Diversity and Commonality in Music Performance - An Analysis of Timing Microstructure in Schumann's UTraumerei
Diversity and Commonality in Music Performance - An Analysis of Timing Microstructure in Schumann's UTraumerei
Diversity and Commonality in Music Performance - An Analysis of Timing Microstructure in Schumann's UTraumerei
This study attempts to characterize the temporal commonalities and differences among
distinguished pianists' interpretations of a well-known piece, Robert Schumann's
"Triiumerei." Intertone onset intervals (lOIs) were measured in 28 recorded performances.
These data were subjected to a variety of statistical analyses, including principal
components analysis of longer stretches of music and curve fitting to series of lOIs within
brief melodic gestures. Global timing patterns reflected the hierarchical grouping
structure of the composition, with pronounced ritardandi at the ends of major sections and
frequent expressive lengthening of accented tones within melodic gestures. Analysis of
local timing patterns, particularly of within-gesture ritardandi, revealed that they often
followed a parabolic timing function. The major variation in these patterns can be
modelled by families of parabolas with a single degree of freedom. The grouping structure,
which prescribes the location of major tempo changes, and the parabolic timing function,
which represents a natural manner of executing such changes, seem to be the two major
constraints under which pianists are operating. Within these constraints, there is room for
much individual variation, and there are always exceptions to the rules. The striking
individuality of two legendary pianists, Alfred Cortot and Vladimir Horowitz, is objectively
demonstrated here, as is the relative eccentricity of several other artists.
INTRODUCTION
A. Diversity and commonality in music
performance
More than at any earlier period in musical
history, the contemporary scene in serious music
is dominated by the performer (see Lipman, 1990).
Music consumers thrive on a limited repertoire of
standard masterworks, primarily from the 19th
century, that are offered again and again in
different performances, both in live concerts and
on recordings. The great musical events of our
time are not the premieres of new compositions,
but the appearances and reappearances of
superstar conductors and instrumentalists.
This research was made possible by the generosity of
Haskins Laboratories (Carol A. Fowler, President). Additional
support came from NIH (BRSG) grant RR05596 to the
Laboratories. I am grateful to the Record Library, School of
Music, Yale University, for providing most of the recordings
analyzed here, and to its manager, Karl Schrom, for his help.
Thanks also to Pat Shove for many stimulating discussions.
227
228
Repp
OpInIOnS about the quality of particular performances by these and other artists, describing
them as "brilliant" or "noble" or "thoughtful."
Some of the more gifted professional critics excel
in characterizing different performances in terms
alternatingly scholarly and poetic.
While the attention of listeners and critics thus
is mainly drawn to the differences among performances, there are also strong commonalities, usually taken for granted and hence unnoticed.
Though there is a large variety of acceptable performances of a given piece of music, there is an
even larger variety of unacceptable performances,
which rarely make their way into the concert halls
or onto records. Unless they have the mark of inspired iconoclasm (as do some of the performances
by the late Glenn Gould; see, e.g., Lipman, 1984),
they quickly succumb to the fierce competition of
the musical marketplace. Music teachers, however, have to deal with them every day and try
their best to mold immature and wayward students into performers that can be listened to with
pleasure. Though teachers may differ considerably
in their methods and goals, and are rarely very
explicit about what these are, they are transmitting the unwritten rules (though see Lussy, 1882)
of a performance tradition that goes back to 19th
century central Europe, where most of the
standard repertoire originated. Despite various
changes in performance practices during the last
200 years, most of them of a narrowly technical
nature, there are generally accepted norms of musical performance, according to which the artist's
actions are largely subordinated to the musical
structure. The artist's primary task is the expression of the musical structure, so it can be grasped
and appreciated by the listener, and make an impression on him or her. (See Lussy, 1882;
Riemann, 1884; Stein, 1962.) This is presumably
done by conventional means that are adapted to
the hearers' perceptual and cognitive abilities.
However, the particular doses in which these
techniques are applied (unconsciously, for the
most part) vary from artist to artist and account
for individual differences in interpretation.
Thus there are two basic aspects of music performance: a normative aspect (i.e., commonality)
that represents what is expected from a competent
performer and is largely shared by different
artists, and an individual aspect (i.e., diversity)
that differentiates performers. The individual aspect may be conceived of as deviations from a single ideal norm; more profitably, however, it may
be thought of as individual settings of free parameters in the definition of the normative behavior.
In
Schumann's "Triiumcrc;P
229
230
Repp
Oiversitll and Commonalitll in lvluS1C Performance: An Anaillsis of Timing Microstructure in ScJlllmann '5 "Tniumerel"
----
;::C
tJ
:'-1
[t
~I
Fr'
Jl'
Ll
..
('jJ
.,
,-
<>
~.
.,"
)_.
I
~I
rn
r--..l-r
l.--'
...... ,
~I
-=1
1 I [
(espr.)
rTil
~.1:
c:
"'<
"it., I
, '"
liLLO/
b.. _.~.
m~
I.
i.
- #
----.--:
....::
~1
~~
",.
n-n
~d
~
(esp'.)
---fiYL
JJ IJll
~h~~~:
(J.'
I......!.-I
[ill
231
I.
~~l r
'L1....W
p.
L...J
",:~-,...
rr
I'$.
l;_
23
r.
~= I
1'=
I I
nl
f.'\
io o .
,":=!.
"
I
~a
Figure 1. The piano score of "Traumerei," created with MusicProse software following the Clara Schumann (Breitkopf
& Harlel) edition (with some deviations in minor details due to software limitations). The layout of the score on the
page is intended to highlight the structure of the music.
232
Repp
p s
p s
Melodic Gestures:
MG'
O[] MG2
D
[l]]] lJI]
Period
Phrase
Type
a,
MG2i
MG6 f
~~ MG3a
tzJ [23 MG4a
MGSa
~
MG6a
2
sa
p s
~~ MG3b
~!6d MG4b
~~ MGSb
[ill
o
S
A
T
B
MG6b
4
S
A
T
B
o
5
b,
T
B
S
A
T
B
S
A
T
B
20
a,
AI
S
A
T
B
S
A
T
B
Figure 2. Schematic representation of the melodiclrhythmic structure of "Traumerei." (See text for explanation.)
in
Schumann's "Triiumerei"
233
Artist
Recording
ARG
ARR
ASH
BRE
BUN
CAP
Cal
C02
C03
CUR
DAV
OEM
ESC
GIA
Hal
H02
H03
KAT
KLI
KRU
KUB
Mal
NEY
NOV
aRT
SCH
SHE
ZAK
Repp
234
e. Measurement error
The measurement procedure would have been
prohibitively time-consuming if maximum accuracy had been aimed for (e.g., by displaying
shorter waveform segments on the computer terminal screen). A certain amount of speed-accuracy
tradeoff had to be taken into account. The magnitude of the resulting measurement error can be
estimated from three performances (BRE, C02,
and the first half of CUR) that, for various reasons, had to be remeasured. Two of them (BRE,
CUR) derived from good LPs with a moderate
amount of surface noise, whereas C02 was of
older vintage and had special problems related to
the pianist's tendency to play the left- and righthand parts asynchronously. There were several
very large discrepancies (in excess of 80 ms)
between the two measurements of the C02
performance, due to a conscious change in the
author's criteria for treating asynchronies; these
discrepancies were not considered true
measurement errors and were omitted from the
data presented below, though some smaller
inconsistencies of the same kind may still be
included.
The measurement error distributions are shown
in Table 2. The combined BRE+CUR data
represent average accuracy, whereas the C02
data constitute a worst-case scenario; only DAV
was even more difficult to measure. It can be seen
that over 90% of the measurement errors in the
BRE+CUR data did not exceed 10 ms, which is
less than 2% of the average 101. The largest error
was under 35 ms, or about 6% ofthe.average 101,
and the average measurement ,error was 4.3 mS,
Percent
77.4
13.8
2.7
1.7
2.2
1.7
0.5
Cum.
77.4
91.3
93.9
95.6
97.8
99.5
100.0
C02 (N=245)
Percent
24.5
40.0
18.8
5.7
3.7
3.3
2.0
1.2
0.4
0.4
Cum.
24.5
64.5
83.3
89.0
92.7
95.9
98.0
99.2
99.6
100.0
A. Overall tempo
Perhaps the most obvious dimension along
which different performances vary is that of
overall tempo. The present set of performances is
no exception. Tentative estimates of global tempo
were obtained by computing the first quartiles
(the 25% point) of the individual eighth-note 101
distributions, multiplying these millisecond values
by 2, and dividing them into 60,000. The choice of
the first quartile was motivated by the
consideration that expressive lengthening of lOIs
is both more frequent and more pronounced than
shortening, and also by the fact that it gave tempi
for two pianists, BRE and DAV, that agreed
closely, respectively, with Brendel's (1981)
statement of his preferred tempo and with Clara
Schumann's (DAV's teacher's) recommended
tempo. (Repp, 1993b, will provide a more extended
discussion.) Table 3 shows that the tempo range
extended from 48 to 79 quarter-notes per minute.
Apart from the fact that the three fastest
performances are all old recordings, there does not
seem to be any systematic relationship between
tempo and the time at which the recording was
l1J
Schumann's "TriiumereI"
235
ESC
KAT, NEY
ZAK
CAP
KLI
CUR
BUN, NOY
DEM, KUB
ARR, KRU, MOl, SCH
H02
ARG
ASH
H03
HOI, SHE
C03
BRE,ORT
GIA
COl
C02
DAY
B. Repeats
The first step in the data analysis was an
attempt to eliminate redundancy and reduce
random measurement error by averaging across
repetitions of the same (or highly similar) musical
material. Of course, such averaging is meaningful
only if there are no systematic differences in
timing microstructure across repeats. The prime
target for averaging was the first period, bars 1-8,
which was repeated literally, according to
Schumann's instructions in the score. All but two
pianists (DAV, KRU) observed this repeat. 2
To compare the first and second repeats for all
pianists, a grand average timing pattern was first
obtained by computing the geometric mean of
corresponding lOIs across all 28 performances.
(For DAV and KRU, the data of bars 1-8 were
Repp
236
Table 4. Correlations of the timing profiles for repeated or similar sections (R = repeat; GM = geometric mean).
Bars
Artist
1-8/1R-8R
ARG
ARR
ASH
BRE
BUN
CAP
COl
CO2
0.781
0.933
0.922
0.953
0.510
0.935
0.859
0.865
0.750
0.937
*
0.938
0.890
0.777
0.918
0.811
0.826
0.825
0.804
*
0.951
0.805
0.920**
0.931
0.677
0.676
0.868
0.839
0.987
cm
CUR
DAY
DEM
ESC
GIA
HOI
H02
H03
KAT
KLI
KRU
KUB
MOl
NEY
NOY
ORT
SCH
SHE
ZAK
GM
*no repeat
**bars 1-4 only (see Footnote 2)
1-4117-20
0.861
0.938
0.951
0.888
0.633
0.922
0.717
0.736
0.741
0.911
0.863
0.924
0.734
0.809
0.958
0.861
0.738
0.901
0.893
0.875
0.913
0.855
0.904
0.906
0.609
0.574
0.831
0.875
0.986
9-12/13-16
0.630
0.850
0.927
0.880
0.632
0.846
0.837
0.851
0.571
0.921
0.863
0.828
0.802
0.914
0.697
0.822
0.770
0.867
0.861
0.940
0.888
0.493
0.872
0.908
0.707
0.748
0.859
0.909
0.950
2500
.........
2000
Ul
E
-.J
<t:
In
--
a, (bars 04)
--
a2 (bars 21-24)
>
a:
--
"Triiumerei"
23;""
b 1 (bars 58)
--
b3 (bars 1316)
f-
1000
fW
~ 800
MG5b
MG3a
r-l
MG4b
ITl
a:
MG3b
z 600
f-
~-M~~~M~~~M~~~M~~
6~~~~NNN~~M~~~~~~
BAR/EIGHTH-NOTE
BAR/EIGHTH-NOTE
Figure 3. Grand average lOIs (geometric means across all 28 performances). The two panels show the timing profiles
for phrases of Type a and b, respectively. Primary melodic gestures (d. Figure 2) are indicated by brackets. The bar
numbers on the abscissa refer to bars 0-8 only; for the later phrases, an appropriate constant must be added.
Repp
238
In
Schumann's "Triiumerei"
239
Repp
240
1OOO-rr---r---,--r--:-::!-:--~-.....---r---,
.s
OIJ
CJ)
a:
CJ)
a:
otD
..
~ 400 -hji-,TTM-rrrrrn-TlTl+n-rrrr-.+nlTTTrmTTrrrr+TTrITTT+-rnT1'Trh-rTTTn-I
1000rr--,---,---,----,----,---,r---,...-----,
400,
....
~-M~~-M~~_M~~_M~~-M~~_M~~_M~~_M~_
O~~~~NNNNMMMM.~.~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~=~
BARS/EIGHTH-NOTES
Figure 4. Timing patterns of the first three principal components for bars 0-8. The standardized factor scores were
converted into milliseconds by multiplying them with the average within-performance 101 standard deviation and
adding this product to the grand mean 101. Bar lines and MG brackets are added for orientation.
0.844
0.760
0.738
0.695
0.695
0.692
0.685
0.672
0.664
0.644
0.637
0.615
0.593
0.551
0.482
0.523
0.509
III
IV
0.423
0.438
0.41 I
0.463
0.460
0.429
0.495
0.495
0.888
0.886
0.837
0.770
0.665
0.626
0.595
0.588
0.570
0.549
0.445
0.524
0.491
0.464
0.876
0.875
0.850
0.501
0.582
In
Schumann', "Triiumerez"
241
242
Repp
Bars 4/5
Bars 0/1
1.6
Argo
1.2-t
I
I
N~
C01.
Bun
1~
--
1.6
' ...
.Arr
Oem.
Sene
She
,lre
...
0.8~
0.4
0.6
0.8
Co2
art
1.2
0.8 ,
0.6
-.,..'
Arre
1.2 _ Oeme
Ho3
Co2.
Col
~ Cur
1.2
1.4
1.8
Gia.
Oem
1.2
Arg
Arr
. , . Ash
,
. . ..~u
Gia
1.6
Nov
1.4
I
I
.c ur.
1
I
1.2
1.4
1.6
0.8 ,
0.8
1.8
1.2
1.6
Bu
Gia'"
1.2,-
,,,
,
~a~.
,
,
,.
0.8 .
2.4
0.4
~
;:,:
1Ii
r;
'\:J
-.
1.4
1.2
Hal
....
Bre
Kub
~;:,:
;;,.
,
,
;:,:
;;,.
;:,:
c'
''-""
r;;
Bun'
1.4,- , art
!
~
Arr
'Ho3.
0.6 .
.
0.6 0.8
3
3
fl
I
n
Q
Bre
,
,
1.6 -
ap
0.8
Bun.
'"
;:,:
M.
Arr.
".8
1.2
Ho1
Bars 16/17
1.8
Nov
0.6
Bars 12/13
Bars 8/9
1.6
1.4
Oav.
Co3
0.8
0.8
Cap
Co3
Co1.
Co2
CO
One
Arg.
1.4 -
Arg.
1.2
--
Nov
.Bun
1.4.-
Ash
Nov.
Bars 20/21
1.6
Oem
1.4~
...,.,
:j
;:;
Ash
$:
r;
~::!
r;
;;;
;:,:
(F>
0.8
1.2
1.6
2.4
:::
S
;:,:
;:,:
A/(B+C)
'I":
-j
...
~;
Figure 5. Timing patterns of the six instances of MG1 and the following chord. (See text for explanation of ratios.) Ratios of 1 represent equal underlying
beats (Le., equal tempo). The upper panels plot the three instances where the upbeat.is nominally a quarter-note, while the lower panels plot the three
instances where the upbeat is nominally an eighth-note (a grace note in bar 16). The dashed lines indicate a doubling or halving of the nominal duration in
execution.
:::
;;;
~.
f-.j
oj;"
\.n
244
Repp
1400
1200
Bars 1/2
Bars 5/6
Bars 9/10
'"
Bars 13/14
1000
Bars 17/18
---- 800
Bars 21/22
---.
(J)
600
400
1-6
1-7
1-8
21
2-2
BAR/EIGHTH-NOTE
l'vll1SIC
111
5chumaml's "Triillmere:"
1200 - , - - - - , - - - - - , - - . , . - - - - , - - - . m
1200 ..,......--r---,--,---r-.,-,
1000
1000
800 - 4 \ - \ - - t - - - - t - - . ; - - - I t - - r l ' - i
1-6
1-7
1-8
2-1
2-2
245
800 -t---4---1---+-_++--"'''''''''
--.
1000
1200-t---+---f---+---ff---;
(f)
.s
800-t-~-+---+----l-,4~+---i
400-!--~~f=:::::=-+--+-_I 400-!---+-~-T~-+--f----i
1-6
1-7
1-8
2-1
2-2
1-6 1-7 1-8 2-1
2-2
1400 -.----,--,---,-----r----, 1600..,......--,----r----,,.---.,---.....,
1400
1200
1000 -+---+----;---++-~
____1
1200 - i - - - + - - - + - - - i f - - - + - , f - - - i
1000 -t---t---+-+-I-f--,.<p>o"6-"71
800 -+--'rl-----;---f-+h~_,f_-__1
1-6
1-7
1-8
2-1
2-2
400 +--+--=+=:::::l!!::::::PL-~-l
1-6 1-7
1-8
2-1
2-2
BAR/EIGHTH-NOTE
Figure 7. MG2 timing data for six individual artists, with best-fitting quadratic functions. Where the pattern deviated
markedly from a parabolic shape, the data points are connected with straight lines. See Figure 6 for the legend of
symbols.
246
Repp
1400
1200
..--...
(j)
1000
---
Q = 20
Q= 40
Q = 60
A.
Q= 80
Q = 100
Q = 120
\I
800
600
400
1-6
1-7
1-8
2-1
2-2
BAR/EIGHTH-NOTE
160
Dav
De_
Na~
140
Esc
(\J
~ 100
She
Cur 0
,,-
80
Gia
0:
0)
60
40
20
o,
o
Ie
Arg
An
Ka~
Sre
Neye
SCh
(\J
(f)
Cap
Ash
Ki~ Zak
ru
120
Kli
Un!.
HaZ.
Ha3
Hal
I
20
40
60
80
100
120
140
160
24;"
Jl
(a)
'3M.
r1J
Jl
(c)
r1J
I.J
(d)
Jl
,r1J
Jl
(e)
Jl
(f)
I
In
2.5
11l
Schumann's "Triiumerei"
249
I
0
2 -f---
Bar 2
Bar 6
V\
Zak
0
Zak
t::. Bar 18
She.6
--
1.5
Co2
Bun
CO
~o
oJa-o_t
t::.
~I
0.5
L...J
0.2
R~
let::.
~_O
t::..
She
t::.
Co10
o Kli
Cao
-t::.
:e~
0.4
~u t
0.6
0.8
1.2
1.4
1.6
A/(B+C)
Figure 11. Relative timing of the grace notes within the last 101 of MG2, for cases (aHc) in Figure 10. The abscissa
shows the ratio of the time before the onset of the first grace note (A) and the remainder of the 101 (Be); the ordinate
shows the ratio of the time between the two grace note onsets (B) and the time from the onset of the second grace note
to the end of the 101 (C). Ratios for bars 2 and 6 are averaged over the two repeats.
Repp
250
1000
Q]
900
Bars 2-4
900
-e-
Bars 18-20
700
700
600
600
500
500
....
c\J
-.-
800
....--..
(f)
1000
800
400
.........
MG6a
(")
If)
....
(")
If)
....
400
(")
If)
....
....
(")
If)
(")
If)
....
<'?
"(
r-;-
1000
1000
OJD
900
900
a:
800
()
700
700
600
600
I- 500
500
....
c\J
800
(f)
a:
0
()
u..
400
....
c\J
(")
If)
....
(")
If)
....
400
~.
1000
(")
If)
....
1000
900
800
800
700
600
600
500
500
....
c\J
@J
900
700
400
....
c\J
(")
If)
....
<'?
~
If)
....
400
....
c\J
C')
If)
....
BAR/EIGHTH-NOTE
Figure 12. Rescaled factor scores (underlying timing patterns) for the MG chain of Type a in bars 2-4 and 18-20. The
abscissa labels refer to bars 2-4.
Diversitll and Commonalitll m ivfusic Performance: An Anaillsis o(Timing Microstructure m Schumann's "Triiumerei"
251
Table 6. Sorted rotated factor loadings from the principal components analysis ofMG3a-MG6a. (Loadings below 0.4
are omitted.)
II
ARR
NEY
BRE
ASH
KAT
ESC
H03
H02
HOI
ARG
COl
CO2
0.773
0.758
0.744
0.708
0.671
0.606
IV
0.446
VI
0.402
0.873
0.832
0.784
-0.593
0.823
0.808
0.786
0.676
0.564
0.456
0.457
0.427
0.431
0.414
0.437
0.416
0.908
0.893
0.830
0.629
cm
ORT
KUB
NOV
SCH
CUR
MOl
SHE
DAV
GIA
KLI
DEM
ZAK
BUN
KRU
CAP
III
0.410
0.576
0.436
0.515
Factor I (VAF = 18%) is characterized by acceleration during MG3a; a fast traversal of MG4a; a
dramatic ritardando within MG5a with maximal
lengthening of the final, unaccented note (position
4-2), further augmented in bars 18-20; and a
pronounced lengthening of the last 101 in MG6a
(position 4-8). The pianists whose interpretations
come closest to this pattern are ARR, NEY, BRE,
and ASH.
Factor II (VAF = 17%) is the "Horowitz factor."
It is characterized by significant lengthening of
the accented tone in MG3a as well as of the preceding unaccented tone; slight deceleration during
MG4a; final lengthening in MG5a; and a ritardando during MG6a, followed by a sudden shortening of the last 101. Some of these tendencies
were exaggerated in bars 18-20 relative to bars 24. Horowitz's actual performances (especially H02
and H03) resemble this pattern, except that the
exaggeration in bars 18-20 is much more dramatic. H03 differs in that bars 2-4 and 18-20 are
executed very similarly, both showing dramatic
lengthening in positions 4-2 and 4-3.
Factor III (VAF = 14%) is the "Cortot factor." It
is characterized by a fast start; progressive slowing down during MG4a and MG5a, with final
0.535
0.541
0.864
0.693
0.574
0.564
0.602
0.565
0.540
0.410
0.491
0.500
0.455
0.436
-0.435
Repp
Diversitll and Commonality in Music Performance: An Anaillsis of Timing Microstructure in Schumanrz's "Triiumerei"
-+- Bars 68
750
[I]
650
( J)
253
550
550
450
450
350
350
7-3
67
7-5
7-7
8-1
750
8-1
6-7
7-1
7-3
75
7-7
6-7
71
7-3
7-5
77 . 8-1
7-5
7-7
750
---(J)
DE]
W 650
650
a:
0
550
<-) 550
(J)
a:
450
450
f-
350
LL
350
67
7-1
7-3
7-5
77
8-1
750
750
650
650
550
550
450
450
C2TI
350
350
6-7
7-1
7-3
7-5
77
8-1
6-7
7-1
7-3
8-1
BAR/EIGHTH-NOTE
Figure 13. Rescaled factor scores <underlying timing patterns) for MG chain of Type b in bars 6-8, and 10-12, and 14-16.
The abscissa labels refer to bars 6-8.
Repp
254
Table 7. Sorted rotated factor loadings from the principal components analysis of MG3b-MG5b. (Loadings below 0.4
are omitted.)
II
ARR
HOI
ARG
H03
KLI
MOl
ASH
DEM
ESC
GIA
CAP
COl
KRU
BRE
CO2
NOV
SHE
CUR
SCH
KUB
NEY
KAT
DAV
ZAK
ORT
BUN
C03
H02
0.923
0.788
0.745
0.740
0.715
0.701
0.648
0.589
0.553
0.531
0.515
0.418
III
IV
VI
0.521
0.582
0.490
0.455
0.438
0.860
0.829
0.733
0.699
0.691
0.675
0.664
0.629
0.530
0.407
0.450
0.407
0.488
0.404
0.416
0.814
0.746
0.721
0.642
0.416
0.893
0.756
0.863
0.413
0.452
III
1000
900
---.
(f)
--0E
700
600
800
:!:1:'
500
12-5
12-7
12-6
2200
---.
12-8
I
V
1800
en
51400
/'
0
/
1000
-.-
~/
600
16-5
16-7
16-6
16-8
1400
1200
---.
(f)
51000
o
800
1/
~
600
23-7
23-8
24-1
24-2
BAR/EIGHTH-NOTE
256
Repp
1200
o ~ -40
o ~ 20
1000
0=0
(/J
.L
0=20
0=40
800
0= 60
"l
0=80
0=100
0=0
0=40
600
400
125
126
12-7
12-8
2400
2100
~1800
0=80
-=-1500
.L
0=120
Q 1200
0= 160
900
0=200
"l
0=240
(/J
i=
600
16-5
16-6
16-7
16-8
1600
1400
:-[1200
1000
800
600
237
23-8
24-1
0=20
0=0
0=20
.L
0=40
0=60
0=80
"l
0= 100
0=120
24-2
BAR/EIGHTH-NOTE
v. GENERAL DISCUSSION
The present comprehensive analysis of
expressive timing patterns in 28 performances of
Schumann's "Traumerei" provides an objective
view of the commonalities and differences among
great artists' interpretations of one of the
masterpieces of the piano literature. At first
glance, the differences are perhaps more striking
than the commonalities. There is ample material
here to support the view that every artist's
performance is, in some sense, unique and unlike
Diversity and Commonalitv in Music Performance: An Analysis of Timing Microstructure in Schumann's "Triiumerei"
257
258
Repp
Diversity and CommonaLitv in Music Performance: An AnaLvsis of Timing Microstructure in Schumann's "Triiumerei"
REFERENCES
Bengtsson, 1., & Gabrielsson, A. (1980). Methods for analyzing
performance of musical rhythm. Scandinavian Journal of
PsychoLogy,21,257-268.
Brendel, A. (1981). Der Interpret muss erwachsen sein. Zu
Schumann's "Kinderszenen." Musica, 6, 429-433.
Chissell, J. (1987). Liner notes for Claudio Arrau's recording
(Philips 420-871-2).
Clarke, E. F. (1982). Timing in the performance of Erik Satie's
'Vexations.' Acta Psychologica, 50, 1-19.
Clarke, E. F. (1983). Structure and expression in rhythmic
performance. In P. Howell, I. Cross, & R. West (Eds.), Musical
structure and cognition (pp. 209-236). London: Academic Press.
Clarke, E. F. (1985). Some aspects of rhythm and expression in
performances of Erik Satie's "Gnossienne No.5." Music
Perception, 2,299328.
Clarke, E. F. (1988). Generative principles in music performance.
In J. A. Sloboda (Ed.), Generative processes in music (pp. 1-26).
Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Cline, E. (1985). Piano competitions: An analysis of their structure,
value, and educational implications. Doctoral dissertation, Indiana
University.
Clynes, M. (1983). Expressive microstructure in music, linked to
liVing qualities. In J. Sundberg (Ed.), Studies of music performance
(pp. 76-181). Stockholm, Sweden: Publication issued by the
Royal Swedish Academy of Music No. 39.
Clynes, M. (1987). What can a musician learn about music
performance from newly discovered microstructure principles
(PM and PAS)? In A. Gabrielsson (Ed.), Action and perception in
rhythm and music (pp. 201-233). Stockholm, Sweden: Publication
issued by the Royal Swedish Academy of Music No. 55.
Friberg, A. (1991). Generative rules for music performance: A
formal description of a rule system. Computer Music Journal, 15,
56-71.
Gabrielsson, A. (1974). Performance of rhythm patterns.
Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 15,63-72.
Gabrielsson, A (1985). Interplay between analysis and synthesis
in studies of music performance and music experience. Music
Perception, 3, 59-86.
Gabrielsson, A. (1987). Once again: the theme from Mozart's
Piano Sonata in A major (K. 331). In A. Gabrielsson (Ed.), Action
and perception in rhythm and music (pp. 81-103). Stockholm,
Sweden: Publication issued by the Royal Swedish Academy of
Music No. 55.
Gabrielsson, A. (1988). Timing in music performance and its
relation to music experience. In J.A. Sloboda (Ed.), Generative
processes in music (pp. 27-51). Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Gabrielsson, A., Bengtsson, I., & Gabrielsson, B. (1983).
Performance of musical rhythm in 3/4 and 6/8 meter.
Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 24,193-213.
Hartmann, A (1932). Untersuchungen tiber metrisches Verhalten
in musikalischen Interpretationsvarianten. Archiv fur die
gesamte Psychologie, 84, 103-192.
259
Horowitz, J. (1990). The ivory trade: Music and the business of music
at the Van Cliburn International Piano Competition. New York:
Summit Books.
Kendall, R. A, & Carterette, E. C. (1990). The communication of
musical expression. Music Perception, 8, 129-163.
Kronrnan, U., & Sundberg, J. (1987). Is the musical ritard an
allusion to physical motion? In A. Gabrielsson (Ed.), Action and
Perception in Rhythm and Music (pp. 57-68). Stockholm:
Publications issued by the Royal Swedish Academy of Music
No. 55.
Lerdahl, F., & Jackendoff, R. (1983). A generative theory of tonal
music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Lipman, S. (1984). Glenn Gould: His dissent/ An obituary
(originally published in 1982). In S. Lipman, The house of music:
Art in an era of institutions (pp. 79-95). Boston: Godine.
Lipman, S. (1990). Arguing for music/Arguing for culture. Boston:
Godine.
Lussy, M. M. (1882). Musical expression: Accents, nuances, and tempo
in vocal and instrumental music. London: Novello.
Palmer, C. (1989). Mapping musical thought to musical
performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Perception and Perfornlance, 15, 331-346.
Povel, D.-J. (1977). Temporal structure of performed music: some
preliminary observations. Acta Psychologica, 41, 309-320.
Repp, B. H. (1990). Patterns of expressive timing in performances
of a Beethoven minuet by nineteen famous pianists. Journal of
the Acoustical Society of America, 88, 622-641.
Repp, B. H. (1992a). Probing the cognitive representation of
musical time: Structural constraints on the perception of timing
perturbations. Cognition, 44, 241-281.
Repp, B. H. (1992b). A constrainton the expressive timing of a
melodic gesture: Evidence from performance and aesthetic
judgment. Music Perception, 10,221-242.
Repp, B. H. (1993a). Objective performance analysis as a tool for
the musical detective. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,
93 (forthcoming).
Repp, B. H. (1993b). On determining the global tempo of a
temporally modulated music performance.
Reti, R. (1951). The thematic process in music. New York: Macmillan.
Riemann, H. (1884). Der Ausdruck in der Musik. In P. Graf
Waldersee (Ed.), Sammlung musikalischer Vortriige (pp. 43-64).
Leipzig: Breitkopf und Hartel.
Seashore, C. E. (1936) (Ed.) Objective analysis of music performance.
Iowa City, IA: The University Press (University of Iowa Studies
in the Psychology of Music, Vol. IV).
Seashore, C. E. (1938). Psychology of music. New York: McGrawHill. (New York: Dover Publications, 1967.)
Seashore, C. E. (1947). In search of beauty in music: A scientific
approach to musical esthetics. New York: Ronald Press.
Shaffer, 1. H. (1980). Analysing piano performance: a study of
concert pianists. In G. E. Stelmach & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials
in motor behavior (pp. 443-455). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Shaffer, 1. H. (1981). Performances of Chopin, Bach, and Bartok:
Studies in motor programming. Cognitive Psychology, 13, 326376.
Shaffer, 1. H. (1984). Timing in solo and duet piano performances.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36A, 577-595.
Shaffer, 1. H. (1989). Cognition and affect in musical performance.
Contemporary Music Review, 4, 381-389.
Shaffer, 1. H., Clarke, E. F., & Todd, N. P. (1985). Metre and
rhythm in piano playing. Cognition, 20, 61-77.
Shaffer, 1. H., & Todd, N. P. (1987). The interpretative component
in musical performance. In A. Gabrielsson (Ed.)! Action and
perception ill rhythm alld music (pp. 139-152). Stockholm,
Sweden: Publication issued by the Royal Swedish Academy of
Music No. 55.
Rqp
260
FOOTNOTES
'Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 92, 2546-2568 (1993).
I An effort is made in this manuscript to distinguish between notes,
which are graphic symbols, and tones, which are sound events.
However, when it comes to grace notes, the distinction cannot
easily be made, since "grace tone" is not an acceptable term and
"grace-note tone" is awkward. It should be understood, then,
that "grace note" refers either to the notated symbol or the
resulting sound, depending on the context. The same ambiguity
holds for "chord," although "tone cluster" is an acceptable term
for the sound event that may be used on occasion.
21n a third case (NEY), it was discovered that the two repeats were
virtually identical from bar 5 on, suggesting duplication by the
recording engineers (see Repp, 1993a).
3In the designation for a "position," such as 5-8, the two
numbers stand for the bar and the eighth-note 101 within it,
respectively. In an expression such as "bars 5-8," however, the
two numbers stand for the first and last bars in the range
referred to.
4Most of the performances in the latter group strike the author as
mannered. It seems that these pianists deliberately tried to play
differently from the norm, but were not willing or able to do so
consistently. Perhaps they intended to convey an
improvisatory quality.
5Todd (1992a, 1992b) has recently revised and extended his
model of expressive timing. An application of this model to the
present data would be most interesting but exceeds the scope
of this paper.
6That is to say, there may be an implicit, expressively modulated
eighth-note pulse going through the longer lOIs. As suggested
by the extent of the MG boxes in Figure 2, the first of these
pulses "belongs to" the preceding MG, but its actual duration
cannot be determined unless it is marked by some tonal event
in another voice. MG2i, which breaks up the inter-gesture
interval in the soprano voice, may track the implicit pulse
induced by the primary MG2 and thus may reveal its final
lengthening.
7Although these performances were by no means identical, they
were more similar than almost any pair of performances by