Locke Social
Locke Social
Locke Social
Locke, Abigail
The Social Psychologising of Emotion and Gender: A Critical Perspective
Original Citation
Locke, Abigail (2011) The Social Psychologising of Emotion and Gender: A Critical Perspective.
In: Sexed Sentiments. Interdisciplinary Perspectives on Gender and Emotion. Rodopi, Amsterdam,
The Netherlands, pp. 185-205. ISBN 9789042032415
This version is available at http://eprints.hud.ac.uk/7834/
The University Repository is a digital collection of the research output of the
University, available on Open Access. Copyright and Moral Rights for the items
on this site are retained by the individual author and/or other copyright owners.
Users may access full items free of charge; copies of full text items generally
can be reproduced, displayed or performed and given to third parties in any
format or medium for personal research or study, educational or not-for-profit
purposes without prior permission or charge, provided:
The authors, title and full bibliographic details is credited in any copy;
A hyperlink and/or URL is included for the original metadata page; and
The content is not changed in any way.
For more information, including our policy and submission procedure, please
contact the Repository Team at: [email protected].
http://eprints.hud.ac.uk/
Abigail Locke
Abstract
Abigail Locke
universal across time and place, and due to innate human physiology.
Although there is still some disagreement as to the number of basic
emotions and the labels given to them, this view within psychology is
immensely popular with many emotion theorists endorsing it (e.g.
Arnold 1960; Frijda 1994; James 1884; Lazarus 1994).
A challenge comes to this side of psychology from social
psychologists who endorse a social-constructionist perspective on
emotion. Social-constructionist approaches to emotion claim that
emotions have a socio-cultural backdrop, and are not simply matters
of biology. According to Vivien Burr (1995, 2003) in her
comprehensive text on the subject, social constructionism holds that
social processes sustain knowledge and that knowledge and action go
together. Thus, in terms of relationships between sex, gender and
emotion, social constructionists consider how emotion terms are
considered within a society, in particular within their assumed
gendered usage. As a theoretical stance within psychology, social
constructionism has presented a challenge to the essentialism so
prevalent within the discipline of psychology, and offered a view that
challenges realist assumptions and considers historical and cultural
specificity. Within social psychology, different methods have
represented themselves as having a social-constructionist backdrop,
including critical psychology, Foucauldian discourse analysis, and
discursive psychology. We will consider examples from discursive
psychology in the field of emotion studies further on in the chapter.
Social constructionist approaches to emotion gained momentum
when issues around cultural and historical differences in emotion and
etymology were taken into consideration. The essentialist idea of a
basic set of emotions was problematized by cross-cultural studies
(Heelas 1996). Anthropologists such as Michelle Rosaldo and
Catherine Lutz found that in certain cultures names for emotions
existed that were not common to Western society. Lutzs work with
the Ifaluk in the Southwest Pacific found that this culture had a
specific term for justified anger song, that was not present in our
society and argued that claims to feel an emotion are bound up with
cultural, moral and political considerations rather than inner, discrete
feelings (Lutz 1988). Similarly, Rosaldos work with the Ilongot, a
Abigail Locke
Abigail Locke
in more recent work, Hall, Carter and Horgan (2000) note that nonverbal behavior does not necessarily signify emotion (97), that is the
experience of emotion may indeed differ from the expression of
emotion. Other studies have reported that women appear more
prepared to talk about and express emotions than men (Fischer 1993).
Huston-Comeaux and Kelly (2002) found a link between the
appropriateness of emotional expression and sex and argue that this
stereotyping leads to a fairly narrow range of possible emotional
expressions for women (7). Similarly, Brody (2000) found that
display rules of emotion generally conform to gender stereotypes, and
that these stereotypes are more robust in interpersonal settings. Simon
and Nath (2004) found that in American culture, the sexes differed in
their reporting of the frequency of positive and negative emotions.
However, they found a strong link between social position and
emotional expression, with those in lower social positions, often
women, reporting more negative affect. This demonstrates that the
relationship between emotion and sex is not a psychological one, but
rather a societal and cultural construct, with factors like class and
ethnicity intersecting with gender. Thus, Fischer (1993) claims that
emotionality should not be considered one of the basic dimensions to
distinguish the sexes, and that the claim that women are more
emotional than men tells us more about our cultural stereotypes than
about actual sex differences in emotions (Fischer 1993, 312).
Psychology has typically offered explanations of phenomena in
biological and cognitive terms. This, as some feminists have argued, is
due to its unacknowledged patriarchal foundations (see Burr, 1998 for
further discussion on this). As Cameron declares [d]ifferences in
mens and womens verbal behaviours are [...] explained in biological
terms (2007, 8). As recent studies on sex differences and emotion in
psychology also demonstrate (e.g. Glenberg, et al. 2009), the
discipline attributes verbal behaviours such as discourse and other
affective displays to biological factors rather than cultural display
norms. In psychology, the outer, discursive and material world
becomes theorised as an inner, emotional essence. However, as
Catherine Lutz (1990) notes from an anthropological stance, emotion
is cultural, constructed by people and not nature (40).
Abigail Locke
10 Abigail Locke
Tolson (2005). When examining emotional expressiveness and gender
differences amongst senior executives, they found that female
executives reported themselves as less emotionally expressive than
male executives. The authors claim that their results are surprising, as
femininity and emotional expressiveness are becoming regarded as
important in the business world. Interestingly, male executives may
report higher levels of expressiveness due to a changing culture
which is just beginning to accept feminine traits such as
expressiveness (521). However, what is also of interest is that the
female executives were not willing to claim to be emotionally
expressive. This example demonstrates not only that there are
contradictions within the discourse of gender, emotionality and
leadership in the workplace, but also that individuals are able to
appropriate these social constructions strategically in their everyday
lives by profiling themselves as adhering to, or deviating from, them.
This interactional nature of emotion discourse has been studied
discursively in psychology (e.g. Buttny 1993; Edwards 1997, 1999;
Locke 2001, 2003; Locke and Edwards 2003). Rather than studying
the actual role or existence of emotional states, emotions are
approached as social and discursive phenomena (Edwards 1999;
Parrot and Harr 1996), produced as part of a narrative framework and
utilised for accounting purposes. Accounting in this sense refers to the
ways in which we use language to justify ourselves or blame others.
Research in this field has demonstrated how emotion discourse and
concepts can be used rhetorically to construct versions of character
and to signify to others how events are problematic or out of the
ordinary (Buttny 1993). It has been proposed that emotion talk or
discourse is an important part of how social accountability is produced
(e.g. Lutz 1988, 1990) and forms an integral part of the accounting
process. It can be used to make sense of peoples actions (Sarbin
1989), or to imply that circumstances are problematic or out of the
ordinary (Buttny 1993) or in contrast to rational thought (Edwards
1999). The literature on emotion discourse within psychology is still
rather limited and has been related to interpersonal areas such as
relationship and couple counselling (Edwards 1999) and legal
discourse (Locke and Edwards 2003). However, it borrows heavily
Clinton testimony
Q: Isnt that correct that you and Mrs Currie were very irate
about that
[4 second pause]
12 Abigail Locke
C: Well I dont remember all that uh what I remember is that
she was very um Monica was very upset, she got upset from
time to time,
[10 lines omitted]
C: And I was upset about her conduct. Im not sure I knew or
focused on at that moment exactly the question you ask. I
remember I was- I thought her conduct was inappropriate
that day.
The prosecutor (Q) invokes both Clintons personal secretary (Mrs
Currie) along with Clinton, as being emphatically, very irate with
Lewinskys actions. In his response, Clinton avoids description of his
own emotions and shifts the attribution to Lewinsky: What he can
recall is how upset she was. Not only was Lewinsky memorably
upset on that occasion, rather we are immediately informed, that she
got upset from time to time. This represents Lewinsky as getting
upset not just on the one occasion in question, but repeatedly. It is
implied that she was perhaps prone to getting upset, such that any
pursuit of the reasons for her getting upset, on any occasion, might
look to reasons within her, and not only to external causes such as
what (in this case) Clinton might have done or said to provoke her.
Thus there is an important rhetorical move here on Clintons part,
deflecting inquiry away from the proximal causes of Lewinskys
emotions (i.e. potentially his actions), and towards her dispositional
tendencies of high emotionality. Rather than being prone to getting
upset, Clinton emerges as understandably reactive to specific
circumstances, which in this case were Lewinskys unreasonable
demands and reactions. What the analysis here demonstrates is how
the rhetorical tropes of reactive versus dispositional emotion work
within our everyday discourse to construct characters and versions of
events. From this extract we have evidence that being ascribed a high
level of emotionality can lead to being situated within a discourse of
vulnerability: that is, it serves to make the social actor (in this
example, female) weaker, and represents them as acting out their
passions, rather than taking rational actions.
KAREN:
JILL:
KAREN:
JILL:
INTERVIEWER:
JILL:
14 Abigail Locke
KAREN:
16 Abigail Locke
References
Arnold, Magda B. 1960. Emotion and personality (2 volumes).
New York: Columbia.
Brebner, John. 2003. Gender and emotions. Personality and
individual differences 34: 387-394.
Brody, Leslie. R. 2000. The socialization of gender differences
in emotional expression: Display rules, infant temperament,
and differentiation. In Gender and emotion: Social
psychological perspectives, ed. Agneta H. Fischer, 24-47.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Brown, Steve. D. and Paul Stenner. 2009. Psychology without
foundations: History, philosophy and psychosocial theory:
Constructionism, mediation and critical psychology. London:
Sage.
Burr, Vivien. 1995. An introduction to social constructionism.
London: Routledge.
Burr, Vivien. 1998. Gender and social psychology. London:
Routledge.
Burr, Vivien. 2003. Social constructionism. Second Edition.
London: Routledge.
Butler, Judith. 1990. Gender trouble: Feminism and the
subversion of identity. London: Routledge.
Buttny, Richard. 1993. Social accountability in communication.
London: Sage.
Cameron, Deborah. 2007. The myth of Mars and Venus: Do men
and women really speak different languages? Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Callahan, Jamie L., Michael G. Hasler and Homer Tolson.
Perceptions of emotion expressiveness: Gender differences
among senior executives. Leadership and Organisation
Development Journal 26: 512-28.
Clore, Gerald L., Allan Ortony and Mark A. Foss. 1987. The
psychological foundations of the affective lexicon. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology 53: 751-66.
Darwin, Charles. 1871. The expression of emotions in animals
and man. London: John Murray.
Davidson, Richard J. 2000. Cognitive neuroscience needs
affective neuroscience (and vice versa). Brain & cognition
42: 89-92.
Denzin, N. 1984. On understanding emotion. London: JosseyBass.
Edwards, Derek. 1997. Discourse and cognition. London: Sage.
Edwards, Derek. 1999. Emotion discourse. Culture &
psychology 5: 271-291.
Eisenberg, Nancy, Amanda Cumberland and Tracy L. Spinrad.
1998. Parental socialization of emotion. Psychological
inquiry 9: 241273.
Ekman, Paul. 1992. Are there basic emotions? Psychological
review 99: 550-3.
Fischer, Agneta H. 1993. Sex differences in emotionality: Fact
or Stereotype? Feminism and psychology 3: 303-318.
London: Sage.
Fischer, Agneta H., ed. 2000. Gender and emotion: Social
psychological perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Fivush, Robyn and Janine P. Buckner. 2000. Gender, sadness
and depression: The development of emotional focus through
gendered discourse. In Gender and emotion: Social
psychological perspectives, ed. Agneta H. Fischer, 232-53.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Frankenhaeuser, M., E. Dunne and U. Lundberg. 1976. Sex
differences in sympathetic adrenal medullary reactions
induced by different stressors. Psychopharmacology 47: 1-5.
18 Abigail Locke
20 Abigail Locke
22 Abigail Locke