Metalinguistic awareness, also known as metalinguistic ability, refers to the ability to consciously reflect on the nature of language and to use metalanguage to describe it. The concept of metalinguistic awareness is helpful in explaining the execution and transfer of linguistic knowledge across languages (e.g. code-switching as well as translation among bilinguals). Metalinguistics expresses itself in ways such as:
- an awareness that language has the potential to go beyond the literal meaning, to further include multiple or implied meanings, formal structures like phonemes, syntax, etc.
- an awareness, therefore, of the flexibility of language through irony, sarcasm and other forms of word play
- an awareness, therefore, too, that language has a structure that can be manipulated
- an awareness that a word is separable from its referent (meaning resides in the mind, not in the name, i.e. Sonia is Sonia, and I will be the same person even if somebody calls me another name)
Metalinguistic awareness is therefore distinct from the notion of engaging with normal language operations, but instead with the process of language use and the exercise of the relevant control.
Currently, the most commonly held conception of metalinguistic awareness suggests that its development is constituted by cognitive control (i.e. selecting and coordinating the relevant pieces of information needed to comprehend the language manipulation) and analysed knowledge (i.e. recognising the meaning and structure of the manipulated language).[1]
Background
editOrigins
editThere are a number of explanations as to where metalinguistic abilities may come from.
One such explanation depends on the notion that metalinguistic ability is developed in tandem with language acquisition, specifically pertaining to spoken language.[2] The development of mechanisms that allow for an individual to detect errors as they speak is, by this account, a manifestation of metalinguistic ability.[2]
Another possible account suggests that metalinguistic awareness and metalinguistic ability are distinct from other sorts of linguistic developments, where these metalinguistic skills are entirely separate from the development and acquisition of basic speaking and listening skills.[2] By this account, metalinguistic abilities necessarily differ from linguistic proficiency.[2]
A third possible account suggests that metalinguistic awareness occurs as a result of language education in schools – this account holds that it is the process of learning to read that nurtures metalinguistic ability.[2]
Later developments
editToday, the most widely-accepted notion of the development of metalinguistic awareness is a framework that suggests it can be achieved through the development of two dimensions: analysed knowledge and cognitive control.[1] As opposed to knowing that is intuitive, analysed knowledge refers to "knowing that is explicit and objective".[1] Cognitive control involves "the selection and coordination of information, usually within time constraint".[1]
In a given proposition, a sentence with wordplay, for instance, metalinguistic awareness plays out in several steps. One has to control selecting and coordinating the relevant information in that proposition, and then analyse the information as it is represented to decipher it.
Bialystok and Ryan argue that achieving metalinguistic awareness is the ability to manipulate both dimensions at an arbitrarily "high" level.[1] In the study of metalinguistic ability in children, the proportional growth of these two dimensions suggests that there may not be a fixed age of onset to trace or measure metalinguistic ability, but rather an emerging proficiency that follows increasingly difficult metalinguistic issues.[1]
Types of metalinguistic awareness
editThere are four major categories to metalinguistic awareness, where this notion of metalinguistic ability may manifest. These categories are: phonological awareness, word awareness, syntactic awareness and pragmatic awareness.[2]
Phonological awareness and word awareness
editPhonological awareness and word awareness work in tandem in order to allow the language user to process, understand, and utilize the constituent parts of the language being used. These forms of metalinguistic awareness are of particular relevance in the process of learning how to read. Phonological awareness may be assessed through the use of phonemic segmentation tasks, though the use of tests utilizing nondigraph, nonword syllables appear to provide more accurate results.[2]
Syntactic awareness
editSyntactic awareness is engaged when an individual engages in mental operations to do with structural aspects of language. This involves the application of inferential and pragmatic rules. This may be measured through the use of correction tasks for sentences that contain word order violations.[2]
Pragmatic awareness
editPragmatic awareness refers to the awareness of the relationships between sentences and their contextual/relational quality.[3] This may include the epistemic context, knowledge of the situation, or any other details surrounding the utterance. This may be measured by assessing the ability to detect inconsistencies between sentences.[2]
Correlations with other linguistic fields
editPast research has attempted to find correlations between the attainment of metalinguistic ability with other language abilities like literacy and bilingualism. However, the paradigm shifted with the idea that metalinguistic ability had to instead be measured through essential underlying skills (i.e. analysed knowledge and cognitive control). This framework – analysing ability through comparing it with skills rather than comparing it with other abilities – came to be applied to other linguistic abilities that bore the need for similar skills.
Literacy
editThe process of learning to read depends heavily on analysed knowledge on the functions and features of reading,[4] control over the knowledge required[5] and control over the formal aspects of the language to extract its meaning.[6] Various research has exhibited that weaknesses in any one of these aspects reflects poorer literacy.[7][8] As such, there suggests a relationship between literacy and metalinguistic awareness.
Separate studies also suggest that the process of learning how to read is strongly influenced by aptitude with metalinguistic factors. In fact, older, literate children often prove to be more adept with metalinguistic skills. It is suggested, though, that the relation may be reversed in that it is improved metalinguistic skill that leads to an improved ability to read, rather than reading that precipitates an improvement in metalinguistic ability.[2]
Bilingualism
editStudies have generally supported the hypothesis that bilingual children possess greater cognitive control than their monolingual counterparts. These studies are conducted with the caveat that monolingual and bilingual children being assessed have, as a baseline, equal competency in the languages that they speak. This would suggest that the differences in performance are to do with a difference in metalinguistic ability rather than differences in linguistic proficiency.
When assessing ability, bilingual children were privileged in their advanced awareness of the arbitrary relationship between words and their meanings, as well as that of structures and meanings.[9][10][11][12][13][14] This advanced awareness could be manifested in the transferability of the idea that language is malleable, across languages.[15] Interestingly, studies seemed to show that bilingual children had higher proficiency in their metalinguistic skills than in the languages themselves.[15]
References
edit- ^ a b c d e f Bialystok, Ellen; Ryan, Ellen Bouchard (1985). "Toward a Definition of Metalinguistic Skill". Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 31 (3): 229–251. JSTOR 23086295.
- ^ a b c d e f g h i j Tunmer, William E.; Herriman, Michael L.; Nesdale, Andrew R. (1988). "Metalinguistic Abilities and Beginning Reading". Reading Research Quarterly. 23 (2): 134–158. doi:10.2307/747799. JSTOR 747799.
- ^ Lena, Ludovica (2020). "Referent introducing strategies in advanced L2 usage". Referring in a Second Language. pp. 164–183. doi:10.4324/9780429263972-9. ISBN 978-0-429-26397-2. S2CID 216254718.
- ^ DOWNING, J. (1979). Reading and reasoning. New York: Springer[page needed]
- ^ Rozin, P.; Gleitman, L. R. (1977). "The structure and acquisition of reading II: The reading process and the acquisition of the alphabetic principle". In Reber, Arthur S; Scarborough, Don L (eds.). Toward a psychology of reading: the proceedings of the CUNY conferences. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 55–142. ISBN 978-0-470-99010-0. OCLC 1277316552. S2CID 41015364.
- ^ Brown, Ann L. (1980). "Metacognitive Development and Reading". In Spiro, Rand J; Bruce, Bertram C; Brewer, William F (eds.). Theoretical issues in reading comprehension: perspectives from cognitive psychology, linguistics, artificial intelligence, and education. L. Erlbaum Associates. pp. 453–482. ISBN 978-0-89859-036-4.
- ^ Eamon, Douglas B. (1978). "Selection and Recall of Topical Information in Prose by Better and Poorer Readers". Reading Research Quarterly. 14 (2): 244–257. doi:10.2307/747476. JSTOR 747476.
- ^ Bransford, John D (1982). "Differences in approaches to learning: An overview". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 111 (4): 390–398. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.111.4.390.
- ^ Cazden, Courtney B. (January 1, 1974). "Play and metalinguistic awareness: One dimension of language experience". The Urban Review. 7 (1): 28–39. doi:10.1007/BF02223202. S2CID 144673278.
- ^ Vygotsky, Lev (1962). Hanfmann, Eugenia; Vakar, Gertrude (eds.). Thought and language. doi:10.1037/11193-000.[page needed]
- ^ Bialystok, Ellen; Ryan, Ellen Bouchard (1985). "A metacognitive framework for the development of first and second language skills". In Forrest-Pressley, Donna-Lynn; MacKinnon, G. E.; Waller, Thomas Gary (eds.). Metacognition, Cognition, and Human Performance. Academic Press. pp. 207–252. ISBN 978-0-12-262301-1.
- ^ Bain, Bruce; Yu, Agnes (1978). "Toward an integration of Piaget and Vygotsky: A cross-cultural replication (France, Germany, Canada) concerning cognitive consequences of bilinguality". In Paradis, Michel (ed.). Aspects of Bilingualism. Hornbeam Press. pp. 83–96. ISBN 978-0-917496-10-3.
- ^ Peal, Elizabeth; Lambert, Wallace E. (1962). "The relation of bilingualism to intelligence". Psychological Monographs: General and Applied. 76 (27): 1–23. doi:10.1037/h0093840.
- ^ Torrance, E. Paul; Gowan, John C.; Wu, Jing-Jyi; Aliotti, Nicholas C. (February 1970). "Creative functioning of monolingual and bilingual children in Singapore". Journal of Educational Psychology. 61 (1): 72–75. doi:10.1037/h0028767.
- ^ a b Barac, Raluca; Bialystok, Ellen (2012). "Bilingual Effects on Cognitive and Linguistic Development: Role of Language, Cultural Background, and Education". Child Development. 83 (2): 413–422. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01707.x. JSTOR 41416093. PMC 3305827. PMID 22313034.
Further reading
edit- Jessner, Ulrike (2019) Language Awareness in Multilingual Learning and Teaching. In: The Routeledge Handbook of Language Awareness, Garrett, P. & Cots, M. (eds.), Routledge, New York.
- Roehr-Brackin, Karen (2018). Metalinguistic Awareness and Second Language Acquisition. doi:10.4324/9781315661001. ISBN 978-1-315-66100-1.
- Trybulec, Marcin (March 2021). "Skillful use of symbolizations and the dual nature of metalinguistic awareness". Language Sciences. 84: 101356. doi:10.1016/j.langsci.2021.101356. S2CID 234023149.
- Magnusson, Eva (2018). "Metalinguistic awareness in phonologically disordered children". Phonological Disorders in Children. pp. 87–120. doi:10.4324/9780429435898-5. ISBN 978-0-429-43589-8. S2CID 150826090.
- Woll, Nina (April 3, 2018). "Investigating dimensions of metalinguistic awareness: what think-aloud protocols revealed about the cognitive processes involved in positive transfer from L2 to L3". Language Awareness. 27 (1–2): 167–185. doi:10.1080/09658416.2018.1432057. S2CID 149064632.
- Kopečková, Romana (April 3, 2018). "Exploring metalinguistic awareness in L3 phonological acquisition: the case of young instructed learners of Spanish in Germany". Language Awareness. 27 (1–2): 153–166. doi:10.1080/09658416.2018.1432629. S2CID 148925611.
- Eviatar, Zohar; Taha, Haitham; Shwartz, Mila (October 1, 2018). "Metalinguistic awareness and literacy among semitic-bilingual learners: a cross-language perspective". Reading and Writing. 31 (8): 1869–1891. doi:10.1007/s11145-018-9850-9. S2CID 149509478.
- Robinson Anthony, Jonathan J.D.; Blumenfeld, Henrike K.; Potapova, Irina; Pruitt-Lord, Sonja L. (March 16, 2022). "Language dominance predicts cognate effects and metalinguistic awareness in preschool bilinguals". International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism. 25 (3): 922–941. doi:10.1080/13670050.2020.1735990. PMC 8992601. PMID 35399223. S2CID 216346220.
- Tighe, Elizabeth L.; Little, Callie W.; Arrastia-Chisholm, Meagan Caridad; Schatschneider, Christopher; Diehm, Emily; Quinn, Jamie M.; Edwards, Ashley A. (March 1, 2019). "Assessing the direct and indirect effects of metalinguistic awareness to the reading comprehension skills of struggling adult readers". Reading and Writing. 32 (3): 787–818. doi:10.1007/s11145-018-9881-2. S2CID 150072721.
- Altman, Carmit; Goldstein, Tamara; Armon-Lotem, Sharon (October 23, 2018). "Vocabulary, Metalinguistic Awareness and Language Dominance Among Bilingual Preschool Children". Frontiers in Psychology. 9: 1953. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01953. PMC 6232916. PMID 30459672.
- Woll, Nina (January 2, 2019). "How French speakers reflect on their language: a critical look at the concept of metalinguistic awareness". Language Awareness. 28 (1): 49–73. doi:10.1080/09658416.2019.1567743. S2CID 150431051.
- Jessner, Ulrike (2006) Linguistic Awareness in Multilinguals Edinburgh University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.3366/edinburgh/9780748619139.001.0001