''The Structure of North Sami''
''The Structure of North Sami''
''The Structure of North Sami''
Department of Linguistics
The University of Utah
Spring 2009
1. INTRODUCTION
1.1. The Saami languages
Saami (alternative spellings: Sami, Smi) is a general name for the indigenous people(s)
in Lapland (including Central and Northern Scandinavia, Northern Finland, and the Kola
Peninsula in Northwestern Russia). Formerly the Saami peoples were generally called
Lapps, but this term has fallen out of use, as it is often considered to have a derogatory
tone.
South Saami
Ume Saami
Pite Saami
Lule Saami
North Saami
Inari Saami
300400
10 at most
20 at most
10002000
33000
400
Skolt Saami
Kemi Saami
Akkala Saami
Kildin Saami
Ter Saami
300
extinct since late 19th century
extinct since 2003
600
10
Six Saami languages South, Lule, North, Inari, Skolt and Kildin Saami have an
independent literary standard. The Kildin Saami writing system employs a slightly
modified Cyrillic alphabet, whereas the others are based on the Latin alphabet.
Genetically, the Saami languages form one subbranch of the Uralic (Finno-Ugric)
language family; the Saami languages are thus remotely related to languages such as
Finnish, Estonian and Hungarian, as well as numerous indigenous languages of European
Russia and western Siberia. In terms of time depth, the Uralic affinity is very remote, and
the various Uralic branches are no more closely related to each other than branches of
Indo-European, for instance. Within the Uralic family Saami shares the most features
with the neighboring Finnic languages (such as Finnish and Estonian); a large part of
these shared features are due to language contact rather than genetic inheritance, though.
vulggi-i PAST.3SG
(s)he left, (s)he went (away)
Some important general features of North Saami morphosyntax are listed below.
o Constituent order:
o North Saami is primarily a SVO language, but in certain (non-finite)
constructions the word order SOV is preferred.
o Postpositions are preferred against prepositions (even though some
prepositions also exist).
o Nouns:
o North Saami has a well-developed case system with six cases. Nouns
distinguish between singular and plural number.
o Nouns can also take possessive suffixes: e.g. dlus in a/the house :
dlustan in my house : dlustat in your house.
o Adjectives:
o Adjectives often have a distinct forms in predicative and attribute position:
e.g. mtki lea guhkki the trip is long vs. guhkes mtki a long trip.
o Pronouns:
o Personal pronouns distinguish three numbers: e.g. mun I, moai we two,
mii we (more than two)
o The system of demonstrative pronouns has five members: dt this, diet
that (near you), duot that (not near either of us), dot that (which is far
away), dat it
o Verbs:
o Verbs have four tenses (present, past, perfect, pluperfect); there is no
separate future tense.
o Verbs have four moods: indicative, imperative (imperative-optative),
conditional, dubitative.
o Verbs with human subjects distinguish between singular, dual and plural:
vuolggn I leave, vulge we (two) leave, vuolgit we (more than two)
leave.
o Verbs have a wide variety of non-finite forms.
o Negation is expressed with a negative verb: in I dont, it you dont, ii
(s)he doesnt, etc.
o There is no separate class of auxiliary verbs.
The western and eastern inland dialects are often called Finnmark dialects (according to the Norwegian
county of Finnmark), with further western and eastern subgroups. This division seems to be mistaken,
however, as the two dialect groups do not share any common traits that would set them apart from Sea and
Torne dialects. Moreover, the term Finnmark dialects is a misnomer, because these dialects are also
spoken outside the Norwegian county of Finnmark, and on the other hand, also Sea dialects are spoken in
Finnmark.
handout, two pronunciation keys are given: one approximating a typical WI and another a
typical EI pronunciation. Minor phonological differences between subdialects will be
ignored. The geographically more marginal Torne and Sea dialects show quite notable
phonological differences from the inland dialects; these will not be dealt with in this
presentation.
p
b
+
p
t
d
+
t
c
z
s
m n
(M) (N)
v
l
(L)
r
(R)
tj
dj
k
g
+
k
nj
j
(J)
The aspirated stops can only occur in initial position, and they are mostly restricted to
recent Scandinavian loanwords such as boasta /psta/ post office, teaksta /tksta/
text and knske /knske/ perhaps (< Norwegian / Swedish kanske perhaps). They do
not occur in the WI dialects and they are not consistently indicated in the orthography.
short
i
e
u
o
long
ii
ee
uu
oo
aa
In the EI dialects there are seven monophthongs, all of which occur in the same three
quantitaties:
short
i
e
u
o
long
ii
ee
aa
uu
oo
The vowel // has a very slight labialization (o-like color) and it is pronounced slightly
more back than /a/.
Diphthongs likewise occur in three quantities: short, long, and finally stressed. The
diphthongs of the WI dialects are:
short
long
ie
ea
uo
oa
finally stressed
ie'
uo'
ea'
oa'
long
ie
i
uo
ua
finally stressed
ie'
ue'
e'
oa'
Finally stressed diphthongs have a short first component and a stressed and lengthened
second component. Their pronunciation is somewhat similar to certain Spanish
diphthongs (cf. bueno, puerto, ciudad).
Note: there is also one more marginal vowel phoneme, the front rounded vowel /y/. It
only occurs in incompletely assimilated borrowings and internationalisms (e.g.
psykologiija psychology, myhta myth, fysihkka physics), but not in native North
Saami vocabulary.
2.1.3. Word-stress
Word-stress in North Saami is almost fully predictable, and stress has no phonologically
contrastive function. The main stress always falls on the first syllable of a word.
Secondary stress normally falls on each subsequent odd syllable, except that the final
8
syllable of a word is never stressed. In the examples below, main stress is indicated by
double underlining, secondary stress by single underlining:
dolla
dolastis
dolastallat
dolastallamin
dolastaladettiin
dolastaladettiinan
fire
in his/her fire
to keep an outdoor fire
keeping an outdoor fire
while keeping an outdoor fire
while I am/was keeping an outdoor fire
There are a couple of minor exceptions to the assignment of secondary stress. As a rule,
clitics attached at the end of words are never stressed, and moreover, secondary stress
cannot be assigned to the syllable immediately preceding a clitic. Consider the following
examples involving the interrogative clitics -go (used for marking questions) and -son
(used to indicate that the question is rhetorical):
gulai
gulaigo
gulaigoson
muitalii
muitaliigo
muitaliigoson
(s)he heard
did (s)he hear?
I wonder if (s)he heard
(s)he told
did (s)he tell?
I wonder if (s)he told
As another type of exception, there are words where secondary stress falls on an even
syllable instead of an odd syllable. These kinds of words are usually either recent
loanwords or irregularly shortened compounds:
reageret to react (< Norwegian reagere id.)
televiuvdna television
vilbealle male cousin (<< viellja brother + bealli half)
There is also one derivational suffix which always receives secondary stress, regardless
of whether it is in an even or an odd syllable: the suffix -()goahtit which forms
inchoative verbs (with the sense of to begin doing X):
dahkat to do
muitalit to tell
Kk
Uu
Bb
Ll
Vv
Cc
Mm
Yy
Nn
Zz
Dd
Oo
Ee
Pp
Ff
Rr
Gg
Ss
Hh
Ii
Tt
The letters c, , z and indicate affricates, is a voiced dental spirant, is its unvoiced
pair, is the velar nasal, and is the hushing sibilant (cf. English sh, German sch).
The vowel y is a high rounded front vowel (cf. German ); it only occurs in recent
loanwords and internationalisms (e.g. myhta myth, psykologiija psychology,
analyseret to analyze).
work
need
I stay
I shoot
A second, related rule is that when the letter p, t, k, c, or occurs after another consonant,
it is usually accompanied by a preceding h-sound (preaspiration) which is not spelled
separately. The following examples illustrate this:
10
(s)he cries
tree bark
stain
serious accident
(s)he must
it bounces off
narrow
tendon (in meat)
bay
trip, journey
spare time
autumn colors on trees
In North Saami, i is both a vocalic and a consonantal letter. Whenever i occurs after
another vowel, it stands for the consonant /j/:
libi (WI /laajpii/, EI /ljpii/)
libi (WI /laajpaaj/, EI /ljpj/)
nieida (WI, EI /njta/)
niidii (WI, EI /nijtij/)
bread
to a/the bread
girl
to a/the girl
fence
storage house
(s)he intends to
old pine tree
In the WI dialects, between vowels b and g are pronounced as the stops /p/ and /k/,
respectively. In EI dialects the pronunciation is different: between vowels b is
pronounced as /v/ and g is pronounced either as /j/ or not at all (as , i.e. zero). The
semivowel /j/ occurs if the preceding syllable is stressed and the following unstressed
vowel is , e or i; otherwise g is not pronounced at all.
lobi (WI /lopii/, EI /loovii/)
agi (WI /akii/, EI /jii/)
viegan (WI /viekaan/, EI /vie.aan/)
dagu (WI /takuu/, EI /t.uu/)
of a permission
of age
I run
of a deed
11
arrow
daughter-in-law
to drive
brother
pole, staff
price
grandfather
hook (and line, for fishing)
nail
urine
In the WI dialects the voicelessness rule also has another exception: if the letter b, d or
g is followed by a nasal, it is pronounced voiced. The EI dialects use an unvoiced stop in
these cases.
gobmi (WI /kobmii/, EI /kopmii/)
ghost
bodni (WI /podnii/, EI /potnii/)
bottom
j j
j j
boadnj (WI /pd n aa/, EI /pt n /) husband
duogat (WI /tdjnjah(t), EI /tkah(t)/ to patch
In the WI dialects, the velar nasal has merged with the palatalized nasal nj; both are
pronounced as /nj/. EI dialects retain the distinction.
mais (WI /manjiis/, EI /miis/) behind (in the direction one is coming from)
manjis (WI /manjiis/, EI /mnjiis/) daughter-in-law (locative form)
jieka (WI /jietnja/, EI /jieka/)
ice
Word-final -t is pronounced as /h/. Before a pause the pronunciation /ht/ may occur, but
never in the middle of a sentence:
Olbmot mannet vuojadit geassit.
WI: /olpmoh manneh vuojaih keassiih(t)/
EI: /olmoh mnneh vuojaeh kissiih(t)/
People go swimming in the summer.
12
In the EI dialects the pronunciation of vowels is even more complicated than in the WI
dialects. At this point, only a couple of general rules regarding vowel quality (but not
quantity!) will be introduced:
In a stressed syllable, the letter a stands for the vowel //:
dat (EI /th(t)/)
mana (EI /m'na/)
salla (EI /slla/)
it
go!
lap, bosom
In a stressed syllable, is pronounced /a/ before a next syllable back vowel (a, o, u) and
// elsewhere:
mnnu (EI /maannuu/)
njrga (EI /njarka/)
dkko (EI /taa'kko/)
mrfi (EI /mrfii/)
mnn (EI /mnn/)
dl (EI /tl/)
moon
cape, land point
this way
sausage
child
now
(s)he goes
13
3. MORPHOPHONOLOGY
North Saami has an intricate system of morphophonological alterations that affect the
shape of word-roots. These alterations are realized in the inflectional paradigms of verbs,
nouns, adjectives, quantifiers, and pronouns, as well as in word-formational operations
(derivation and compounding).
There are six main categories of regular morphophonological alterations, the most
important being the first two:
A) Consonant gradation refers to a systematic alteration of consonants and
consonant clusters on the border of a stressed and an unstressed syllable (usually
the first and the second syllable). The following serve as examples of consonant
gradation in the declension of nouns: vrri mountain : vris on the mountain,
goahti Saami tent : goais in the tent, lgu beginning : lggus in the
beginning.
B) Diphthong simplification refers to a process whereby diphthongs become
monophthongs in certain word-forms. The following serve as examples of
diphthong simplification in verb conjugation: vuolgit to go away : moai vulge
the two of us go away, ierrut to cry : moai irro the two of us cry.
C) Unstressed vowel alteration refers to qualitative alterations of vowels in an
unstressed syllable (usually the second syllable). Such alterations are frequent in
both noun and verb paradigms, e.g. hi father : hn my father; bhit to
shoot : bn I shoot : bhen I shot.
D) Margin consonant alterations are alterations of consonants at the end of wordstems (i.e., on the stem margin). The alterations may be qualitative, as in nvet
cowhouse : nveh-is in the cowhouse. On the other hand, there are also
alterations between the presence of a consonant vs. its absence (, zero): for
example : m in eana earth, ground, eatnamis on the ground; : g in otta
throat : oddagis in the throat.
E) Stressed vowel length alteration refers to the alteration in the length of a
stressed vowel (usually the vowel in the first syllable). There are major dialectal
differences in the nature and scope of these alterations, and for this reason they
are not indicated in the literary language. Examples of alteration between short
and long vowels in the EI dialects: vulgen /vulken/ I went away : vulggii
/vuulkkij/ (s)he went away; vuorddn /vuorttn/ I wait : vuord /vrt/
(s)he waits.
F) Alteration between so-called largo and allegro forms. Many word-forms,
such as finite verb forms and certain case forms of nouns, have two alternative
14
15
hi
geahi
guos'si
guossi
geassi
goho
Quantity I
geasi
gou
Quantity I is always a single consonant, whereas Quantity II and III are always either
geminates (double consonants) or consonant clusters. In each word, the gradation takes
place between two quantity groups. Most cases fall into two types:
Note that the distinction between long and short geminates is not indicated in normal spelling. The
symbol ' (in e.g. guos'si) is used in linguistic notation to indicate a long geminate. In normal literary usage
the SG.NOM and the SG.GEN of these kinds of words are spelled identically (guossi), even though there is a
difference in pronunciation!
16
1) the strong grade is Quantity II and the weak grade is Quantity I (e.g. geahi,
geassi)
2) the strong grade is Quantity III and the weak grade is Quantity II (e.g. hi,
guos'si)
There is also a rare third type, which only occurs in the paradigms of certain types of
nouns (the so-called contracting s-nouns and mutating u-nouns), but never in verbs.
3) the strong grade is Quantity III and the weak grade is Quantity I (e.g. gou
soot)
This exceptional type of gradation results from the a morphophonological process called
strong grade strengthening, which is explained in subsection 3.1.3. below.
3.1.2. Types of consonant gradation
In the subsections below, the gradating pairs are grouped into categories, each of which
behaves in a distinct manner phonologically. For each group, two forms from the
paradigm of one example word are cited to illustrate each possible case of consonant
gradation. (In the case of nouns, the forms are SG.NOM and SG.GEN, in the case of verbs
INF and 1SG.)
3.1.2.1. Geminate consonant as the strong grade, single consonant as the weak grade
(Q II Q I)
This is a very common type of alteration. The strong grade consists of a geminate
continuant consonant, the weak grade of a corresponding single consonant.
SG
WG example
WI pronunciation
EI pronunciation
gloss
oait oan
/oaiih(t) oaaan/
/uaiih(t) uan/
to sleep
ff
geaffi geafi
/keaffii keafii/
/kiffii kifii/
poor
ll
giella giela
/kiella kiela/
/kiella kiela/
language
mm
namma nama
/nammaa namaa/
/nmma nma/
name
nn
mnn mn
/maannaa maanaa/
/mnn mn/
child
nnj
nj
mannji manji
j j
j j
/mn n ii mn ii/
daughter-in-law
j j
j j
j j
mai mai
/mii mii/
rear, back
rr
muorra muora
/muorra muora/
/muorra muora/
tree
ss
gussa gusa
/kussaa kusaa/
/kuussa kuusa/
cow
vai vai
/vaii vaii/
/vii vii/
hatred
Ruoa Ruoa
/ruoa ruoa/
/ruoa ruoa/
Sweden
vv
suovva suova
/suovva suova/
/suovva suova/
smoke
17
WG example
WI pronunciation
EI pronunciation
gloss
f'f
ff
jf'fu jffu
/jaaf'fuu jaaffuu/
/jaffuu jaaffuu/
flour
l'l
ll
ul'lu ullu
/ul'luu ulluu/
/ulluu uulluu/
wool
llj
lj
j j
j j
j j
j j
viellja vielja
/vl l a viel l a/
brother
m'm mm
cum'm cumm
/cum'maa cummaa/
/cumm cuumm/
kiss
n'n
bean'na beanna
/pn'na peanna/
/pnna pinna/
pen
nn
j j
'
ha' ha
/h h/
long-tailed duck
r'r
rr
skear'ru skearru
/skr'ruu skearruu/
/skrruu skirruu/
disc
s's
ss
bus's buss
/pus'saa pussaa/
/puss puuss/
cat
'
v'v
vv
neav'vu neavvu
/nv'vuu neavvuu/
/nvvuu nivvuu/
advice; tool
WG example
j
vuodja vuoja
WI pronunciation
j j
/vuot t a vuoja/
EI pronunciation
j j
/vuot t a vuoja/
gloss
butter, grease
3.1.2.4. Preaspirated stop or affricate in the strong grade, single consonant in the
weak grade (Q II Q I)
This is a common type of alteration. In the strong grade there is a preaspirated consonant
cluster, consisting of h + a following stop or affricate. The corresponding weak grade is a
single, non-preaspirated stop or affricate except in the case of the cluster -ht-, which has
the spirant -- as its weak grade.
18
A note on dialectal pronunciation: in EI dialects, the weak grade -b- is pronounced as /v/,
and the weak grade -g- as either /-j-/ or nothing at all (zero, ). Some older speakers
pronounce the weak grade -g- as a voiced velar spirant /--/, but this has become very
rare.
SG
WG example
WI pronunciation
EI pronunciation
gloss
hp
lohpi lobi
/lohpii lopii/
/loohpii loovii/
permission
ht
giehta giea
/kiehta kiea/
/kiehta kiea/
hand, arm
hk
johka joga
/johkaa jokaa/
ahki agi
/ahkii akii/
age
hc
beahci beazi
/peahcii peacii/
/pihcii picii/
pine tree
geahi geai
/keahii keaii/
/kihii kiii/
end, tip
WG example
WI pronunciation
EI pronunciation
gloss
hpp
hp
/vhpa vuohpa/
father-in-law
htt
ht
Mhtte Mhte
/mah'te maahte/
/mhte mhte/
Matthew
hkk
hk
lohkka lohka
/loh'ka lohkaa/
/lohka loohka/
lock
hcc
hc
fhcca fhca
/fah'ca faahca/
/fahca faahca/
mitten
hi hi
/ah'ii aahii/
/hii hii/
father
3.1.2.6. Stop + nasal in the strong grade, a single nasal in the weak grade (Q II Q
I)
Clusters written with an unvoiced stop letter (p, t, k) followed by a nasal have single nasal
consonants as their weak grade counterparts.
SG
WG example
WI pronunciation
EI pronunciation
gloss
pm
Spmi Smi
/saapmii saamii/
/spmii smii/
Lapland
tn
vuotna vuona
/vuotna vuona/
/vuotna vuona/
tnj
k
nj
botnjat bonjan
jieka jiea
/jietn a jien a/
fjord
j
ice
19
WG example
WI pronunciation
EI pronunciation
gloss
bm
pm
gobmi gopmi
/kobmii kopmii/
/kopmii koopmii/
ghost
dn
tn
bodni botni
/podnii potnii/
/potnii pootnii/
bottom
dnj
g
tnj
k
boadnji boatnji
husband
WG example
WI pronunciation
EI pronunciation
gloss
bb
pp
oabb oapp
/bpaa oappaa/
/bp uapp/
sister
dd
tt
haddi hatti
/hadtii hattii/
/hdtii httii/
price
gg
kk
biegga biekka
/pgka piekka/
/pgka piekka/
wind
j j
j j
j j
j j
ddj
dj
ddj dj
/d t t t /
grandfather
zz
cc
gazza gacca
/kazca kaccaa/
/kzca kcca/
nail, hoof
vieat viean
/vah(t) viean/
20
SG
WG example
WI pronunciation
EI pronunciation
gloss
bb
beabi beabbi
/ppii peappii/
/ppii pippii/
shoulder-blade
gg
geagi geaggi
/kkii keakkii/
/kkii kikkii/
stone, rock
vv
bva bvva
/paava paavva/
/pava paavva/
curly birch
ib
ibb
libi libbi
/laajpii laajppii/
/ljpii ljppii/
bread
id
idd
nieida nieidda
/njta niejtta/
/njta niejtta/
girl, daughter
if
iff
riifu riiffu
/rijfuu rijffuu/
/rijfuu riijffuu/
snail
ig
igg
igi iggi
/aajkii aajkkii/
/jkii jkkii/
time
ihl
ihll
mihli mihlli
/maajLii maajLLii/
/mjLii mjLLii/
sap
ihm
/tujMii tuujMMii/
stupid
ihn
/aajNii aajNNii/
/jNii jNNii/
woodpecker
il
ill
biila biilla
/pijla pijllaa/
/pijla piijlla/
car
ir
irr
liri lirri
/laajrii laajrrii/
/ljrii ljrrii/
clay
is
iss
gis giss
/kaajsaa kaajssaa/
/kjs kjss/
iv
ivv
moivi moivvi
/mojvii mojvvii/
/mojvii moojvvii/
mess
lb
lbb
silba silbba
/silpa silppaa/
/silpa siilppa/
silver
ld
ldd
guoldu guolddu
/kltuu kuolttuu/
/kltuu kuolttuu/
snow storm
lf
lff
skuolfi skuolffi
/sklfii skuolffii/
/sklfii skuolffii/
owl
lg
lgg
lgu lggu
/aalkuu aalkkuu/
/alkuu aalkkuu/
to leave
ls
lss
goalsi goalssi
/klsii koalssii/
/klsii kualssii/
merganser (a bird)
la la
/aala aala/
/ala aala/
energy, perserverence
lv
lvv
balva balvva
/palva palvvaa/
/plva plvva/
cloud
mb
/pumpaa pumppaa/
/pump puumpp/
nd
ndd
gnda gndda
/kaanta kaantta/
/kanta kaantta/
boy
ns
nss
nsu nssu
/aansuu aanssuu/
/ansuu aanssuu/
merit
stni stni
/staanii staanii/
/stnii stnii/
mud
gg
mga mgga
/maanka maankka/
/maka maakka/
many
rb
rbb
rbi rbbi
/aarpii aarppii/
/rpii rppii/
inheritance
rd
rdd
earda eardda
/rta eartta/
/rta irtta/
tribe
rf
rff
mrfi mrffi
/maarfii maarffii/
/mrfii mrffii/
sausage
rg
rgg
biergu bierggu
/prkuu pierkkuu/
/prkuu pierkkuu/
meat
rj
rjj
vearju vearjju
/vrjuu vearjjuu/
/vrjuu virjjuu/
weapon
rs
rss
bursa burssa
/pursa purssaa/
/pursa puurssa/
wallet
mora mora
/mora moraa/
/mora moora/
walrus
rv
rvv
arvi arvvi
/arvii arvvii/
/rvii rvvii/
rain
gri gri
/kaarii kaarii/
/krii krii/
narrow
sk
skk
goaski goaskki
/kskii koaskkii/
/kskii kuaskii/
sm
/pisma pismmaa/
/pisma piisma/
bishop
21
st
stt
astat asttan
/astah(t) asttaan/
/stah(t) staan/
to have time
kk
/rkit rikn/
mm umi ummi
/umii ummii/
/umii uumii/
pp
rupi ruppi
/rupi ruppii/
/rupi ruupii/
carrot
tk
tkk
mtki mtkki
/maatkii maatkkii/
/mtkii mtkii/
trip, journey
tm
/faatmii faatmmii/
/ftmii ftmii/
fathom
vd
vdd
ravda ravdda
/ravta ravttaa/
/rvta rvtta/
edge
vg
vgg
/vka uovkka/
light
vhl
vhll
skvhli skvhlli
gull
vj
vjj
oavji oavjji
/vjii oavjjii/
/vjii uavjjii/
stomach
vl
vll
guovlu guovllu
/kvluu kuovlluu/
/kvluu kuovlluu/
area, region
vr
vrr
jvri jvrri
/jaavrii jaavrrii/
/jvrii jvrrii/
lake
vz
vzz
svza svzza
/saavca saavcca/
/savca saavcca/
sheep
/kva kuova/
bear
WG example
icc
ikk
ipp
itt
lk
gica gicca
uoika uoikka
biipu biippu
iti itti
mielki mielkki
WI pronunciation
h
/kaaj ca kaajhca/
h
/j ka uojhka/
h
/ml ki mielhkii/
EI pronunciation
h
/kaj ca kaajhca/
h
/j ka uojhka/
h
/j tii jhtii/
h
/ml ki mielhkii/
gloss
goat
mosquito
pipe
storehouse
milk
lp
lpp
lt
ltt
slti sltti
mp
k
rc
bku bkku
bircu birccu
/saalhtii saalhtii/
h
/kum pe kumhpe/
/slhtii slhtii/
h
/kum pe kuumhpe/
salt
wolf
bank
dice
22
/skurhuu skurhuu/
skuru skuru
rk
rkk
rp
rt
vk
vp
rpp
rtt
vkk
vpp
rpu rppu
brti brtti
lvka lvkka
gvpi gvppi
/laav ka laavhka/
h
mark
thread
(serious) accident
bag
store, shop
WG example
WI pronunciation
EI pronunciation
gloss
ibm
/aajpmuu aajmmuu/
/ajmuu aajmmuu/
air
idn
inn
/sjtnii suojnnii/
/sjnii suojnnii/
grass, hay
/vja vuoja/
breath, spirit
suoidni suoidni
j j
ig
lbm
/alpmii almmii/
/lmii lmmii/
eye
ldn
lnn
/sltnii suolnnii/
/slnii suolnnii/
dew
suoldni suolnni
j j
lg
alga ala
/la la/
gums
vdn
vnn
svdni svnni
/saavtnii saavnnii/
/svnii svnnii/
sauna
vdnj vnjj
lavdnji lavnnji
j j
j j
peat, sod
WG example
WI pronunciation
EI pronunciation
gloss
rbm
rpm
orbma orpma
/or.pma orp.maa/
/orma oorpma/
fist
rdn
rtn
brdni brtni
/paar.tnii paart.nii/
/prnii prtnii/
boy, son
rdnj
rg
rtnj
rk
wretch
23
WG example
WI pronunciation
EI pronunciation
gloss
kc
vcc
gakcut gavccun
aka ava
/aka avha/
/ka vha/
autumn
ks
vss
uksa uvssa
/uksa uvssa/
/uksa uuvssa/
door
dikut divun
/tikuuh(t) tivuun/
/tikuuh(t) tiivuun/
kst
vstt
teaksta teavstta
/tksta teavstta/
kt
vtt
okta ovtta
/okta ovhta/
/okta oovhta/
to climb
to take care of
one
WG example
WI pronunciation
isk
iskk
ist
istt
mistit misttn
mk
nst
nstt
EI pronunciation
gloss
/tujska tuujska/
Germany
/limkii liimkii/
rag
/knsta kuansta/
rst
rstt
stm
Girste Girstte
/kirste kirstte/
/kirste kiirste/
(womans name)
/aastmaa aastmmaa/
/stm stm/
asthma
vsk
/haavskii haavskkii/
/hvskii hvskii/
fun, pleasant
vst
vst
/naavstuu naavsttuu/
/nvstuu nvstuu/
nvstu nvsttu
24
strong grade Q II
weak grade Q I
boahtit
come.INF
to come
boan
come.1SG
I come
boahtti
come.IMP.2DU
come! (you two)
Note that the process does not affect words which show consonant gradation between
Quantity III and Quantity II (since there is no quantity longer than Quantity III to which
the strong grade could be strengthened). Consider the following example:
strong grade Q III
weak grade Q II
luohttit
trust.INF
to trust
luohtn
trust.1SG
I trust
luohtti
come.IMP.2DU
trust! (you two)
The following table shows the results of strong grade strengthening in each case of
gradation of the type Q II : Q I.
SG
WG strengthened
INF 1SG
IMP.2DU
> '
oait oan
oa'i
to sleep
ll
> l'l
doallat doalan
doal'li
to hold
mm
> m'm
njammat njaman
njam'mi
to suck
nn
> n'n
mannat manan
man'ni
to go
rr
> r'r
jearrat jearan
jear'ri
to ask
ss
> s's
bassat basan
bas'si
to wash
> '
viat vian
vi'i
to bother to
vv
> v'v
njuovvat njuovan
njuov'vi
to flay, to slaughter
hp
> hpp
rahpat raban
rahppi
to open
ht
> htt
boahtit boan
boahtti
to come
hk
> hkk
juhkat jugan
juhkki
to drink
hc
> hcc
bhcit bzn
bhcci
to stay, to remain
> h
bhit bn
bhi
to shoot
pm
> bm
doapmat doaman
doabmi
to hurry
tn
> dn
atnat anan
adni
to tie, to bind
tnj
nj
> dnj
botnjat bonjan
bodnji
to twist
> g
kat an
gi
to creep in
dj
> ddj
vuodjit vuojn
vuoddji
to drive
25
INF
ierrut
eallit
vuolgit
goarrut
2DU
irro
elle
vulge
gorro
to cry
to live
to leave, go away
to sew
applies:
often
often
nearly always
rarely
Before the sequence -ui-, it is the diphthong simplification itself which is exceptional: it
only occurs in very few forms. One such form is the SG.ILL of vowel-stem nouns ending
in -u: viessu [house.SG.NOM] : vissui [house.SG.ILL]. In most forms, however, the
sequence -ui- does not trigger diphthong simplification: cf. viesuin [house.SG.COM],
viesuid [house.PL.GEN], viesuide [house.PL.ILL], etc.
26
e
o
a
i
u
alterations conditioned:
(does not alterate)
phonologically
phonologically
morphologically
morphologically
morphologically
fertet
digot
3SG
ferte
digo
PAST.3SG
fertii
digui
2DU
fertejetne
digojetne
PAST.2DU
fertiime
diguime
to have to
to order, to subscribe
INF
boahtit
giessat
goarrut
1SG
boan
giesan
goarun
to come
to wrap
to sew
27
2) i > , a > . This set of changes occurs, e.g., in the 3SG forms of gradating verbs:
change
i>
a>
u: no change
INF
boahtit
giessat
goarrut
3SG
boaht
giess
goarru
to come
to wrap
to sew
3) i > e, u > o. This set of changes occurs, e.g., in the CONNEG forms of gradating verbs:
change
i>e
a: no change
u>o
INF
CONNEG
boahtit
giessat
goarrut
boae
giesa
goaro
to come
to wrap
to sew
4) i > e, a > e, u > o. This set of changes occurs, e.g., in the 3PL forms of gradating
verbs:
change
i>e
a>e
u>o
INF
boahtit
giessat
goarrut
3PL
bohtet
gisset
gorrot
to come
to wrap
to sew
SG.NOM
PL.NOM
boaris
fanas
boaus
boarrsat
fatnasat
bohtosat
PL.NOM
forms of consonant-stem
old person
boat
result
6) i > , a > i. This set of changes is apparently confined to only one particular form, the
SG.ILL of vowel-stem nouns:
change
i>
a>i
u: no change
SG.NOM
SG.ILL
vuovdi
giehta
dllu
vuovdi
gihtii
dllui
forest
hand, arm
house
7) a > i (no other changes). This change occurs, e.g., in the IMP.2DU of gradating verbs:
change
i: no change
a>i
u: no change
INF
IMP.2DU
boahtit
giessat
goarrut
boahtti
gies'si
goar'ru
to come
to wrap
to sew
28
In addition to the alterations listed above, unstressed vowels can also be affected by stem
vowel deletion. In stems ending in a vowel, the final vowel becomes deleted when a
suffix beginning with a vowel is added. This process is fairly rare in inflectional
paradigms, but it does occur in gradating verbs when certain imperative suffixes are
added, such as the IMP.1DU suffix -u or the IMP.1PL suffix -ot:
INF
vuolgit
geahat
lvlut
stem
vuolgigeahalvlu-
IMP.1DU
IMP.1PL
vuolgu
geahu
lvlu
vulgot
gehot
lvlot
to leave, go away
to look, watch
to sing
PL.NOM
gvpot
beana
gvpogat
beatnagat
city
dog
underlying consonant:
g
g
29
30
31
4. NOMINALS
In North Saami the group of nominal word-classes consists of nouns, pronouns,
adjectives, and quantifiers. All nominals can be inflected for case and (with the
exception of personal pronouns) for singular and plural number. In addition to case and
number, nouns can also be inflected for possession by means of possessive suffixes.
dllu
dlu
dlus
dllui
dluin
dllun
The main functions of the cases are described in more detail in the subsections below.
4.1.1. Nominative
The nominative singular is morphologically unmarked, and it is the citation form of
nominals in dictionaries. The nominative has two core syntactic functions: it is used as a
case of a subject and a predicative.
The grammatical subjects of all types of finite verbs are always in the nominative case:
Ipmil lea.
god(.NOM) be.3SG
God exists. (existential verb)
Duot dlut leat ruoksadat.
that.PL.NOM house.PL.NOM be.3PL red.PL.NOM
Those houses are red. (copula)
Ruoksat lea mu fvorihttaivdni.
red(.NOM) be.3SG 1SG.GEN favorite.color
Red is my favorite color. (equative verb)
32
33
Also the verbs addat to become and orrut to seem to be3 can take a predicative in the
nominative case:
Dat attai buorre fanas.
it become.PAST.3SG good boat(.NOM)
It became a good boat.
Dat orui buorre fanas.
it seem.PAST.3SG good boat(.NOM)
It seemed to be a good boat.
The nominative also functions as the case of the object if the object is a singular form
of a numeral higher than one, or one of the quantifiers mga many, moadde a
couple of, or gal'le how many?.4 In all other instances, objects take the genitive case;
see 4.2.2. below. Hence, these quantifiers form a curious exception to the general pattern
of object marking. Examples:
Gal'le ostet?
how_many(.NOM) buy.PAST.2SG
How many did you buy?
Osten golbma.
buy.PAST.1SG three(.NOM)
I bought three.
Osten moadde.
buy.PAST.1SG a_couple(.NOM)
I bought a few.
Note that both verbs of these verbs are highly polysemous, and also appear in other meanings in quite
different syntctic constructions: addat also means to grow, to be born, and to end up in; to end up
having to do something, and orrut also has the meanings to live (somewhere), to dwell, to stay, and in
certain contexts even to be.
4
Note that even numeral and other quantifier objects take the genitive case if they are in the plural. Plural
numerals and quantifiers are used for counting pairs or sets of objects (see X.X. for details on the use of
quantifier constructions):
Osten golmmaid.
buy.PAST.1SG three.PL.GEN
I bought three [pairs of something / sets of something].
34
4.1.2. Genitive
Nearly all grammatical descriptions of North Saami distinguish between a genitive and
an accusative case. These are described as homonymous in nearly all instances, and thus
also the term genitive-accusative is used. Despite this tradition, there seem to be no
acceptable morphological or morphosyntactic criteria for distinguishing the accusative
as a case of its own in North Saami. Hence, only the term genitive will be used in this
grammar sketch.
The genitive is used as an attribute in noun phrases. It most commonly expresses
possession (in the wide sense):
Mhte biila [Matthew.GEN car]
Matthews car
beatnaga namma [dog.GEN name]
the dogs name
su jurdda [3SG.GEN thought]
his/her idea
mnid skuvla-beaivi [child.PL.GEN school-day] the childrens day at school
riikka rdj [state.GEN border]
state border
meara brut [sea.GEN wave.PL.NOM]
the waves of the ocean
35
b) time
maebrgga viisa [tuesday.GEN newspaper] tuesdays newspaper
diimm ml'le [last_year.GEN model]
last years model
c) measure
miilla mtki [ten_kilometers.GEN trip]
a distance of 10 kilometers
diimmu boddu [hour.GEN break]
a one hour break
via kilo oarbealle [five.GEN kilo.GEN rump] a 5-kilo reindeer rump roast
d) subject of a verb that semantically underlies the head noun
doaktra dutkamu [doctor.GEN examination]
bissuid bvkkas [gun.PL.GEN banging]
on the table
after the holidays
without money
all over the world
36
Objects of transitive verbs are always in the genitive case (except if the object is a
singular form of a numeral higher than one or one of the quantifiers mga, moadde, or
gal'le; see 4.1.1.2.):
Oidnen Mhte.
see.PAST.3SG Matthew.GEN
I saw Matthew.
Hliidehpetgo deaja?
want.2PL.Q tea.GEN
Would you (3+) like some tea?
hi doalvu dudno skuvlii.
father take.3SG 2DU.PL.GEN school.ILL
Father will take you (2) to school.
Osten ruoksada.
buy.PAST.1SG red.GEN
I bought a red one.
Osten due ovtta.
buy.PAST.1SG only one.GEN
I only bought one.
Note: there are very many verbs that are transitive in North Saami, even though their
semantic equivalents in English (and many other langauges) are not transitive. Some
examples:
Mnt urvot hi.
child.PL.NOM shout.3PL father.GEN
The children are calling for father.
Lean regon Mhte mannan dlvvi.
be.1SG work_as_a_hired_hand_for.PAST_PTCL Matthew.GEN last winter.GEN
Ive worked as a hired hand for Matthew last winter.
The subjects of non-finite verb-forms are in the genitive case:
Son muitalii dan Mhte guladettiin.
3SG tell.PAST.3SG it.GEN Matthew.GEN hear.GER
(S)he told it while Matthew was hearing.
Dt du libun ghkku lea njlgga.
this 2SG.GEN bake.PASS_PAST_PTCL cake be.3SG tasty
This cake you have baked is good.
37
4.1.3. Locative
The locative is one of the two local cases in North Saami (cf. the illative, 4.1.4). It has
two basic local functions.
a) The locative is a static local case that expresses location at the point of reference:
Mhtte lea gvppis.
Matthew be.3SG store.LOC
Matthew is at the (grocery) store.
Fierpmit leat zis.
net.PL.NOM be.3PL water.LOC
The nets are in water.
Ig vulggii ku gieas muoraid uohppat.
Ing leave.PAST.3SG ax hand.LOC tree.PL.GEN cut.INF
Ing went cutting wood with an ax in her hand.
Eadni geah televiuvnna orrun-lanjas.
mother look.3SG television.GEN living-room.LOC
Mother is watching TV in the living room.
38
39
40
41
4.1.4. Illative
The illative is the second of the two local cases in North Saami (cf. the locative, 4.1.3.).
The illative is a dynamic local case that expresses movement to a place or point of
reference:
Mrj mann gvpi.
Mary go.3SG store.ILL
Mary is going to the store.
Mhtte doalvu mnid skuvlii.
Matthew take.3SG child.PL.GEN school.ILL
Matthew takes the children to school.
Soai suhppiiga firpmiid hci.
3DU throw.PAST.3DU net.PL.GEN water.ILL
They cast the nets in water.
There are also many kinds of metaphoric extensions of this basic local function:
Ohcejogas lea Guovdageidnui guoktelogivihtta miilla.
Ohcejohka.LOC be.3SG Guovdageaidnu.ILL twentyfive ten_kilometres.GEN
It is 250 kilometres from Ohcejohka to Guovdageaidnu.
Mhtte geahai munnje.
Matthew look.PAST.3SG 1SG.ILL
Matthew looked at me.
Viellja vulggii guollebivdui.
brother leave.PAST.3SG fish.hunt.ILL
Brother went fishing.
The second basic function of the illative is habitive it marks a recipient, as in the
following examples:
Mhtte attii girjji Igi.
Matthew give.PAST.3SG book.GEN Ig.ILL
Matthew gave the book to Ig.
Osten mnide oa dlvejhkaid.
buy.PAST.1SG child.PL.ILL new.ATTR winter.coat.PL.GEN
I bought the children new winter coats.
42
43
4.1.5. Comitative
The comitative case has two main functions.
a) It signifies an instrument or means for something:
Ig goarru gvtti goarrunmaiinnain.
Ing sew.3SG Saami_costume.GEN sewing_machine.COM
Ig sews a Saami costume with a sewing machine.
Sii uhppe muoraid kuiguin.
3PL cut.PAST.3PL tree.PL.GEN ax.PL.COM
They cut down the trees with axes.
44
45
bhpa guovttos
priest.GEN guovttos
Note also:
gndda guovttos
boy.GEN guovttos
The two boys
duot guovttos
that.PL.NOM guovttos (nominative!)
The two over there
46
4.1.6. Essive
In intransitive sentences, the essive is a case of both predicatives and adverbs that
express:
a) what the subjects (usually temporary) task or function is, or what state the subject
is in;
b) what the subject becomes, changes or turns into, becomes like, or what state the
subject ends up in.
Consider the examples:
Mhtte barg gielddas l'lin.
Matthew work.3SG municipality.LOC secretary.ESS
Matthew works as a secretary at the municipality office.
Mis ledje kssat stuollun.
1PL.LOC be.PAST.3PL box.PL.NOM chair.ESS
We had boxes as chairs.
Mhtte veal'l seaggas buohccin.
Matthew lie.3SG bed.LOC sick.ESS
Matthew lies sick in bed.
Ig stuere doavttirin.
Ig study.3SG doctor.ESS
Ig is studying to become a doctor.
Idja jorai beaivin.
night spin.PAST.3SG day.ESS
Night turned into day.
dj jpmima ma Ig lea addan nu lundin.
grandfather.GEN death.GEN after Ig be.3SG become.PAST_PTCL so depressed.ESS
After grandfathers death Ig has become so depressed.
Son attai fuones oahpaheaddjin.
3SG become.PAST.3SG bad.ATTR teacher.ESS
(S)he became a bad teacher. [i.e., (s)he was a teacher already, and then became
a bad one]
47
48
49
SET 2:
SG
PL
SG
PL
-s
-i
-in
-n
-t
-id
-in
-ide
-iguin
-a
-is
-ii
-iin
-in
-at
-iid
-iin
-iidda
-iiguin
Note that the PL.LOC and SG.COM case endings are identical; the two case forms are
actually always homonymous for all words.
50
SG
PL
strong
weak
weak
strong
weak
strong
weak
weak
weak
weak
weak
51
In quadrisyllabic a-stems the final vowel is deleted in SG.NOM. As a result of this, also
the final consonant or consonant cluster is simplified due to phontactic restrictions on
consonants in final position.
Some i-stem nominals exhibit an exceptional final e in the SG.NOM (e.g., buorre
good, guokte two). These kinds of nominals are very rare.
The following example paradigms illustrate the declension of vowel stem-nominals:
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
guolli fish:
mnn child:
SG
PL
SG
PL
SG
PL
guolli
guoli
guolis
guolli
guliin
guollin
guolit
guliid
guliin
guliide
guliiguin
mnn
mn
mns
mnni
mnin
mnnn
mnt
mnid
mnin
mnide
mniguin
giehta
giea
gieas
gihtii
gieain
giehtan
gieat
gieaid
gieain
gieaide
gieaiguin
biergu meat:
reive letter:
SG
PL
SG
PL
SG
PL
biergu
bierggu
bierggus
birgui
biergguin
biergun
bierggut
biergguid
biergguin
biergguide
biergguiguin
reive
reivve
reivves
reivii
reivviin
reiven
reivvet
reivviid
reivviin
reivviide
reivviiguin
belko
belkko
belkkos
belkui
belkkuin
belkon
belkkot
belkkuid
belkkuin
belkkuide
belkkuiguin
televiuvdna television:
SG
PL
SG
PL
mielaheapmi
mielaheami
mielaheamis
mielaheapmi
mielahemiin
mielaheapmin
mielaheamit
mielahemiid
mielahemiin
mielahemiide
mielahemiiguin
televiuvdna
televiuvnna
televiuvnnas
televiuvdnii
televiuvnnain
televiuvdnan
televiuvnnat
televiuvnnaid
televiuvnnain
televiuvnnaide
televiuvnnaiguin
52
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
logaldat lecture:
SG
PL
SG
PL
spmela
spmelaa
spmelaas
spmelaii
spmelaain
spmelaan
spmelaat
spmelaaid
spmelaain
spmelaaide
spmelaaiguin
logaldat
logaldaga
logaldagas
logaldahkii
logaldagain
logaldahkan
logaldagat
logaldagaid
logaldagain
logaldagaide
logaldagaiguin
buorre good:
guokte two:
SG
PL
SG
PL
buorre
buori
buoris
buorri
buriin
buorrin
buorit
buriid
buriin
buriide
buriiguin
guokte
guovtti
guovttis
guokti
guvttiin
guoktin
guovtti
guvttiid
guvttiin
guvttiide
guvttiiguin
hildu shelf:
vuoddji driver
SG
PL
SG
PL
GEN
hildu
hildu
hildut
hilduid
vuoddji
vuoddji
LOC
hildus
hilduin
vuoddjis
ILL
hildui
hilduide
vuoddji
COM
hilduin
hilduiguin
ESS
hildun
vuddjiin (WI)
~ vuoddjiin (EI)
vuoddjin
vuoddjit
vuddjiid (WI)
~ vuoddjiid (EI)
vuddjiin (WI)
~ vuoddjiin (EI)
vuddjiide (WI)
~ vuoddjiide (EI)
vuddjiiguin (WI)
~ vuoddjiiguin (EI)
NOM
53
SG.NOM
njoammil hare
gvpot city
boaris old
beana dog
eana earth, land
oblique stem
njoammilgvpogboarrsbeatnageatnam-
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
njoammil hare:
dihtor computer:
SG
PL
SG
PL
njoammil
njoammila
njoammilis
njoammilii
njoammiliin
njoammilin
njoammilat
njoammiliid
njoammiliin
njoammiliidda
njoammiliiguin
dihtor
dihtora
dihtoris
dihtorii
dihtoriin
dihtorin
dihtorat
dihtoriid
dihtoriin
dihtoriidda
dihtoriiguin
gievkkan kitchen:
gvpot city:
SG
PL
SG
PL
gievkkan
gievkkana
gievkkanis
gievkkanii
gievkkaniin
gievkkanin
gievkkanat
gievkkaniid
gievkkaniin
gievkkaniidda
gievkkaniiguin
gvpot
gvpoga
gvpogis
gvpogii
gvpogiin
gvpogin
gvpogat
gvpogiid
gvpogiin
gvpogiidda
gvpogiiguin
54
ustit friend:
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
fiskat yellow:
SG
PL
SG
PL
ustit
ustiba
ustibis
ustibii
ustibiin
ustibin
ustibat
ustibiid
ustibiin
ustibiidda
ustibiiguin
fiskat
fiskada
fiskadis
fiskadii
fiskadiin
fiskadin
fiskadat
fiskadiid
fiskadiin
fiskadiidda
fiskadiiguin
SG
PL
weak
strong
strong
strong
strong
weak
strong
strong
strong
strong
strong
The weak grade occurs only in the SG.NOM and the ESS case. All the other forms in the
paradigm are formed from a strong-grade oblique stem, which also undergoes the
unstressed vowel alterations i > and u > o. The unstressed -o- resulting from the latter
change triggers diphthong simplification. Moreover, nominals of this type may show
margin consonant alterations with zero: the stem-final consonant is deleted in SG.NOM
and ESS, but surfaces in the oblique stem.
Example paradigms:
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
duottar tundra:
boagn belt:
SG
PL
SG
PL
duottar
duoddara
duoddaris
duoddarii
duoddariin
duottarin
duoddarat
duoddariid
duoddariin
duoddariidda
duoddariiguin
boagn
boahkna
boahknis
boahknii
boahkniin
boagnin
boahknat
boahkniid
boahkniin
boahkniidda
boahkniiguin
55
boaris old:
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
boaus result:
SG
PL
SG
PL
boaris
boarrsa
boarrsis
boarrsii
boarrsiin
boarisin
boarrsat
boarrsiid
boarrsiin
boarrsiidda
boarrsiiguin
boaus
bohtosa
bohtosis
bohtosii
bohtosiin
boausin
bohtosat
bohtosiid
bohtosiin
bohtosiidda
bohtosiiguin
beana dog:
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
SG
PL
SG
PL
beana
beatnaga
beatnagis
beatnagii
beatnagiin
beanan
beatnagat
beatnagiid
beatnagiin
beatnagiidda
beatnagiiguin
eana
eatnama
eatnamis
eatnamii
eatnamiin
eanan
eatnamat
eatnamiid
eatnamiin
eatnamiidda
eatnamiiguin
vuoni mother-in-law:
luomi cloudberry:
SG
PL
SG
PL
vuoni
vuotnma
vuotnmis
vuotnmii
vuotnmiin
vuonin
vuotnmat
vuotnmiid
vuotnmiin
vuotnmiidda
vuotnmiiguin
luomi
luopmna
luopmnis
luopmnii
luopmniin
luomin
luopmnat
luopmniid
luopmniin
luopmniidda
luopmniiguin
56
oblique stem
njlg- (Q III)
vielp- (Q III)
stuor'r- (Q III)
fl'l- (Q III)
njlggis (Q II)
vielppis (Q II)
stuoris (Q I)
flis (Q I)
The case forms are formed by adding case endings in set 1 to the oblique stem, save for
the essive, which is formed by adding the essive suffix in set 2 directly to the nominative
singular.
Example paradigms:
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
vielppis puppy:
stuoris big:
SG
PL
SG
PL
vielppis
vielp
vielps
vielpi
vielpin
vielppisin
vielpt
vielpid
vielpin
vielpide
vielpiguin
stuoris
stuor'r
stuor'rs
stuor'ri
stuor'rin
stuorisin
stuor'rt
stuor'rid
stuor'rin
stuor'ride
stuor'riguin
57
The strong grade undergoes strong grade strengthening (see 3.1.3); hence, if the
SG.NOM has a Quantity I weak grade, this results in gradation between Quantity III
and Quantity I.
The stem vowel -u changes to -o-, except before case suffixes beginning with -ibecause the sequence -oi- is not permitted in unstressed syllables (see 3.3.).
If the noun has a diphthong, it undergoes diphthong simplification (see 3.2.) in all
forms based on the oblique stem. This happens before both second-syllable -o- and ui-. Note that this is one of the rare cases where the sequence -ui- triggers diphthong
simplification.
Some examples:
SG.NOM
oblique stem
nurvo- / nurvu- (Q III)
sul'lo- / sul'lu- (Q III)
bohcco- / bohccu- (Q III)
edno- / ednu- (Q III)
nuorvvu (Q II)
suolu (Q I)
boazu (Q I)
eanu (Q I)
The case forms are formed by adding case endings of set 1 to the oblique stem, except for
ESS, where the ending is attached directly to the SG.NOM form.
Example paradigms:
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
suolu island:
boazu reindeer:
SG
PL
SG
PL
suolu
sul'lo
sul'los
sul'lui
sul'luin
suolun
sul'lot
sul'luid
sul'luin
sul'luide
sul'luiguin
boazu
bohcco
bohccos
bohccui
bohccuin
boazun
bohccot
bohccuid
bohccuin
bohccuide
bohccuiguin
58
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
almmi man:
SG
PL
SG
PL
olmmo
olbmo
olbmos
olbmui
olbmuin
olmmoin
olbmot
olbmuid
olbmuin
olbmuide
olbmuiguin
almmi
albm
albms
albmi
albmin
almmjin
albmt
albmid
albmin
albmide
albmiguin
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
1SG
mun
mu
mus
munnje
muinna
munin
2SG
don
du
dus
dutnje
duinna
dunin
3SG
son
su
sus
sutnje
suinna
sunin
1PL
mii
min
mis
midjiide
minguin
minin
2PL
dii
din
dis
didjiide
dinguin
dinin
3PL
sii
sin
sis
sidjiide
singuin
sinin
1DU
moai
mun'no
mun'nos
mun'nuide
mun'nuin
mun'non
2DU
doai
dudno
dudnos
dudnuide
dudnuin
dudnon
3DU
soai
sudno
sudnos
sudnuide
sudnuin
sudnon
59
Demonstrative pronouns.
There are five demonstrative pronouns in North Saami:
dat it; that (not present in the context of the conversation)
dt this
diet that (which is near or in the possession of the person(s) spoken to)
duot that (visible, but not near either the speaker or the person spoken to)
dot that (visible but distant)
The declension of all five pronouns is identical, except for the first syllable vowel. The
paradigm of dat is given as an example below. (Note that the SG.NOM and PL.NOM forms
are homonymous!)
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
SG
PL
dat
dan
das
dasa
dainna
danin
dat
daid
dain
daidda
daiguin
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
SG
PL
SG
PL
mii
man (maid)
mas
masa
mainna
manin
mat
maid
main
maidda
maiguin
gii
gean
geas
geasa
geainna
geanin
geat
geaid
geain
geaidda
geaiguin
The pronoun mii shows a unique morphosyntactic peculiarity. Its normal SG.GEN form is
man, but when the singular form of this pronoun appears as a direct object, the form man
is only used in the meaning which one, but maid in the meaning what. Otherwise,
what and which are not distinguished. Compare the examples:
Man don ostet?
what/which.SG.GEN 2SG buy.PAST.2SG
Which one did you buy?
60
SET 2:
SG
PL
SG
PL
-st-s-in-n-
-id-in-idasa-id-PX-guin
-istt-/-istti-asa-iinn-/-iinni
-iidd-/-iiddi-iinn-/-iinni-iidds-iidd-/-iiddi-PX-guin
Like the case endings, also possessive suffixes come in two sets. The possessive suffixes
in set 1 are added after case suffixes ending in a vowel, those in set 2 after case suffixes
ending in a consonant.
SET 1:
SET 2:
PS.
SG
DU
PL
SG
1
2
3
-n
-t
-s
-me
-de
-ska
-met
-det
-set
DU
PL
61
Example possessive paradigms are given separately for each case-form below. The
example words used in the paradigms are:
vowel stems:
consonant stem:
contracting is-stem:
mutating u-stem:
SG . NOM
Vowel stem nouns undergo the unstressed vowel changes i > and u > o before 1.
and 2. person possessive suffixes.
Second syllable o triggers diphthong simplification.
The possessive SG.NOM of consonant stem nouns, contracting is-nouns and mutating
u-stem nouns is formed of the oblique stem; the forms thus look quite different from
the corresponding absolute forms.
PS.
SG
DU
PL
1
2
3
vielljan
vielljat
vielljas
vielljame
vielljade
vielljaska
vielljamet
vielljadet
vielljaset
1
2
3
goahtn
goahtt
goahtis
goahtme
goahtde
goahtiska
goahtmet
goahtdet
goahtiset
1
2
3
visson
vissot
viessus
vissome
vissode
viessuska
vissomet
vissodet
viessuset
1
2
3
rhkksan
rhkksat
rhkksis
rhkkseame
rhkkseatte
rhkkseaskka
rhkkseamet
rhkkseattet
rhkkseaset
1
2
3
vielpn
vielpt
vielps
vielpme
vielpde
vielpska
vielpmet
vielpdet
vielpset
1
2
3
ednon
ednot
ednos
ednome
ednode
ednoska
ednomet
ednodet
ednoset
62
SG . GEN
Vowel stem nouns undergo the same vowel changes as in SG.NOM: i > and u > o
before 1. and 2. person possessive suffixes; the unstressed o triggers diphthong
simplification.
Unlike in the absolute declension, vowel stem nouns have the strong grade before 1.
person possessive suffixes (but the expected weak grade before the 2. and 3. person
possessive suffixes). Hence, the forms with 1. person suffixes are homonymous with
SG.NOM forms (cf. the table above).
The possessive SG.GEN forms of all other noun types are entirely homonymous with
the possessive SG.NOM forms (see the table above).
PS.
SG
DU
PL
1
2
3
vielljan
vieljat
vieljas
vielljame
vieljade
vieljaska
vielljamet
vieljadet
vieljaset
1
2
3
goahtn
goat
goais
goahtme
goade
goaiska
goahtmet
goadet
goaiset
1
2
3
visson
visot
viesus
vissome
visode
viesuska
vissomet
visodet
viesuset
SG . LOC
The SG.LOC case ending is -st- after stems ending in a vowel, and -istt- / -istti- after
stems ending in a consonant. The former variant occurs before 1. and 2. person
possessive suffixes, the latter before 3. person possessive suffixes.
PS.
SG
DU
PL
1
2
3
vieljastan
vieljastat
vieljastis
vieljasteame
vieljasteatte
vieljasteaskka
vieljasteamet
vieljasteattet
vieljasteaset
1
2
3
goaistan
goaistat
goaistis
goaisteame
goaisteatte
goaisteaskka
goaisteamet
goaisteattet
goaisteaset
1
2
3
viesustan
viesustat
viesustis
viesusteame
viesusteatte
viesusteaskka
viesusteamet
viesusteattet
viesusteaset
63
1
2
3
rhkksisttn
rhkksisttt
rhkksisttis
rhkksisttme
rhkksisttde
rhkksisttiska
rhkksisttmet
rhkksisttdet
rhkksisttiset
1
2
3
vielpstan
vielpstat
vielpstis
vielpsteame
vielpsteatte
vielpsteaskka
vielpsteamet
vielpsteattet
vielpsteaset
1
2
3
ednostan
ednostat
ednostis
ednosteame
ednosteatte
ednosteaskka
ednosteamet
ednosteattet
ednosteaset
SG . ILL
The SG.ILL suffix is -s- after stems ending a vowel and -asa- after stems ending in a
consonant. (Note that in the absolute declension the SG.ILL suffix is altogether
different: -i / -ii.)
Vowel stem nouns undergo the unstressed vowel modifications i > and u > o. The o
resulting from the latter change causes diphthong simplification. (Note that there is no
vowel modification a > i as in the absolute declension.)
PS.
SG
DU
PL
1
2
3
vielljasan
vielljasat
vielljasis
vielljaseame
vielljaseatte
vielljaseaskka
vielljaseamet
vielljaseattet
vielljaseaset
1
2
3
goahtsan
goahtsat
goahtsis
goahtseame
goahtseatte
goahtseaskka
goahtseamet
goahtseattet
goahtseaset
1
2
3
vissosan
vissosat
vissosis
vissoseame
vissoseatte
vissoseaskka
vissoseamet
vissoseattet
vissoseaset
1
2
3
rhkksasan
rhkksasat
rhkksasas
rhkksasame
rhkksasade
rhkksasaska
rhkksasamet
rhkksasadet
rhkksasaset
1
2
3
vielpsan
vielpsat
vielpstis
vielpseame
vielpseatte
vielpseaskka
vielpseamet
vielpseattet
vielpseaset
1
2
3
ednosan
ednosat
ednosis
ednoseame
ednoseatte
ednoseaskka
ednoseamet
ednoseattet
ednoseaset
64
SG . COM
The SG.COM case ending is -in- after stems ending in a vowel (like in the absolute
declension), and -iinn- / -iinni- after stems ending in a consonant. The former variant
occurs before 1. and 2. person possessive suffixes, the latter before 3. person
possessive suffixes.
PS.
SG
DU
PL
1
2
3
vieljainan
vieljainat
vieljainis
vieljaineame
vieljaineatte
vieljaineaskka
vieljaineamet
vieljaineattet
vieljaineaset
1
2
3
goiinan
goiinat
goiinis
goiineame
goiineatte
goiineaskka
goiineamet
goiineattet
goiineaset
1
2
3
viesuinan
viesuinat
viesuinis
viesuineame
viesuineatte
viesuineaskka
viesuineamet
viesuineattet
viesuineaset
1
2
3
rhkksiinnn
rhkksiinnt
rhkksiinnis
rhkksiinnme
rhkksiinnde
rhkksiinniska
rhkksiinnmet
rhkksiinndet
rhkksiinniset
1
2
3
vielpinan
vielpinat
vielpinis
vielpineame
vielpineatte
vielpineaskka
vielpineamet
vielpineattet
vielpineaset
1
2
3
ednuinan
ednuinat
ednuinis
ednuineame
ednuineatte
ednuineaskka
ednuineamet
ednuineattet
ednuineaset
ESS
The possessive essive forms are very rarely used. They can only be formed from
vowel stem nouns and mutating u-stem nouns, but not of consonant stem nouns or
contracting is-stem nouns.
PS.
SG
DU
PL
1
2
3
vielljanan
vielljanat
vielljanis
vielljaneame
vielljaneatte
vielljaneaskka
vielljaneamet
vielljaneattet
vielljaneaset
65
1
2
3
goahtinan
goahtinat
goahtinis
goahtineame
goahtineatte
goahtineaskka
goahtineamet
goahtineattet
goahtineaset
1
2
3
viessunan
viessunat
viessunis
viessuneame
viessuneatte
viessuneaskka
viessuneamet
viessuneattet
viessuneaset
1
2
3
ednonan
ednonat
ednonis
ednoneame
ednoneatte
ednoneaskka
ednoneamet
ednoneattet
ednoneaset
PL . NOM
The PL.GEN case ending is -id- after stems ending in a vowel (like in the absolute
declension), and -iidd- / -iiddi- after stems ending in a consonant. The former variant
occurs before 1. and 2. person possessive suffixes, the latter before 3. person
possessive suffixes.
PS.
SG
DU
PL
1
2
3
vieljaidan
vieljaidat
vieljaidis
vieljaideame
vieljaideatte
vieljaideaskka
vieljaideamet
vieljaideattet
vieljaideaset
1
2
3
goiidan
goiidat
goiidis
goiideame
goiideatte
goiideaskka
goiideamet
goiideattet
goiideaset
1
2
3
viesuidan
viesuidat
viesuidis
viesuideame
viesuideatte
viesuideaskka
viesuideamet
viesuideattet
viesuideaset
1
2
3
rhkksiiddn
rhkksiiddt
rhkksiiddis
rhkksiiddme
rhkksiiddde
rhkksiiddiska
rhkksiiddmet
rhkksiidddet
rhkksiiddiset
1
2
3
vielpidan
vielpidat
vielpidis
vielpideame
vielpideatte
vielpideaskka
vielpideamet
vielpideattet
vielpideaset
66
1
2
3
ednuidan
ednuidat
ednuidis
ednuideame
ednuideatte
ednuideaskka
ednuideamet
ednuideattet
ednuideaset
PL . LOC
The locative plural forms are always homonymous with the comitative singular forms, as
in the absolute declension.
PL . ILL
The PL.ILL case ending is -idasa- after stems ending in a vowel (like in the absolute
declension), and -iidds- after stems ending in a consonant.
PS.
SG
DU
PL
1
2
3
vieljaidasan
vieljaidasat
vieljaidasas
vieljaidasame
vieljaidasade
vieljaidasaska
vieljaidasamet
vieljaidasadet
vieljaidasaset
1
2
3
goiidasan
goiidasat
goiidasas
goiidasame
goiidasade
goiidasaska
goiidasamet
goiidasadet
goiidasaset
1
2
3
viesuidasan
viesuidasat
viesuidasas
viesuidasame
viesuidasade
viesuidasaska
viesuidasamet
viesuidasadet
viesuidasaset
1
2
3
rhkksiiddsan
rhkksiiddsat
rhkksiiddsis
rhkksiiddseame
rhkksiiddseatte
rhkksiiddseaskka
rhkksiiddmet
rhkksiidddet
rhkksiiddset
1
2
3
vielpidasan
vielpidasat
vielpidasas
vielpidasame
vielpidasade
vielpidasaska
vielpidasamet
vielpidasadet
vielpidasaset
1
2
3
ednuidasan
ednuidasat
ednuidasas
ednuidasame
ednuidasade
ednuidasaska
ednuidasamet
ednuidasadet
ednuidasaset
PL . COM
The PL.COM possessive forms are morphologically exceptional: the PL.COM suffix
consists of two separate parts, and the possessive suffix is insesrted between them.
The PL.COM forms can always be formed from the PL.GEN forms by adding the
morpheme -guin after the possessive suffix.
67
PS.
SG
DU
PL
1
2
3
vieljaidanguin
vieljaidatguin
vieljaidisguin
vieljaideameguin
vieljaideatteguin
vieljaideaskkaguin
vieljaideametguin
vieljaideattetguin
vieljaideasetguin
1
2
3
goiidanguin
goiidatguin
goiidisguin
goiideameguin
goiideatteguin
goiideaskkaguin
goiideametguin
goiideattetguin
goiideasetguin
1
2
3
viesuidanguin
viesuidatguin
viesuidisguin
viesuideameguin
viesuideatteguin
viesuideaskkaguin
viesuideametguin
viesuideattetguin
viesuideasetguin
1
2
3
rhkksiiddnguin
rhkksiiddtguin
rhkksiiddisguin
rhkksiiddmeguin
rhkksiidddeguin
rhkksiiddiskaguin
rhkksiiddmetguin
rhkksiidddetguin
rhkksiiddisetguin
1
2
3
vielpidanguin
vielpidatguin
vielpidisguin
vielpideameguin
vielpideatteguin
vielpideaskkaguin
vielpideametguin
vielpideattetguin
vielpideasetguin
1
2
3
ednuidanguin
ednuidatguin
ednuidisguin
ednuideameguin
ednuideatteguin
ednuideaskkaguin
ednuideametguin
ednuideattetguin
ednuideasetguin
68
Rekohin biilan.
wreck.PAST.1SG car.SG.GEN.1SG
I wrecked my car.
Vldet biergasiidddet!
take.IMP.2PL thing.PL.GEN.2PL
Take (pl.) your stuff!
b) The possessive suffix may also refer to the subject of a non-finite verb form rather than
the subject of the whole sentence:
Mun gohon din vldit biergasiidddet.
1SG order.PAST.1SG 2PL.GEN take.INF thing.PL.GEN.2PL
I told you (pl.) to take your stuff.
c) In a possessive sentence (see 4.1.3.) the possessive suffix refers to the possessor in the
locative form (rather than the grammatical subject which is the thing possessed):
Mii dus lea gieastat?
what 2SG.LOC be.3SG hand.LOC.2SG
What have you got in your hand?
d) Sometimes the possessive suffix may also refer to the object of a transitive verb, but
this is rare:
Bidjen girjji sadjsis.
put.PAST.1SG book.GEN place.ILL.3SG
I put the book [back] in its place.
Oinniime su ovttas mnidisguin.
see.PAST.1DU 3SG together child.PL.3SG.COM
We (2) met him/her together with his/her children.
Other type of use of possessive forms is generally not possible; e.g., in the following
types of sentences the use of possessive forms would be ungrammatical, because the
suffixes do not refer to any constituent of the sentence:
Rekohin du biilla. (*biillat)
wreck.PAST.1SG 2SG.GEN car.GEN (*car.GEN.2SG)
I wrecked your car.
Mun gohon din vldit sin biergasiid. (*biergasiiddiset)
1SG order.PAST.1SG 2PL.GEN take.INF 3PL.GEN thing.PL.GEN (*thing.PL.GEN.3PL)
I told you (pl.) to take their stuff.
69
There is one exception to these restrictions, though. Kinship terms can take a 2. person
possessive suffix even if the suffix does not refer to another constituent of the sentence:
Oidnen ht.
see.PAST.1SG father.GEN.2SG
I saw/met your father.
Gulaime dan hkusteatte.
hear.PAST.1DU it.GEN grandmother.LOC.2DU
We (du.) heard it from your (du.) grandmother.
4.3.3. The paradigm of the reflexive pronoun ie
When the reflexive pronoun ie is inflected for case, the use of possessive suffixes is
obligatory. The inflection of the reflexive pronoun is highly irregular; the locative and
illative case forms are suppletive.
The nominative forms of the reflexive pronoun do not take possessive suffixes; in hte
nominative there are distinct forms for singular and plural (but not for dual). In all other
case forms the reflexive pronoun occurs in combination with a possessive suffix:
CASE
PS.
NOM
SG
DU
PL
ie
iea
iea
GEN
1
2
3
iean
ieat
ieas
ieame
ieade
ieaska
ieamet
ieadet
ieaset
LOC
1
2
3
alddn
alddt
alddis
alddme
alddde
alddiska
alddmet
aldddet
alddiset
ILL
1
2
3
alccen
alccet
alcces
alcceme
alccede
alcceska
alccemet
alccedet
alcceset
COM
1
2
3
ieainan
ieainat
ieainis
ieaineame
ieaineamet
ieaineatte
ieaineattet
ieaineaskka ieaineaset
ESS
1
2
3
iehanan
iehanat
iehanis
iehaneame iehaneamet
iehaneatte iehaneattet
iehaneaskka iehaneaset
The use of the reflexive pronoun is explained later in the section on the morphosyntax of
pronouns.
70
4.4. Adjectives
4.4.1. Adjectives as heads of noun phrases
In North Saami, adjectives take case marking just like nouns when they occur
independently as heads of noun phrases. This can be illustrated with the following
examples:
Ruoksat lea mu fvorihttaivdni.
red(.NOM) be.3SG 1SG.GEN favorite.color
Red is my favorite color.
Osten ruoksada.
buy.PAST.1SG red.GEN
I bought a red one.
Balan ruoksadis.
be_afraid.1SG red.LOC
I am afraid of red (color).
Liikon ruoksadii.
like.1SG red.ILL
I like red.
Mlejin dan ruoksadiin.
paint.PAST.1SG it.GEN red.COM
I painted it with red (color).
Mlejin dan ruoksadin.
paint.PAST.1SG it.GEN red.ESS
I painted it red.
Case forms of adjectives can also occur as adverbs and genitive attributes just like nouns
do, even though this is very rare.
Unlike nouns, however, adjectives cannot take possessive suffixes. For example, even
though ruoksada [red.GEN] a red one can by itself constitute a noun phrase and appear
as a direct object of a transitive verb, it seems to be impossible to attach a possessive
suffix to it: hence, *ruoksadan my red one is unacceptable.
4.4.2. Attribute forms of adjectives
When adjectives appear as modifiers of nouns, they occur in a specific attribute form.
Many adjectives have distinct attribute forms, whereas for others the attribute form is
identical to the SG.NOM of the adjective. In the following examples, the adjective guhkki
71
long has a morphologically distinctive attribute form (guhkes), whereas nuorra young
does not:
Mtki lea guhkki.
trip be.3SG long
The trip / the distance is long.
Dat lea guhkes mtki.
it be.3SG long.ATTR trip
Its a long trip / a long distance.
Mhtte lea nuorra.
Matthew be.3SG young
Matthew is young.
Mhtte lea nuorra almmi.
Matthew be.3SG young(.ATTR) man
Matthew is a young man.
The attribute forms of adjectives are indeclinable: whenever an adjective occurs as a
modifier of a noun, it stays in the indeclinable attribute form regardless of what case the
noun itself takes. For example:
guhkes mtki
long.ATTR trip
a long trip
guhkes mtkkis
long.ATTR trip.LOC
on/from a long trip
guhkes mtki
long.ATTR trip.ILL
on(to) a long trip
The formation of attribute forms of adjectives shows several irregularities, but
nevertheless, in the majority of cases it can be predicted 1) whether the adjective will
have a distinct attribute form, and 2) how this attribute form is formed. In terms of their
attribute forms, adjectives can be divided into four different categories, plus a few
irregular cases. Which category an adjective belongs to is mostly determined on the basis
of phonological and morphological criteria; the semantics of the adjectives play no role in
the classification.
72
A) No separate attribute form. This group includes the following types of adjectives:
A1) Quadrisyllabic vowel stem adjectives with a
example:
SG.NOM
SG.GEN
ATTR
dbla
dehla
duhtava
gierdava
lihkola
rfla
vrala
issoras
dblaa
dehlaa
duhtavaa
gierdavaa
lihkolaa
rflaa
vralaa
issorasa
dbla i
dehla i
duhtava olmmo
gierdava ustit
lihkola nieida
rfla olmmo
vrala biki
issoras i
SG.NOM
A2) Most consonant stem adjectives which show consonant gradation, except the ones
ending in -is as well as several ending in -as (cf. group C).
SG.NOM
SG.GEN
ATTR
amas
baha
iegus
dearvvas
dievas
guohca
rabas
riha
suohtas
varas
apmasa
bahaga
ihkosa
dearvasa
dievvasa
guohccaga
rahpasa
rihaga
suohttasa
varrasa
amas olbmot
baha gffe
iegus i
dearvvas gnda
dievas gohppa
guohca biergu
rabas uksa
riha vuolla
suohtas olmmo
varas luossa
SG.GEN
ATTR
buoret
buoremus
buorebu
buoremusa
buoret filbma
buoremus filbma
B) The attribute form is formed by adding the suffix -s / -es / -is. This group includes
several types of adjectives:
B1) Most bisyllabic vowel stem adjectives. The attribute forms ending in -s undergo
several morphophonemic changes: they have the weak grade (unless the adjective lacks
73
consonant gradation); in most cases also the second-syllable vowel undergoes the change
i > e or u > o, but the resulting e or o does not trigger diphthong simplification (see 3.2.).
Examples:
SG.NOM
SG.GEN
ATTR
lki
bah
buoidi
eahppi
uorbi
falli
fasti
gri
gievra
guhkki
hlbi
headju
jal'la
jllu
ropmi
som
lki (~ lkki)
bah
buoiddi
eahpi
uorbbi
fali
fastti
gri
gievrra
guhki
hlbbi
heaju
jalla
jlu
romi
som
B2) Most consonant-stem adjectives that lack consonant gradation. These form two
subgroups:
B2a) The attribute suffix -is added to the stem; the only morphophonological alternation
that can occur in this group is margin consonant alteration (see 3.4.). Examples:
SG.NOM
SG.GEN
ATTR
bastil
bivval
heittot
johtil
bastila
bivvala
heittoga
johtila
bastilis niibi
bivvalis dlki
heittogis biila
johtilis biila
B2b) Consonant-stem adjectives that in -at and have an oblique stem in -ad- show
complex morphophonological changes in the attribute form: 1) the -ad- in the oblique
stem is deleted as the ending -es is attached; 2) the consonant in the adjective root
undergoes strong grade strengthening, if applicable (see 3.1.3.); 3) the second syllable -ecauses diphthong simplification. Examples:
SG.NOM
SG.GEN
ATTR
hppat
fiskat
hppada
fiskada
hppes beana
fiskes dllu
74
jaskat
ruoksat
earrat
seavdnjat
suohkat
jaskada
ruoksada
earrada
seavdnjada
suohkada
jaskes dllu
rukses lssaliinnit
er'res beaivi
sevdnjes idja
suhkkes vuovdi
B3) Caritive adjectives ending in -heapmi have an attribute form ending in -his:
SG.NOM
SG.GEN
bealjeheapmi -heami
almmeheapmi
lihkoheapmi
lobiheapmi
ATTR
bealjehis olmmo
almmehis olmmo
lihkohis olmmo
lobihis dahku
B4) Adjectives ending in -i, -ii, or -ui have attribute forms ending in -s, -es or -os,
respectively. (Note: in most dialects this adjective type is indeclinable, so there is no
SG.GEN form.)
SG.NOM
SG.GEN
ATTR
as'si
jierbmi
vuddjii
birgui
as'ss girji
jierbms olmmo
vuddjes biebmu
birgos mlli
C) The attribute form is formed by deleting the -s in the predicative form. This
group includes many adjectives ending in -as (but not all of them; cf. group A2). It is
quite paradoxical that the attribute forms of adjectives of this type involve the deletion of
-s, whereas the attribute forms of many other types of adjectives is formed by addition of
an ending -s / -es / -is (see category B above). Examples:
SG.NOM
SG.GEN
ATTR
oas
ielggas
galmmas
garas
liekkas
njuoskkas
oasa
ielgasa
galbmasa
garrasa
lieggasa
njuoskasa
oa mohtorgielk
ielga i
galbma hci
garra biegga
liegga viessu
njuoska biergu
D) The attribute form is formed by changing -i- to -e- in the second syllable. This
group includes most consonant stem adjectives with an SG.NOM ending in -is (but not
contracting -is-stem adjectives with a SG.NOM ending in -is!)
75
SG.NOM
SG.GEN
ATTR
boaris
buhtis
grvvis
oahpis
ollis
rnis
boarrsa
buhttsa
grvsa
oahppsa
ol'lsa
rtnsa
boares gpmagat
buhtes biktasat
grvves biebmu
oahpes olmmo
olles beaivi
rnes sof'f
buori
duoa
nuora
buorre filbma
duohta muitalus
nuorra nieida
(cf. B2a:)
alit
aliha
alit albmi
E2) A separate attribute form ending in -is, even though no such form is expected:
(cf. A1:)
aseha
oaneha
vuollega
asehaa
oanehaa
vuollegaa
asehis girji
oanehis bddi
vuollegis beavdi
(cf. A2:)
ieal
iekala
iekalis jvri
bb
njlg
stuor'r
ppa almmi
njlgga libi
stuorra dllu
(cf. B1)
seaggi
uhcci
un'ni
seakki
uhci
unni
seakka muorra
uhca gndda
unna mn
76
(cf. B2a)
allat
gassat
govdat
allaga
gassaga
govdaga
alla vrri
gassa almmi
govda johka
(cf. B2b)
rrat
lossat
rrada
lossada
rra iit
lossa diga
(cf. D)
dimis
dipmsa
dipma libi
variant 1:
-t : -bu-mus : -mussa- / -musa-
variant 2:
-et : -eabbo- / -eappo-eamos : -epmos-
The formation rules for comparative and superlative derivatives are fairly simple:
vowel stems:
consonant stems:
contracting is-stems:
adjectives ending in -i
77
Examples:
SG.NOM
SG.GEN
COMPAR
SUPERL
nuorra young
hlbi cheap
uhcci small, little
buorre good
lki easy
som pleasant, fun
lihkola happy
nuora
hlbbi
uhci
buori
lki
som
lihkolaa
nuorat
hlbbit
uhcit
buoret
lkit
somt
lihkolaat
nuoramus
hlbbimus
uhcimus
buoremus
lkimus
sommus
lihkolaamus
oas new
boaris old
seavdnjat dark
oasa
boarrsa
seavdnjada
oaset
boarrset
seavdnjadet
oaseamos
boarrseamos
seavdnjadeamos
ppis beautiful
njlggis tasty
bb
njlg
bbt
njlgt
bbmus
njlgmus
as'st
as'smus
A few adjectives have irregular comparative and superlative forms; the following are the
most common:
SG.NOM
SG.GEN
COMPAR
SUPERL
stuoris big
allat high
gassat thick (round things)
govdat broad
rrat early
lossat heavy
stuor'r
allaga
gassaga
govdaga
rrada
lossada
stuort, stuorit
alit
gasit
govddit
rat
losit
stuormus, stuorimus
alimus
gasimus
govddimus
ramus
losimus
Declination:
Comparatives formed with the suffix variant -t show a morphophonological peculiarity,
which not encountered in any other words: in the oblique stem the comparative suffix
appears as -bu-, and the comparative is declined as a vowel stem noun, even though it has
an odd number of syllables:
78
SG
PL
SG
PL
buoret
buorebut
lihkolaat
lihkolaabut
GEN
buorebu
buorebuid
lihkolaabu
lihkolaabuid
LOC
buorebus
buorebuin
lihkolaabus
lihkolaabuin
ILL
buorebui
buorebuidda
lihkolaabui
lihkolaabuidda
COM
buorebuin buorebuiguin
lihkolaabuin
lihkolaabuiguin
ESS
buorebun
lihkolaabun
Comparatives taking the suffix variant -et have oblique stems ending with -eabbo- / eappo-; they are declined according like a disyllabic nominal *eabbo would be, save for
the final vowel deletion and other phonological simplifications in the SG.NOM form:
NOM
SG
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
PL
oaset
oaseappot
oaseappo
oaseappuid
oaseappos oaseappuin
oasebbui
oaseappuide
oaseappuin oaseappuiguin
oaseabbon
Superlatives formed with the suffix variant -mus are vowel stems that are declined like a
disyllabic nominal *mussa would be; the final vowel in the SG.NOM form is deleted.
SG
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
PL
buoremus
buoremusat
buoremusa buoremusaid
buoremusas buoremusain
buoremussii buoremusaide
buoremusain buoremusaiguin
buoremussan
SG
PL
lihkolaamus
lihkolaamusat
lihkolaamusa lihkolaamusaid
lihkolaamusas lihkolaamusain
lihkolaamussii lihkolaamusaide
lihkolaamusain lihkolaamusaiguin
lihkolaamussan
Superlatives formed with the suffix variant -eamos are consonant stems that are declined
like consonant stems ending -us (e.g. boaus result; see 4.2.2.), whith the excepetion
that also the SG.NOM and ESS have the vowel -o-. Note that SG.NOM and ESS lack of
diphthong simplification despite the fact that -o- triggers simplification in the rest of the
paradigm!
79
SG
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
PL
oaseamos
oasepmosat
oasepmosa oasepmosiid
oasepmosis oasepmosiin
oasepmosii oasepmosiidda
oasepmosiin oasepmosiiguin
oaseamosin
Comparative constructions:
There are two alternative comparative constructions in North Saami. There is no
difference of meaning between the two constructions, but the first one is more commonly
used than the second one:
A) COMPAR + go than + NOM
Ig lea guhkit go Mhtte.
Ing be.3SG tall.COMPAR than Matthew(.NOM)
Ing is taller than Matthew.
B) SG.GEN +
COMPAR
80
COMPAR
SUPERL
davvi north
mddi south (EI)
lulli south (WI)
nuorti east
oarji west
gddi shore
vrri mountain
davimus northernmost
mttimus
lulimus
nuorttimus
oarjjimus
gttimus the one closest to the
shore
vrimus the one closest to the
mountain
Comparatives and superlatives of nouns are used in essentially the same way as
adjectives are, e.g. as predicatives and as attributes of nouns.
81
4.5. Quantifiers
4.5.1. Basic numerals
The morphologically underived numerals in North Saami are:
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
100
1000
SG.NOM
SG.GEN
nol'la
okta
guokte
golbma
njeallje
vihtta
guhtta
iea
gvcci
ovcci
logi
uoi
duht
nolla
ovtta
guovtti
golmma
njealji
via
gua
iea
gvcci
ovcci
logi
uoi
duhha
The rest of the numerals are formed by compounding. The numerals 1119 are formed by
compounding the numerals 19 with -nuppe-lohki [second.GEN.ten.ILL], which could be
translated as into the second ten: oktanuppelohki 11, guoktenuppelohki 12, etc. In
pronunciation -nuppelohki is often more or less reduced: e.g., oktanuppelohki can be
pronounced as /oktanuplohkaj/, /oktanumlohkaj/, /oktanuploh/, /oktanumloh/, and
/oktamloh/.
All numerals higher than 19 are formed by simply compounding the underived basic
numerals and the numerals for teens, as follows:
20
21
22
37
112
120
4725
guoktelogi [two.ten]
guoktelogiokta [two.ten.one]
guoktelogiguokte [two.ten.two]
golbmalogiiea [three.ten.seven]
uoiguoktenuppelohki [hundred.two.second.GEN.ten.ILL]
uoiguoktelogi [hundred.two.ten]
njealljeduhtieauoiguoktelogivihtta
[four.thousand.seven.hundred.two.ten.five]
etc.
82
The case paradigms of the numerals okta 1, guokte 2, vihtta 5, iea 7, gvcci 8,
and duht 1000 are given below as examples. (The numeral golbma 3 behaves like
okta 1, njeallje 4 like guokte 2, guhtta 6 like vihtta 5, and ovcci 9, logi 10 and
uoi 100 like gvcci 8). Note that the numerals vihtta 5 and guhtta 6 show
irregular consonant gradation (htt : ), and that the numerals gvcci 8, ovcci 9, logi
10 and uoi 100 lack consonant gradation.
okta 1:
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
guokte 2:
SG
PL
SG
PL
SG
PL
okta
ovtta
ovttas
oktii
ovttain
oktan
ovttat
ovttaid
ovttain
ovttaide
ovttaiguin
guokte
guovtti
guovttis
guokti
guvttiin
guoktin
guovttit
guvttiid
guvttiin
guvttiide
guvttiiguin
vihtta
via
vias
vihttii
viain
vihttan
viat
viaid
viain
viaide
viviaiguin
iea 7:
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
vihtta 5:
gvcci 8:
duht 1000:
SG
PL
SG
PL
SG
PL
iea
iea
ieas
iii
ieain
iean
ieat
ieaid
ieain
ieaide
ieaiguin
gvcci
gvcci
gvccis
gvcci
gvcciin
gvccin
gvccit
gvcciid
gvcciin
gvcciide
gvcciiguin
duht
duhha
duhhis
duhhii
duhhiin
duhhin
duhhat
duhhiid
duhhiin
duhhiidda
duhhiiguin
The case paradigms of compounded numerals are usually formed by declining the last
component of the numeral; the other parts of the compound remain unchanged. (There
are certain dialectal exceptions to this, though.) The paradigm of oktanuppelohki 11 is
given as an example.
oktanuppelohki 11:
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
SG
PL
oktanuppelohki
oktanuppelogi
oktanuppelogis
oktanuppelohki
oktanuppelogiin
oktanuppelohkin
oktanuppelogit
oktanuppelogiid
oktanuppelogiin
oktanuppelogiide
oktanuppelogiiguin
83
guokte dlu
two house.GEN
two houses
golbma dlu
three house.GEN
three houses
Plural numeral constructions are only used when counting sets of objects each of which
has a fixed number of members. Things occurring in sets include, e.g., shoes and gloves
(pairs) or tires of a car (sets of four). When quantifying such objects, singular numeral
constructions are used for counting individual objects and plural numeral constructions
for counting sets of objects. Compare the examples:
okta gma
one shoe(.NOM)
one shoe
guokte gpmaga
two shoe.GEN
two shoes
golbma gpmaga
three shoe.GEN
three shoes
ovttat gpmagat
one.PL.NOM shoe.PL.NOM
one pair of shoes
guovttit gpmagat
two.PL.NOM shoe.PL.NOM
two pairs of shoes
golmmat gpmagat
three.PL.NOM shoe.PL.NOM
three pairs of shoes
okta riekkis
one tire(.NOM)
one tire
guokte riegg
two tire.GEN
two tires
golbma riegg
three tire.GEN
three tires
ovttat rieggt
one.PL.NOM tire.PL.NOM
one set of tires
guovttit rieggt
two.PL.NOM tire.PL.NOM
two sets of tires
golmmat rieggt
three.PL.NOM tire.PL.NOM
three sets of tires
Numeral constructions also show a few other peculiar morphosyntactic features. First, the
numeral and the quantified noun do not always take the same case marking. Second, it is
84
to some extent ambiguous which word is the head of the construction; in certain case
forms the numeral appears to function as the head, whereas in others the noun could be
better analyzed as the head of the phrase.
As regards morphosyntax, the numeral okta 1 behaves differently from all other
numerals, so it can be dealt with first. When okta quantifies a noun, the case marking
pattern for the whole numeral phrase is the following:
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
okta dllu
ovtta dlu
ovtta dlus
ovtta dllui
ovttain dluin
oktan dllun
As the paradigm shows, the locative and illative case is marked only on the quantified
noun, whereas the quantifier okta one occurs in the genitive case. This phenomenon is
called partial agreement; it is also exhibited by some other modifiers of nouns, such as
demonstrative pronouns that occur as modifiers as nouns.
The case marking pattern in plural numeral constructions involving okta one is
somewhat different, and also shows dialectal differences. In the locative, the illative and
the comitative, okta may either take the same case as the noun, or occur in the PL.GEN
form. (Note that the essive has no distinct plural form; see 4.2.)
PL.NOM
PL.GEN
PL.LOC
PL.ILL
PL.COM
ESS
ovttat gpmagat
ovttaid gpmagiid
ovttaid gpmagiin
~ ovttain gpmagiin
ovttaid gpmagiidda
~ ovttaide gpmagiidda
ovttaid gpmagiiguin
~ ovttaiguin gpmagiiguin
oktan gman
Numeral constructions with numerals other than okta 1 differ in one particular form:
when the whole phrase is in the nominative singular, only the numeral takes the SG.NOM
marking, whereas the noun occurs in the SG.GEN. In this form, thus, it would seem as if
the numeral was the head of the construction, as it carries the marking for the nominative
case. However, in the locative and illative singular the case marking is carried by the
noun, whereas the numeral occurs in the SG.GEN!
85
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
guokte dlu
guovtti dlu
guovtti dlus
guovtti dllui
guvttiin dluin
guoktin dllun
In the plural, there is no difference between constructions involving okta 1 and other
numerals:
PL.NOM
PL.GEN
PL.LOC
PL.ILL
PL.COM
ESS
guovttit gpmagat
guvttiid gpmagiid
guvttiid gpmagiin
~ guvttiin gpmagiin
guvttiid gpmagiidda
~ guvttiide gpmagiidda
guvttiid gpmagiiguin
~ guvttiiguin gpmagiiguin
guoktin gman
The numeral-like quantifiers mga many, moadde a few, a couple of, and gal'le
how many behave identically to numerals higher than one. The paradigm of
consructions with mga is given as an example:
ESS
mga dlu
mgga dlu
mgga dlus
mgga dllui
mggain dluin
mgan dllun
PL.NOM
mggat gpmagat
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
PL.GEN
PL.LOC
PL.ILL
PL.COM
ESS
86
In addition to numerals and the three numeral-like quantifiers, there are many other
quantifiers; some of the most common are soames some, muhtun some, ollu ~ ol'lu
much, eanet more, uhcn little, uhcit less, veh a little, a bit. These quantifiers
behave like adjectives modifying nouns: they are indeclinable when they occur in
quantifier constructions.
87
4.6. Pronouns
The North Saami pronoun system consists of both independent pronouns and pronoun
constructions. Pronouns are independent lexical items (e.g. mun I, dat it, gii who),
whereas pronoun contructions are phrases consisting of two distinct lexical items which,
nevertheless, have a fixed pronominal function (e.g. nubbi nuppi each other, mii beare
whatever, gii nu someone).
4.6.1. Personal pronouns
Personal pronouns distinguish between singular, dual and plural, and between first,
second and third person. There is no distinction between inclusive and exclusive we.
The personal pronouns are given in the table below; the case paradigms of the pronouns
can be found in section 4.2.5.
1
2
3
SG
DU
PL
mun
don
dat; son
moai
doai
dat; soai
mii
dii
dat; sii
As the table above shows, there are two different third person pronouns. Note that dat is
also a demonstrative pronoun roughly corresponding to English it and they; see the
section on demonstrative pronouns below (4.6.3). In most contexts the difference
between these two is stylistic: dat is colloquial and the one used in normal speech,
whereas son, soai, and sii are used in more formal speech and in most genres of written
language.
Son/dat boii ikte.
3SG come.PAST.3SG yesterday
(s)he came yesterday (formal / colloquial)
Muitalin dan sudnuide/daidda.
tell.PAST.1SG it.SG.GEN 3DU.ILL
I told it to them (2). (formal / colloquial)
Jerretgo sis/dain?
ask.PAST.2SG.Q 3PL.LOC
Did you ask them? (formal / colloquial)
However, there are some more intricate rules regarding the use of the two sets of third
person pronouns: son, soai and sii are preferred when the person or the people the
pronoun refers to hear the conversation. Apparently, this is at least partially motivated by
88
the fact that dat is also a demonstrative pronoun that refers to something outside the
context of the conversation (see 4.6.3).
In relative clauses there is a grammatical opposition between dat and son/soai/sii: the
latter refers back to the subject of the main clause, but the former does not. Compare the
following minimal pair:
Mhtte logai, ahte son boaht.
Matthew say.PAST.1SG that 3SG come.3SG
Matthew said that he [= Matthew] will come.
Mhtte logai, ahte dat boaht.
Mathhew say.PAST.1SG that 3SG come.3SG
Matthew said that (s)he [= someone else, not Matthew] will come.
89
Reflexive pronouns are often used in non-finite constructions to refer back to the subject
of the finite verb:
Soai dajaiga alddiska leat biilla.
3DU say.PAST.3DU REFL.LOC.3DU be.INF car.GEN
They said they have a car.
The genitive forms of the reflexive pronoun are used in the meaning ones own:
Dat lea mu iean biila.
it be.3SG 1SG.GEN REFL.1SG car
It is my own car.
Vldde ieat biilla.
take.IMP.2SG REFL.2SG car.GEN
Take your own car.
A reflexive pronoun following a pronoun or a noun, taking the same case as the noun, can
be used as a form of emphasis:
Mhtte ie doppe lei.
Matthew REFL there be.PAST.3SG
Indeed, Matthew was there.
Dus alddt mun jearan.
2SG.LOC REFL.LOC.2SG 1SG ask.1SG
Im asking you (personally).
A nominative form of the reflexive pronoun before a personal pronoun subject indicates
that the action the verb pertains to has no particular aim or purpose:
Ie mun djun.
REFL.SG 1SG joke.1SG
Im just joking.
Iea mii dppe vccait.
REFL.PL 1PL here walk_about.1PL
Were just taking a stroll here.
90
GLOSS
dat
it; that
REFERS TO
dt
diet
duot
dot
PL
LOC
dt biila
dn biilla
dn biillas
ILL
dn biilii
COM
dinna biillain
dt biillat
did biillaid
din biillain
~ did biillain
didda biillaide
~ did biillaide
diguin biillaiguin
~ did biillaiguin
ESS
dnin biilan
NOM
GEN
91
NEG.2SG
PL
mii biillaid
man biilla
man biillas
mat biillat
maid biillaid
main biillain
~ maid biillain
man biilii
maidda biillaide
~ maid biillaide
mainna biillain maiguin biillaiguin
~ maid biillaiguin
manin biilan
SG
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
SG
PL
gii olbmuid
gean olbmo
gean olbmos
geat olbmot
geaid olbmuid
geain olbmuin
~ geaid olbmuin
gean olbmui
geaidda olbmuide
~ geaid olbmuide
geainna olbmuin geaiguin olbmuiguin
~ geaid olbmuiguin
geanin olmmoin
PL
goabb gma
goapp gpmaga
goapp gpmagis
goappt gpmagat
goappid gpmagiid
goappin gpmagiin
~ goappid gpmagiin
goapp gpmagii
goappide gpmagiidda
~ goappid gpmagiidda
goappin gpmagiin goappiguin gpmagiiguin
~ goappid gpmagiiguin
goabbn gman
93
94
something
someone
either of two, one of two
The paradigms of the indefinite pronoun constructions are formed by declining the
pronoun mii, gii, goabb or guhte; the other lexical component remains unchanged. The
paradigms of vaikko mii and gii nu are given as examples below:
NOM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
ESS
SG
PL
SG
PL
vaikko mii
vaikko man
vaikko mas
vaikko masa
vaikko mainna
vaikko manin
vaikko mat
vaikko maid
vaikko main
vaikko maidda
vaikko maiguin
gii nu
gean nu
geas nu
geasa nu
geainna nu
geanin nu
geat nu
geaid nu
geain nu
geaidda nu
geaiguin nu
95
Usage examples:
Dt ii leat miige oahppogirjjiid.
this NEG.3SG be.CONNEG what.also learning.book.PL.GEN
This is no textbook.
Dppe ii leat giige.
here NEG.3SG be.CONNEG who.also
Theres no one here.
Ii mis guhtege leat oaidnn Mhte.
NEG.3SG 1PL.LOC who.also be.CONNEG see.PAST_PTCL Matthew.GEN
No one of us has seen Matthew.
Goabbge ii beassan vuolgit.
which_of_two.also NEG.3SG get_to.PAST.CONNEG leave.INF
Neither of them could go / had the chance to go.
96
Note, however, that the SG.NOM form miige is never used alone as an indefinite pronoun;
instead, a form mihkkege is used:
Mus ii leat mihkkege.
1SG.LOC NEG.3SG be.CONNEG mihkkege
I dont have anything.
(cf. the example Dt ii leat miige oahppogirjjiid above!)
each other
The nubbi nuppi construction can be declined in the singular or in the plural. The singular
forms have a dual function, and the plural forms a plural function. The latter component
carries case and number marking, but the former only number marking (SG.NOM or
PL.NOM). Possessive suffixes are never used in the nubbi nuppi construction. The case
paradigm of nubbi nuppi is given below (reciprocal pronoun constructions do not have
NOM or ESS forms):
SG (dual
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
function)
nubbi nuppi
nubbi nuppis
nubbi nubbi
nubbi nuppiin
PL (plural
function)
nuppit nuppiid
nuppit nuppiin
nuppit nuppiide
nuppit nuppiiguin
In the goabbat guoibmi ja guhtet guoibmi constructions the noun guoibmi always takes
possessive suffixes. The goabbat guoibmi construction is used in dual fucntion, and
hence dual possessive suffixes are used; the construction guhtet guoibmi has a plural
function, and takes plural possessive suffixes. In the guhtet guoibmi construction the
97
noun guoibmi may be conjugated either in the singular or in the plural; both forms are
used, and there is no difference of meaning between them.
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
GEN
LOC
ILL
COM
1DU
goabbat guoibmme
goabbat guoimmisteame
goabbat guoibmseame
goabbat guimmiineame
1PL
guhtet guoibmmet (~ guimmiideamet)
guhtet guoimmisteamet (~ guimmiineamet)
guhtet guoibmseamet (~ guimmiidasamet)
guhtet guimmiineamet (~ guimmiideametguin)
2DU
goabbat guoimmde
goabbat guoimmisteatte
goabbat guoibmseatte
goabbat guimmiineatte
2PL
guhtet guoimmdet (~ guimmiideattet)
guhtet guoimmisteattet (~ guimmiineattet)
guhtet guoibmseattet (~ guimmiidasadet)
guhtet guimmiineattet (~ guimmiideattetguin)
3DU
goabbat guoimmiska
goabbat guoimmisteaskka
goabbat guoibmseaskka
goabbat guimmiineaskka
3PL
guhtet guoimmiset (~ guimmiideaset)
guhtet guoimmisteaset (~ guimmiineaset)
guhtet guoibmseaset (~ guimmiidasaset)
guhtet guimmiineaset (~ guimmiideasetguin)
98
North Saami also has quite a few reciprocal verbs, such as oaidnalit to see/meet each
other, suhtadit to get mad at each other, bealkkait to bicker at each other, soabadit
to make up with each other, heaitalit to stop doing X to each other; to break up. If
reciprocity is already coded in the verb, a reciprocal pronoun construction cannot appear
as an overt object in the sentence:
Moai oaidnaletne ihttin.
1DU see_each_other.1DU tomorrow
Well meet tomorrow.
Mhtte ja Ig suhtadeigga.
Matthew and Ig get_mad_at_each_other.PAST.3DU
Matthew and Ing got mad at each other.
(cf.:)
*Moai oaidnaletne nubbi nuppi ~ goabbat guoibmme ihttin.
1DU see_each_other.1DU another another.GEN ~ each companion.GEN.1DU
tomorrow
99
1., 2., 3.
singular, dual, plural
non-past, past, perfect, pluperfect
indicative, imperative, conditional, dubitative
neutral, progressive
These categories are partially marked by verb conjugation, and partially by periphrastic
constructions applying the verb leat be or the negative verb in addition to
morhpological marking. For instance:
Pure morphological marking:
bora-n eat-1SG
borre-n eat.PAST-1SG
bora--in eat-COND-1SG
100
101
The stem can be formed by removing the ending -t form the infinitive:
stem
geahat
geahaboahtit
boahtigoarrut
goarrurehkenastit
rehkenastidolastallat
dolastallamuitaligoahtit muitaligoahtiINF
to watch, to look
to come
to sew
to count, to calculate
to keep a campfire
to begin telling
B) Trisyllabic verbs. The infinitive of the verb nearly always has three syllables and
ends in -it, e.g. muitalit to tell, berotit to care for, care about. Also verbs with five
syllables, such as rehkenastalit to try to count, belong in this category (except for the
inceptive derivatives in -(i)goahtit mentioned above). As verbs with five syllables are
rare, the category is named according to the more common three-syllable type.
Trisyllabic verbs do not participate in consonant gradation.
You get the stem of the verb by removing the ending -it form the infinitive:
INF
muitalit
berotit
rehkenastalit
stem
muitalberotrehkenastal-
to tell
to care for, care about
to try to count
C) J-stem verbs. The infinitive form of these verbs always ends in -t, -et or -ot. The
infinitive usually has two syllables, e.g. ohkkt to sit, sddet to send, digot to order,
to subscribe. Some have four syllables, e.g. vldojuv'vot to be taken, and even six
syllables is possible: rehkenastojuv'vot to be counted. An odd number of syllables is
also possible, even though quite rare: e.g., rigeret to make a racket. Like the trisyllabic
verbs, j-stem verbs never participate in consonant gradation.
The group is called the J-stem verbs because all verbs of this type have two stems: a
shorter stem which you get by removing the infinitve ending -t, and a longer stem with
the consonant increment -j-. Certain forms of these verbs are based on the shorter stem,
while others are formed from the longer j-stem:
INF
ohkkt
sddet
digot
vldojuv'vot
rigeret
short stem
ohkksddedigovldojuv'vorigere-
j-stem
ohkkjsddejdigojvldojuv'vojrigerej-
to sit
to send
to order, to subscribe
to be taken
to make a racket
102
vowel stem
consonant stem
1SG
2DU
1DU
IMP.2SG
bora-n
borra-beahtti
borre(-)
bora(-)
muital-an
muital-eahppi
muital-etne
muital(-)
In the paradigms of j-stem verbs, a shorter stem ending in a vowel and a longer stem
ending in the consonant -j- alterate. Certain forms are based on the shorter stem, while
others are based on the j-stem. As a result of this, in certain forms these verbs take vowel
stem endings, whereas other forms in the paradigm take consonant stem endings.
Consider the following forms of the verb ohkkt to sit:
form
vowel stem
1SG
2DU
1DU
IMP.2SG
ohkk-n
ohkk-beahtti
ohkkj-etne
ohkk(-)
103
The functions of the past, perfect and pluperfect tenses correspond in a rather
straightforward way to the functions of those tenses in English, for instance. The non-past
tense functions as both a present and future tense. No grammatical distinction between a
present and future tense exists in North Saami.
5.4.1.1. Indicative, non-past tense
The indicative mood non-past tense endings are the following:
vowel stems:
1
2
3
consonant stems:
SG
DU
PL
SG
DU
PL
-n
-t
-
-
-beahtti
-ba
-t
-behtet
-t
-an
-at
-a
-etne
-eahppi
-eaba
-it
-ehpet
-it
Gradating verbs take the vowel stem endings. The following morphophonological
alterations occur in the verb root:
the weak grade occurs in the 1SG and 2SG forms (the rest of the forms have the strong
grade)
the 1SG and 2SG forms undergo unstressed vowel alteration 1 (i > ; see 3.3.)
the 3SG form undergoes unstressed vowel alteration 2 (i > , a > ; see 3.3.)
the 1DU and 3PL forms undergo unstressed vowel alteration 4 (i > e, a > e, u > o; see
3.3.)
in the 1DU and 3PL forms, second-syllable e and o trigger diphthong simplification
(see 3.2.)
Trisyllabic verbs take the consonant-stem endings; the verb root undergoes no
morphophonological alterations.
J-stem verbs show the longer j-stem in the 1DU and 3PL forms, whereas the rest of the
forms are based on the shorter vowel stem.
Example paradigms (geahat to look, to watch, vuolgit to leave, to go (away),
doalvut to take (somewhere), muitalit to tell, ohkkt to sit, sddet to send, digot
to order, to subscribe):
SG
1 geahan
2 geahat
3 geah
DU
PL
gehe
geahat
geahabeahtti geahabehtet
geahaba
gehet
SG
DU
PL
vuolggn vulge
vuolgit
vuolggt vuolgibeahtti vuolgibehtet
vuolg
vuolgiba
vulget
104
SG
DU
PL
SG
doalvut
doalvubehtet
dolvot
muitalan muitaletne
muitalit
muitalat muitaleahppi muitalehpet
muitala muitaleaba
muitalit
DU
PL
SG
DU
ohkkjetne
ohkkbeahtti
ohkkba
ohkkt
ohkkbehtet
ohkkjit
sdden
sddet
sdde
sddejetne
sddet
sddebeahtti sddebehtet
sddeba
sddejit
DU
PL
digojetne
digobeahtti
digoba
digot
digobehtet
digojit
1 doalvvun dolvo
2 doalvvut doalvubeahtti
3 doalvu
doalvuba
SG
1 ohkkn
2 ohkkt
3 ohkk
SG
1 digon
2 digot
3 digo
DU
PL
PL
The paradigm of the verb leat to be is partially irregular and has to be learned by rote;
the indicative mood non-past tense forms are:
SG
1 lean
2 leat
3 lea
DU
PL
letne
leahppi
leaba
leat
lehpet
leat
The negative non-past indicative forms are formed by combining a form of the negative
verb (marked for person and number) with the non-past indicative connegative form of
the main verb. The indicative paradigm of the negative verb is:
SG
1 in
2 it
3 ii
DU
PL
ean
eahppi
eaba
eat
ehpet
eai
The rules for forming the non-past indicative connegative forms of verbs are:
a) gradating verbs:
no ending (-)
weak grade
unstressed vowel alteration 3 (i > e, u > o; see 3.3.)
note that the second-syllable e and o do not cause diphthong simplification!
b) trisyllabic verbs:
no ending (-)
margin consonant alteration may occur (see 3.4.)
c) j-stem verbs:
no ending (-)
the connegative form is identical to the short stem (vowel stem)
105
d) the verb leat to be has the irregular connegative form leat (which is homonymous
with the infinitive).
Examples:
INF
CONNEG
geahat
vuolgit
doalvut
muitalit
berotit
boradit
ohkkt
sddet
digot
geaha
vuolgge
doalvvo
muital
bero
borat
ohkk
sdde
digo
to look, to watch
to leave, to go (away)
to take (somewhere)
to tell
to care
to have a meal
to sit
to send
to order, to subscribe
I dont look/watch
you (SG) dont leave/go
(s)he doesnt take
we (2) dont tell
you (2) dont care
they (2) dont have a meal
we (3+) dont sit
you (3+) dont send
they (3+) dont order/subscribe
Im not
consonant stems:
SG
DU
PL
SG
DU
PL
-n
-t
-i
-ime
-ide
-iga
-imet
-idet
-
-in
-it
-ii
-eimme
-eidde
-eigga
-eimmet
-eiddet
-edje
106
Gradating verbs take the vowel stem endings. The following morphophonological
alterations occur in the verb root:
the strong grade occurs in the 1SG, 2SG and 3PL forms (the rest of the forms have the
weak grade)
the 1SG, 2SG and 3PL forms undergo unstressed vowel alteration 4 (i > e, a > e, u >
o; see 3.3.)
second-syllable e, o and ii trigger diphthong simplification (see 3.2.)
Trisyllabic verbs take the consonant-stem endings; the verb root undergoes no
morphophonological alterations.
J-stem verbs show the longer j-stem in the 1SG, 2SG and 3PL forms, whereas the rest of
the forms are based on the shorter vowel stem;
second-syllable e and o undergo the unstressed vowel changes e > i and o > u
whenever a suffix beginning with -i- follows (see 3.3).
Example paradigms (geahat to look, to watch, vuolgit to leave, to go (away),
doalvut to take (somewhere), muitalit to tell, ohkkt to sit):
SG
1 gehen
2 gehet
3 geahai
SG
1 dolvon
2 dolvot
3 doalvvui
SG
DU
PL
SG
DU
PL
geahaime
geahaide
geahaiga
geahaimet
geahaidet
gehe
vulgen
vulget
vulgii
vulggiime
vulggiide
vulggiiga
vulggiimet
vulggiidet
vulge
DU
PL
SG
DU
PL
doalvvuime
doalvvuide
doalvvuiga
doalvvuimet
doalvvuidet
dolvo
muitalin
muitalit
muitalii
muitaleimme muitaleimmet
muitaleidde muitaleiddet
muitaleigga muitaledje
DU
PL
SG
DU
PL
ohkkimet
ohkkidet
ohkkjedje
sddejin
sddejit
sddii
sddiime
sddiide
sddiiga
sddiimet
sddiidet
sddejedje
1 ohkkjin ohkkime
2 ohkkjit ohkkide
3 ohkki ohkkiga
SG
1 digojin
2 digojit
3 digui
DU
PL
diguime
diguide
diguiga
diguimet
diguidet
digojedje
1 ledjen
2 ledjet
3 lei
DU
PL
leimme
leidde
leigga
leimmet
leiddet
ledje
107
The negative verb does not have separate past tense forms. Instead, the negative past
tense is formed by combining a form of the negative verb (see 5.4.1.1. above) by a
separate past tense connegative form of the main verb. The rules for forming the past
tense connegative forms of verbs are:
a) gradating verbs:
ending -n
strong grade
unstressed vowel alteration 5 (i > , u > o; see 3.3.)
second-syllable o causes diphthong simplification (see 3.2.)
b) trisyllabic verbs:
ending -an
no morphoponological alterations
c) j-stem verbs:
ending -n added to the vowel stem
no morphophonological alterations
d) the verb leat to be has the past tense connegative form lean.
Examples:
INF
CONNEG
geahat
vuolgit
doalvut
muitalit
berotit
boradit
ohkkt
sddet
digot
geahan
vuolgn
dolvon
muitalan
berotan
boradan
ohkkn
sdden
digon
to look, to watch
to leave, to go (away)
to take (somewhere)
to tell
to care
to have a meal
to sit
to send
to order, to subscribe
108
The patterns for forming the perfect and pluperfect tenses are the following:
perfect:
a non-past tense indicative form of leat + past participle
negative perfect:
negative verb + non-past tense connegative of leat + past participle
pluperfect:
a past tense indicative form of leat + past participle
negative pluperfect: negative verb + past tense connegative of leat + past participle
Examples:
(geahat to look, to watch)
perfect:
letne geaha-n
be-1DU watch-PAST_PTCL
we (2) have watched
negative perfect:
pluperfect:
le-imme geaha-n
be-PAST.1DU watch-PAST_PTCL
we (2) had watched
lehpet lea-ma
be.2PL be-PAST_PTCL
you (3+) have been
negative perfect:
pluperfect:
leiddet lea-ma
be.PAST.2PL be-PAST_PTCL
you (3+) had been
109
consonant stems:
SG
DU
PL
SG
DU
PL
-
-os
-u
-
-oska
-ot
-t
-oset
-
-ehkos
-eadnu
-ehkot ~ -eatnot
-eahkki
-ehket
-ehkoska -ehkoset
There are two notable things regarding the shape of the endings. First, the vowel stem
and consonant stem allomorphs of most endings are phonologiclly highly dissimilar.
Second, the the vowel stem endings in the first and third person impertaive forms are
exceptional in that they begin with a vowel rather than a consonant. When these endings
are attached to vowel stems, the final vowel of the verb stem disappears through stem
vowel deletion. This morphophonological process occurs in no other inflectional forms,
but is common in derivation (see 3.3. for discussion).
The rules for forming the impertative paradigms are the following:
Gradating verbs take the vowel stem endings. The following morphophonological
alterations occur in the verb root:
the weak grade occurs in the 2SG form; the rest of the forms have a strong grade
the 1DU and 2DU forms (and in some dialects also the 1PL and 2PL forms) undergo
strong grade strengthening (see 3.1.3.)
the 2SG form undegoes unstressed vowel alteration 1 (i > e, u > o; see 3.3.)
the 2DU form undergoes unstressed vowel alteration 7 (a > i; see 3.3.)
the 2PL form undergoes unstressed vowel alteration 4 (i > e, a > e, u > o; see 3.3.)
all 1. and 3. person forms undergo stem vowel deletion (see above and 3.3.)
all forms with second-syllable -e- or -o- except for 2SG undergo diphthong
simplification
Trisyllabic verbs take the consonant-stem endings. In the 2SG form, which lacks an
ending, a margin consonant alteration can occur (see 3.4.).
J-stem verbs have the vowel stem only in the 2SG form; all the other forms are based on
the longer j-stem, and hence show the consonant-stem endings.
110
DU
PL
SG
DU
jear'ru
boahttu
jeara
jear'ri
jerros
jerroska
jerrot
~ jer'rot
jerret
~ jer'ret
jerroset
SG
DU
PL
goar'ru
goaro
gorros
boae
bohtos
PL
bohtot
~ bohttot
boahtti
bohtet
~ bohttet
bohtoska bohtoset
gorrot
~ gor'rot
goar'ru
gorrot
~ gor'rot
gorroska gorroset
SG
DU
PL
veahkeheadnu
2
3
veahket
veahkehehkos
veahkeheahkki
veahkehehkoska
veahkehehkot
~ vehkeheatnot
veahkehehket
veahkehehkoset
SG
DU
PL
ohkkjeadnu
2
3
ohkk
ohkkjehkos
ohkkjeahkki
ohkkjehkoska
ohkkjehkot
~ ohkkjeatnot
ohkkjehket
ohkkjehkoset
SG
DU
PL
leage
lehkos
Negative imperative-optatives are periphrastic constructions consisting of an imperativeoptative form of the negative verb followed by the non-past tense connegative form of
the main verb. The formation of the non-past connegative form is explained in 5.4.1.1.
The imperative-optative paradigm of the negative verb is:
111
1
2
3
SG
DU
PL
ale
allos
al'lome
al'li
alloska
allot ~ al'lot
allet ~ al'let
alloset
112
COND substem
geaha-vuolgg-dolvvo-- ~ doalvvo-muital-iveohkk--
to look, watch
to leave, go (away)
to take (somewhere)
to tell
to sit
The conditional non-past forms take the same set of personal endings as the indicative
past tense (see 5.4.1.2.), not the non-past tense personal endings (5.1.1.1.). The
conditional substems with the suffix in -- take the consonant stem endings, whereas the
substems with the suffix -ive- take the vowel stem endings. Note that the -e- in the
allomorph -ive- automatically changes to -i- (> -ivi-) before any ending beginning
with -i- (see the unstressed vowel alteration rules in 3.3.).
113
DU
PL
1
2
3
geahain
geahait
geahaii
geahaeimme
geahaeidde
geahaeigga
geahaeimmet
geahaeiddet
geahaedje
1
2
3
ohkkin
ohkkit
ohkkii
ohkkeimme
ohkkeidde
ohkkeigga
ohkkeimmet
ohkkeiddet
ohkkedje
1
2
3
muitaliven
muitalivet
muitalivii
muitaliviime
muitaliviide
muitaliviiga
muitaliviimet
muitaliviidet
muitalive
SG
DU
PL
liven
livet
livii
liviime
liviide
liviiga
liviimet
liviidet
live
There are two periphrastic constructions for exprssing the conditional past tense. As far
as is known, the two constructions have an identical meaning. Nevertheless, since both
types of constructions are attested in all dialects, the difference does not seem to be
dialectal; hence, it is possible that there is some kind of as yet undiscovered semantic
difference between the two conditional past tense constuctions.
type 1:
type 2:
For example:
liven vuolgn
be.COND.1SG leave.PAST_PTCL
I would have gone
~ ledjen vuolgit
be.PAST.1SG leave.INF
115
DUB substem
geaha-vulgge-dolvvo-ohkk--
to look, watch
to leave, go (away)
to take (somewhere)
to sit
The dubitative takes the non-past tense personal endings (see 5.4.1.1.). The substems
ending in -- take the consonant stem endings. There is one exception, though: the
DUB.3SG form takes no personal ending (-), in contrast to the 3SG ending -a in the
indicative non-past (cf., e.g. muital-a tell.3SG (s)he tells). Due to the lack of a personal
ending in the DUB.3SG form, the DUB suffix -- ends up in final position and is realized as
- (because affricates are not permitted word-finally).
Example dubitative paradigms:
SG
1
2
3
1
2
3
DU
PL
SG
DU
geahaan geahaetne
geahait
geahaat geahaeahppi geahaehpet
geaha
geahaeaba
geahait
vulggean
vulggeat
vulgge
vulggeetne
vulggeit
vulggeeahppi vulggeehpet
vulggeeaba
vulggeit
SG
DU
SG
DU
dolvvoan
dolvvoat
dolvvo
dolvvoetne
dolvvoit
dolvvoeahppi dolvvoehpet
dolvvoeaba
dolvvoit
ohkkan
ohkkat
ohkk
ohkketne
ohkkit
ohkkeahppi ohkkehpet
ohkkeaba
ohkkit
PL
PL
PL
The dubitative forms of trisyllabic verbs are formed in a manner similar to the indicative
non-past forms of quadrisyllabic gradating verbs. The dubitative suffix -eaa- itself
116
1
2
3
SG
DU
PL
veahkeheaan
veahkeheaat
veahkehea
~ veahkehe(a)
veahkehee
veahkeheaabeahtti
veahkeheaaba
veahkeheaat
veahkeheaabehtet
veahkeheet
SG
DU
PL
viean
vieat
vie
vie
vieat
vieabeahtti vieabehtet
vieaba
viet
SG
DU
leaan
leaat
lea
~ le(a)
lee
leaabeahtti
leaaba
PL
leaat
leaabehtet
leet
117
trisyllabic verbs:
j-stem verbs:
The progressive aspect can be combined with all moods and tenses except for the
imperative-optative mood (see 5.4.2.) and the conditional past tense type 2 (see 5.4.3.).
The following examples illustrate the conversion of various types of constructions into
the progressive mood. As the lexical verb of the construction is put into the progressive
form, all information regarding mood and tense is marked on the auxiliary leat:
118
Mhtte mann
Matthew go.3SG
Matthew goes
Mhtte ii mana
Matthew NEG.3SG go.CONNEG
Matthew doesnt go
Mhtte manai
Matthew go.PAST.3SG
Matthew went
Mhtte ii mannan
Matthew NEG.3SG go.PAST.CONNEG
Matthew didnt go
Mhtte manaii
Matthew go.COND.3SG
Matthew would go
Mhtte ii manae
Matthew NEG.3SG go.COND.CONNEG
Matthew would not go
119
Mhtte mana
Matthew go.DUB.3SG
Matthew might go
Mhtte ii manae
Matthew NEG.3SG go.DUB.CONNEG
Matthew presumably doesnt go
There are certain semantic limitations to the use of the progressive aspect. Typically,
progressive aspect is only used when the verb has a high degree of agentivity i.e., when
the subject is a conscious agent that is perceived as actively doing something. The less
agentivity the verb shows, the less probable and less grammatical the use of the
progressive aspect is. Consider the following continuum:
Mhtte lea uohppamin muoraid.
Matthew be.3SG cut.PROG tree.PL.GEN
Matthew is cutting wood.
Mhtte lea mannamin gvpi.
Matthew be.3SG go.PROG store.ILL
Matthew is going to the store.
Mhtte lea smiehttamin iht logaldaga.
Matthew be.3SG ponder.PROG tomorrows lecture.GEN
Matthew is thinking about tomorrows lecture.
Mhtte uou ~ ?lea uoumin duoppe.
Matthew stand.3SG ~ be.3SG stand.PROG there.LOC
Matthew is standing over there.
Mhtte jurddaa ~ ?lea jurddaeamen.
Matthew think.3SG ~ be.3SG think.PROG
Matthew is thinking.
120
122
Heivego borgguhit?
be_appropriate_to.3SG.Q smoke.INF
Can I smoke here? (Is it appropriate to smoke?)
c) a mental state or a mental action, e.g. igut to intend to, jurddait to think,
hliidit to want, lohpidit to promise.
Mhtte iggui oa svnni hukset.
Matthew intend_to.PAST.3SG new.ATTR sauna.GEN build.INF
Matthew was going to build a new sauna.
Lean jurddaan olgoriikii frret.
be.1SG think.PAST_PTCL out.country.ILL move.INF
Ive been thinking of moving abroad. (= and I intend to do it)
Hliidehpetgo filmma geahat?
want.2PL.Q film.GEN watch.INF
Do you (pl.) want to watch a movie?
Dal'le mun easkka fuomin ohcagoahtit Mhte.
then 1SG only_then notice.PAST.1SG search.INCEPT.INF Matthew.GEN
Only then I noticed that I must/should start looking for Matthew.
Mhtte lohpidii vieat mu biillain.
Matthew promise.PAST.3SG fetch.INF 1SG.GEN car.COM
Matthew promised to pick me up by car.
d) beginning (e.g., lgit to begin)
Dasto lggii uorpmastit.
after_that begin.PAST.3SG hail.INF
Then it began to hail. / Then a hail storm began.
e) motion, as a verbal argument that in terms of directionality corresponds to an NP in
the illative case:
Mannen libbi oastit.
go.PAST.1SG bread.GEN buy.ILL
I went to buy some bread.
(cf. mannen gvpi [go.PAST.1SG store.ILL] I went to the store)
123
124
trisyllabic verbs:
j-stem verbs:
The second infinitive has two main functions. Firstly, it is used as an argument of verbs
that signify that an action, event or process stops, ends, is not carried out, does not
occur, or is avoided:
De heittii arvimis.
then stop.PAST.3SG rain.2INF
Then it stopped raining.
Mhtte gearggai boradeames.
Matthew be_done_with.PAST.3SG have_a_meal.2INF
Matthew finished his meal.
Mhtte dolkkai ~ vibbai luopmniid oaggimis.
Matthew get_bored_with.PAST.3SG ~ get_tired_with.PAST.3SG
cloudberry.PL.GEN gather.2INF
Matthew got bored ~ tired with picking cloudberries [and stopped].
(compare:)
Mhtte dolkkai ~ vibbai luopmniid oaggit.
Matthew get_bored_with.PAST.3SG ~ get_tired_with.PAST.3SG
cloudberry.PL.GEN gather.INF
Matthew got bored ~ tired with picking cloudberries [but didnt stop].
Mhtte gjui mu heavvaneames.
Matthew rescue.PAST.3SG 1SG.GEN drown.2INF
Matthew saved me from drowning.
Mhtte gilddii mnid gddi mannamis.
Matthew forbid.PAST.3SG child.PL.GEN shore.ILL go.2INF
Matthew told the children not to go to the shore.
125
trisyllabic verbs:
j-stem verbs:
126
The following examples illustrate the use of the gerund. Normally the underlying subject
of the gerund is the same as the subject of the finite verb in the sentence. A possessive
suffix that refers to the subject can be attached to the gerund form, but this is not
obligatory:
Mhtte lvllui goarodettiin(is).
Matthew sing.PAST.3SG sew.GER(.3SG)
Matthew sang while (he was) sewing.
Mnt galget ohkkt siivvus boradettiin(easet).
child.PL.NOM must.3PL sit.INF quietly have_a_meal.GER(.3PL)
Children are supposed to sit quietly while (they are) eating.
Moai nohkaime filmma geahadettiin(eame).
1DU fall_asleep.PAST.1DU film.GEN watch.GER(.1DU)
We (2) fell asleep while watching the film.
Mhtte bii lotti uoiggadettiin(is).
Matthew shoot.PAST.3SG bird.GEN ski.GER(.3SG)
Matthew shot a bird while skiing.
The underlying subject of the gerund can also be the direct object of the finite verb in the
sentence, even though this is rather rare. In such a case, no possessive suffix can be
attached to the gerund:
Mhtte bii lotti girddedettiin.
Matthew shoot.PAST.3SG bird.GEN fly.GER
Matthew shot a bird in flight.
Gerund forms, which have a subject that does not correspond either to the subject or the
object of the finite verb, are quite rare. Even so, such constructions are possible at least if
the subject of the gerund is marked as an experiencer in the main clause, or if the
subject is an indeterminate person or group of people:
Gov'vedettiin mus doarggistigohte gieat.
take_a_picture.GER 1SG.LOC tremble.INCEPT.PAST.3PL hand.PL.NOM
Taking a picture my hands started trembling on me.
Mnn lei gietkama siste joedettiin ja orodettiin.
child be.PAST.3SG cradle.GEN inside.LOC travel.GER and stay.GER
The child was kept in the cradle both during travel and when people stayed
in one place.
127
trisyllabic verbs:
j-stem verbs:
128
The underlying subject of a negative converb has to be a conscious being, and usually
(though not necessarily) the subject is a true agent who is conceived as consciously
leaving something undone. Therefore, the first of the following sentences is grammatical
but the second is not:
Lssa lea cuvkekeahtt.
glass be.3SG break(TR).NEG_CONV
The glass / the window has not been broken.
(but someone is supposed to or is going to break it)
*Lssa lea cuovkankeahtt.
glass be.3SG break(INTR).NEG_CONV
(The glass / the window has not broken.)
129
trisyllabic verbs:
j-stem verbs:
The instrumental converb is a rather late development in the North Saami verb system. It
is originally a comitative case form of a deverbal noun: e.g., bivdimiin hunt.INSTR_CONV
by hunting is at least historically analyzable as bivdim-iin (hunting-COM), which is a
regular comitative form of the noun bivdin hunting. However, morphosyntactic criteria
warrant the analysis of these forms as independent converbs: the instrumental converb
takes complements typical of verbs rather than nouns (e.g., adverbs and direct objects).
Rievssahiid bivdimiin ill gal eall.
ptarmigan.PL.GEN hunt.INSTR_CONV hardly really live.3SG
One hardly makes a living (lives) by hunting ptarmigans.
Geahalemiin dat ielg.
try.INSTR_CONV it become_clear.3SG
Trying will make it clear. (= one will see how it goes when one tries)
Vialit stueremiin beasat guhks eallimis.
eagerly study.INSTR_CONV get_somewhere.2SG far life.LOC
You will accomplish a lot (you will get far) in life by studying eagerly.
Due oskumiin Kristusii gul add vnhurskkisin.
only believe.INSTR_CONV Christ.ILL it_is_said become.3SG righteous.ESS
They say that only by believing in Christ one will become righteous.
gradating verbs:
trisyllabic verbs:
j-stem verbs:
The purposive converb has only very recently developed from a postpostional
construction consisting of the genitive form of a verbal noun and the postposition dihte ~
dihtii because of. Nevertheless, it must be analyzed as an independent non-finite verb
form for several reasons:
1) The purposive converb is no longer formally identical to a normal postpositional
construction: compare bargandihte [work.PURP_CONV] vs. bargama dihte
[working.GEN because_of].
2) There is a semantic difference between purposive converbs and true postpositional
constructions with a verbal noun as a complement: bargandihte in order to work vs.
bargama dihte because of working.
3) The purposive converbs take arguments typical of verbs, such as direct objects.
Examples of the use of the purposive converb:
Mhtte vulggii gvpogii ohcandihte barggu.
Matthew leave.PAST.3SG city.ILL seek.PURP_CONV work.GEN
Matthew went to the city in order to look for a job.
Mhtte dajai dan due bilkidandihte mu.
Matthew say.PAST.3SG it.GEN only mock.PURP_CONV 1SG.GEN
Matthew said that only to mock me.
Due to its postpositional background, the purposive converb allows conjunction
reduction (i.e., morphological marking is present only on the latter member of a
conjoined phrase):
iehkdin uvssa duohkai oaidnin- ja gullandihte maid Mhtte igu dahkat.
hide.PAST.1SG door.GEN behind.ILL see-n and hear.PURP_CONV what Matthew
intend_to.3SG do.INF
I hid behind the door in order to see and hear what Matthew was going to do.
131
trisyllabic verbs:
j-stem verbs:
Like the purposive converb, the converb in -nlhkai has only very recently developed
from a postpostional construction; its original components are the genitive form of a
verbal noun and the postposition lhkai in the manner of. Nevertheless, it must be
analyzed as an independent non-finite verb form for the same reasons as the purposive
converb: both in terms of form and function it has become independent of the
postpositional constructions.
Some usage examples:
Virgi lea ohcanlhkai.
post be.3SG seek.nlhkai
The post/position is open. (= in the state that it can be applied to [sought])
Mhtes lea vuolla beavddi alde juhkanlhkai.
Matthew.LOC be.3SG beer table.GEN on.LOC drink.nlhkai.
Matthew has a beer to drink on the table.
Mhtte iccai lotti ja rohttii hvlra bhinlhkai.
Matthew notice.PAST.3SG bird.GEN and pull_quickly.PAST.3SG shotgun.GEN
shoot.nlhkai
Matthew noticed a bird and quickly drew out the shotgun. (= so that it was ready
to shoot with)
132
5.5.4. Participles
5.5.4.1. The present participle
The present participle is marked by a zero suffix (-) on gradating verbs and by a suffix
-eaddji on trisyllabic verbs and j-stem verbs. To form the present participle, apply the
following rules:
gradating verbs:
trisyllabic verbs:
j-stem verbs:
a sleeping dog
waxing moon
a live fish
sick people
dying people
When used as a modifier, the present participle is indeclinable: just like attribute forms of
adjectives (see 4.4.2.), it shows no agreement with the noun.
133
Numerous present participle forms have been lexicalized as nouns, for example:
bargat to work
dutkat to examine, to investigate
buohcat to be ill
jorrat to roll, to spin, to go around
girdit to fly
llit to write
vuodjit to drive
eallit to live
njoammut to crawl
oahpahit to teach
doaimmahit to edit; to deliver
bargi employee
dutki researcher
buohcci patient
jor'ri cart, carriage, stroller
girdi airplane; pilot
l'li secretary
vuoddji driver
eal'li animal
njoam'mu reptile
oahpaheaddji teacher
doaimmaheaddji journalist; editor
These nouns can be declined for case, number and possession just like other nouns.
However, they show a few morphophonological peculiarities: they lack consonant
gradation and in the Eastern Inland dialects also diphthong simplification. See 4.2.1. for
an example paradigm of vuoddji driver.
an educated person
a drunk person
a dead person
a burned house
a broken glass/window
Note that also passive verbs (see 5.6.1.1. below) can be put in a passive past participle
form:
juhkkov'von gffe [drink.PASS.PAST_PTCL coffee]
coffee that has been drunk
boldojuv'von dllu [burn(TR.).PASS.PAST_PTCL house]
a burned house (= a house that someone has burned on purpose)
cuvkejuv'von lssa [break(TR.).PASS.PAST_PTCL glass]
a broken glass/window (= that someone has broken on purpose)
134
The past participle can also occue as a complement of modal verbs, as well as verbs
signifying knowledge, belief or verbal expression:
Mhtte soait ~ did vuolgn.
Matthew perhaps_be_the_case.3SG ~ probably_be_the_case.3SG leave.PAST_PTCL
Matthew may have left. ~ Presumably Matthew has left.
Son dieht ~ jhkk ~ gdd Mhte vuolgn.
3SG know.3SG ~ believe.3SG assume.3SG Matthew.GEN leave.PAST_PTCL
(S)he knows ~ believes ~ thinks Matthew has left.
Son dajai ~ muitalii Mhte vuolgn.
3SG say.PAST.3SG ~ tell.PAST.3SG Matthew.GEN leave.PAST_PTCL
(S)he said ~ told that Matthew has left.
The past participle is also used as component of several periphrastic constructions: the
indicative perfect and pluperfect tenses (see 5.4.1.3.), the conditional past tense (see
5.4.3.), and the dubitative past tense (see 5.4.4.).
trisyllabic verbs:
j-stem verbs:
The past agentive participle can be formed of transitive verbs and used as a modifier of
nouns which are the underlying object of the past agentive participle; the underlying
subject of the participle is expressed by a genitive complement:
hi oastin biila
father.GEN buy.PAST_AG_PTCL car
the car bought by father
135
trisyllabic verbs:
j-stem verbs:
136
an unread book
unmilked cows
an unsewed Saami costume
untold rumors/news
unsawed logs
passive construction. The subject is often (but not always) topicalized by placing it first
in the sentence.
Intransitive sentence
passive sentence
Nu dalle ellojuv'vui.
so then live.PASS.PAST.3SG
One lived like that back then.
(no subject)
Transitive sentence
passive sentence
Girji l'lojuv'vui.
book write.PASS.PAST.3SG
A/The book was written.
Note that because the function of the Saami passive is to conceal the subject of the
corresponding active sentence, it is not possible to express the agent in a Saami passive
construction. While English passive constructions, for instance, can have an overt agent
(e.g. The book was written by Matthew), such constructions have no equivalents in
Saami.
There is also another crucial difference between Saami and English passives: even though
the agent is concealed and thus never expressed in a Saami passive contruction, the
passive always presupposes that there is an underlying conscious agent. The passive also
tends to imply that the act was consciously committed (as opposed to being accidental).
This can be illustrated with the following example:
Mhtte cuvkii lsa.
Matthew break.PAST.3SG window.GEN
Matthew broke the window.
Lssa cuvkejuv'vui.
window break.PASS.PAST.3SG
The window was broken.
(i.e., someone broke it on purpose)
The following passive construction seems strange because the act is not purposeful:
Mhtte cuvkii lsa vahgis.
Matthew break.PAST.3SG window.GEN accident.LOC
Matthew accidentally broke the window.
138
In the following case, no corresponding passive construction exists, because the subject
of the active sentence is not a true agent:
Biegga cuvkii lsa.
wind break.PAST.3SG window.GEN
The wind broke the window.
Formation of passive verbs. In Saami, passive verbs are independent derived verbs
that have conjugations of their own. Passive verbs are formed are formed from active
verbs according to the following rules:
gradating verbs:
trisyllabic verbs:
j-stem verbs:
Examples:
llit
atnat
lohkat
dadjat
vldit
oaidnit
uohppat
gvdnot
jearrat
muitalit
rhkadit
loktet
hukset
lebbet
to write
to tie
to read
to say
to take
to see
to cut
to find
to ask
to tell
to make
to lift
to build
to spread
> l'lojuv'vot
> adnojuv'vot
> lohkkojuv'vot
> daddjojuv'vot
> vldojuv'vot
> oidnojuv'vot
> uhppojuv'vot
> gvdnojuv'vot
> jer'rojuv'vot
> muitaluv'vot
> rhkaduv'vot
> loktejuv'vot
> huksejuv'vot
> lebbejuv'vot
to be written
to be tied
to be read
to be said
to be taken
to be seen
to be cut
to be found
to be asked
to be told
to be made
to be lifted
to be built
to be spread
The passive verbs of most gradating verbs also have a shorter variant, where the passive
suffix is merely -o-. Examples:
llit
atnat
lohkat
dadjat
vldit
jearrat
to write
to tie
to read
to say
to take
to ask
> l'lot
> adnot
> lohkkot
> daddjot
> vldot
> jer'rot
to be written
to be tied
to be read
to be said
to be taken
to be asked
139
However, some verbs formed with the shorter suffix -o- have a special automative
meaning these express that the action denoted by the root verb occurs automatically
and there is no true agent:
oaidnit
gullat
haksit
gvdnat
doadjit
geassit
oaggit
to see
to hear
to smell (trans.)
to find
to break in two
to pull, to drag
to gather, collect
> oidnot
> gul'lot
> haksot
> gvdnot
> doddjot
> ges'sot
> oggot
Many automative verbs do not translate very well to English; the following usage
examples illustrate the automative semantics involved in these words:
Duoppe oidnojit muhtun olbmot.
there see.AUTOM.3PL some person.PL.NOM
There are some people over there
(some people are visible over there)
Olgun gul'lui biilla jietna.
outside.LOC hear.AUTOM.PAST.3SG person.PL.GEN sound
The sound of a car could be heard from outside [the house]
(a cars sound was audible from outside)
Meahcis gvdnui boares goahtesadji.
wilderness.LOC find.AUTOM.PAST.3SG old.ATTR tent.place
An old dwelling site was found in the forest
(but no one was looking for it)
Mus giehta doddjui.
1SG.LOC hand break.AUTOM.PAST.3SG
I broke my arm
(at me an arm got broken)
The contrast between a passive and an automative verb can be illustarted with the
following examples:
Mhtte oidnui gvppis.
Matthew see.AUTOM.PAST.3SG store.LOC
Matthew was seen in the store (just by coincidence)
Mhtte oidnojuv'vui gvppis.
Matthew see.PASS.PAST.3SG store.LOC
Matthew was seen in the store (and he had been looked for)
140
As noted above, passive verbs can be formed out of intransitive verbs as well. In such a
case, the resulting passive sentence does not have a subject; the passive verb appears in
the 3SG form. Such passive sentences are often difficult to translate into English:
141
Dohko ii vulgojuv'vo.
there.ILL NEG.3SG leave.PASS.3SG
Nobodys going there.
(It is not gone there)
Examples:
borrat to eat
llit to write
ierrut to cry
uohppat to cut X
goarrut to sew
rhkadit to make X
hukset to build X
142
143
144
6. ADPOSITIONS
6.1. Basic features
There is a large number of adpositions (pre- and postpositions) in North Saami.
Postpositions are more common than prepositions; there are also some ambipositions
(i.e., adpositions that can be used as both pre- and postpositions). The complements of all
adpositions are always in the genitive case. For example:
postposition:
presposition:
ambiposition:
PRE
, and
145
LOC
ILL
PROL
upper surface
above
next to
side
around (= in a circle)
around (= here and there)
opposite to
behind (= the back side)
between
middle
someones home
ala onto
bajbeallai
bldii
beallai
=
=
=
duohkai
gaskii
=
geahai
duoge ~ duogi
gaskka
146
middle
beside
near
immediate vicinity
behind (= the direction
one is coming from)
all over
along with
front
guovdu AMBI
guoras
lahka AMBI
luhtte
mais
=
gurrii
=
lusa
mai
guora
=
=
mail
mieht PRE
mielde
ovddas
=
=
ovdii
inside
under
against
siste
vuolde
vuost
sisa
vuollai
=
=
=
ovddal towards (from
opposite direction)
vuole
=
In addition to the ones listed in the table above, there are four functionally PROL
postpositions that are not members of adpositional sets: bokte ~ bakte via, aa AMBI
through, meatt past and rast AMBI across.
Some further usage examples:
Girji lea beavddi alde.
book be.3sg table.gen on.loc
The book is on the table.
Vlden girjji beavddi alde.
take.PAST.1SG book.GEN table.GEN on.LOC
I took the book off the table.
Bija girjji beavddi ala.
put.IMP.2SG book.GEN table.GEN on.ILL
Put the book on the table.
Dllu lea meara bealde.
house be.3SG sea.GEN side.LOC
The house is on the seaward side.
Mhtte huksii dlu vri beallai.
Matthew build.PAST.3SG house.GEN mountain.GEN side.ILL
Matthew built a house on the side towards the mountain.
(= as opposed to some other side, e.g. the seaward side)
Mhtte boii vri beale.
Matthew come.PAST.3SG mountain.GEN side.PROL
Matthew came by the way along the mountain side.
147
148
NEG.2SG
leave.CONNEG
149
150
151
e) The sentence expresses what is considered normal or usual for people to do; in such
sentences the verb takes the 3PL form:
Suomas juhket ollu mielkki.
Finland.LOC drink.3PL much milk.GEN
In Finland people drink a lot of milk.
akat bidjet bohccuid idi.
in_fall put.3PL reindeer.PL.GEN corral.ILL
In fall the reindeer are gathered (put) in a corral.
152
question concerns; the phrase must occur as the first member in the sentence (excluding
preceding discourse particles).
Boahtgo Mhtte maid?
come.3SG.go Matthew also
Is Matthew coming as well?
Leago Mhtte dppe?
be.3SG.go Matthew here.LOC
Is Matthew here?
Dppego Mhtte lea?
here.LOC.go Matthew be.3SG
Is Matthew here? (and not somewhere else)
Mhttego dppe lea?
Matthew.go here.LOC be.3SG
Is it Matthew who is here? (and not someone else)
Note that an interrogative clitic can only be attached to the head of a phrase. Hence, the
first of the following sentences is grammatical, the second is not:
Rukses dlusgo Mhtte orru?
red.ATTR house.LOC.go Matthew live.3SG
Is it a red house that Matthew lives in?
*Ruksesgo dlus Mhtte orru?
red.ATTR.go house.LOC Matthew live.3SG
Is it a red house that Matthew lives in?
When attached to the adverbs olu ~ ollu much and guhk for a long time, the clitic -go
forms questions of degree:
Olugo mkset biillas?
much.go pay.PAST.2SG car.LOC
How much did you pay for the car?
Guhkgo oet?
for_a_long_time.go sleep.PAST.2SG
How long did you sleep?
153
Another clitic -son may be attached after -go. The clitic -son indicates that either the
question is rhetorical (i.e. that the speaker is not really expecting an answer for it), or that
the speaker expects a negative answer.
Boahtgoson Mhtte?
come.3SG.go.son Matthew
I wonder if Matthew is coming.
There is also another interrogative clitic -bat. Compared to the neutral interrogative clitic
-go, the clitic -bat adds an impression of surprise to the question:
Boahtbat Mhtte?
come.3SG.bat Matthew
Is Matthew coming? (= Im surprised that he might be coming)
Inbat oao oppa aviissa rfis lohkat?
NEG.3SG.bat be_allowed.CONNEG even newspaper.GEN peace.LOC read.INF
Cant I even read the newspaper in peace?
The clitic -bat can even be added to an interrogative word to express the speakers
surprise:
Gosbat Mhtte lea?
where.loc.bat Matthew be.3SG
Wheres Matthew? (= Im surprised he isnt here)
Note that the rhetorical question clitic -son cannot be attached after the interrogative clitic
-bat.
154