Sleep Dreaming and Mental Health
Sleep Dreaming and Mental Health
Sleep Dreaming and Mental Health
CLINICAL REVIEW
a r t i c l e i n f o
s u m m a r y
Article history:
Received 26 February 2010
Received in revised form
14 July 2010
Accepted 14 July 2010
Available online 19 September 2010
Theories as to the function of sleep and dreaming and their relationship to emotions have been studied
since the beginning of recorded history. Earliest historical records show the predominant view to be that
dreams were considered divine in origin and only later did dream theory become linked with the
functioning of the brain, perhaps most famously in psychoanalytic theory. The development of sleep
laboratory techniques ushered in a new era of the dream study and their relationship to mental health. In
this review we outline the history of theories about the genesis and function of dreams and sleep and
their relationship to mental illness from ancient mythic and religious views to the rst tentative scientic
approaches to the ascendency of psychoanalysis and ultimately to the modern era of neuroscience.
! 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords:
Sleep disorders
Dreams
Depression
Psychiatric disorders
Introduction
Sleep and dreaming have always aroused our curiosity and
theories as to their cause and function have been described since the
beginning of recorded history.1e4 This fascination is widely evidenced in the arts and literature.5,6 In earliest times artists, philosophers, poets, as well as clerics, considered dreams to be divine
manifestations.1,4,7e9 They have also been an important topic of
inquiry throughout the history of philosophy.10 At the end of 19th
century the interpretation of dream content and its relationship
with human emotions became the focus of psychoanalytic
theory.11,12 In the early decades of this psychoanalytic era dreaming
was regarded as the meaningful reection of unconscious mental
functioning. In the 1950s dream research had a central role in the
elds of psychiatry and psychology.13 The development of sleep
laboratory techniques ushered in a new era of sleep/dream study
and their relation to mental health. The discovery of REM (rapid eye
movement) sleep and other advances in sleep research caused a shift
in focus. After a half century as the dominant paradigm of dream
analysis, psychology was largely eclipsed by neurobiology as
dreaming became closely identied with the physiological events of
REM sleep14 as well as NREM (non-rapid eye movement) sleep.15,16
Abbreviations: CDFF, corollary discharge or feed-forward; EEG, electroencephalography; NREM, non-rapid eye movement; rCBF, regional cerebral blood ow;
REM, rapid eye movement.
* Corresponding author. Tel.: 39 050 992 658; fax: 39 050 992 965
E-mail address: [email protected] (L. Palagini).
1087-0792/$ e see front matter ! 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2010.07.003
180
181
182
183
dreaming, and examine why many REM and NREM dreams are
indistinguishable.15,16,85e91 As yet, relatively few neuroimaging
studies have been dedicated to human sleep disorders, dreaming
and their relationship to mental illness. Future studies could attempt
to link dreaming experiences with patterns of regional cerebral
activity in psychiatric disorders. Thus far, precise neural correlates of
dreaming remain elusive and the denitive functions of dreaming
and of sleep itself e as well as their relationship to normal and
pathological emotions in humans e continue to elude us and remain
the subject of considerable debate.
One of the most intriguing similarities of dreams and psychosis
is that, in both states, neural activity in an individuals brain is
experienced as produced by the external environment. It has been
hypothesized that such experiences can be produced when the
corollary discharge or feed-forward (CDFF) mechanisms of the
mind are disabled. Such mechanisms were rst identied in
sensorimotor systems where they operate to distinguish sensory
experiences produced by self-initiated action (neural commands)
from sensory experiences produced by the environment. Feinberg128 hypothesized that similar systems exist for mental activity,
which Hughlings Jackson129 proposed is simply the highest level of
motor activity. In psychoses such as schizophrenia, neural activity
produced in the patients brain is experienced as produced from
activity coming from outside, often in the form of voices. It is true,
of course, that in the dreams of normal individuals, the hallucinatory experiences are more often visual than auditory. Nevertheless,
in both brain states e dreaming and psychosis e this disorder of
agency can be understood as a failure of CDFF systems, as Feinberg and Guazzelli130 pointed out. This eld is now ripe for
sophisticated experimentation with new imaging techniques.
Conclusion
In this review we have traced the evolution of our knowledge
and beliefs about sleep and dreaming, their function, and their
connection to mental illness. The various theories attempting to
explain these phenomena have shifted dramatically through
history; for centuries dreams were a means to manage worries
about the future, rst as messengers from gods, later as
a messenger from the unconscious. With new and improved
techniques for assessing brain function in vivo, attention has shifted away from studying the subjective characteristics of dreaming
to examining specic neurophysiology. Still, the integration of sleep
and dream study methodology with recent developments in neuroimaging and functional brain exploration provides an exciting
and promising area of inquiry that may one day help answer the
many remaining questions about the nature and function of sleep
and dreams, and their relationship to psychiatric illness.
Research agenda
1. The integration of sleep and dream study methodology
with the study of functional brain exploration in vivo in
psychiatric illness is an exciting and promising area of
inquiry that may one day help answer the many
remaining questions about the nature and function of
sleep and dreams in humans, and the pathophysiology
of the major psychiatric disorders.
2. Some potentially useful findings from earlier studies on
dreaming in patients with mood or anxiety disorders
may guide clinical care, as well as help guide future
research. Studies on dream content and recall in larger
samples of specific psychiatric disorders in future
research can increase our knowledge in this field.
184
Practice points
1. Since earliest times dream content has been studied in
relationship with human health and function.
2. The development of sleep laboratory techniques
ushered in a new era of the study of dreams and mental
health. Over time, this focus has become more neurobiological. Studies on dreaming have focused especially on dreaming mechanisms, functions and
relationship with cognitive human functions and sleep
research within a more neuroscientific perspective, and
shifted from dreaming to the neurobiological activity of
sleep and its relationship with mental health.
3. Perturbations of sleep have then been studied as
related to pathological emotional states: sleep disorders are thus commonly considered risk factors in the
development of psychiatric disorders, particularly
mood disorders. Pathological emotional states may
lead to sleep disturbance, and vice versa. The neurobiology of sleep may be moreover important in the
development and maintenance of brain function and
may have a possible role in the maintenance of
synaptic homeostasis. Prolonged restriction or
disruption of sleep may have cumulative effects leading
to a major decrease in hippocampal cell proliferation,
cell survival and neurogenesis.
4. Neuroscience and the development of neuroimaging
techniques have made possible the characterization
of many aspects of cerebral function throughout the
sleepewake cycle, but to date we do not yet fully
understand the significance of sleep and dreaming. The
various theories attempting to explain these
phenomena have shifted dramatically through history.
Acknowledgment
Thanks to Aldo Palagini for his important support.
References
1. Kurland ML. Oneiromancy: an historic review of dream interpretation. Am J
Psychother 1972;26(3):408e16.
2. Rupprecht CS. Our unacknowledged ancestors: dream theorists of antiquity,
the middle ages, and the renaissance. Psychiatr J Univ Ott 1990;15(2):117e22.
3. Leven KH. History of medicine. Through the horny or ivory gate e dream
interpretation in the antiquity. I. From Homer to Aristotle. Praxis 2004, Apr
14;93(16):684e8.
4. Hughes JD. Dream interpretation in ancient civilizations. Dreaming 2000;10
(1):7e18.
5. Rosselli D. Brief history of dreams. Rev Neurol 2000,16e31;30(2):195e8.
6. Stoll RT. Sleep and dreams in pictures. Praxis 1995, 11;84(15):432e9.
7. Oppenheim L. The interpretation of dreams in the ancient Near East, with
a translation of an Assyrian dream-book. Trans Am Phil Soc 1956;46
(3):179e373. New Series.
8. Szpakowska K. Through the looking glass. Dreams in ancient Egypt. In:
Bulkeley K, editor. Dreams: a reader on the religions, cultural and psychological dimensions of dreaming. New York: Palgrave; 2001. p. 29e43.
9. Barbera J. Sleep and dreaming in Greek and Roman philosophy. Sleep Med
2008;9:906e10.
10. Dreisbach C. Dreams in the history of philosophy. Dreaming 2000;10:31e41.
11. Freud S. The interpretation of dreams. Modern Library Edition. Toronto:
Random House Inc.; 1994.
12. Freud S. Project for a scientic psychology. Psychoanal Q; 1950. The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud.
13. Robbins M. Primary mental expression: freud, klein and beyond. J Am
Psychoanal Ass 2008;56:177.
14. Aserinsky E, Kleitman N. Regularly occurring periods of eye motility, and
concomitant phenomena during sleep. Science 1953;118:273e4.
15. Foulkes D, Vogel G. Mental activity at sleep onset. J Abnorm Psychol
1965;34:104e10.
185
92. Ansseau M, Kupfer DJ, Reynolds 3rd CF, McEachran AB. REM latency
distribution in major depression: clinical characteristics associated with
sleep onset REM periods. Biol Psychiatry 1984;19(12):1651e66.
93. Giles DE, Biggs MM, Rush AJ, Roffwarg HP. Risk factors in families of unipolar depression. I. Psychiatric illness andreduced REM latency. J Affect
Disord 1988;14(1):51e9.
94. Giles DE, Kupfer DJ, Rush AJ, Roffwarg HP. Controlled comparison of electrophysiological sleep in families of probands with unipolar depression. Am
J Psychiatry 1998;155(2):192e9.
95. Lauer CJ, Schreiber W, Holsboer F, Krieg JC. In quest of identifying vulnerability markers for psychiatric disorders by all-night polysomnography.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1995;52(2):145e53.
96. Kupfer DJ, Broudy D, Spiker DG, Neil JF, Coble PA. EEG sleep and affective
psychoses: I. Schizoaffective disorders. Psychiatry Res 1979;1(2):17.
97. Ford D, Kamerow D. Epidemiologic study of sleep disturbances and
psychiatric disorders: and opportunity for prevention? J Am Med Assoc
1989;262(11):1479e84.
98. Ohayon M, Roth T. Place of chronic insomnia in the course of depressive
and anxiety disorders. J Psychiatr Res 2003;37:9e15.
99. Szklo-Coxe M, Young T, Peppard PE, Finn LA, Benca RM. Prospective associations of insomnia markers and symptoms with depression. Am J Epidemiol 2010, 15;171(6):709e20.
100. Bryant R, Creamer M, ODonnell M, Silove D, McFarlane AC. Sleep disturbance immediately prior to trauma predicts subsequent psychiatric
disorder. Sleep 2010;33(1):69e74.
101. Svestka J. Sleep deprivation therapy. Neuro Endocrinol Lett 2008;29(1):65e92.
102. Manber R, Chambers AS. Insomnia and depression: a multifaceted interplay. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2009;11(6):437e42.
103. Tononi G, Cirelli C. Sleep and synaptic homeostasis: a hypothesis. Brain Res
Bull 2003, 15;62(2):143e50.
104. Tononi G.Slow wave homeostasis and synaptic plasticity. J Clin Sleep Med
2009;15(5):16e9.
105. Cirelli C. Cellular consequences of sleep deprivation in the brain. Sleep Med
Rev 2006;10:307e21.
106. Meerlo P, Mistlbeerger RE, Jacobs BL, Heller HC, McGinty D. New neurons in
the adult brain: the role of sleep and consequences of sleep loss. Sleep Med
Rev 2009;20(1):1e17.
*107. Abrous DN, Koehl M, Le Moal M. Adult neurogenesis: from precursors to
network and physiology. Physiol Rev 2005;85:523e69.
*108. Kempermann G, Wiskott L, Gage FH. Functional signicance of adult neurogenesis. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2004;14:186e91.
*109. Phelps EA. Human emotion and memory: interactions of the amygdala and
hippocampal complex. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2004;14:198e202.
110. Roman V, Van der Borght K, Leemburg SA, Van der Zee EA, Meerlo P. Sleep
restriction by forced activity reduces hippocampal cell proliferation. Brain
Res 2005;1065(1e2):53e9.
111. Tung A, Takase L, Fornal C, Jacobs B. Effects of sleep deprivation and
recovery sleep upon cell proliferation in adult rat dentate gyrus. Neuroscience 2005;134(3):721e3.
112. Guzman-Marin R, Bashir T, Suntsova N, Szymusiak R, McGinty D. Hippocampal neurogenesis is reduced by sleep fragmentation in the adult rat.
Neuroscience 2007, 10;148(1):325e33.
113. Hairston IS, Little MT, Scanlon MD, Barakat MT, Palmer DT, Sapolsky RM,
et al. Sleep restriction suppresses neurogenesis induced by hippocampusdependent learning. J Neurophysiol 2005;94(6):4224e33.
114. Lucassen PJ, Meerlo P, Naylor AS, van Dam AM, Dayer AG, Fuchs E, et al.
Regulation of adult neurogenesis by stress, sleep disruption, exercise and
inammation: implications for depression and antidepressant action.
Neuropsychopharmacology 2010;20(1):1e17.
115. Ad Muller, Pollock MS, Lieblich SE, Epp JR, Galea LA, Mistberger RE. Sleep
deprivation can inhibit adult hippocampal neurogenesis independent of adrenal
stress hormones. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 2008;294(5):1693e703.
116. Stickgold R. Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature 2005;437:1272e8.
*117. Ruskin DN, Liu C, Dunn KE, Bazan NG, LaHoste GJ. Sleep deprivation impairs
hippocampus-mediated contextual learning but not amygdala-mediated
cued learning in rats. Eur J Neurosci 2004;19:3121e4.
118. Peigneux P, Laureys S, Fuchs S, Collette F, Perrin F, Reggers J, et al. Are spatial
memories strengthened in the human hippocampus during slow wave
sleep? Neuron 2004;44:535e45.
119. Orban P, Rauchs G, Balteau E, Degueldre C, Luxen A, Maquet P, et al. Sleep
afterspatial learning promotes covert reorganization of brain activity. Proc
Natl Acad Sci U S A 2006;103:7124e9.
120. Backhaus J, Junghanns K, Born J, Hohaus K, Faasch F, Hohagen F. Impaired
declarative memory consolidation during sleep in patients with primary
insomnia: inuence of sleep architecture and nocturnal cortisol release.
Biol Psychiatry 2006;60:1324e30.
121. Riemann D, Voderholzer U, Spiegelhalder K, Psych D, Hornyak M, Buysse DJ,
et al. Chronic insomnia and MRI-measured hippocampal volume: a pilot
study. Sleep 2007;30:955e85.
122. Jacobs BL, Praag H, Gage FH. Adult brain neurogenesis and psychiatry:
a novel theory of depression. Mol Psychiatry 2000;5:262e9.
123. Maquet P, Degueldre C, Delore G, Aerts J, Peters JM, Luxen A, et al. Functional
neuroanatomy of human slow wave sleep. J Neurosci 1997;17(8):2807e12.
124. Maquet P. Functional neuroimaging of normal human sleep by positron
emission tomography. J Sleep Res 2000;9(3):207e31.
186