Grammatical Cohesion and Textuality
Grammatical Cohesion and Textuality
Grammatical Cohesion and Textuality
INTRODUCTION
The study and analysis of actual language in used is the goal of text and discourse
analysis. Michael Halliday, one of the linguists credited with the development of systemic
linguistics and functional grammar, defines text as any authentic stretch of written or spoken
language. According to Hallyday (1994:14) the historical study of linguistics first involved
studying the morphology of language followed by studying the meaning of words at the sentence
level. Ultimately the goal of such analysis was to find the meaning of the forms of language.
interpreted as a system of meanings, accompanied by forms through which the meaning can be
expressed”. Beyond the grammar and lexis of language, understanding the mechanisms for how
text is structured is the basis for his work. What makes any length of text meaningful and
coherent has been termed texture. Texture is the basis for unity and semantic interdependence
within text and a text without texture would just be a group of isolated sentences with no
relationship to one another. Eggins (1994:85) refers to the term put forth by schegloff and Sacks
sequence where one line of text follows another with each line being linked or related to the
previous line. This linear progression of text creates a context for meaning. Contextual meaning,
at the paragraph level is referred to as “coherence” while the internal properties of meaning are
referred to as “cohesion”. Coherence has both situational coherence when field, tenor, and mode
can be identified for a certain group of clauses and “generic” coherence when the text can be
recognized as belonging to a certain genre. Cohesion relates to the semantic ties within text
where by a tie is made when there is some dependent link between items that combine to create
1
meaning. Therefore, texture is created within text when there are properties of coherence and
cohesion, outside of the apparent grammatical structure of the text. The principles of referencing,
substitution, ellipsis, conjunction, and lexical cohesion put forth by Halliday and Hasan (1976)
and Bloor and Bloor (1995) will be applied to the article and analyzed to demonstrate the
relevance of the cohesive elements that are present in texts which contribute to the overall
meaning of the text. Understanding how cohesion functions within text to create semantic links
meaning.
2
CHAPTER II
CRITICISM
A. Principle of Cohesion
the structure. Cohesion refers to the non-structural text-forming relations. (Halliday and Hasan
1976:7) the concept of cohesion in text is related to semantic ties or relations of meanings that
exist within the text, and that define it as a text (ibid:4). Within text, if a previously mentioned
item is referred to again and is dependent upon another element, it is considered a tie. Without
semantic ties, sentences or utterances would seem to lack any type of relationship to each other
and might not be considered text. Halliday and Hasan (ibid:4) refer to this intertextual link as the
presupposing and the presupposed. Using the authors’ example: “Wash and core six cooking
apples. Put them into a fireproof dish.” The word “them” presupposes “apples” and provides a
semantic tie between the two sentences, thus creating cohesion. Cohesion creates
interdependency in text.
B. Reference
able for it to be considered as cohesive. In written text, referencing indicates how the writer
(Eggins 1994:95) there are three general types of referencing are as follows:
1. Exophoric referencing, this refers to information from the immediate context of situation.
2. Endophoric referencing, which refers to information that can be retrieved from within the
3
Endophoric referencing can be divided into three areas are:
a) Anaphoric
information in text.
For cohesion purpose, anaphoric referencing is the most relevant as it provides a link
with a preceding portion of the text (Halliday and Hasan 1976:51). Functional
speaking, there are three main types of cohesive references, they are:
Person
Person reference keeps track of function through the speech situation using
noun pronouns like “He, him, she, her, etc”. And possessive determiners like
For example: Petter has many books. Those books are his.
Demonstrative
proximity references like “this, these, that, those, here, there, then, and the.”
Comparative
references using adjectives like “same, equal, similar, different, else, better,
more.” And adverbs like “so, such, similarly, otherwise, so, more, etc.” (ibid:
37-39)
4
For example: Jack has a white shirt. I bought the same shirt with him.
b) Cataphoric
Cataphoric refers to any reference that point forward to information that will be
c) Esphoric
Esophoric refers to any reference within the same nominal group or phrase which
Whereas referencing functions to link semantic meanings within text, substitution and
ellipsis differs in that it operates as a linguistic link at the lexicogrammatical level. In Bloor and
Bloor (1995:96), substitution and ellipsis is used when a speaker or writer wishes to avoid the
repetition of a lexical item and is able to draw on one of the grammatical resources of the
For example: Wealthier Italians whose parents quit farms for the cities in the 60’s are
coming back.
The word “coming back” refers to line before in which the elliptical references to the
children of farmers are returning to the farms that their parents quit.
The three types of classification for substitution and ellipsis that reflect its grammatical function
are as follows:
5
1. Nominal
In nominal substitution, the most typical substitution words are “one and ones” and they
substitute nouns.
2. Verbal
In verbal substitution, the most common substitute is the verb “do” and is sometime used
in conjunction with “so” as in “do so” and substitute verbs. Halliday and Hasan (ibid:
125-126) point out that “do” often operate with the reference items “it” and “that” but
still have the main function as a verbal substitute because of its grammatical role.
From: you buy a new book and she buys a ne book too.
3. Clause
In clause substitution, an entire clause is substituted and though it may seem to be similar
reference.
When something in text is being substituted, it follows that the substituted item maintains
the same structural function as the presupposed item. Though substitution and ellipsis are similar
in their function as the linguistic link for cohesion, ellipsis differs in that it is substitution by zero
(ibid: 142). Ellipsis refers to a presupposed anaphoric item although the reference is not through
a place-marker like in substitution. The presupposed item is understood through its structural
link. As it is a structural link, ellipsis operates through nominal, verbal and clausal levels.
Halliday and Hasan further classify ellipsis in systemic linguistic terminology as deictic,
6
D. Conjunction
Conjunction, as described by Bloor and Bloor (1995:98) acts as a cohesive tie between
clause or sections of text in such a way as to demonstrate a meaningful pattern between them,
though Halliday and Hasan (ibid: 227) indicate that conjunction relations are not tied to any
particular sequence in the expression. Therefore, amongst the cohesion forming devices within
Conjunction acts as a semantic cohesive tie within text in four categories, they are:
Additive
item and are signaled through “and, also, too, furthermore, additionally”, etc. additive
conjunction may also act to negate the presupposed item and is signaled by “nor, and…
Adversative
Adversative conjunctions act to indicate “contrary to expectation” (ibid: 250) and are
Causal
Causal conjunction expresses “result, reason and purpose” and is signaled by “so, then,
For example: She studied match hardly as a result she passed the exam.
7
Temporal
The last conjunctive category is temporal and links by signaling sequence or time.
Some samples temporal conjunctive signals are “then, next, after, that, next day, until
Most learners, when learning the grammar of a foreign language, spend time assimilating
the structure of clauses in that language, i.e. where subjects, objects and adverbials are placed in
relation to the verb, and what options are available for rearranging the most typical sequences.
Discourse analysis are interested in the implications of these different structural options for the
creation of text, and as always, it is from the examination of natural data that patterns of use are
seen to emerge. Some of the structural options frequently found in natural data are ignored or
underplayed in language teaching (especially those found in spoken data, which are often
dismissed as degraded or bad style), probably owing to the continued dominance of standards
English is what is often called an ‘SVO’ language, in that the declarative clause requires
a verb at its centre, a subject before it and any object after it. This is simply a labeling device
which enables comparisons to be made with declarative realizations in different languages, some
of which will be ‘VSO’ or ‘SOV’ languages. This pattern is often recast in English, no least in
interrogative structures, where the verbal group is split by the subject (“does she like cats?”), and
(OSV Object-fronted)
8
There are in English a variety of ways in which the basic clause elements of subject, verb,
and complement object, adverbial can be rearranged by putting different elements at the
Structures such as sentence 2 and 3 are far from infrequent in spoken data, but are often
for no obvious reason, not presented in books claiming to describe grammatical options for the
learner. If we look again at our examples from the point of view of how the information in them
is presented, we can see how different options enable us to focus on or highlight certain
elements: sentence1 seems to be saying something about the Guardian rather than about Joyce;
sentence 2 and 3 seem to be telling us something about Joyce. This aboutness is the sort of
notion discourse analysis are concerned with for it is a speaker/writer choice made independently
of the propositional content of the message; the speaker/writer decides how to stage the
In English, what we decide to bring to the front of the clause (by whatever means) is a
signal of what is to be understood as the framework within which what we want to say is to be
understood. The rest of the clause can then be seen as transmitting what we want to say within
this framework. Items brought to front-place in this way we shall call the themes (or topics) of
their clauses in what has been called the Prague school of linguistics, the relationship of the
theme to the rest of the sentence is viewed as part of communicative dynamism, that is the
9
assessment of the extent to which each element contributes to the development of the
communication. Alternatively, the theme can be seen as the point of departure of the message
(Halliday 1985:38). For the moment, we shall take as the theme of a clause the subject noun-
phrase, or, if this is not initial, then we shall include whatever comes before it. It seems that first
position in the clause is important in many of the world’s languages, and that creating a theme in
the clause is a universal feature, though its realizations may vary from language to language.
Concentrating on the themes (or topics) of clauses does not tell us much about the rest of
the clause, which may be called the rheme or comment of the clause. In fact, when we look at
themes and rhemes together in connected text, we see further patterns emerging we can divide
1. I ‘m sitting here…
2. Outside my window is a big lawn…
3. In the middle of the lawn is a flower bed
4. The bed was full of daffodils…
5. You ‘d love it here
6. You must come and stay
7. We ‘ve got plenty of room
Two different options can be sees to be realized here: (a) the rheme of sentence 3
contains an element (the flower bed) which becomes the theme of sentence 4; (b) the theme of
10
CHAPTER III
CONCLUSION
This chapter has taken a selection of grammatical concepts and has attempted to show
how discourse analysis has contributed to our understanding of the relationship between local
choice within the clause and sentence and the organization of the discourse as a whole. When
speakers and writers are producing discourse, they are at the same time as they are busy
constructing clause, monitoring the development of the larger discourse, and their choices at the
local level can be seen simultaneously to reflect the concerns of the discourse as an unfolding
grammar would suggest not only a grater emphasis on contexts larger than the sentence, but also
a reassessment of priorities in terms of what is taught about such things as word order, articles,
ellipsis, tense and aspect, and some of the other categories discussed here.
If grammar is seen to have a direct role in welding clauses, turns and sentences into
discourse, what of words themselves? What role does vocabulary choice play in the discourse
11
Answer question
c. Look at that
2. There are three types of classification for ellipsis and substitution that reflect its
a. Nominal
b. Verbal
c. Homophoric
d. Clausal
d. Jack has white shirt. I bought the same shirt with him
4. Ellipsis is the omission of elements normally required by the grammar which the
speaker/writer assumes are obvious from the context and therefore need not be raised.
a. Ellipsis
b. Conjunction
c. Clausal
12
d. Reference
a. Personal
b. Demonstrative
c. Comparative
d. Cataphoric
Answer key
2. C. Homophoric
3. D. Jack has white shirt. I bought the same shirt with him
4. A. Ellipsis
5. D. Cataphoric
13
REFERENCES
Brown Gillian & George Yule. 1984. Discourse Analysis. Cambridge University Press.
Crane Paul A. 1994. Texture in Text: A Discourse Analysis of A News Article Using
Halliday and Hasan’s Model of Cohesion. Retrived (09 nov 2009) from
http://library.nakanishi.ac.ip/kiyou/gaidai%2830%29/08.pdf.
14