1. Introduction
Understanding the fundamental principles governing motions observed in Nature can be thought of as one of humanity’s earliest philosophical endeavours: Making sense of the perceived physical world naturally leads to the study of mechanics. After the pioneering works of Newton (1643–1727), mechanics considerably evolved thanks to the reformulations of Lagrange (1736–1813) and Hamilton (1805–1865), see e.g. [
1] for a detailed discussion of these formulations. The Newtonian approach relies on the fundamental equation
in Cartesian coordinates, where the total external force
acting on a pointlike body of inertial mass
m gives it an acceleration
, which in turn allows to yield the velocity
and position
of the body after successive integrations and upon imposing initial conditions. By invoking a principle of least action, Lagrange replaced Newton’s equation by the search for a single function named after him and denoted by
. In its original formulation, the Lagrangian
L depends on the configuration (position) space variables
and their first-order time derivative
, where
and the integer
n is the dimension of the configuration space, i.e., the number of degrees of freedom of the system once all the holonomic constraints have been taken into account. Typically,
, the difference between the kinetic and potential energy functions of the system. Lagrange’s postulate is that the dynamical system, during its temporal evolution between two instants
and
, will always minimize the action
. The motion is then given by Hamilton’s variational principle
subjected to the boundary conditions
.
In the realm of motor control, that resides at the intersection of biomechanics, neuroscience, and mathematics, there has been a paradigm shift akin to the one from Newtonian to Lagrangian mechanics. Instead of merely describing motion, researchers have begun to ask why organisms move the way they do. Indeed, certain actions or behaviours are repeated in a consistent manner. The concept of stereotyped movements has been observed centuries ago, among others by Sherrington in 1906 [
2]. The answer may lie in the minimization of a cost function, a mathematical representation of the effort, energy, or some other metric that a biological system tries to optimize. The interested reader may find reviews about optimal control theory in motor control in [
3,
4]. By quantifying the cost linked with diverse trajectories or control strategies, these functions illuminate pathways to optimal movement strategies.
Traditionally applied to describe the dynamics of non-living physical systems, the recent introduction of Lagrangians as cost functions offers new insights accompanied by new challenges. One frequently cited cost functions in motor control relies on what is called the
jerk,
, a vectorial quantity related to movement smoothness [
5]. Research suggests that when humans make smooth, unperturbed movements, the trajectory they follow tends to minimize the averaged squared jerk, i.e., the movement minimizes the following action [
6]
An apparent paradox that we aim to solve in the present study is that, from a mechanical point of view, the Lagrangian
does not lead to bounded trajectories. Indeed, the solutions
of the Euler-Lagrange equations
based on the Lagrangian
are polynomials of degree three in the time parameter
t; they are clearly not (quasi-)periodic solutions. The jerk has however been considered in the study of bounded trajectories, which is clearly problematic for describing voluntary movements such as the drawing of ellipses on a sheet of paper. We propose another framework in which higher-derivative actions produce bounded trajectories satisfying two-thirds law, see below, and in which the minimization of jerk appears to be a dynamical consequence of the variational principle. By higher-derivative we mean a Lagrangian
depending on the first
derivatives of the dynamical variables
, i.e., depending on
,
,
, etc. Three-dimensional vectors are assumed throughout. Study of such higher-derivative classical systems with finitely many degrees of freedom will be achieved by resorting to Ostrogradski’s approach [
7].
Although cost functions can be central in elucidating optimal movement trajectories, they are not the only framework in motor control research. Another approach is to identify conserved quantities or invariants. Invariants, as the term implies, are quantities whose value stays constant during dynamical evolution. A particularly illuminating example of this field is the empirical two-thirds power law, hereafter referred to as 2/3-PL. It has been found in [
8] and can be written as
with velocity
, speed
and
a constant. One speaks of “two-thirds" because this law’s original formulation involved the angular speed
where
is the curvature (the inverse of the radius
R of the osculating circle) of the trajectory at the given time, leading to
, i.e. the
exponent. The initial observation was that the speed of a drawing or writing movement is related to the curvature of the drawing. This law has since been observed in a wide range of planar movements, see e.g. the review [
9], and especially in elliptic trajectories. Note that a more general link of the type
may be observed in an even wider class of trajectories [
10], though we will not consider this generalization here. There has been a lot of debate on 2/3-PL, some authors claiming that it is mostly an artefact due to fitting procedures, while others (including the authors of this paper) argue that this law is indeed a behavioural consequence of a fundamental law in human motion [
9]. Results from various studies challenge a purely kinematic interpretation of the 2/3-PL and highlight the role of the central nervous system in motion planning, that leads to (
2), using motor imagery paradigms for covert movements [
11,
12].
This investigation aims to extend the understanding of cost functions through the lens of higher-derivative Lagrangians. It proposes that a class of higher-derivative actions broader than (
1) leads to the 2/3-PL, offering new insights into cost functions in human motion. This is developed in
Sec. 3 after general considerations about 2/3-PL in
Sec. 2.
Complementing the Lagrangian perspective, the Hamiltonian formalism provides a phase-space representation of the dynamics of a system as well as a general way to compute invariants through action-angle variables [
1]. The proposed higher-derivative Lagrangians will give rise to corresponding Hamiltonian functions through Ostrogradsky’s procedure [
7], after which the invariants will be computed in
Sec. 4. A contextualisation of these results in the framework of motor control will then be discussed in
Sec. 5.
2. Two-Thirds Law: Kinematical Considerations
In the context of three-dimensional motion in Euclidean space, the curvature and torsion of a given trajectory are obtained through the well-known formulae
where
and where the symbol × stands for the usual vector product in three-dimensional space. From the above definitions, one can write
Hence, 2/3-PL is valid if the norm of the vector
is constant. A sufficient condition is
, implying that
The coefficient function
may explicitly depend on time and on the various derivatives
. Therefore, a trajectory such that
satisfies (
2). The simplest choice is that of a constant function
, leading to elliptic trajectories. In [
13], different choices of the form
are explored, leading to trajectories that all comply with 2/3-PL.
Condition (
6) implies that the motion is planar, so that the torsion vanishes,
. Hence, non planar trajectories should not be related to (
2). Although it is not the main topic of our paper, it has been proposed in [
14] that the law
should hold in non-planar, three-dimensional motion, with
C a positive constant. From (
4) it can be deduced that
. In other words, the law is valid if
is constant, or
In the spirit of [
13], one can say that any trajectory solution of
will satisfy (
7).
5. Conclusion: First Principles Shaping Voluntary Motion
The application of Lagrangian and Hamiltonian formalisms in motor control demands a multidisciplinary approach that respects both a well-established mathematical formalism and the intricacies of human physiology. This paper proposed a broader class of higher-derivative Lagrangians that, upon defining appropriate initial conditions, lead to trajectories complying with the 2/3-PL, thus providing new insights into cost functions critical to human motion. These Lagrangians are given in Eq. (
10). A salient issue for the observation of 2/3-PL is the necessity of setting accurate initial conditions: If a Lagrangian involving up to the
time derivative
is used, it is necessary, in order to have a bounded motion, to set the Ostrogradski momenta
, with
. We recall that, for the class of Lagrangians (
10), the momenta are given by Eq. (
31). If these
initial conditions are unaligned with the natural capabilities of the human motor system, the considered Lagrangian is not qualified to model voluntary human movement. This consideration leads us to the conclusion that a minimal
N is the most natural choice. Therefore we think that
actions of the form
are favoured:
They naturally lead to 2/3-PL provided one individual is able to fix the initial condition . For a motion with vanishing initial speed one only needs to impose , irrespective of the choice of potential function U .
They may lead to a great variety of trajectories satisfying 2/3-PL according to the choice made for U. In the case of harmonic potential, elliptic trajectories are recovered, which are the best known case in which this law appears.
The action variable
makes explicitly appear the mean squared jerk, and it is known that minimizing this function (maximizing smoothness) is an experimentally observed principle in motor control [
15]. Mechanics imposes that
is constant but not necessarily minimal. However, provided
is a mass scale,
has the dimension of the mechanical power. Minimizing
during motion is therefore a way to minimize power expenditure.
Figure 1 gives a schematic representation of
in this case.
Beyond the confines of the 2/3-PL, humans exhibit various stereotyped behaviours, such as bell-shaped velocity profiles [
21] and adherence to Fitt’s law [
22]. In the first case, the kinematics of horizontal reaching movements invariably shows a peak velocity in the middle of the trajectory, with a slight asymmetry depending on whether the movement is upward or downward [
23]. These phenomena, which describe kinematic features of horizontal reaching movements and the compromise between speed and accuracy, are underpinned by the central nervous system’s capacity to maintain consistency in the face of environmental variability. Our study suggests that theoretical constructs like action variables may mirror such neural processes, serving as constants/constraints in the complex generation of human motion. The central nervous system, equipped with its intricate network of neurons and sensory receptors, plays a pivotal role in orchestrating movements. The brain, acting as the command centre, receives sensory feedback, processes it, and generates precise motor commands. It is conceivable that the brain, through its complex computations and feedback loops, sets dynamical principles and initial conditions that align with the physiological capabilities of the human body, ultimately giving rise to observed movement patterns, including the 2/3-PL.
An open challenge lies in identifying the neural correlates for these theoretical constructs. While certain brain regions responsible for sensory processing are well-understood, those involved in integrating multimodal sensory information to estimate physical forces like gravity remain less defined [
24]. Moreover, the potential role of the brain and nervous system in shaping appropriate initial conditions and motor control strategies is a crucial aspect of our proposal that remains to be elucidated. This not only affects the trajectories described by our mathematical models but also fundamentally influences the very ability of these models to mirror real human movement.
As a last comment we note that 2/3-PL and minimal jerk, that are closely linked within our framework, are not the only proposed models to describe voluntary motion. Let us consider Lagrangian (
47) in which the position degree of freedom is actually an angular degree of freedom describing a circular motion in a plane, i.e., a circular motion restricted to a single constant axis of rotation. In this case the movement would tend to minimize
, in which
is the angular acceleration. By virtue of Euler equation with a single rotation axis,
with
M the external torque applied on the considered body and
. This is a simplified version of the minimal torque-change model [
25], suggesting that voluntary human motion minimizes the rate of change of total external torque. This last comment suggests that, beyond 2/3-PL, the integration of Lagrangian and Hamiltonian formulations of mechanics may fit within a general motor control model of voluntary motion encompassing several kinds of cost functions.