Guy Barry1,2Lyndsey M. Konen,,Lyndsey M. Konen1,3,4Sandy S. Pineda,Sandy S. Pineda1,2Boris Guennewig,&#x;Boris Guennewig1,2Lotta AvessonLotta Avesson1Raphael Zinn,,Raphael Zinn1,3,4Nicole Schonrock,Nicole Schonrock1,2Maina Bitar,&#x;Maina Bitar1,2Nicky Jonkhout,Nicky Jonkhout1,2Lauren CrumlishLauren Crumlish5Dominik C. KaczorowskiDominik C. Kaczorowski1Andrew Gong&#x;Andrew Gong1Mark Pinese,Mark Pinese1,2Gloria R. FrancoGloria R. Franco6Carl R. WalkleyCarl R. Walkley7Bryce Vissel,,Bryce Vissel1,3,4John S. Mattick,*John S. Mattick1,2*
  • 1Garvan Institute of Medical Research, Sydney, NSW, Australia
  • 2St. Vincent's Clinical School, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW, Australia
  • 3Centre for Neuroscience and Regenerative Medicine, Faculty of Science, University of Technology Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
  • 4St. Vincent's Centre for Applied Medical Research (AMR), Sydney, NSW, Australia
  • 5Institute for Molecular Bioscience, The University of Queensland, St. Lucia, QLD, Australia
  • 6Departamento de Bioquímica e Imunologia, Instituto de Ciências Biológicas, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais, Belo Horizonte, Brazil
  • 7St. Vincent's Institute of Medical Research, Department of Medicine, St. Vincent's Hospital, The University of Melbourne, Fitzroy, VIC, Australia

The amount of regulatory RNA encoded in the genome and the extent of RNA editing by the post-transcriptional deamination of adenosine to inosine (A-I) have increased with developmental complexity and may be an important factor in the cognitive evolution of animals. The newest member of the A-I editing family of ADAR proteins, the vertebrate-specific ADAR3, is highly expressed in the brain, but its functional significance is unknown. In vitro studies have suggested that ADAR3 acts as a negative regulator of A-I RNA editing but the scope and underlying mechanisms are also unknown. Meta-analysis of published data indicates that mouse Adar3 expression is highest in the hippocampus, thalamus, amygdala, and olfactory region. Consistent with this, we show that mice lacking exon 3 of Adar3 (which encodes two double stranded RNA binding domains) have increased levels of anxiety and deficits in hippocampus-dependent short- and long-term memory formation. RNA sequencing revealed a dysregulation of genes involved in synaptic function in the hippocampi of Adar3-deficient mice. We also show that ADAR3 transiently translocates from the cytoplasm to the nucleus upon KCl-mediated activation in SH-SY5Y cells. These results indicate that ADAR3 contributes to cognitive processes in mammals.

Introduction

The human brain has evolved to enable unique cognitive capabilities and has tripled in size since the split from the chimpanzee lineage around 5 million years ago. Current hypotheses suggest that the advancement of human cognition most likely arose through the combined effects of the expansion of brain size and complexity, protein evolution (including new splice variants) and the emergence of RNA-based regulatory mechanisms that facilitate the epigenetic reformation of neural circuitry in response to experience (Barry and Mattick, 2012).

One of the RNA-diversification/regulatory mechanisms is RNA editing. Two main types of RNA editing are known, one involving the deamination of cytidine to create uridine (C-to-U), and the other, the deamination of adenosine to inosine (A-to-I). RNA editing not only alters the nucleotide sequence of target RNAs, but presumably also their structure-function relationships and interactions (Bass, 2002; Jantsch and Öhman, 2008).

The existence of RNA editing in mammals was first discovered in mRNAs for important neuroreceptors, such as the glutamate and serotonin receptors, and was initially thought to be an interesting but idiosyncratic mechanism to change their amino acid sequence (Maas et al., 2003), presumably to alter the electrophysiological properties of synapses in response to activity or other cues. Subsequently, transcriptome-wide analyses revealed that A-I editing is widespread in humans, occurring in thousands of transcripts, mostly in Alu sequences within intronic and intergenic sequences, varying in different tissues (Athanasiadis et al., 2004; Blow et al., 2004; Kim et al., 2004; Levanon et al., 2004; Ramaswami et al., 2012; Huntley et al., 2016). Although operating via a conserved mechanism (Jin et al., 2009), the rates of A-to-I editing have increased dramatically throughout vertebrate, mammalian and especially primate evolution, with RNA editing in humans being more than an order of magnitude higher than in mouse (Kim et al., 2004; Levanon et al., 2004). Moreover, more editing occurs in the human brain than in other primates (Paz-Yaacov et al., 2010).

Adenosine Deaminase Acting on RNA (ADAR) proteins, are responsible for the execution of the A-to-I RNA editing through hydrolytic deamination (Bass, 2002). Three ADAR enzymes (ADAR1-3) are encoded in the vertebrate genome, with ADAR3 being vertebrate-specific (Chen et al., 2000). Common to all ADARs is a C-terminal catalytic domain and multiple double-stranded RNA binding domains (Nishikura, 2010). In particular, ADAR3 shares 50% amino-acid sequence identity with ADAR2 (Melcher et al., 1996a) and is almost exclusively expressed in the nervous system, but its role is unknown. Unlike the other ADAR proteins, ADAR3 contains a novel arginine rich motif (herein, R-domain), which allows the binding of single stranded RNA (Chen et al., 2000), activity that may result in novel functions. Based on in vitro evidence, ADAR3 is suggested to act as a dominant-negative regulator of A-to-I RNA editing (Chen et al., 2000). The R-domain has also been proposed to serve as a functional nuclear localization signal (NLS) as it mediates interactions between ADAR3 and the Importin α protein complex enabling ADAR3 to locate to the nucleus (Maas and Gommans, 2009) where A-to-I RNA editing is believed to occur (Jin et al., 2009).

Due to the novelty and relatively unknown function of ADAR3 and its high expression in the nervous system, we investigated the role of this protein in cognition and behavior in ADAR3 deficient mice. We also demonstrate that mice lacking exon 3 of Adarb2 (referred to herein as Adar3exon3), containing the double stranded RNA binding domains, display increased anxiety levels and impaired short and long-term hippocampus-dependent memory formation. RNA sequencing of mouse hippocampal tissue indicates roles for Adar3 in synaptic function. Finally, our results also show that ADAR3 transiently translocates to the nucleus in response to neuronal activation in SH-SY5Y cells. Collectively, our data strongly suggest that ADAR3 is essential for correct cognitive functioning of the mammalian brain.

Materials and Methods

Mouse Lines: Generation of Adar3exon3 Mice

ES cells containing the targeted Adarb2tm1a(KOMP)Mbp allele (IKMC project number 39714; hereafter referred to as Adar3tm1a) were generated by the trans-NIH Knock-Out Mouse Project (KOMP) and obtained from the KOMP Repository (www.komp.org). The Adarb2tm1a(KOMP)Mbp allele contains a splice acceptor-beta-geo-polyA (SA-βgeo-pA) flanked by FRT sites located in intron 2 and loxP elements flanking exon 3 (KOMP designation: KO first allele (reporter-tagged insertion with conditional potential). Correct targeting was confirmed and transgenic mice were generated from embryonic stem cell clones DEPD0006_14_A03 & DEPD0006_14_A05. Animals were generated by the Australian Phenomics Network ES to Mouse service at Monash University. Positive mice were backcrossed to C57BL/6N background and genotyped by PCR. Primers P1 (located at the 3′ end of the 5′ homology arm) and P2 (located at the 5′ end of exon ENSMUSE00000465454) will amplify a product of 554 bp from the wild type allele. Primers P1 and P3 (located in the en-2 intron) will amplify a product of 246 bp from the targeted allele. P1 = 5′ CAATATACCACAACGAACATCTTTG 3′; P2 = 5′ GTCCCCAGGTTGCTCACATTTCG 3′; P3 = 5′ CAACGGGTTCTTCTGTTAGTCC 3′. PCR conditions were: an initial denaturation step at 94°C 3 min, followed by 35 cycles of 94°C 30 s, 57°C 30 s, 72°C 45 s and a final cycle of 72°C for 5 min.

Heterozygous Adar3tm1a animals (identified in-house as ADAR2BlacZ line) were crossed with heterozygotes of a ubiquitous expressing Cre-recombinase mouse line (C57BL/6NTac-Gt(ROSA)26Sortm16(cre)Arte; Taconic). The Cre-recombinase gene was identified using the following primers, which yielded a 408 bp fragment: forward 5′ GCATTACCGGTCGATGCAACGAGTGATGAG 3′; and reverse 5′ GAGTGAACGAACCTGGTCGAAATCAGTGCG 3′. Thus, in addition to expressing Cre-recombinase, double heterozygous mice from the resulting cross, possessed one WT allele and one allele in which Cre-mediated deletion of the loxP-flanked portion of the Adar3 gene had occurred. To maintain the strain background and breed out the Cre-recombinase allele, double heterozygous animals were then crossed back to WT mice from the ADAR2BlacZ line. Resulting heterozygous mice for the KO and also Cre-negative, were used to maintain the colony by breeding heterozygous by heterozygous. Homozygotes possess a non-functional ADAR3 protein and were thus referred to here as Adar3exon3.

Histology

Mice were deeply anesthetized and perfused transcardially with 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in PBS. Brains were post-fixed in 4% PFA (in PBS) overnight and then cryoprotected in 30% sucrose. Tissue was cryosectioned into 40 μm thick coronal slices and stored at 4°C in PBS + 0.02% sodium azide. Free-floating sections were mounted onto gelatin-coated slides and air-dried prior to cresyl violet staining in a 0.1% solution (ProSciTech, QLD, Australia) according to standard protocols.

Behavioral Testing

The following testing order was performed on all mice: open field testing in the morning of Day 1; elevated plus maze in the afternoon of Day 1; rotarod test 1 on Day 2; rotarod test 2 on Day 3; rotarod test 3 on Day 4; and Y-maze testing on Day 5. Animals used in fear conditioning experiments were not tested in any other behavioral test.

Open Field Test

Motor activity and anxiety were evaluated in the open field test (OFT). Mice were placed in the center of the OFT arena (40 × 40 cm; Med Associates, Georgia, VT, USA), which was a large box with clear Plexiglas walls, no ceiling, and a white floor. Each chamber was set inside a larger sound-attenuating cubicle and with two ceiling mounted house lights at the rear corners to fully illuminate the cubicle. A small fan mounted at the top of the right wall of each chamber provided background noise. Testing chambers were cleaned with 70% ethanol (EtOH) before each run to prevent the transfer of odors between animals. Mice were allowed to freely explore the test box for 10 min while a computer software program (Activity Monitor, Med Associates, Georgia, VT, USA) recorded activity via photo-beam detection inside the testing chambers. Data collected includes total distance traveled, distance traveled in the center and periphery of test box, and time spent in the center and periphery.

Elevated Plus Maze

Following the OFT, anxiety-like behavior was assessed using the elevated plus maze (EPM). The EPM apparatus (Med Associates, Georgia, VT, USA) used in this study consisted of two platforms (77 × 10 cm) opposite each other and two platforms (77 × 10 cm) enclosed by three 20 cm high walls opposite each other so that they form a plus shape. The maze was elevated 70 cm off the floor. Mice were placed at the center of the maze facing one of the closed arms and allowed to explore freely for 5 min. Each test was video recorded and anxiety-like behavior including the number of entries into each arm and time spent in each arm were measured. The maze was wiped cleaned with 70% EtOH and dried between each mouse.

Rotarod

Mice were placed on the suspended beam of the rotarod facing away from the viewer for 5 min. The rotarod was started once all mice were placed on the beams and rotated at a rate of 4 rpm and increased to 40 rpm over the course of 5 min. Animals were taken off the rotarod once they fell to the catch tray below or after 5 min had elapsed and the latency to fall off the beam was recorded. Animals were exposed to the test once a day for three consecutive days. The device was cleaned with 70% EtOH between each mouse.

Y-Maze

The Y-maze was adapted from a previously described protocol (Wolf et al., 2016), with slight modification. Testing was conducted in an opaque plexiglas Y-maze consisting of three arms (40 × 4 × 17 cm high) diverging at a 120° angle. Each mouse was placed in the center of the Y-maze and allowed to explore freely through the maze during a 5-min session. The experimenter remained in the room but was not visible to the test mouse. The sequence and total number of arms entered was video recorded. Arm entry was counted when the hind paws of the mouse had been completely placed in the arm. The percent alternation was calculated as the number of trials containing entries into all three arms divided by the maximum possible alternations (the total number of arms entered minus 2) × 100. The maze was cleaned with 70% EtOH and dried between each mouse.

Contextual Fear Conditioning Experiments

In contextual fear conditioning, animals learn to associate an aversive foot shock (unconditional stimulus; US) with the neutral context in which it was elicited (conditional stimulus; CS). This learning is thought to lead to the formation of a CS-US associative memory which, when retrieved through re-exposure to the context in which conditioning occurred, leads to expression of conditional fear (displayed as freezing behavior in mice). This fear expression is a direct correlate of contextual memory (Maren et al., 2013).

On day one of fear conditioning, mice were placed into a fear-conditioning chamber (32 × 27 × 26 cm; Med Associates, Georgia, VT, USA). The chamber had aluminum sides and clear plexiglas back, top, and front walls, with the front being a hinged door. The floor of the chambers consisted of a metal grid with 36 stainless steel rods spaced 8 mm apart and connected to a current generator for delivery of an electric foot shock. There was also a removable metal tray underneath the grid. Attached to the left wall of each chamber was a small house light measuring 3.8 cm2 containing a light bulb that emitted 780 lux at a distance of 2.5 cm and covered by a diffuser. The foot-shock and house light were controlled by a computer software program (FreezeFrame, Med Associates, Georgia, VT, USA). For all fear conditioning experiments, the chambers were setup with the following parameters: (1) chambers were cleaned with 70% EtOH and the grid floor dried; (2) the tray was scented with aniseed essence; and (3) the test room lighting was set at full fluorescence.

On conditioning day, mice were placed into the chambers and after a placement-to-shock interval (PSI) of 3 min a single 2 s 1.0 mA foot shock was delivered. Mice were removed from the chambers 30 s after the shock ended and returned to their home cages. For short-term contextual memory assessment, animals were placed back into the conditioning context 2 h post-conditioning for a 3 min test in which no foot shock was elicited. Upon completion of the context test, mice were removed from the fear conditioning chambers and placed back into their home cages. A separate cohort of animals was used for long-term and remote contextual memory assessment. Animals were conditioned as described above. At 24 h post-conditioning, mice were placed back into the conditioning context for a 3 min test in which no foot shock was elicited. Upon completion of the context test, mice were removed from the fear conditioning chambers and placed back into their home cages. To test remote contextual memory, the same animals were re-tested in the conditioning context for 3 min at 3 weeks post-conditioning.

All fear conditioning experiments were video-recorded and the conditioned fear responses measured by using the number of times the animal was determined to be freezing by observing it every 4 s over the length of the testing period and converting that number into a percentage of time spent freezing over the total time. Freezing behavior is characterized by the cessation of movement except that needed for respiration (Fanselow and Poulos, 2005). Freezing was scored by one investigator, who was blind to the treatment groups.

RNA Extraction

Three wild type and three Adar3exon3 mice were used for RNA-seq experiments. Briefly, each individual was deeply anesthetized before harvesting and dissecting the hippocampal tissue. Total RNA was extracted from mouse hippocampus, using the AllPrep DNA/RNA Mini Kit (QIAGEN, Hilden, Germany) with an on-column DNAse treatment step (to remove genomic DNA), following the manufacturer's instructions. Mouse tissue was homogenized using the Qiagen TissueLyser II according to the manufacturer's instructions. Isolated RNA quality (integrity) was assessed on an Agilent RNA 6000 Nano Chip (Agilent Technologies, Santa Clara, CA, USA) using the Agilent 2100 BioAnalyser (Agilent Technologies, Santa Clara, CA, USA), following the BioAnalyser instructions. RNA concentration was quantified using NanoDrop 2000TM Spectrophotometer (Thermo Scientific, Waltham, MA).

Library Preparation and RNAseq

Libraries were prepared with TruSeq Stranded Total RNA Library Prep Kit with Ribo-Zero (Illumina, San Diego, CA, USA). Libraries were quantified with Qubit™ 3.0 Fluorometer dsDNA HS kit (ThermoFisher Scientific Inc., Waltham, MA, USA). Indexed DNA libraries were analyzed individually using an Agilent Technologies 2100 Bioanalyzer with the DNA 1000 kit (Agilent Technologies, Santa Clara, CA, USA). Libraries were diluted and pooled to a final concentration of 10 nM. Pooled libraries were quantitated with Qubit™ 3.0 Fluorometer dsDNA HS kit (ThermoFisher Scientific Inc., Waltham, MA, USA). RNA sequencing was performed using Illumina HiSeq 2500 System with 100 bp paired-end sequencing.

Analysis of RNA Editing

Additional description of the Bioinformatics analysis approach is included in Data Sheet 1 and Data Sheet 2. Briefly, trimmed paired-end RNA-seq reads were aligned with bowtie v1.1.0 (http://bowtie-bio.sourceforge.net/index.shtml) (Langmead et al., 2009) to the mouse reference genome (mm10, Genome Reference Consortium GRCm38). A-to-G mismatches were called with Samtools v1.12 (http://www.htslib.org/doc/samtools.html) (Li et al., 2009). The positions of common SNPs from dbSNPs138 were removed.

Only positions covered by at least 20 reads with the A-to-G mismatch found on average 5% of the reads were retained. Candidate editing sites were inspected for Strand Bias, Variant Distance Bias and Read Position Bias (Supplementary Methods). To estimate the significance of A-to-I editing level changes with age, we fitted the changes of A-to-G substitution frequency for the 282 filtered high-confidence candidate editing sites (Data Sheet 2) using the logistic regression model. The logistic regression model (function glm in R, v3.2.3) was used to fit the A-to-G substitution frequency for the 282 filtered high-confidence candidate-editing sites with genotype as a predictor. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to obtain which edited sites showed significant change with genotype, applying a false discovery rate threshold of 5%.

Differential Gene Expression Analysis

Trimmed paired-end reads were aligned against assembly GRCm38.p4 of the mouse genome with STAR v2.5.1a (Dobin et al., 2013) using a pre-built index based on GENCODE m10. Statistics and quality control of the alignment was performed with RSeQC v2.6.1 (Wang et al., 2012). Quantification of the aligned reads was performed with RSEM v1.2.26 (Li and Dewey, 2011). Genes with low counts were filtered out and only genes with 10 counts in at least 2 samples (6 in total) were considered in the downstream analysis in R v3.2.3 (Retrieved from http://www.R-project.org).

Transcript normalization was performed using the trimmed mean of M values (TMM) applied through the edgeR package v3.14 (Robinson et al., 2010). An identifier was called significant if its false discovery rate is below 0.01 after Benjamini-Hochberg correction. The differentially expressed transcripts are additionally annotated through the BioMart portal (http://www.biomart.org/).

Targeted Deep Re-sequencing

Candidate editing sites found to have statistically significant differences in editing levels between the WT and Adar3exon3 (9 loci containing 10 editing sites), along with three additional well-characterized editing targets (serotonin receptor 5ht2cr, Blcap and Gabra3) were selected for targeted re-sequencing and were amplified using PCR.

Briefly, 1 μg RNA was used to synthesize cDNA with the SuperScriptTM IV First-Strand Synthesis System for RT-PCR kit (Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA, USA). PCR was performed in a multi-well plate using one primer pair per sample (mouse), the primer sequences and the PCR conditions used for the PCR reactions are detailed in Supplementary Table 2. PCR products were electrophoresed on a 3% agarose gel, to confirm expected product size and the absence of additional bands. All amplicons were quantitated individually using a Qubit™ 3.0 Fluorometer dsDNA HS kit (ThermoFisher Scientific Inc., Waltham, MA, USA). Subsequently the nM concentration for each amplicon was calculated. The 12 PCR amplicons for each sample (mouse) were pooled in equimolar ratios. The pooled PCR products were purified using a MinElute PCR Purification Kit (QIAGEN, Hilden, Germany). 200 ng of each pool was used for library preparation with NEBNext Ultra II DNA Library Prep Kit for Illumina (New England Biolabs Inc, Ipswich, MA, USA; E7645S) with the NEBNext Multioplex Oligos for Illumina (Index Primers Set 1; E7335S) according to the manufacturer's instructions.

The quality and quantification control was performed with a LabChipGX (Perkin Elmer, Waltham, MA, USA) and the KAPA Library Quantification Kit (KAPA Biosystems, Wilmington, MA, USA) with the ViiA7TM Real-Time PCR System (ThermoFisher Scientific Inc., Waltham, MA, USA). 7pM library plus 10% PhiX spike-in control were loaded on MiSeq System (Illumina, San Diego, CA, USA) using the MiSeq Reagent Kit v2, 300 Cycles (Illumina). RNA editing analysis was performed on both paired and unpaired reads mapped to the mouse genome (mm10, Genome Reference Consortium GRCm38) with bowtie1 v1.1.0 (http://bowtie-bio.sourceforge.net/index.shtml) as described above. All potential editing sites were manually inspected using the Integrative Genomics Viewer (IGV v2.3, http://software.broadinstitute.org/software/igv/) (Robinson et al., 2011). A-to-G mismatches in high-quality score bases with frequency of 1% or greater were recorded (Data Sheet 2) and analyzed as shown in Supplementary Figure 2).

Serotonin Receptor Editing Analysis

The serotonin receptor 5HT2CR is known to be edited on 5 closely positioned adenosines on exon 5, i.e., ATACGTAATCCTA (highlighted in bold). The editing of all or any combination of these adenosines can result in 32 mRNA variants. To investigate the editing levels at each adenosine position in exon 5 and all its possible combinations, an in-house script was used to generate all possible combinations of the amplified 178 bp PCR 5HT2CR product (chrX:147169590-147169767). Based on these 32 variants, 32 “artificial serotonin chromosomes” were created (Data Sheet 3) and indexed with bowtie1 v1.1.0. (http://bowtie-bio.sourceforge.net/index.shtml) Reads from the targeted deep re-sequencing experiment were aligned to all possible “artificial serotonin chromosomes” with bowtie1 v1.1.0. Bedtools v2.22.0 (Quinlan and Hall, 2010) was used to count the number of reads aligning to each of the 32 possible “artificial serotonin chromosomes.” Statistical significance was determined using a one-way ANOVA in R v3.2.3.

qPCR Validations

To further validate hits with significant difference between WT and Adar3exon3, qPCR reactions were also performed. Briefly, 200 ng of RNA extracted from the right hippocampus was used for a cDNA synthesis. RNA was converted into cDNA in a 20 μl reaction with the SuperScriptTM III First- Strand Synthesis System for RT-PCR kit (Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA, USA), following the manufacturer's instructions. For qPCR, 3 μl cDNA as obtained above was used (diluted 1:20). Negative controls included a non-template (water) control and a reaction without reverse transcriptase added. Reactions were performed using UPL assays (Roche Applied Science, Basel, Switzerland) with LightCycler 480 Probes Master, on the Roche LightCycler 480 (Roche Applied Science, Basel, Switzerland).

Samples were amplified using the standard qPCR program for the Roche LightCycler 480 program as follows: initial denaturation at 95°C 10 min, followed by 40 cycles of: 95°C for 10 s, 60°C for 30 s and 72°C 5 s. The threshold cycle (Ct) was set using the LightCycler 480 Software v1.5.0. (Roche Applied Science, Basel, Switzerland). Amplification efficiency was determined by including a standard curve of cDNA serial dilutions for each primer set. All tested primers (target and standard house-keeping genes) had comparable amplification efficiency (Supplementary Table 3). Gene expression analysis was performed with the comparative Ct method (Schmittgen et al., 2008). All primer sets used are detailed in Supplementary Table 3. Statistical significance was determined with two-tailed unpaired t-test in GraphPad Prism v7.00, (GraphPad Software, La Jolla, CA, USA).

SH-SY5Y Cells, Staining, and Microscopy

Coverslips were coated with Poly-L-Lysine for 5 min, washed with H2O four times, and dried overnight at room temperature. SH-SY5Y neuroblastoma cells (CRL-2266TM, ATCC®, Manassas, VA, USA) cells were plated and grown overnight at a density of 2.5 × 105 cells in 500 μl DMEM, 10% FBS. Cells were depolarized by adding KCl at a concentration of 50 mM to the medium for 30 sec, after which the medium was replaced, and cells were incubated for 1, 3, and 6 h before fixing. Cells were fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde/4% sucrose in phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) at room temperature for 15 min and washed three times in PBS. Cells were permeabilized in 0.1% Triton X-100/0.1% Na-Citrate/PBS for 3 min at room temperature and washed three times with PBS. Cells were blocked in 10% FBS/PBS for 1 h at room temperature and incubated overnight at 4°C with primary antibody, (1:100, ADARB2; NBP1-57558, Novus Biologics, CO, USA) in blocking solution. The following day, cells were washed 3 times in PBS and then incubated with secondary antibody (1:500 Abcam; ab6939, Goat pAb to Rb IgG-H&L (Cy3®) pre-adsorbed, CBG, UK) for 90 min at room temperature and washed 4 times in PBS and once in Milli-Q® H2O before mounting onto microscopy slides with MP Biomedical immune-fluoro mounting medium. A Zeiss LSM700 confocal microscope was used with 63x oil objective. The settings were: pinhole 34 μm, speed 1.58 μs per pixel, 512 × 512 pixels, 4 times averaging, laser intensity 2, and gain was adjusted on the fly.

Statistical Analysis

The description of specific statistical methods, are described in detail in each relevant section. Briefly, for mouse behavioral experiments unpaired two-tail t-tests were calculated using GraphPad Prism v7.0 (GraphPad Software, La Jolla, CA, USA). For qPCR gene expression analysis unpaired t-test was estimated in GraphPad Prism v7.0 (GraphPad Software, La Jolla, CA, USA). The A-to-G frequency for the 282-filtered candidate editing sites was fitted with the glm model in R v3.2.3, followed by ANOVA. For deep sequencing experiments one-way ANOVA analysis was performed in R v3.2.3.

Data Availability

Metadata and nucleotide sequences generated and utilized in this work were deposited to NCBI under accessions: Bioproject PRJNA434770, BioSamples: SAMN08574369, SAMN08574370, SAMN08574371, SAMN08574372, SAMN08574373, SAMN08574374 and SRA accessions: SRR6757007, SRR6757008, SRR6757009, SRR6757010, SRR6757011, SRR6757012.

Results

Expression Analysis of Adar3

In mouse, the Adar3 gene is located on chromosome 13, with the gene spanning over ~557 Kbp (Figure 1A). We performed an expression analysis of ADAR3 that interrogated publicly available datasets derived from different technologies to exclude any bias. Firstly, data from the FANTOM5 project, which shows transcriptional activity as measured by Cap Analysis of Gene Expression (CAGE), showed Tags produced from a ~838 bp region that surrounds the Adar3 promoter (Figure 1A). Out of the 366 mouse tissue samples analyzed, ADAR3 CAGE tags showed expression in 51 tissues above background levels (Figure 1B). Importantly, this analysis highlighted that the majority of these highly expressed tags (indicating high ADAR3 transcription) are from neuronal tissues, with exceptions being the eyeball (red bars), embryonic forelimb (green bars), pituitary gland and prostate (blue bars). Secondly, we performed in-silico analysis of microarray data obtained from the BioGPS database of gene expression across 53 mouse tissues (http://biogps.org). Both ADAR3 probes in this dataset showed that ADAR3 was most highly expressed in the adult mouse brain, with highest expression observed in hippocampus, thalamus, amygdala and olfactory region (Figure 1C). This expression pattern suggests that Adar3 may be involved in the function of the brain and sensory information processing, i.e., learning and memory formation.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. ADAR3 is most highly expressed in mouse nervous tissue. (A) Screen shot from ZENBU Genome Browser showing the ADAR3 (Adarb2) promoter region (mm9 - chr13:8201842-8202679). Expression analysis of ADAR3, derived from the FANTOM5 project, which shows transcriptional activity measured by Cap Analysis of Gene Expression (CAGE), shows the sense strand Tag expression of a ~838 bp region that surrounds the Adar2b gene promoter. (B) Out of the 366-mouse tissue samples analyzed from the FANTOM5 project, ADAR3 CAGE tags showed expression above background levels in 51 tissues. Importantly, the majority of these highly expressed tags are from neuronal tissues, with exceptions being the eyeball (red bars), embryonic forelimb (green bars), pituitary gland and prostate (blue bars). (C) Similarly, in-silico analysis of microarray data obtained from the BioGPS database (http://biogps.org) for ADAR3 expression in 53 mouse tissues using two different probes, shows highest expression in brain. Different brain regions are indicated as green dots, with highest expression in olfactory bulb [Ob], hippocampus [H] and amygdala [A]. The retina is indicated as an orange dot and all other tissues are gray. Two highly expressed housekeeping genes Gapdh and Hptr are shown for comparison.

Adar3exon3 Mice Are Grossly Normal

In order to test whether ADAR3 is involved in learning and memory, we generated a knockout mouse line, using the Cre/lox gene targeting system. The newly created line lacks exon 3 of the Adarb2 gene, which encodes two double stranded RNA binding domains (Figures 2A–C). By removing the ability of ADAR3 to bind double stranded RNA, any adverse effects on the RNA editing-dependent and independent functions can be investigated. Quantitative PCR verified that exon 3 was absent in homozygous mice and showed that the level of the (truncated) transcript was decreased (Figure 2D). Adar3exon3 mice were born following the predicted Mendelian ratio and displayed no size or weight difference nor any atypical developmental characteristics compared with controls. As ADAR3 is expressed almost exclusively in the brain we investigated brain morphology in adult mice. Adar3exon3 mice showed no obvious differences in brain morphology including the hippocampal formation (Figures 2E,F).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Generation of Adar3exon3mice. (A) Schematic representation of Adar2b spliced mRNA structure with exons numbered and with the Ds-RNA binding and the Adenosine Deaminase domains shown. (B) Schematic of Adar3 knock-out allele. Adar3 intronic (lines) and exonic (bars) sequences are drawn to scale. (C) Schematic representation of the strategy used to remove exon 3 (containing the Ds-RNA binding domains). (D) Relative expression of Adar3 with respect to Gapdh, the relative mRNA level analysis was done by the ΔΔCt method (primers used were as follow 5′-3′: JM371_mADAR3_Ex1/2_f: GAGGTCCAAGAGGAAAGACA; JM372_mADAR3_Ex1/2_r: AGGTTATCTTCATCCTCTGTG; JM373_mADAR3_Ex3_r: TTCACTTCAGCACTGCTGGT; JM374_mADAR3_Ex4/5_f: TCGTCATGACCAAAGGCTTG). *Indicates P < 0.05; (E,F) No gross differences in the hippocampal formation between the WT and Adar3exon3 mice were found.

Adarexon3 Mice Display Deficits in Anxiety and Learning

Taking into account that Adar3exon3 mice are grossly normal, and that the function of Adar3 in mice is more likely to be cognitively subtle, we proceeded with behavioral analyzes. Locomotor activity in Adar3exon3 mice was normal over three consecutive days of testing using the rotarod test (Figure 3A). As ADAR3 is highly expressed in the hippocampus we sought to test Adar3exon3 mice using hippocampal-dependent behavioral analyzes. Firstly, we employed the open field test to investigate locomotor activity (Figure 3B1) and anxiety (Figure 3B2) and found no significant differences between control and Adar3exon3 mice, although there was a trend toward decreased anxiety in Adar3exon3 mice (p = 0.0633).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Adar3exon3 mice show increased anxiety in the elevated plus maze test. (A) No differences were observed between Adar3exon3 (n = 10) and WT littermates (n = 11) in the accelerating rotarod over a 3 day testing period indicating normal motor coordination and balance skills. (B) Adar3exon3 mice displayed normal locomotor (B1) and anxiety (B2) behavior as neither the total distance traveled nor the time spent in the center of an open field test were statistically significant between Adar3exon3 (n = 10) and WT littermates (n = 11). (C) However, in the elevated plus maze test of anxiety, Adar3exon3 (n = 10) mice spent significantly less time in the open arms as compared to WT littermates (n = 11) indicating an anxiogenic phenotype in the Adar3exon3 mice. (D) No significant differences were found between Adar3exon3 (n = 10) and WT littermates (n = 10) in either the percentage of spontaneous alternations (D1) or total arm entries (D2) in the Y-maze suggesting normal spatial learning and memory. Error bars indicate SEM. Star (***) indicates P < 0.05.

In contrast, in an alternate test for anxiety-like behavior, Adar3exon3 mice subjected to the elevated plus maze showed significant differences in anxiety suggesting impairment (Figure 3C) and potentially reflecting alterations in some forms of hippocampal processing (Bannerman et al., 2014). Finally, we used the Y maze test, which is a spatial working memory test that measures the willingness of mice to explore new environments. We found no significant differences between Adar3exon3 and control mice in percentage alternations (Figure 3D1) or total arm entries (Figure 3D2).

The hippocampus and amygdala, where ADAR3 expression is high (Figure 1C) are also central in fear conditioning and the hippocampus is implicated in the formation of context-dependent memory while the amygdala coordinates the paired association of the stimuli resulting in a fear response (Selden et al., 1991). Therefore, we used a classical fear conditioning experiment to test the contextual memory of Adar3exon3 mice. In this experiment mice are placed in a novel environment (fear conditioning chamber), where a foot shock is delivered. Mice learn to associate the contextual cues of the chamber with the aversive stimulus (see Methods). Re-exposure to the conditioning context retrieves the fear memory (displayed as freezing behavior). For short-term memory we re-exposed mice to the context (fear conditioning chamber) 2 h after the first shock was delivered. We found robust learning in control mice and, while some learning was observed in Adar3exon3 mice, the response was significantly decreased (Figure 4A). These results were mirrored in both long-term (24 h post-shock; Figure 4B) and remote memory after 3 weeks post-shock; Figure 4B) tests. These tests showed that Adar3exon3 mice display an anxiogenic-like phenotype in the elevated plus maze test and impaired contextual fear learning and memory.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Impaired hippocampal-dependent context fear learning in Adar3exon3 mice. (A) Short-term memory. Adar3exon3 mice displayed significantly reduced freezing behavior compared to WT littermates when tested 2 h after a mild foot-shock in a contextual fear-conditioning task (n = 10/genotype). (B) Long-term and remote memory. Adar3exon3 animals showed significantly impaired freezing behavior when tested both 24 h and 3 weeks post-conditioning as compared to WT littermates (n = 10/genotype). In both tests, all groups displayed minimal freezing behavior prior to the foot-shock being delivered indicating no alterations to baseline fear levels in either genotype. Error bars indicate SEM. Star (****) indicates P < 0.0001.

Adar3 Deficiency Results in a Subtle Modulation of Hippocampal Gene Expression

As the hippocampus is essential in contextual fear conditioning (Frankland et al., 1998; Maren et al., 2013) and the ventral hippocampus has been shown to play a role in the mechanisms of stress and anxiety (Bannerman et al., 2003, 2014), we reasoned that ADAR3 deficiency may affect hippocampal function. In order to assess how ADAR3 deficiency modulates gene expression in the hippocampus we performed next generation sequencing on total RNA extracted from hippocampal tissue of WT and Adar3exon3 mice. Total RNA from the hippocampus was sequenced and differential gene expression analysis was performed (see Methods and Supplementary Table 1).

Principal component analysis (herein, PCA) separated WT from Adar3exon3 samples (Figures 5A,B). A total of 550 differentially expressed genes (DEGs) were identified at a false discovery rate (FDR) of <0.01 and a logFC of at least 1. The majority of DEGs were up-regulated in the Adar3exon3 hippocampi (423), while 127 genes were down-regulated (apart from Adar2b), as shown in Figures 5A,B. The results from the DEG analysis were validated for 16 transcripts using quantitative polymerase chain reaction on an independent cohort of RNA samples (Supplementary Figure 1). Sipa1l3, Kcnc3, Dagla, Pgbd1, and Dkkl1 genes, showed statistically significant changes in gene expression in agreement with the RNA-seq analysis, while the majority of the rest of the tested genes showed the expected trend but did not reach significance.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Adar3 deficiency results in a mild modulation of gene expression in the hippocampus. (A) A Principal Component Analysis of normalized gene counts showing the transcriptomic variation between the samples in two dimensions. Most of the variation could be explained by PC1 and PC2. (B) A heat map of z-scores of differentially expressed genes (FDR < 0.01) of WT and Adar3exon3 mice. X/Y axes are ordered based on unsupervised clustering. (C) An MA plot of differentially expressed genes with FDR <0.01. Genes with log2FC≥ 2 are highlighted in red. Image generated with Degust, v.0.20 (http://vicbioinformatics.com/degust/index.html). (D) Panther gene ontology (GO) analysis of 550 (508 with mapped IDs) differentially expressed genes with FDR <0.01, log2FC≥ 1). Analysis Type: PANTHER Overrepresentation Test (release 20160715), GO Ontology database version 1.2, released 2017-01-26 (Ashburner et al., 2000; Mi et al., 2013, 2017).

Overall, most of the differentially regulated genes (with statistical significance), showed subtle fold changes in gene expression, with just a few genes with logFC ≥ 2 (Figure 5C). Gene ontology analysis of the DEGs with logFC ≥ 1 showed enrichment for biological processes such as regulation of synaptic plasticity and stem cell population maintenance (Figure 5D). Moreover, Ingenuity Pathway Analysis (IPA) showed that the most significantly affected pathways from the DEG analysis are all pathways related to the nervous system and oncogenesis, i.e., Rac Signaling, Axonal Guidance Signaling, Glutamate Receptor Signaling and Wnt/β-catenin Signaling (Data Sheet 4).

We also analyzed the sub-hippocampal expression pattern of the most significantly up/downregulated genes by FDR rate, using a recently published public gene expression resource from excitatory neuron-populations in sub-hippocampal regions (http://hipposeq.janelia.org) (Cembrowski et al., 2016). Interestingly, the 20 most significantly upregulated genes in the hippocampi of Adar3exon3 mice, were co-expressed in the dorsal dentate gyrus, whereas the 20 most significantly down-regulated genes did not show a co-expression pattern in any particular region (Figure 6A). Furthermore, in order to determine if the subset of up-regulated genes form a co-expression network brain-wide, we utilized the brain architecture database (http://addiction.brainarchitecture.org/) with 3,041 genes available for mouse, whose expression energies have the highest correlation between the coronal and sagittal images from the Allen Mouse Brain Atlas database brain-wide (available from: mouse.brain-map.org) (Lein et al., 2007; Ng et al., 2009; Hawrylycz et al., 2011a,b).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. The top most significantly overexpressed genes in the Adar3exon3 hippocampi show signature of co-expression. (A) A heatmap of FPKM expression values for the 20 most significantly up-regulated genes in the Adar3exon3 hippocampi with entries in the Hipposeq database. Computed from the Hipposeq Web portal (http://hipposeq.janelia.org), replicates included (numbers 0, 1, and 2 on the x axis), normalized values (FPKM values are normalized to the highest expressing population of each gene). (B) A co-expression network of a subset of 9 genes (the 20 most significantly up-regulated genes in the in the Adar3exon3 hippocampi with entries in the mouse brain architecture database http://addiction.brainarchitecture.org). (C) Adjacency matrix for (B), numbers denote cosine similarity values. (D) Cumulative distribution function (CDF) for the subset of 9 genes from (B) (in red). The red curve sits below the mean CDF of random draws of genes (in blue) (simulated set). Therefore the subset of 9 genes appears to be more co-expressed than expected by chance. Legend: dg-d, dorsal dentate gyrus granule cell, dg-v, ventral dentate gyrus granule cell, ca1-d, dorsal CA1 pyramidal cell, ca1-v, ventral CA1 pyramidal cell, ca3-d, dorsal CA3 pyramidal cell, ca3-v, ventral CA3 pyramidal cell, mc, dorsal dentate gyrus mossy cell, ca2, dorsal CA2 pyramidal cell.

In the brain architecture database, a gene-by-gene co-expression matrix is computed for the 3,041 genes as described in Menashe et al., Equation 1 (Menashe et al., 2013). The more co-expressed any pair of genes are, the closer their cosine similarity is to 1. The results showed a total of 9 genes were found in the intersection of the 20 most significantly upregulated genes in Adar3exon3 mice and the available genes in the mouse brain architecture database. The 9 genes showed a significant degree of co-expression in the mouse brain (Figures 6B,C), with cosine similarity threshold of 0.8 connecting 8 of the 9 genes in a network (Figure 6B). To determine whether the subset of up-regulated genes is more co-expressed than expected by chance, the cumulative distribution function (CDF) of the entries of the 9 genes and the mean CDF resulting from random draws of genes were computed through the Brain Architecture Web portal (Figure 6D). As the red curve of the CDF (subset of 9 genes) sits below the simulated average of the CDF of random sets of genes, the results suggest that the 9-gene subset may be co-expressed at a higher level than that expected by chance (blue and green lines; Figure 6D). The most significantly down-regulated genes showed less co-expression than expected by chance (data not shown).

Adar3 Deficiency Does Not Substantially Modulate RNA Editing Activity

ADAR3 has been thought to play a regulatory role by inhibiting the editing activities of ADAR1 and ADAR2 (Chen et al., 2000). In these studies, human purified recombinant ADAR3 lacked editing activity against well-known edited substrates (GluR-B and 5-HT2CR human gene transcript fragments) in vitro. In addition, ADAR3 inhibited the site-selective editing of ADAR1 and ADAR2 on adenosines situated on the 5-HT2CR fragment. However, ADAR1 and ADAR2 also interfered with each other's site-selective editing activity (Chen et al., 2000), suggesting that possibly the precise ratio of the three ADARs in vivo may be important for a tight regulation of the editing levels of a particular editing site.

Despite the lack of measurable editing activity in vitro (Melcher et al., 1996a; Chen et al., 2000), it has been noted that ADAR3 deaminase domain is probably functional based on the sequence comparison with the deaminase domains of ADAR1 and ADAR2. All amino acid residues that are believed to be part of the deaminase domain in ADAR3 apart from a few amino acids are well conserved among the ADAR members (Chen et al., 2000). It has also been shown that unlike the recombinant ADAR3, the native ADAR3 from mouse brain, appears to form a homodimeric-complex and homodimerization may be required for the function of ADAR1 and ADAR2 (Cho et al., 2003). Therefore it has been speculated that ADAR3 activity could be different in vivo compared to the in vitro studies (Cho et al., 2003).

To investigate whether a measurable alteration in editing activity could be identified in the Adar3exon3 mice, RNA-editing analysis from the mouse hippocampi was performed. The results showed that out of the 282 high-confident editing sites (see Methods, Data Sheet 1 and Data Sheet 2), 52.5% of them known editing targets in the RADAR v2 database (http://rnaedit.com), only 16 genomic positions showed statistically significant differences in editing frequency between the WT and Adar3exon3 mice as summarized in Figure 7 and Supplementary Figure 2. In order to validate these differences, targeted PCR of the locus surrounding selected candidate editing sites was performed, followed by deep sequencing with the Illumina MiSeq system (see Methods and Supplementary Figure 2). Out of the 10 sites chosen for validation, only one site (in the gene Wipi2) showed significant difference of more than 2% (2.87 and 4.67% for a neighboring site) between the WT and Adar3exon3 mice (Figure 7).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Adar3 deficiency does not have a substantial impact on RNA editing levels. Editing levels of 16 sites that showed significant differences between WT and Adar3exon3 mice in the RNA-seq analysis (purple and blue bars) compared to the editing level of 10 sites chosen for validation through targeted PCR re-sequencing (red and green bars). (n = 3/genotype) Error bars indicate SEM. Star (*) indicates P < 0.05.

In addition we chose to assess how Adar3 deficiency modulates the editing frequency of three protein-coding genes with well studied and characterized editing sites–Blcap, Gabra3 and serotonin receptor 5-Ht2cr. Bladder cancer-associated protein (BLCAP), also known as BC10, is a highly conserved 87 Amino Acid protein with an unknown function and multiple editing sites in the coding (Y/C, Q/R, K/R) and non-coding sequences (Gromova et al., 2001, 2002; Clutterbuck et al., 2005). Both ADAR1 and ADAR2 can edit Blcap coding sites but with different site-selective efficiencies (Galeano et al., 2010). Gabra3 codes for the alpha 3 subunit of the GABA(A) receptors, ligand-gated chloride channels and the major inhibitory neurotransmitter receptors in the CNS. Gabra3 is edited in the coding I/M site with high frequency in the adult brain (close to 100%) (Ohlson et al., 2007). Both ADAR1 and ADAR2 can edit Gabra3 and the non-edited and edited Gabra3-containing channels show different properties (Rula et al., 2008). The serotonin receptor 5-HT2CR is a G-coupled protein receptor and binds the endogenous excitatory neurotransmitter serotonin. Five adenosines located in close proximity on exon 5 (coding for the intracellular II loop region) are edited and these sites are known as the A, B, C, D, and E sites. Editing of any of these sites or a combination of them has a dramatic effect on the G-protein coupling efficiency of the receptor (Burns et al., 1997; Fitzgerald et al., 1999; Niswender et al., 1999; Wang et al., 2000). ADAR1 selectively edits the A and B site while ADAR2 selectively edits the D site (Nishikura, 2006).

Adar3 deficiency did not affect significantly any of the above-mentioned well-known editing sites, apart from a significant but small change in the editing of site C of the serotonin receptor as shown in Supplementary Figure 3. The results also showed that although there was a trend for a decrease in the editing frequency of site A and B of the serotonin receptor and the I/M site in Gabra3, this was not significant (Supplementary Figure 2).

The deep sequencing of the serotonin receptor exon 5 region also allowed us to study the frequency of the 32 possible mRNA isoforms, resulting from all possible combination of the 5 edited adenosines (Abbas et al., 2010). The largest differences between transcript frequencies were non-significant trend for a decrease in the frequency of the fully unedited transcript (aaaaa), accompanied by an increase in the edited transcripts (aaaag and ggaag) Supplementary Figure 3A). The serotonin transcripts can also be grouped in larger transcript groups (Abbas et al., 2010) and we found a non-significant trend for a decrease in the frequency of the unedited sites in A and B transcript groups with a concurrent increase in the edited (A and B sites) group in the Adar3exon3 animals (Supplementary Figure 3B).

We also examined in detail the editing of the Q/R and the R/G editing sites of the a-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methylisoxazole-4-propionic acid (AMPA) receptor GluR-B (Gria2) subunit. The Q/R site is known to be edited with close to 100% efficiency almost exclusively by ADAR2 (Sommer et al., 1991; Melcher et al., 1996b). The frequency of editing of the R/G site, which is embedded in the splice donor upstream of the acceptors of the mutually exclusive “flip” and “flop” modules, differs in the two alternative isoforms, whose expression is precisely regulated in sub-hippocampal regions (Sommer et al., 1990; Lomeli et al., 1994). We found no differences in the editing frequency of the Q/R or R/G site in the hippocampi of WT and Adar3exon3 mice (Supplementary Figure 4). While no large differences in editing were found, we also did not find that ADAR3 deficiency results in an increase in the editing frequency of any of the serotonin receptor sites, arguing that unlike in vitro, ADAR3 in the hippocampus does not show an inhibitory activity on the editing frequency of any of the known edited 5-Ht2cr sites in vivo.

Activity-Dependent ADAR3 Translocation to the Nucleus

ADAR3 is known to contain a nuclear import signal where the N terminal R domains bind to the Importin α protein family member KPNA2 (Maas and Gommans, 2009). Furthermore, our data suggests ADAR3 is involved in activity-dependent processes, such as synaptic plasticity, so we reasoned that ADAR3 cellular localization might be influenced by neuronal activity. To check this hypothesis we utilized stimulated SH-SY5Y cells with 50 mM KCl to depolarize the cells in order to mimic neuronal activity.

Indeed, we found that in stimulated cells, ADAR3 was localized to both the cytoplasm and nucleus as shown in Figure 8A. However after 3 h post treatment with KCL, localization of ADAR3 was more evident in the nucleus rather than both (Figure 8B). After 6 h post KCl activation, the ADAR3 protein was markedly diminished in the nucleus (Figure 8C).

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. ADAR3 translocates transiently from the cytoplasm to the nucleus upon neuronal activation. (A) ADAR3 immunohistochemistry in SH-SY5Y cells shows mostly diffuse ADAR3 staining in the cytoplasm at 0 h. (B) At 3 h post KCl-mediated activation, ADAR3 protein shows mostly nuclear localization (C) but by 6 h ADAR3 is seen to be returning to a more cytoplasmic pattern.

Discussion

In this study we show for the first time that, Adar3exon3 mice are viable, appear to be developmentally normal and are fertile, but exhibit cognitive and behavioral changes, specifically deficits in contextual fear learning and increased anxiety-like behavior; deficits that have been previously linked to the hippocampus (Kheirbek et al., 2013). We also show that both synaptic function-related genes and stem cell maintenance/neurogenesis genes are dysregulated in the hippocampi of Adar3exon3 mice.

In contrast, Adar1-deficient mice are embryonically lethal due to unsuppressed global innate immune response (Hartner et al., 2009), while Adar2-deficient animals die shortly after birth due to epileptic seizures, caused by deficiency in editing of the Q/R site of GluR-B (Brusa et al., 1995; Higuchi et al., 2000). ADAR1 function has been linked to the innate cytoplasmic dsRNA, which can trigger toxic innate immune response (Hartner et al., 2004; Mannion et al., 2014; Liddicoat et al., 2015; Pestal et al., 2015). Apart from editing the essential for life evolutionary conserved Q/R site of the glutamate receptor GluR-B subunit, ADAR2 also preferentially edits a number of other sites located on protein-coding regions such as those on the serotonin receptor (Nishikura, 2006). However, the biological function of editing, except for the known cases where editing results in non-synonymous amino acid changes, remains largely unknown. This is because the vast majority of editing occurs on repetitive elements in the UTRs and intronic sequences of mRNAs and in intergenic transcripts (Athanasiadis et al., 2004; Blow et al., 2004; Ramaswami et al., 2012; Huntley et al., 2016). It is unclear how editing in noncoding sequences might influence gene expression in a cis or trans manner. However, possibilities include modulation of miRNA binding sites, regulation of miRNA biogenesis and cross-talk with the short interference pathway (RNAi) (Nishikura, 2006; Liu et al., 2014; Warnefors et al., 2014; Bahn et al., 2015).

RNA-seq analysis of Adar3exon3 mouse hippocampi revealed a modest alteration in the expression of genes enriched for biological processes such as synaptic plasticity and stem cell population maintenance. In the latter there is a partial overlap with genes involved in Wnt/β-catenin signaling, a significantly affected pathway based on the Ingenuity Pathway Analysis in Adar3exon3 mice. The dentate gyrus in the hippocampus is also known to be one of the two places where neurogenesis in the adult brain can occur and the Wnt/β-catenin signaling pathway is primarily involved in regulating adult hippocampal neurogenesis (Lie et al., 2005; Deng et al., 2010). Neurogenesis is also required for memory formation, although its importance in contextual fear conditioning remains controversial (Deng et al., 2010). We also found that the 20 most significantly overexpressed genes in the Adar3exon3 hippocampi seem to form a co-expression network. Interestingly, at least 4 of them, Fosb, Nrna1, Arc, and Egr1, are known activity-dependent immediate early genes, implicated in synaptic plasticity, whose expression has been associated with learning and memory (Guzowski et al., 2005; Deng et al., 2010; West and Greenberg, 2011; Madabhushi et al., 2015).

The expression of Nrna1, Arc, Egr1, and Egr2 has also been shown to decrease with age in the medial prefrontal cortex in rats (Ianov et al., 2016). These results taken together lead us to hypothesize that Adar3 deficiency may lead to alteration of genes, involved in synaptic function, cognition and possibly regulation of neurogenesis, which may contribute to the memory and learning deficit phenotype in Adar3exon3 mice, however, the precise molecular mechanism remains unclear. Although the changes in gene expression are subtle, the 20 most significantly overexpressed genes form a co-expression network. Thus, it is possible that the mild modulation of whole networks has implications in the function of the hippocampus, particularly in relation to contextual learning and memory.

While we observed substantial changes in RNA editing for several candidate editing sites in the RNA-seq experiment, targeted PCR re-sequencing of selected target sites revealed only subtle changes in their editing frequencies. It is difficult to determine the biological relevance of such small changes. For example, zebrafish larvae deficient for fragile X mental retardation (fmr1) showed an increase of 1.2–3.7 fold in the expression levels of all ADAR genes (Shamay-Ramot et al., 2015). However, only mild changes in editing were observed in whole larvae, while the changes in editing levels somewhat increased for selected targets when brains were analyzed alone (Shamay-Ramot et al., 2015). Therefore, it is possible that the small and subtle differences found in editing in the Adar3exon3 hippocampi are amplified in specific sub-hippocampal regions.

ADAR3 has been shown to inhibit the editing of the serotonin receptor 5-HT2CR in vitro (Chen et al., 2000) and we expected to see an up-regulation in the editing frequency of the 5 edited sites on exon 5 of the receptor in Adar3exon3. However, targeted deep sequencing of the receptor revealed a trend for down-regulation of the editing level of 4 of the known edited sites, but no up-regulation was observed for any of the 5 sites. Therefore, unlike in vitro experiments, ADAR3 may not be a negative regulator of serotonin receptor editing in vivo in the mouse hippocampus. Thus, it is possible that the main biological activity of ADAR3 is exerted during neuronal stimulation. In human cell cultures, stimulated neuroblastoma SH-SY5Y cells showed a transient increase in ADAR3 nuclear localization. Moreover, activity-dependent temporal dynamics in the nuclear concentration of ADAR3 may change the local concentration ratio of the three ADARs and subsequently have a significant impact on gene expression or editing activity.

In our study, we identified that Adar3 deficiency resulted in the increased expression of genes involved in synaptic signaling and plasticity such as the Potassium Voltage-Gated Channel Subfamily C Member 3 [Kcnc3 (Kv3.3)] and Diacylglycerol Lipase Alpha (Dagla). It has been shown that tuning the activity of Kv channels can lead to broadening of the presynaptic action potential (AP) and affecting synaptic transmission (Carta et al., 2014), which in turn would influence the hippocampal CA3-dentate gyrus interaction strength, and thus memory formation (Carta et al., 2014). Interestingly, the modulation of Kv channels can be achieved in a retrograde fashion, by the lipid arachidonic acid, released in post-synaptic hippocampal CA3 pyramidal cells upon activation, leading to a lower threshold to induce long term potentiation on the same post-synaptic CA3 neurons (Carta et al., 2014). Arachidonic acid could be formed from the highly abundant in the CNS endocannabinoid 2-Arachidonoylglycerol (2-AG), synthesized from diacylglycerol almost exclusively by DAGLA (Tanimura et al., 2010). Thus, the potassium channel KCNC3 and DAGLA activities could be interconnected through lipid intermediates (such as arachidonic acid). It remains to be determined how increased expression of Kcnc3 and Dagla in the Adar3exon3 mice modulates synaptic signaling and whether it contributes to the fear conditioning deficit and increased anxiety phenotype in these mice. However, there is mounting evidence to suggest that ion channels (such as potassium channels) are involved in the pathogenesis of a range of psychiatric conditions (Imbrici et al., 2013).

In summary, our results showed that Adar3exon3 mice (lacking exon 3 of the Adar2b gene) are viable and appear to be developmentally normal. However, our Adarexon3 mice display cognitive and behavioral changes i.e., have increased levels of anxiety and deficits in hippocampus-dependent short- and long-term memory formation. RNA sequencing experiments from Adar3exon3 mice hippocampi, revealed a dysregulation of genes involved in synaptic function, which results in a subtle modulation of hippocampal gene expression. Moreover, ADAR3 deficiency does not substantially modulate the RNA editing activity of genes known to be edited. Finally, we also show that ADAR3 transiently translocated from the cytoplasm to the nucleus upon KCl-mediated activation in SH-SY5Y cells. Adar3exon3 mice provide an important tool that can be used further to investigate the role and mechanisms of action of ADAR3 in mammalian brain function.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Australian code for the care and use of animals for scientific purposes. The protocol was approved by the Garvan/St Vincent's Animal Ethics Committee.

Author Contributions

DM, GB, LK, BG, LA, RZ, MB, NJ, LC, DK, AG, NS, MP, CW, and GF performed the experiments or provided data. DM, GB, LA, BV, and JM conceived the experiments. DM, GB, LK, SP, and JM wrote the manuscript. JM and BV provided laboratory facilities. All authors commented and contributed toward the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was funded by the Australian Research Council, Discovery Project DP120100729; JM was an NHMRC Australia Fellow (631668) during much of this work; DM is a recipient of the Science and Industry Endowment Fund John Stocker Postdoctoral Fellowship PF13-040; CW was a Leukemia Foundation Phillip Desbrow Senior Research Fellow and LA was recipient of the Swedish Research Council grant N: 350-2012-375. We acknowledge funding from the NIH through grants awarded to Velocigene at Regeneron Inc (U01HG004085) and the CSD Consortium (U01HG004080). CSD funded the generation of gene-targeted ES cells for 8500 genes in the KOMP Program and archived and distributed by the KOMP Repository at UC Davis and CHORI (U42RR024244; for more information or to obtain KOMP products go to www.komp.org or email service@komp.org).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful for the contribution by and discussions with Martin Smith, Eva Novoa Pardo, Helaine Graziele Santos Vieira, Matthew Hobbs, Aaron Stratham, Luis R. Arriola Martinez, Lilly Bojarski, Clement Loy, Shalima Nair, Phuc Loi Luu, and Ksenia Skvortsova.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2018.00243/full#supplementary-material

References

Abbas, A. I., Urban, D. J., Jensen, N. H., Farrell, M. S., Kroeze, W. K., Mieczkowski, P., et al. (2010). Assessing serotonin receptor mRNA editing frequency by a novel ultra high-throughput sequencing method. Nucleic Acids Res. 38:e118. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkq107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashburner, M., Ball, C. A., Blake, J. A., Botstein, D., Butler, H., Cherry, J. M., et al. (2000). Gene ontology: tool for the unification of biology. The gene ontology consortium. Nat. Genet. 25, 25–29. doi: 10.1038/75556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Athanasiadis, A., Rich, A., and Maas, S. (2004). Widespread A-to-I RNA editing of Alu-containing mRNAs in the human transcriptome. PLoS Biol. 2:e391. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0020391

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bahn, J. H., Ahn, J., Lin, X., Zhang, Q., Lee, J. H., Civelek, M., et al. (2015). Genomic analysis of ADAR1 binding and its involvement in multiple RNA processing pathways. Nat. Commun. 6:6355. doi: 10.1038/ncomms7355

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bannerman, D. M., Grubb, M., Deacon, R. M., Yee, B. K., Feldon, J., and Rawlins, J. N. (2003). Ventral hippocampal lesions affect anxiety but not spatial learning. Behav. Brain Res. 139, 197–213. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00268-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bannerman, D. M., Sprengel, R., Sanderson, D. J., McHugh, S. B., Rawlins, J. N., Monyer, H., et al. (2014). Hippocampal synaptic plasticity, spatial memory and anxiety. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 181–192. doi: 10.1038/nrn3677

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barry, G., and Mattick, J. S. (2012). The role of regulatory RNA in cognitive evolution. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed). 16, 497–503. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bass, B. L. (2002). RNA editing by adenosine deaminases that act on RNA. Annu. Rev. Biochem. 71, 817–846. doi: 10.1146/annurev.biochem.71.110601.135501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blow, M., Futreal, P. A., Wooster, R., and Stratton, M. R. (2004). A survey of RNA editing in human brain. Genome Res. 14, 2379–2387. doi: 10.1101/gr.2951204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brusa, R., Zimmermann, F., Koh, D. S., Feldmeyer, D., Gass, P., Seeburg, P. H., et al. (1995). Early-onset epilepsy and postnatal lethality associated with an editing-deficient GluR-B allele in mice. Science 270, 1677–1680.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Burns, C. M., Chu, H., Rueter, S. M., Hutchinson, L. K., Canton, H., Sanders-Bush, E., et al. (1997). Regulation of serotonin-2C receptor G-protein coupling by RNA editing. Nature 387, 303–308.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Carta, M., Lanore, F., Rebola, N., Szabo, Z., Da Silva, S. V., Lourenco, J., et al. (2014). Membrane lipids tune synaptic transmission by direct modulation of presynaptic potassium channels. Neuron 81, 787–799. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cembrowski, M. S., Wang, L., Sugino, K., Shields, B. C., and Spruston, N. (2016). Hipposeq: a comprehensive RNA-seq database of gene expression in hippocampal principal neurons. Elife 5:e14997. doi: 10.7554/eLife.14997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, C. X., Cho, D. S., Wang, Q., Lai, F., Carter, K. C., and Nishikura, K. (2000). A third member of the RNA-specific adenosine deaminase gene family, ADAR3, contains both single- and double-stranded RNA binding domains. RNA 6, 755–767. doi: 10.1017/S1355838200000170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cho, D. S., Yang, W., Lee, J. T., Shiekhattar, R., Murray, J. M., and Nishikura, K. (2003). Requirement of dimerization for RNA editing activity of adenosine deaminases acting on RNA. J. Biol. Chem. 278, 17093–17102. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M213127200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clutterbuck, D. R., Leroy, A., O'Connell, M. A., and Semple, C. A. (2005). A bioinformatic screen for novel A-I RNA editing sites reveals recoding editing in BC10. Bioinformatics 21, 2590–2595. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/bti411

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deng, W., Aimone, J. B., and Gage, F. H. (2010). New neurons and new memories: how does adult hippocampal neurogenesis affect learning and memory? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 339–350. doi: 10.1038/nrn2822

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dobin, A., Davis, C. A., Schlesinger, F., Drenkow, J., Zaleski, C., Jha, S., et al. (2013). STAR: ultrafast universal RNA-seq aligner. Bioinformatics 29, 15–21. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/bts635

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fanselow, M. S., and Poulos, A. M. (2005). The neuroscience of mammalian associative learning. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 56, 207–234. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzgerald, L. W., Iyer, G., Conklin, D. S., Krause, C. M., Marshall, A., Patterson, J. P., et al. (1999). Messenger RNA editing of the human serotonin 5-HT2C receptor. Neuropsychopharmacology 21(2 Suppl.), 82S-90S. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(99)00004-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frankland, P. W., Cestari, V., Filipkowski, R. K., McDonald, R. J., and Silva, A. J. (1998). The dorsal hippocampus is essential for context discrimination but not for contextual conditioning. Behav. Neurosci. 112, 863–874. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.112.4.863

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galeano, F., Leroy, A., Rossetti, C., Gromova, I., Gautier, P., Keegan, L. P., et al. (2010). Human BLCAP transcript: new editing events in normal and cancerous tissues. Int. J. Cancer 127, 127–137. doi: 10.1002/ijc.25022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gromova, I., Gromov, P., and Celis, J. E. (2001). A novel member of the glycosyltransferase family, beta 3 Gn-T2, highly downregulated in invasive human bladder transitional cell carcinomas. Mol. Carcinog. 32, 61–72. doi: 10.1002/mc.1065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gromova, I., Gromov, P., and Celis, J. E. (2002). bc10: a novel human bladder cancer-associated protein with a conserved genomic structure downregulated in invasive cancer. Int. J. Cancer 98, 539–546. doi: 10.1002/ijc.10244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guzowski, J. F., Timlin, J. A., Roysam, B., McNaughton, B. L., Worley, P. F., and Barnes, C. A. (2005). Mapping behaviorally relevant neural circuits with immediate-early gene expression. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, 599–606. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2005.08.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartner, J. C., Schmittwolf, C., Kispert, A., Müller, A. M., Higuchi, M., and Seeburg, P. H. (2004). Liver disintegration in the mouse embryo caused by deficiency in the RNA-editing enzyme ADAR1. J. Biol. Chem. 279, 4894–4902. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M311347200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartner, J. C., Walkley, C. R., Lu, J., and Orkin, S. H. (2009). ADAR1 is essential for the maintenance of hematopoiesis and suppression of interferon signaling. Nat. Immunol. 10, 109–115. doi: 10.1038/ni.1680

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawrylycz, M., Baldock, R. A., Burger, A., Hashikawa, T., Johnson, G. A., Martone, M., et al. (2011a). Digital atlasing and standardization in the mouse brain. PLoS Comput. Biol. 7:e1001065. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1001065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawrylycz, M., Ng, L., Page, D., Morris, J., Lau, C., Faber, S., et al. (2011b). Multi-scale correlation structure of gene expression in the brain. Neural Netw. 24, 933–942. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2011.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higuchi, M., Maas, S., Single, F. N., Hartner, J., Rozov, A., Burnashev, N., et al. (2000). Point mutation in an AMPA receptor gene rescues lethality in mice deficient in the RNA-editing enzyme ADAR2. Nature 406, 78–81. doi: 10.1038/35017558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huntley, M. A., Lou, M., Goldstein, L. D., Lawrence, M., Dijkgraaf, G. J., Kaminker, J. S., et al. (2016). Complex regulation of ADAR-mediated RNA-editing across tissues. BMC Genomics 17:61. doi: 10.1186/s12864-015-2291-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ianov, L., Rani, A., Beas, B. S., Kumar, A., and Foster, T. C. (2016). Transcription profile of aging and cognition-related genes in the medial prefrontal cortex. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:113. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Imbrici, P., Camerino, D. C., and Tricarico, D. (2013). Major channels involved in neuropsychiatric disorders and therapeutic perspectives. Front. Genet. 4:76. doi: 10.3389/fgene.2013.00076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jantsch, M. F., and Öhman, M. (2008). “RNA editing by adenosine deaminases that act on RNA (ADARs),” in RNA Editing, ed H. U. Göringer (Berlin: Springer), 51–84.

Google Scholar

Jin, Y., Zhang, W., and Li, Q. (2009). Origins and evolution of ADAR-mediated RNA editing. IUBMB Life 61, 572–578. doi: 10.1002/iub.207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kheirbek, M. A., Drew, L. J., Burghardt, N. S., Costantini, D. O., Tannenholz, L., Ahmari, S. E., et al. (2013). Differential control of learning and anxiety along the dorsoventral axis of the dentate gyrus. Neuron 77, 955–968. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.12.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, D. D., Kim, T. T., Walsh, T., Kobayashi, Y., Matise, T. C., Buyske, S., et al. (2004). Widespread RNA editing of embedded alu elements in the human transcriptome. Genome Res. 14, 1719–1725. doi: 10.1101/gr.2855504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langmead, B., Trapnell, C., Pop, M., and Salzberg, S. L. (2009). Ultrafast and memory-efficient alignment of short DNA sequences to the human genome. Genome Biol. 10:R25. doi: 10.1186/gb-2009-10-3-r25

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lein, E. S., Hawrylycz, M. J., Ao, N., Ayres, M., Bensinger, A., Bernard, A., et al. (2007). Genome-wide atlas of gene expression in the adult mouse brain. Nature 445, 168–176. doi: 10.1038/nature05453

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levanon, E. Y., Eisenberg, E., Yelin, R., Nemzer, S., Hallegger, M., Shemesh, R., et al. (2004). Systematic identification of abundant A-to-I editing sites in the human transcriptome. Nat. Biotechnol. 22, 1001–1005. doi: 10.1038/nbt996

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, B., and Dewey, C. N. (2011). RSEM: accurate transcript quantification from RNA-Seq data with or without a reference genome. BMC Bioinformatics 12:323. doi: 10.1186/1471-2105-12-323

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, H., Handsaker, B., Wysoker, A., Fennell, T., Ruan, J., Homer, N., et al. (2009). The sequence alignment/map format and SAMtools. Bioinformatics 25, 2078–2079. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btp352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liddicoat, B. J., Piskol, R., Chalk, A. M., Ramaswami, G., Higuchi, M., Hartner, J. C., et al. (2015). RNA editing by ADAR1 prevents MDA5 sensing of endogenous dsRNA as nonself. Science 349, 1115–1120. doi: 10.1126/science.aac7049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lie, D. C., Colamarino, S. A., Song, H. J., Desire, L., Mira, H., Consiglio, A., et al. (2005). Wnt signalling regulates adult hippocampal neurogenesis. Nature 437, 1370–1375. doi: 10.1038/nature04108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, H., Ma, C. P., Chen, Y. T., Schuyler, S. C., Chang, K. P., and Tan, B. C. (2014). Functional impact of RNA editing and ADARs on regulation of gene expression: perspectives from deep sequencing studies. Cell Biosci. 4:44. doi: 10.1186/2045-3701-4-44

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lomeli, H., Mosbacher, J., Melcher, T., Hoger, T., Geiger, J. R., Kuner, T., et al. (1994). Control of kinetic properties of AMPA receptor channels by nuclear RNA editing. Science 266, 1709–1713.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Maas, S., and Gommans, W. M. (2009). Identification of a selective nuclear import signal in adenosine deaminases acting on RNA. Nucleic Acids Res. 37, 5822–5829. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkp599

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maas, S., Rich, A., and Nishikura, K. (2003). A-to-I RNA editing: recent news and residual mysteries. J. Biol. Chem. 278, 1391–1394. doi: 10.1074/jbc.R200025200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madabhushi, R., Gao, F., Pfenning, A. R., Pan, L., Yamakawa, S., Seo, J., et al. (2015). Activity-induced DNA breaks govern the expression of neuronal early-response genes. Cell 161, 1592–1605. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.05.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mannion, N. M., Greenwood, S. M., Young, R., Cox, S., Brindle, J., Read, D., et al. (2014). The RNA-editing enzyme ADAR1 controls innate immune responses to RNA. Cell Rep. 9, 1482–1494. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2014.10.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maren, S., Phan, K. L., and Liberzon, I. (2013). The contextual brain: implications for fear conditioning, extinction and psychopathology. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 417–428. doi: 10.1038/nrn3492

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melcher, T., Maas, S., Herb, A., Sprengel, R., Higuchi, M., and Seeburg, P. H. (1996a). RED2, a brain-specific member of the RNA-specific adenosine deaminase family. J. Biol. Chem. 271, 31795–31798.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Melcher, T., Maas, S., Herb, A., Sprengel, R., Seeburg, P. H., and Higuchi, M. (1996b). A mammalian RNA editing enzyme. Nature 379, 460–464.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Menashe, I., Grange, P., Larsen, E. C., Banerjee-Basu, S., and Mitra, P. P. (2013). Co-expression profiling of autism genes in the mouse brain. PLoS Comput. Biol. 9:e1003128. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mi, H., Huang, X., Muruganujan, A., Tang, H., Mills, C., Kang, D., et al. (2017). PANTHER version 11: expanded annotation data from Gene Ontology and Reactome pathways, and data analysis tool enhancements. Nucleic Acids Res. 45, D183–D189. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkw1138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mi, H., Muruganujan, A., Casagrande, J. T., and Thomas, P. D. (2013). Large-scale gene function analysis with the PANTHER classification system. Nat. Protoc. 8, 1551–1566. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2013.092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ng, L., Bernard, A., Lau, C., Overly, C. C., Dong, H. W., Kuan, C., et al. (2009). An anatomic gene expression atlas of the adult mouse brain. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 356–362. doi: 10.1038/nn.2281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nishikura, K. (2006). Editor meets silencer: crosstalk between RNA editing and RNA interference. Nat. Rev. Mol. Cell Biol. 7, 919–931. doi: 10.1038/nrm2061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nishikura, K. (2010). Functions and regulation of RNA editing by ADAR deaminases. Annu. Rev. Biochem. 79, 321–349. doi: 10.1146/annurev-biochem-060208-105251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niswender, C. M., Copeland, S. C., Herrick-Davis, K., Emeson, R. B., and Sanders-Bush, E. (1999). RNA editing of the human serotonin 5-hydroxytryptamine 2C receptor silences constitutive activity. J. Biol. Chem. 274, 9472–9478. doi: 10.1074/jbc.274.14.9472

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohlson, J., Pedersen, J. S., Haussler, D., and Ohman, M. (2007). Editing modifies the GABA(A) receptor subunit alpha3. RNA 13, 698–703. doi: 10.1261/rna.349107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paz-Yaacov, N., Levanon, E. Y., Nevo, E., Kinar, Y., Harmelin, A., Jacob-Hirsch, J., et al. (2010). Adenosine-to-inosine RNA editing shapes transcriptome diversity in primates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 12174–12179. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1006183107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pestal, K., Funk C. C., Snyder, J. M., Price, N. D., Treuting, P. M., and Stetson, D. B. (2015). Isoforms of RNA-editing enzyme ADAR1 independently control nucleic acid sensor MDA5-driven autoimmunity and multi-organ development. Immunity 43, 933–944. doi: 10.1016/j.immuni.2015.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quinlan, A. R., and Hall, I. M. (2010). BEDTools: a flexible suite of utilities for comparing genomic features. Bioinformatics 26, 841–842. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btq033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramaswami, G., Lin, W., Piskol, R., Tan, M. H., Davis, C., and Li, J. B. (2012). Accurate identification of human Alu and non-Alu RNA editing sites. Nat. Methods 9, 579–581. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1982

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, J. T., Thorvaldsdóttir, H., Winckler, W., Guttman, M., Lander, E. S., Getz, G., et al. (2011). Integrative genomics viewer. Nat. Biotechnol. 29, 24-26. doi: 10.1038/nbt.1754

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, M. D., McCarthy, D. J., and Smyth, G. K. (2010). edgeR: a Bioconductor package for differential expression analysis of digital gene expression data. Bioinformatics 26, 139–140. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btp616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rula, E. Y., Lagrange, A. H., Jacobs, M. M., Hu, N., Macdonald, R. L., and Emeson, R. B. (2008). Developmental modulation of GABA(A) receptor function by RNA editing. J. Neurosci. 28, 6196–6201. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0443-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmittgen, T. D., Lee, E. J., and Jiang, J. (2008). “High-Throughput Real-Time, P. C. R,” in Molecular Beacons: Signalling Nucleic Acid Probes, Methods, and Protocols, eds A. Marx and O. Seitz (Totowa, NJ: Humana Press), 89–98.

Google Scholar

Selden, N. R., Everitt, B. J., Jarrard, L. E., and Robbins, T. W. (1991). Complementary roles for the amygdala and hippocampus in aversive conditioning to explicit and contextual cues. Neuroscience 42, 335–350. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(91)90379-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shamay-Ramot, A., Khermesh, K., Porath, H. T., Barak, M., Pinto, Y., Wachtel, C., et al. (2015). Fmrp interacts with Adar and regulates RNA editing, synaptic density and locomotor activity in zebrafish. PLoS Genet. 11:e1005702. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1005702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sommer, B., Keinanen, K., Verdoorn, T. A., Wisden, W., Burnashev, N., Herb, A., et al. (1990). Flip and flop: a cell-specific functional switch in glutamate-operated channels of the CNS. Science 249, 1580–1585.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Sommer, B., Kohler, M., Sprengel, R., and Seeburg, P. H. (1991). RNA editing in brain controls a determinant of ion flow in glutamate-gated channels. Cell 67, 11–19. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(91)90568-J

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanimura, A., Yamazaki, M., Hashimotodani, Y., Uchigashima, M., Kawata, S., Abe, M., et al. (2010). The Endocannabinoid 2-Arachidonoylglycerol produced by Diacylglycerol Lipase α mediates retrograde suppression of synaptic transmission. Neuron 65, 320–327. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, L., Wang, S., and Li, W. (2012). RSeQC: quality control of RNA-seq experiments. Bioinformatics 28, 2184–2185. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/bts356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Q., O'Brien, P. J., Chen, C. X., Cho, D. S., Murray, J. M., and Nishikura, K. (2000). Altered G protein-coupling functions of RNA editing isoform and splicing variant serotonin2C receptors. J. Neurochem. 74, 1290–1300. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2000.741290.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warnefors, M., Liechti, A., Halbert, J., Valloton, D., and Kaessmann, H. (2014). Conserved microRNA editing in mammalian evolution, development and disease. Genome Biol. 15:R83. doi: 10.1186/gb-2014-15-6-r83

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

West, A. E., and Greenberg, M. E. (2011). Neuronal activity-regulated gene transcription in synapse development and cognitive function. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Biol. 3:a005744. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a005744

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolf, A., Bauer, B., Abner, E. L., Ashkenazy-Frolinger, T., and Hartz, A. M. S. (2016). A comprehensive behavioral test battery to assess learning and memory in 129S6/Tg2576 mice. PLoS ONE 11:e0147733. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0147733

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: ADAR3, Adar3exon3 mouse model, RNA editing, learning and memory, Adarb2

Citation: Mladenova D, Barry G, Konen LM, Pineda SS, Guennewig B, Avesson L, Zinn R, Schonrock N, Bitar M, Jonkhout N, Crumlish L, Kaczorowski DC, Gong A, Pinese M, Franco GR, Walkley CR, Vissel B and Mattick JS (2018) Adar3 Is Involved in Learning and Memory in Mice. Front. Neurosci. 12:243. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00243

Received: 22 January 2018; Accepted: 27 March 2018;
Published: 13 April 2018.

Edited by:

Michael F. Miles, Virginia Commonwealth University, United States

Reviewed by:

Sulev Kõks, University of Tartu, Estonia
Karine Merienne, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), France

Copyright © 2018 Mladenova, Barry, Konen, Pineda, Guennewig, Avesson, Zinn, Schonrock, Bitar, Jonkhout, Crumlish, Kaczorowski, Gong, Pinese, Franco, Walkley, Vissel and Mattick. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: John S. Mattick, [email protected]

Present Address: Guy Barry and Maina Bitar, QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute, Brisbane, QLD, Australia; The School of Medicine, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, QLD, Australia
Boris Guennewig, Sydney Medical School, Brain and Mind Centre, The University of Sydney, Camperdown, NSW, Australia
Andrew Gong, School of Medical Sciences, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW, Australia

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.