Attachment
Attachment
Table of content
(Last update: 05-19-2010)
To read the Key Messages related to this topic, consult the Encyclopedia at:
http://www.child-encyclopedia.com/en-ca/parents-child-attachment/key-messages.html
Synthesis on attachment
(Published online November 26, 2006)
All infants require attention, comfort and a sense of security. Infants who feel
threatened will turn to their caregiver for protection and comfort; over time, the
caregiver’s response helps mold the relationship into a pattern of interaction.
Bowlby’s attachment theory describes the importance of the early relationship that
develops between the infant and the primary caregiver. This affective bond, called
attachment, provides the foundation for the child’s later social, emotional and even
cognitive development. In addition, attachment relationships continue to influence
thoughts, feelings, motives and close relationships throughout life.
Research shows that secure attachment is a protective factor that leads to more
optimal developmental outcomes, while children with insecure attachment are more
prone to social and maladjustment problems and children with disorganized
attachment are at highest risk for psychopathology and poor outcomes.
What do we know?
There are four patterns of infant-caregiver attachment. Infants who actively seek
proximity to their caregivers on reunion, communicate their feelings of stress and
distress openly and then readily return to exploration are classified as secure. This
type of attachment is believed to develop when the caregiver consistently responds
to the child’s distress in a sensitive manner. Infants who ignore or avoid the
caregiver after being reunited are classified as insecure-avoidant. This is believed to
develop when the caregiver consistently responds to the child’s distress in ways that
are rejecting.
Infants who combine strong contact maintenance with contact resistance, or who
remain inconsolable without being able to return to explore the environment, are
classified as insecure-ambivalent. This develops when the caregiver responds in ways
that are inconsistent and unpredictable. Finally, some infants do not seem able to
resort to a single, organized attachment pattern. This is called disorganized
attachment, and is believed to develop when the caregiver displays unusual and
ultimately frightening behaviours in the presence of the child.
For a normative population, it is reported that about 62% of infants are classified as
secure, 15% as insecure-avoidant, 8% as insecure-ambivalent and 15% as
disorganized.1
Children with disorganized attachment are at the highest risk for psychopathology.
There is a high percentage of attachment disorganization among children who have
been victims of maltreatment. An array of parental behaviours has been linked to
infant disorganization. These include affective communication errors (such as
contradictory responses to infant signals), parental withdrawal, negative-intrusive
responses, role-confused responses, disoriented responses and frightened or
frightening behaviours.
Negative life events (such as divorce) can compromise attachment security, but
differences in attachment security result primarily from the children’s interactions
with their social environment during the first few years of life. Parenting therefore
plays a crucial role. For this reason, preventive interventions in early childhood have
enormous potential to alter behavioural and developmental trajectories, especially in
high-risk families.
Currently, the research evidence for enhancing the attachment relationship favours
brief, highly targeted interventions, once the child is at least six months old.
Reference
Topic
Attachment
Introduction
Bowlby’s and Ainsworth’s approach to personality development relied on ethology1,2 and
cross-cultural research3, preserving the central questions of traditional psychoanalysis2
and drawing on the concept of mental representation as suggested by cognitive
psychology. The ethological approach implies: a) a careful description and classification
of infant and child behaviour4; b) reference to a posited environment of evolutionary
adaptedness for humans, as evidenced by young humans’ intense responsiveness to being
left alone in a strange environment with strange people; and c) analyzing the function of
emotions and behaviours in a social context.5 Attachment serves to ensure protection and
care, and secure attachment serves to relieve distress, restore physiological homeostasis
and encourage exploration. The impact of attachment in terms of biology and
neurobiology has also been documented in recent studies. 7,18 For example, it is through
attachments relationships, that young children first learn to link emotions to external
events in a linguistically meaningful manner. Further, non-pathological attachment
relationships are the basis for becoming emotionally, socially and cognitively
acculturated.6
In the early years, attachment relationships to parents and consistent caregivers are the
predominant and most influential relationships in children’s lives. These relationships set
the stage for infants’ physiological functioning, their emotional and cognitive
interpretations of social- and non-social experiences, their language development, and
their acquisition of meaning about themselves and others in complex social situations.
Later, the attachment relationships mediate children’s acceptance and acquisition of their
culture.14 Joint attention appears to be the central process;8 it emerges at around nine
months, at the height of stranger anxiety. In this way, nature ensures that infants learn
first about their family’s culture in the mother tongue. Attachment relationships that were
vital for infant survival during human evolution9 continue to influence thoughts, feelings
and motives and therefore close relationships throughout life. Early experiences of care,
and the attachment relationship with the caregiver, have a long lasting impact on the
child’s reactivity to stress.18
Within the framework of modern evolutionary biology, attachment theory focuses on the
“gene-selfish” interest of children in receiving as much of their parents’ physical as well
as psychological resources as possible.5,9 In terms of Trivers’10 parent-offspring conflict
paradigm, attachment theory focuses on the offspring’s side of the conflict, and on the
parent’s willingness or unwillingness to invest in any particular individual offspring.
However, parental lifespan planning may help to explain possible differences in parental
investment in care and differential parental sensitivity towards different children.26 This
may also explain the moderate concordance of patterns of attachment even in
monozygotic twins.11
Subject
Attachment theory posits a causal relationship between individuals’ experience with their
parents or attachment figures and their capacity to form affectional bonds later on. If a
child receives tender loving care when in need, and support for autonomy during
exploration from mother as well as father, such experiences are assumed to a) give the
child a sense of worth, a belief in the helpfulness of others and enable the child to explore
the environment with confidence; b) be an optimal precondition for mutually supportive,
enduring adult partnerships; and c) provide a model for later parenthood.12,6 Confident,
competent exploration is equivalent to our concept of “secure” exploration.13 Combining
the concept of secure attachment with secure exploration yields the concept of
“psychological security” that we advocate.13
Problems
Originally, attachment research provided only one method to assess quality of attachment
in infancy, using a separation-reunion paradigm (the strange situation). However,
research results indicated a low validity of the infant-father strange situation assessment
for predicting subsequent psychosocial development.15 Rather, father-child interactive
quality during play or exploration, and sensitive challenges to the young child’s
competencies seem to be better predictors of child development.16,24 Another challenge to
attachment research is more a measurement than a conceptual issue: How do behavioural
patterns of infant attachment become patterns of verbal discourse about attachment
later?23
Research Context
Two longitudinal studies of children’s social and emotional development in not-at-risk
middle-class two-parent families were started in the mid- and late 1970s: the Bielefeld
project, or Project 1, which started with the birth of the infants, and the Regensburg
project, or Project 2, which started when the infants were 11 months old.19 The children’s
experiences in the domains of attachment and exploration were assessed in infancy,
childhood and adolescence, with both mother and father using standardized or free
observations. Semi-structured interviews about family matters were conducted with the
parents on many occasions and later with the children. Representations of attachment
were assessed at ages 10, 16 and 22, representations of friendship at 16, and
representations of partnership at 20 or 22. For the analysis of early influences on the
representation of close relationships, data on child attachment and exploratory strategies,
maternal and paternal sensitivity and support were aggregated for the periods of infancy
(birth to age three), childhood (five to 10) and adolescence (16 to 18)19. In addition, we
conducted various studies in other cultures,20 adding to the long tradition of cross-cultural
research on attachment.21
Research Results
Our longitudinal projects revealed several major findings:19
3. Mothers’ and fathers’ sensitivity during joint play with their children in various
settings in the first six years of life contributed significantly to the child’s later quality of
partnership representation. Parental sensitivity during play was characterized by parental
support, and behaviours that promote cooperation and independent problem solving.
7. By age 22, however, a number of subjects had reflected thoroughly on their attachment
experience such that parental divorce was no longer a major but only a mediating
variable. The most powerful predictor of attachment and partnership representation at age
22, was the child’s representation of maternal and paternal support during middle
childhood age and mothers’ and/or fathers’ rejection of the child, as indicated in a lengthy
semi-structured interview when the children were 10 years old.27
Our own cross-cultural research on Japanese and Trobriand infants confirmed three of the
four core hypotheses of attachment theory21: 1. Infant attachment to at least one caring
adult is universal; 2. the secure pattern of attachment was also the norm in both groups;
and 3. security of attachment is positively related to competence.5,20 In our recent review,
we summarize many studies that support the concept of psychological security indicating
the combined influence of secure exploration and secure attachment. Psychological
security was linked to cognitive competence, flexible gender-role behaviour, as well as
resourceful transition and adaptions within the school system.13
Conclusion
Young children’s experiences of sensitive, accepting, supportive mothers and fathers start
a pathway of positive psychosocial development for the child. Such experiences in the
domains of attachment as well as exploration are at the roots of secure models of close
relationships and healthy self-reliance in the academic domain.28 They are likely to be
carried forward to other close relationships in childhood, adolescence and young
adulthood. Changes in parental acceptance or disruption of the family can alter the
pathway in either direction, temporarily or permanently.6,19
Parents who have experienced difficult childhoods themselves or who have an infant with
special needs benefit from help in three pivotal domains: 1) understanding child
development in all domains; 2) learning to respond sensitively to their individual child;30
3) finding enjoyment and sufficient time for sensitive, supportive interactions with the
child in attachment- and exploration-relevant situations. In subsequent years, support in
more domains become important, such as finding invested, knowledgeable mentors and
educators for the child and monitoring the child’s friendship group.. This is especially
important when parents’ own education or acculturation leave too many gaps. Secure
attachment is a necessary but not sufficient prerequisite for becoming a cooperative,
valuable and accepted member of one’s group and society. Secure exploration must
complement secure attachments so that children can successfully meet the many
challenges posed by their social relationships.
REFERENCES
Grossmann K, Grossmann KE. The impact of attachment to mother and father at an early age on children’s
psychosocial development through young adulthood. Rev ed. In: Tremblay RE, Barr RG, Peters RDeV,
Boivin M, eds. Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development [online]. Montreal, Quebec: Centre of
Excellence for Early Childhood Development; 2009:1-8. Available at: http://www.child-
encyclopedia.com/documents/GrossmannANGxp_rev.pdf. Accessed [insert date].
Copyright © 2005-2009
This article is funded by the Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development
(CEECD), the Strategic Knowledge Cluster on ECD (SKC-ECD) and the Alberta
Centre for Child, Family and Community Research
Topic
Attachment
Introduction
The attachment relationship between parent and child refers to those aspects of the
relationship that serve to regulate the infant’s stressful arousal or sense of felt security.
The quality of regulation of fearful affect available in attachment relationships is
fundamental to the developing child's freedom to turn attention away from issues of
threat and security toward other developmental achievements, such as exploration,
learning and play. Under normal conditions, an adequately functioning attachment
relationship buffers the infant against extreme levels of fearful arousal. However, the
attachment relationship may also malfunction. Based on accumulated research findings,
disorganized and controlling forms of attachment behaviour are now thought to represent
signs of malfunction of the attachment relational system. Both caregiver and infant
contribute to the infant-caregiver negotiations that occur around distress and comfort, as
well as to the potential defensive adaptations that may result from those negotiations.
ceasing distress after being picked up. Infants who display disorganized versions of
secure strategies constitute a slight majority (approx. 52%) of infants classified as
disorganized.3,4
Conclusions
Disorganized attachment processes are early predictors of both internalizing and
externalizing forms of psychopathology from the preschool period onward. These
attachment processes are distinct from child temperament and appear to reside in child-
caregiver relational processes rather than in the child or parent alone. Attachment
disorganization is likely to constitute a broad relational risk factor for psychopathology
that cuts across conventional diagnostic categories and interacts with individual
biological vulnerability, producing a range of psychiatric symptoms. Variability in
behavioural profiles within the disorganized group suggests that multiple etiological
models may be needed. Differing biological vulnerabilities interacting with differing
experiences of loss, abuse and/or chronically hostile or neglecting relationships may lead
to quite different developmental trajectories and adult outcomes. Current frontiers include
investigation of gene-environment interaction in the etiology of disorganized
attachment,22,23,24,25 and differentiation of correlates and outcomes related to
indiscriminate attachment behaviour compared to disorganized attachment behaviour.
26,27
TABLE 1
REFERENCES
17. Toth SL, Rogosch FA, Manly JT, Cicchetti D. The efficacy of toddler-parent
psychotherapy to reorganize attachment in the young offspring of mothers with
major depressive disorder: A randomized preventive trial. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology 2006;74(6):1006-1016.
18. Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA, Toth SL. Fostering secure attachment in infants in
maltreating families through preventive interventions. Development and
Psychopathology 2006;18(3):623-649.
19. Olds D, Henderson CJr, Kitzman H, Eckenrode J, Cole R, Tatelbaum R. The
promise of home visitation: Results of two randomized trials. Journal of
Community Psychology 1998;26(1):5-21.
20. Schweinhart LJ, Barnes H, Weikart D. Significant benefits: The High/Scope Perry
Preschool study through age 27. Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press; 1993.
21. Lally JR, Mangione PL, Honig AS. The Syracuse University Family Development
Research Program: Long-range impact on an early intervention with low-income
children and their families. In: Powell DR, ed. Parent education as early
childhood intervention: Emerging directions in theory, research and practice.
Westport, CT: Ablex Publishing; 1988:79-104. Annual advances in applied
developmental psychology; vol. 3.
22 . Lakatos K, Toth I, Nemoda Z, Ney K, Sasvari-Szekely M, Gervai J. Dopamine
D4 receptor (DRD4) gene polymorphism is associated with attachment
disorganization in infants. Molecular Psychiatry 2000;5(6):633-637.
23. Gervai J, Novak A, Lakatos K, Toth I, Danis I, Ronai, Z, Nemoda Z, Sasvari-
Szekely M, Bureau JF, Bronfman E, Lyons-Ruth K. Infant genotype may
moderate sensitivity to maternal affective communications: Attachment
disorganization, quality of care, and the DRD4 polymorphism. Social
Neuroscience 2007;2(3-4):307-319.
24. van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg M. DRD4 7-repeat polymorphism
moderates the association between maternal unresolved loss or trauma and infant
disorganization. Attachment and Human Development 2006;8(4):291-307.
25. Roisman GI, Fraley RC. A behavior-genetic study of parenting quality, infant
attachment security, and their covariation in a nationally representative sample.
Developmental Psychology 2008;44(3):831-839.
26. Zeanah CH, Smyke AT, Koga SF. Bucharest Early Intervention Project Core
Group; Attachment in institutionalized and community children in Romania.
Child Development 2005;76(5):1015-1028.
27. Lyons-Ruth K, Bureau JF, Riley CD, Atlas-Corbett AF. Socially indiscriminate
attachment behavior in the Strange Situation: Convergent and discriminant
validity in relation to caregiving risk, later behavior problems, and attachment
insecurity. Development and Psychopathology 2009;21(2):355-372.
28. Karoly LA, Greenwood PW, Everingham SS, Hoube J, Kilburn R, Rydell P,
Sanders M, Chiesa J. Investing in our children: What we know and don’t know
about the costs and benefits of early childhood interventions. Santa Monica, CA:
RAND Corporation; 1998. Available at:
http://www.rand.org/publications/MR/MR898/. Accessed January 4, 2010.
Copyright © 2005-2010
This article is funded by the Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development
(CEECD), the Strategic Knowledge Cluster on ECD (SKC-ECD) and the Alberta
Centre for Child, Family and Community Research
Topic
Attachment
Introduction
What is attachment? Children are considered to be attached if they tend to seek proximity
to and contact with a specific caregiver in times of distress, illness and tiredness.1
Attachment to a protective caregiver helps infants to regulate their negative emotions in
times of stress and distress and to explore the environment, even if it contains somewhat
frightening stimuli. Attachment, a major developmental milestone in the child’s life,
remains an important issue throughout the lifespan. In adulthood, attachment
representations shape the way adults feel about the strains and stresses of intimate
relationships, including parent-child relationships, and the way in which the self is
perceived.
Development of attachment
Attachment develops in four phases.1 In the first phase — indiscriminately orienting and
signalling to people — the baby seems “tuned” to certain wave-lengths of signals from
the environment. These signals are mostly of human origin (e.g. the sound of voices).
During the second phase, probably first by smell and then by sight, the baby develops
preference for one or more caregivers — the phase of orienting and signalling to one or
several specific persons. Not until the infant is able to show active attachment behaviour,
such as actively seeking proximity to and following the attachment figure, does the infant
enters the third phase, the phase of attachment proper — staying near a specific person by
means of signalling and movement. Children enter the fourth phase of the goal-corrected
partnership when they can imagine the parent or caregiver’s plans and perceptions and fit
their own plans and activities according to these.
Three patterns of attachment can be distinguished on the basis of infants' reactions to the
reunion with the parent or other caregiver. Infants who actively seek proximity to their
caregivers on reunion, communicate their feelings of stress and distress openly and then
readily return to exploration are classified as secure (B). Infants who do not seem to be
distressed and ignore or avoid the caregiver after being reunited (although physiological
research shows their arousal)3 are classified as insecure-avoidant (A). Infants who
combine strong contact maintenance with contact resistance, or remain inconsolable
without being able to return to explore the environment, are classified as insecure-
ambivalent (C). Besides the classic tripartite ABC classifications, Main and Solomon4
proposed a fourth classification, disorganized attachment (D), which is not discussed
here.
An overview of all American studies with non-clinical samples (21 samples with a total
of 1,584 infants, conducted between 1977 and 1990) shows that about 67% of the infants
were classified as secure, 21% as insecure-avoidant and 12% as insecure-ambivalent.5 A
central issue in attachment theory and research is what causes some infants to develop an
insecure attachment relationship while other infants feel secure.
Research Context
The basic model of explaining differences in attachment relationships assumes that
sensitive or insensitive parenting determines infant attachment (in-)security. Ainsworth2
and colleagues originally defined parental sensitivity as the ability to perceive and
interpret children’s attachment signals correctly and respond to these signals promptly
and adequately. Lack of responsiveness or inconsistent sensitivity has indeed been found
to be associated with insecurity in children, and consistent sensitive responsiveness with
secure bonds.6
However, some proponents of the behavioural genetic approach have declared most
correlational findings on child development to be seriously flawed because they are based
on traditional research designs focusing on between-family comparisons, which confound
genetic similarities between parents and children with supposedly shared environmental
influences.7 Harris,8 for example, claims that there is an urgent need to radically rethink
and de-emphasize the role of parents in child development. Despite the prevalence of this
current of thought, attachment theory continues to emphasize the important role of
parental sensitivity.
Conclusions
Attachment, the affective bond of infant to parent, plays a pivotal role in the regulation of
stress in times of distress, anxiety or illness. Human beings are born with the innate bias
to become attached to a protective caregiver. But infants develop different kinds of
attachment relationships: some infants become securely attached to their parent, and
others find themselves in an insecure attachment relationship. These individual
differences are not genetically determined but are rooted in interactions with the social
environment during the first few years of life. Sensitive or insensitive parenting plays a
key role in the emergence of secure or insecure attachments, as has been documented in
twin studies and experimental intervention studies. In the case of attachment theory, the
nurture assumption8 is indeed warranted. Numerous findings confirm the core hypothesis
that sensitive parenting causes infant attachment security, although other causes should
not be ruled out.
REFERENCES
1. Bowlby J. Attachment. New York, NY: Basic Books; 1969. Attachment and loss;
vol. 1.
2. Ainsworth MDS, Blehar MC, Waters E, Wall S. Patterns of attachment: a
psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates; 1978.
3. Spangler G, Grossmann KE. Biobehavioral organization in securely and
insecurely attached infants. Child Development 1993;64(5):1439-1450.
4. Main M, Solomon J. Procedures for identifying infants as
disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth Strange Situation. In: Greenberg
MT, Cicchetti D, Cummings EM, eds. Attachment in the preschool years: Theory,
research, and intervention. Chicago, Ill: University of Chicago Press; 1990:121-
160.
5. van IJzendoorn MH, Goldberg S, Kroonenberg PM, Frenkl OJ. The relative
effects of maternal and child problems on the quality of attachment: A meta-
analysis of attachment in clinical samples. Child Development 1992;63(4):840-
858.
6. De Wolff MS, van IJzendoorn MH. Sensitivity and attachment: A meta-analysis
on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Development 1997;68(4):571-
591.
7. Rowe DC. The limits of family influence: genes, experience, and behavior. New
York, NY: Guilford Press; 1994.
8. Harris JR. The nurture assumption: Why children turn out the way they do. New
York, NY: Free Press; 1998.
9. Ricciuti AE. Child-mother attachment: A twin study. Dissertation Abstracts
International 1992;54:3364. University Microfilms No. 9324873.
10. O’Connor TG, Croft CM. A twin study of attachment in preschool children. Child
Development 2001;72(5):1501-1511.
11. Bokhorst CL, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Fearon RMP, van IJzendoorn MH,
Fonagy P, Schuengel C. The importance of shared environment in mother-infant
attachment security: A behavioral genetic study. Child Development
2003;74(6):1769-1782.
12. Finkel D, Matheney APJr. Genetic and environmental influences on a measure of
infant attachment security. Twin Research 2000;3(4):242-250.
13. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH, Juffer F. Less is more: Meta-
analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood.
Psychological Bulletin 2003;129(2):195-215.
14. Clinton HR. It takes a village: and other lessons children teach us. New York,
NY: Simon & Schuster; 1996.
van IJzendoorn M. Attachment at an early age (0-5) and its impact on children’s development. In:
Tremblay RE, Barr RG, Peters RDeV, eds. Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development [online].
Montreal, Quebec: Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development; 2005:1-6. Available at:
http://www.child-encyclopedia.com/documents/van_IJzendoornANGxp.pdf. Accessed [insert date].
Copyright © 2005
Topic
Attachment
Introduction
Attachment theory and research has established for itself a central place in the study of
human social and affective development. Building on the foundational concepts of
Bowlby1 and Ainsworth’s2 translation of these into a framework for empirical study,
researchers worldwide have used attachment as a vehicle to increase our understanding of
the life-long consequences of the human infant’s first relationship and of the factors in
the mother’s own childhood that are central to the shaping of that relationship.
Karin and Klaus Grossmann and Marinus van IJzendoorn are outstanding representatives
of the researchers who have built on the legacy of Bowlby and Ainsworth. They have
taken different but complementary approaches to their tasks. van IJzendoorn provides a
straightforward, authoritative overview of attachment theory and a description of patterns
of attachment relationships.3 His description of research findings focuses on the question
of whether variation in attachment is a function of early social experience with the
caregiver or genetic factors, including temperament. He cites recent behavioural genetic
analyses of attachment in twins that convincingly support the experiential side in this
debate. This evidence is all the more notable because it contrasts sharply with the results
of parallel studies of the origins of many behavioural and personality traits and attitudes
for which evidence of substantial genetic influence has been found.4
The Grossmanns, on the other hand, take a more personal approach to their task,
beginning with an account that stresses attachment’s role, both in evolutionary and
developmental time, in providing the infant with a social apprenticeship with the
caregiver – an opportunity to acquire the social and emotional skills required to adapt to
the society and culture of his or her birth. Their review of research involves extracts from
two extensive longitudinal studies carried out in Germany by the Grossmanns and their
colleagues. These results stress both life-long continuity and the potential for change, for
better or worse, as a result of substantial fluctuations in the social environment.
1
Comments on original paper published by Karin Grossmann & Klaus E. Grossmann in 2005. To have
access to this article, contact us at [email protected]
Bowlby captured the most important implications of attachment theory and research for
social policy and services in a report that was written over half a century ago:
“Just as children are absolutely dependent on their parents for sustenance, so in
all but the most primitive communities, are parents, especially their mothers,
Bowlby’s comment to some extent reflects the language and culture of the day, but
remains urgently accurate. Research on attachment over the past three decades has
confirmed his central hypothesis that the sensitivity and responsiveness of the caregiver
is instrumental in shaping the human infant’s first relationship. This relationship, in turn,
has been shown to be a powerful predictor of later important social outcomes. Our efforts
to ensure that this outcome is adaptive rather than maladaptive for both the individual
and society must, therefore, focus on our support of the infant’s caregiver, most often the
mother. In today’s society, this translates most urgently, at a policy level, into ensuring
that families at developmental risk, including single mothers, are provided with the social
and financial resources necessary to provide their children with a supportive social
environment – the prerequisite of a healthy attachment relationship. As suggested by van
IJzendoorn, in many cases this will mean the provision of quality daycare for these same
families. For service-providers, attachment theory and research call for a focus on early
social interaction and on the primary mediator of such interaction, the mother. Patterns of
attachment behaviour and mental representations become less flexible and less open to
change with developmental time. Investment through social policy and service delivery
in the earliest years is thus a more efficient and feasible approach than reactive
intervention delayed until the negative consequences of inadequate early experiences
become apparent.
REFERENCES
Moran G. Attachment in early childhood: Comments on van IJzendoorn, and Grossmann and Grossmann.
In: Tremblay RE, Barr RG, Peters RDeV, eds. Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development [online].
Montreal, Quebec: Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development; 2005:1-4. Available at:
http://www.child-encyclopedia.com/documents/MoranANGxp.pdf. Accessed [insert date].
Copyright © 2005
Topic
Attachment
Introduction
More than 50 years ago, a British child psychiatrist named John Bowlby was
commissioned by the World Health Organization to write a monograph about the mental-
health needs of young children. Bowlby’s conclusion was that “what is believed to be
essential for mental health is that an infant and young child should experience a warm,
intimate and continuous relationship with his mother (or mother substitute – or permanent
mother substitute – one person who steadily mothers him) in which both find satisfaction
and enjoyment.”1 Grossmann and Grossmann, van IJzendoorn, and Hennighausen and
Lyons-Ruth all review the current status of more than 35 years of research that has
affirmed, refined and extended Bowlby’s central thesis. In this commentary, we review
the authors’ interpretations of research, implications for policy, and highlight additional
areas of emphasis.
1
Comments on original paper published by Karin Grossmann & Klaus E. Grossmann in 2005. To have
access to this article, contact us at [email protected]
2
Comments on original paper published by Kate Hennighausen & Karlen Lyons-Ruth in 2005. To have
access to this article, contact us at [email protected]
than “nature” that accounts for differences in attachment security. His hypothesis is well
substantiated by the research he cites and is further supported by repeated findings that a
child may have different attachment classifications with different caregivers.2
(2) If attachment patterns reflect relationship characteristics rather than traits in the
child, one would expect that characteristics of dyadic interaction would be associated
with patterns of attachment. The research cited by van IJzendoorn provides support for a
causal role of parental sensitivity in the development of attachment security, though much
less research has addressed the interactive patterns that precede avoidant and resistant
attachment. Research reviewed by Hennighausen and Lyons-Ruth has also demonstrated
that certain parental behaviours, such as withdrawal, negative-intrusive responses, role-
confused responses, disoriented responses, frightened or frightening behaviours and
affective communication errors, which include contradictory responses to infant signals,
are likely to be more evident in the context of certain types of parental psychopathology,
and have been documented to be associated with disorganized attachment.3,4
(3) A central tenet of attachment theory has been that early experiences between
young children and their caregivers provide a model for intimate relationships in later
life. Although this model is believed to be modifiable by subsequent experiences, the
theory has posited a conservative tendency to resist change. These propositions suggest
that in a stable caregiving environment, one would expect to find stable patterns of
attachment, but in environments characterized by significant changes, one would expect
less stability. On balance, these assertions are supported by research, although results
from four longitudinal studies of attachment from infancy to adulthood do not support a
linear relationship,5-8 as these studies do not uniformly demonstrate stability of
attachment classifications from infancy to adulthood. They do, however, provide support
for a relationship between life events and changes in attachment classifications. In the
Grossmanns’ work, negative life events and stresses were also found to compromise
attachment security. Individuals whose attachment classifications changed from secure in
infancy to insecure in adulthood were more likely to have experienced negative life
events (such as divorce), and children who demonstrated insecure attachment in infancy
were more likely to remain insecure if they experienced negative life events. Studies
conducted and reviewed by Grossmann and Grossmann (this volume) have helped
illuminate some of the complexities of developmental pathways.
Additional Issues
What is missing from these contributions is a consideration of attachment in more
extreme populations, such as maltreated or severely deprived young children. In contrast
to the developmental perspective that considers the quality of a young child’s attachment
to a caregiver as a risk or protective factor for the development of psychopathology, the
clinical tradition considers that attachments may be so disturbed as to constitute an
already established disorder. Reactive attachment disorder (RAD) describes a
constellation of aberrant attachment behaviours and other social behavioural anomalies
that are believed to result from “pathogenic care.”10 Two clinical patterns have been
described:
(a) An emotionally withdrawn/inhibited pattern, in which the child exhibits limited or
absent initiation or response to social interactions with caregivers, and a variety of
aberrant social behaviours, such as inhibited, hyper-vigilant or highly ambivalent
reactions; and (b) an indiscriminately social/disinhibited pattern, in which the child
exhibits lack of expectable selectivity in seeking comfort, support and nurturance, with
lack of social reticence with unfamiliar adults and a willingness to “go off” with
strangers.
Although the systematic study of attachment disorders is quite recent, these disorders
have been described for more than half a century. From a handful of recent studies, it
seems clear that signs of attachment disorders are rare to non-existent in low-risk
samples,11-13 increased in higher-risk samples,14,15 and readily identifiable in maltreated16
and institutionalized samples.12,13 Interestingly, the emotionally withdrawn/inhibited type
of RAD is readily apparent in young children living in institutions and in young children
when they are first placed in foster care for maltreatment, but it is rarely evident in
samples of children adopted out of institutions.11,17 In contrast, the indiscriminately
social/disinhibited type of RAD is discernable in maltreated,16 institutionalized12,13,18 and
post-institutionalized children 11,13, 17,19-20
All three contributors describe implications for policy. van IJzendoorn emphasizes that
policies should be developed to encourage parental sensitivity in the infancy period.
No doubt all of the contributors would agree that we already know enough to identify
children at risk for disturbances of attachment and its associated psychopathology.
Nonetheless, preventive interventions, perhaps even before the child is born, have
enormous potential to alter the behavioural and developmental trajectories that may befall
children born into multi-risk families. The contributors further assert that policy and
practice should focus on the early identification of parent-child relationship difficulties in
hopes of providing services that may ameliorate the risk for the development of later
psychopathology.
Policies should identify the means by which families can access consistent parenting and
psychological support throughout the lifetime of their child. Primary health-care
providers and child-care professionals are two groups that have contact with most
families of children and adolescents. How these professionals may best support the needs
of parents and which interventions are most beneficial to enhance parental sensitivity and
infant attachment remains a matter of debate. A recent meta-analysis of early childhood
interventions asserted that brief interventions (<5 sessions) focusing on increasing
maternal sensitivity and enhancing infant attachment security were more effective than
long-term intervention.23 In contrast, Hennighausen and Lyons-Ruth cited evidence that
disorganized attachment responds best to home-based, intensive and long-term
interventions. In other words, from a health-promotion perspective (promoting secure
attachments), shorter and more focused interventions may be preferable, but from a risk-
reduction perspective (reducing disorganized attachment), longer and more intensive
interventions may be necessary. Challenges that remain are demonstrating valid
approaches to identifying different levels of risk in families and cost-effective
interventions to optimize later developmental and behavioural outcomes for young
children.
REFERENCES
1. Bowlby J. Child care and the growth of love. Melbourne, Australia: Penguin
Books; 1953;13.
2. Howes C. Attachment relationships in the context of multiple caregivers. In:
Cassidy J, Shaver PR, eds. Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and
clinical applications. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1999:671-687.
3. Lyons-Ruth K, Bronfman E, Parsons E. Atypical attachment in infancy and early
childhood among children at developmental risk. IV. Maternal frightened,
frightening, or atypical behaviour and disorganized infant attachment patterns.
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 1999;64(3):67-96.
4. Green J, Goldwyn R. Annotation: Attachment disorganisation and
psychopathology: new findings in attachment research and their potential
implications for developmental psychopathology in childhood. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry 2002;43(7):835-846.
5. Hamilton CE. Continuity and discontinuity of attachment from infancy through
adolescence. Child Development 2000;71(3):690-694.
6. Lewis M, Feiring C, Rosenthal S. Attachment over time. Child Development
2000;71(3):707-720.
7. Waters E, Merrick S, Treboux D, Crowell J, Albersheim L. Attachment security
in infancy and early adulthood: A twenty-year longitudinal study. Child
Development 2000;71(3):684-689.
8. Weinfeld NS, Sroufe LA, Egeland B. Attachment from infancy to early adulthood
in a high-risk sample: Continuity, discontinuity, and their correlates. Child
Development 2000;71(3):695-702.
9. Zeanah CH, Keyes A, Settles L. Attachment relationship experiences and
childhood psychopathology. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
2003;1008:22-30
10. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (DSM-IV). 4th ed. Text revision. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
Association;2000;130.
11. O’Connor TG, Marvin RS, Rutter M, Olrick JT, Britner PA, English and
Romanian Adoptees (ERA) Study Team. Child-parent attachment following early
institutional deprivation. Development and Psychopathology 2003;15(1):19-38.
12. Smyke AT, Dumitrescu A, Zeanah CH. Attachment disturbances in young
children. I: The continuum of caretaking casualty. Journal of the American
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2002;41(8):972-982.
13. Zeanah CH, Smyke AT, Koga S, Carlson E. Attachment in institutionalized
children. Paper presented at: Biennial meeting of the Society for Research in
Child Development; March, 2003; Tampa, Fla.
14. Boris NW, Zeanah CH, Larrieu JA, Scheeringa MS, Heller SS. Attachment
disorders in infancy and early childhood: A preliminary investigation of
diagnostic criteria. American Journal of Psychiatry 1998;155(2):295-297.
15. Boris NW, Hinshaw-Fuselier SS, Smyke AT, Scheeringa MS, Heller SS, Zeanah
CH. Comparing criteria for attachment disorders: Establishing reliability and
Zeanah CH Jr., Shah P. Attachment and its impact on child development: Comments on van IJzendoorn,
Grossmann and Grossmann, and Hennighausen and Lyons-Ruth. In: Tremblay RE, Barr RG, Peters
RDeV, eds. Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development [online]. Montreal, Quebec: Centre of
Excellence for Early Childhood Development; 2005:1-6. Available at: http://www.child-
encyclopedia.com/documents/Zeanah-ShahANGxp.pdf. Accessed [insert date].
Copyright © 2005
Topic
Attachment
Prevention of child maltreatment (abuse/neglect)
Introduction
Extremely insensitive and maltreating caregiving behaviors may be among the most
important precursors involved in the development of attachment insecurity and
disorganization. Egeland and Sroufe1 pointed out the dramatically negative impact of
neglecting or abusive maternal behavior for attachment and personality development, for
which they accumulated unique prospective evidence in later phases of the Minnesota
study.2 What do we know about the association between child maltreatment and
attachment, what are the mechanisms linking maltreatment with attachment insecurity
and disorganization, and what type of attachment-based interventions might be most
effective?
Subject
Following Cicchetti and Valentino,3 we include in our definition of child maltreatment
sexual abuse, physical abuse, neglect and emotional maltreatment. Besides these “family-
context” types of maltreatment, we also draw attention to structural neglect from which
world-wide millions of orphans and abandoned children suffer. Structural neglect points
to the inherent features of institutional care that preclude continuous, stable and sensitive
caregiving for individual children: caregiver shifts, high staff-turnover rates, large groups,
strict regimes, and sometimes physical and social chaos.4
inconsistent care. Maltreating insensitive parents do not regulate or buffer their child’s
experience of distress, but they also activate their child’s fear and attachment systems at
the same time. The resulting experience of fright without solution is characteristic of
maltreated children. According to Hesse and Main,5 disorganized children are caught in
an unsolvable paradox: their attachment figure is a potential source of comfort and at the
same time a source of unpredictable fright.
Problems
We speculate that multiple pathways to attachment disorganization exist involving either
child maltreatment by abusive parents or neglect in chaotic multiple-risk families or
institutions.
The pathway of abuse is based on the idea of (physically or sexually) maltreating parents
creating fright without solution for the child who cannot handle the paradox of a
potentially protective and, at the same time, abusive attachment figure, and thus becomes
disorganized.
Third, marital discord and domestic violence may lead to elevated levels of
disorganization as the child is witnessing an attachment figure unable to protect herself in
her struggle with a partner. Zeanah et al.7 documented a dose-response relation between
mothers’ exposure to partner violence and infant disorganization. Witnessing parental
violence may elicit fear in a young child about the mother’s well-being and her ability to
protect herself and the child against violence.
Research context
Collecting data on maltreatment samples is difficult. Maltreated children are often
victims of multiple forms of abuse, making it difficult to compare the different types of
maltreatment. Conjoint work with the child welfare system may raise legal and ethical
issues involving sharing information with clinical workers or being asked to provide a
statement in court.
Remarkable and rigorous but scarce work has been conducted by research groups
pioneering this challenging area. Seven studies on attachment security/disorganization
and child maltreatment in families have been reported, and six studies on attachment in
institution-reared children using the (modified) Strange Situation procedure to assess
attachment.8 In order to examine the impact of child maltreatment on attachment we
compare the studies’ combined distribution of attachment patterns to the normative low-
B B
A
C D A
D
C
D
institutions C maltreatment
B
A
B= secure
A= avoidant
C= ambivalent
D= disorganized
typical families
Figure 1: Attachment Distributions (Proportions) in Maltreatment
Samples, Institutions and Typical Families
Research gaps
How do some institution-reared and maltreated children develop secure attachment, and
what characterizes these children? Does attachment security constitute a protective factor
in high-risk contexts? Does it interact with other protective factors such as the child’s
biological constitution or the caregivers’ psychosocial resources? Little is know about the
differential effects of the various types of abuse and neglect – co-morbidity may hamper
a clear distinction of differential effects. Lastly, long-term effects of child maltreatment
should be studied more closely.
The lack of evidence-based interventions for maltreatment may have led some clinicians
to rely on so-called holding therapies, in which children are forced to make physical
contact with their caregiver although they strongly resist these attempts. Holding therapy
has not been proven to be effective,24,25 and in some cases has been harmful for children
to the level of casualties.26 Holding therapy is not implied at all by attachment theory. In
fact, therapists force the caregiver to be extremely insensitive and to ignore clear child
signals.
A major randomized control study by Cicchetti, Rogosch, and Toth27 has demonstrated
the effectiveness of an attachment-based intervention for maltreating families with child-
parent psychotherapy, enhancing maternal sensitivity through reinterpretation of past
attachment experiences. The intervention resulted in a substantial reduction in infant
disorganized attachment, and an increase in attachment security.
Maltreatment prevalence data show a large impact of risk factors associated with a very
low education and unemployment of parents (e.g., Euser et al.28). A practical implication
of this observation is the recommendation to pursue a socio-economic policy with a
strong emphasis on education and employment. Since unemployed and school dropped-
out parents are the most frequent perpetrators of child maltreatment, policies enhancing
education and employment rates are expected to effectively decrease child maltreatment
rates.
REFERENCES
15. Cyr C, Euser EM, Bakermans-Kranenburg M.J, Van IJzendoorn MH. Attachment
security and disorganization in maltreating and high-risk families: A series of
meta-analyses. Development & Psychopathology 2010;22(1):87-108.
16. The St. Petersburg – USA Orphanage Research Team. The effects of early social-
emotional and relationship experience on the development of young orphanage
children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development
2008;73(3):1-262.
17. Vorria P, Papaligoura Z, Dunn J, van IJzendoorn MH, Steele H, Kontopoulou A,
Sarafidou J. Early experiences and attachment relationships of Greek infants
raised in residential group care. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
2003;44(8):1208-1220.
18. Zeanah CH, Smyke AT, Koga SF, Carlson E, Bucharest Early Internvention
Project Core Group. Attachment in institutionalized and community children in
Romania. Child Development 2005;76(5):1015-1028.
19. Steele M, Steele H, Jin X, Archer M, Herreros F. Effects of lessening the level of
deprivation in Chinese orphanage settings: Decreasing disorganization and
increasing security. Paper presented at: The Biennial Meeting of the Society for
Research in Child Development. April 2-4, 2009; Denver, CO.
20. Herreros F. Attachment security of infants living in a Chilean orphanage. Poster
session presented at: The Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child
Development. April 2-4, 2009; Denver, CO.
21. Dobrova-Krol NA, Bakermans-Kranenburg MH, van Ijzendoorn MH, Juffer J.
The importance of quality of care: Effects of perinatal HIV infection and early
institutional rearing on preschoolers’ attachment and indiscriminate friendliness.
In: Dobrova-Krol NA, eds. Vulnerable children in Ukraine impact of institutional
care and HIV on the development of preschoolers. Leiden, the Netherland:
Mostert en van Onderen; 2009.
22. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH, Juffer F. Less is more: Meta-
analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood.
Psychological Bulletin 2003;129(2):195-215.
23. Berlin LJ, Ziv Y, Amaya-Jackson L, Greenberg MT, eds. Enhancing Early
Attachments: Theory, Research, Intervention, and Policy. New York, NY:
Guilford Press; 2005.
24. O’Connor MJ, Zeanah CH. Introduction to the special issue: Current perspectives
on assessment and treatment of attachment disorders. Attachment & Human
Development 2003;5(3):221-222.
25. Sroufe A, Erickson MF, Friedrich WN. Attachment theory and “attachment
therapy.” APSAC Advisor 2002;14:4-6.
26. Chaffin M, Hanson R, Saunders B, Barnett D, Zeanah C, Berliner L, Egeland B,
Lyon T, Letourneau E, Miller-Perrin C. Report of the APSAC Task Force on
attachment therapy, reactive attachment disorder, and attachment problems. Child
Maltreatment 2006;11(1):76-89.
27. Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA, Toth SL. Fostering secure attachment in infants in
maltreating families through prevention interventions. Development and
Psychopathology 2006;18:623-649.
28. Euser EM, Van IJzendoorn MH, Prinzie P, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. The
prevalence of child maltreatment in Netherlands. Child Maltreatment
2010;15(1):5-17.
van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. Attachment security and disorganization in maltreating
families and orphanages. In: Tremblay RE, Barr RG, Peters RDeV, Boivin M, eds. Encyclopedia on Early
Childhood Development [online]. Montreal, Quebec: Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood
Development; 2009:1-7. Available at: http://www.child-encyclopedia.com/documents/van_IJzendoorn-
Bakermans-KranenburgANGxp-Attachment.pdf. Accessed [insert date].
Copyright © 2009
This article is funded by the Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development
(CEECD), the Strategic Knowledge Cluster on ECD (SKC-ECD) and the Alberta
Centre for Child, Family and Community Research
Topic
Attachment
Introduction
A key biologically-based task for infants and toddlers is developing attachment
relationships with caregivers. The quality of attachment that children develop appears
largely dependent on caregivers’ availability.1 When caregivers are responsive, children
tend to develop secure attachments, seeking out caregivers directly when distressed.
When caregivers reject children’s bids for reassurance, children tend to develop avoidant
attachments, turning away from caregivers when distressed. When caregivers are
inconsistent in their availability, children tend to develop resistant attachments, showing a
mixture of proximity-seeking and resistance. Although it may be optimal for children in
our society to develop secure attachments,2-6 each of these three attachment types can be
seen as well-suited to caregivers’ availability. When caregivers are frightening to
children, though, children have difficulty developing organized attachments and instead
often develop disorganized attachments, which leave children without a consistent
strategy for dealing with their distress. Attachment quality has been linked with later
problem behaviours, with disorganized attachment especially predictive of dissociative
symptoms (e.g. seeming spacey, “in a fog” etc.),7 and internalizing and externalizing
problems.6,8-10 A number of prevention and intervention programs have been developed
that aim to improve infant attachment quality.
Subject
The strongest predictor of infant attachment is parental state of mind with regard to
attachment.11 State of mind refers to the manner in which adults process attachment-
related thoughts, feelings and memories. When parents are coherent in discussing their
own attachment experiences, they are said to have “autonomous states of mind” with
regard to attachment. When parents are not coherent in discussing their own attachment
experiences, they are said to have “non-autonomous states of mind” with regard to
attachment. Parents with autonomous states of mind are most likely to have babies with
secure attachments, whereas parents with “non-autonomous” states of mind are most
likely to have babies with insecure attachments. Given this association between parental
state of mind and infant attachment, some interventions12-15 target parent state of mind as
a means of changing infant attachment.
Problems
Programs that share the goal of enhancing attachment may differ in their focus, in their
intervention strategy, and in the populations targeted. Whereas an overall objective may
be to enhance attachment quality, other goals of improving quality of life, increasing life
skills and reducing symptomatology may differ, depending on the intervention and the
population served, as well as the level of fidelity to the treatment model. There is
disagreement among experts in the area regarding the nature of what is needed. For
example, some13 suggest that intensive interventions that start prenatally are essential,
whereas others19 suggest that targeted, short-term interventions are needed. Assessment
of treatment process and treatment fidelity is crucial to knowing what is being provided
in an intervention. For example, Korfmacher et al.20 found that their intervention,
intended to modify parental state of mind, rarely engaged parents in insight-oriented
work.
Research Context
In 2003, a meta-analysis reported by Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn and
Juffer19 found 29 studies that included attachment security as an outcome. Of these, 23
were randomized clinical trials, with a total of 1,255 participants. The nature of the
interventions, and the populations served, differed widely from one study to another.
Nonetheless, meta-analytic results allow assessment of the importance of factors such as
intervention intensity and population. Attachment quality was assessed in the Strange
Situation in most studies, although the Attachment Q-set was used in some investigations.
underlying characteristics, for several reasons. First, state of mind and other parent
characteristics are not as amenable to change as are parental behaviours. Second,
presumably, it is parental behaviours that directly affect children’s expectations of
parental availability.
Interventions that started after the child was at least six months old have been more
effective than those starting earlier.19 Although it is speculative, interventions may be
most efficient and effective when children have begun to show attachments to specific
caregivers.
For the most part, intervention effects have not proven to be significantly different for
different types of study populations. For example, intervention effects have been
generally comparable across risk status and socioeconomic status. Nonetheless, samples
with higher levels of insecurity have shown the largest effect sizes.
Conclusions
1. Interventions are effective in enhancing children’s attachment quality.
2. Interventions that target specific issues, most especially parental sensitivity,
appear more effective than interventions with more global goals.
3. Interventions that are brief are at least as effective as those that are of longer
duration.
4. Interventions that begin when attachment quality has begun to emerge (after about
six months of age) appear more effective than those begun earlier.
Implications
Currently, the research evidence favours brief, highly targeted interventions to enhance
attachment quality among infants. Given that the current evidence base is relatively
small, it makes sense to continue to examine associations between intervention
characteristics and outcome. Nonetheless, at this point, time-limited interventions
beginning in the second half of the first year of life appear most promising.
REFERENCES
14. Heinicke CM, Fineman NR, Ruth G, Recchia SL, Guthrie D, Rodning C.
Relationship-based intervention with at-risk mothers: Outcome in the first year of
life. Infant Mental Health Journal 1999;20(4):349-374.
15. Baradon T, Steele M. Integrating the AAI in the clinical process of psychoanalytic
parent-infant psychotherapy in a case of relational trauma. In: Steele H, Steele M,
eds. Clinical applications of the Adult Attachment Interview. New York, NY:
Guilford Press; 2008;195-212.
16. van den Boom DC. The influence of temperament and mothering on attachment
and exploration: An experimental manipulation of sensitive responsiveness
among lower-class mothers with irritable infants. Child Development
1994;65(5):1457-1477.
17. van den Boom DC. Do first-year intervention effects endure? Follow-up during
toddlerhood of a sample of Dutch irritable infants. Child Development
1995;66(6):1798-1816.
18. Juffer F, Hoksbergen RAC, Riksen-Walraven JM, Kohnstamm GA. Early
intervention in adoptive families: Supporting maternal sensitive responsiveness,
infant-mother attachment, and infant competence. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 1997;38(8):1039-1050.
19. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH, Juffer F. Less is more: Meta-
analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood.
Psychological Bulletin 2003;129(2):195-215.
20. Korfmacher J, Adam E, Ogawa J, Egeland B. Adult attachment: Implications for
the therapeutic process in a home visitation intervention. Applied Developmental
Science 1997;1(1):43-52.
Dozier M, Bernard K. The impact of attachment-based interventions on the quality of attachment among
infants and young children. Rev ed. In: Tremblay RE, Barr RG, Peters RDeV, Boivi M, eds. Encyclopedia
on Early Childhood Development [online]. Montreal, Quebec: Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood
Development; 2009:1-5. Available at: http://www.child-encyclopedia.com/documents/Dozier-
BernardANGxp_rev.pdf. Accessed [insert date].
Copyright © 2004-2009
This article is funded by the Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development
(CEECD), the Strategic Knowledge Cluster on ECD (SKC-ECD) and the Alberta
Centre for Child, Family and Community Research
Topic
Attachment
Introduction
Bowlby’s attachment theory is a theory of both psychopathology and normal socio-
emotional development. It is based on the idea that the early relationship that develops
between the infant and caregiver provides the foundation for later development.
Bowlby’s theory attempts to explain how the early relationship contributes to
psychological well-being or later psychopathology. The term attachment is used to
describe the affective bond that develops between an infant and caregiver.1,2 Attachment
is not a characteristic of the infant, nor is it a characteristic of the caregiver. Rather, it is a
pattern of emotional and behavioural interaction that develops over time as the infant and
caregiver interact, particularly in the context of the infant’s needs and bids for attention
and comfort.
Through repeated interactions with the same adults, an infant begins to recognize
caregivers and to anticipate the behaviour of the primary caregiver, usually the mother.
Bowlby described the infant as biologically predisposed to use the caregiver as a haven of
safety, or a secure base, while exploring the environment.1 So an infant who feels
threatened will turn to the caregiver for protection and comfort. The caregiver’s responses
to such bids help mould the attachment relationship into a pattern of interaction that
develops over time. By the end of the first year of life, the history of the relationship
between infant and caregiver allows the infant to begin to anticipate the caregiver’s
response to her bids for comfort, and to act in accordance with those expectations.
Another tenet of attachment theory is that from these first relationships, infants form
mental representations of the self, others and the relationship between self and other.
Bowlby called these representations inner working models.1 As the infant develops and
encounters the world beyond that first relationship, the inner working model guides her
behaviour and expectations in subsequent relationships as well.
Mothers who are sensitive and comforting when the infant makes bids for comfort will
have infants who continue to seek out the mother when distressed, and will be calmed by
contact with the mother. The infant’s inner working model will lead her to see others as
reliable and compassionate, and herself as worthy of this kind of attention. This pattern
has been labelled secure.1,2
Subject
Research has demonstrated that security of attachment during infancy predicts aspects of
social development during childhood and adolescence, such as empathy,3,4,5 social
competence5,6,7,8,9 and behaviour problems,10,11,12 with secure attachment predicting more
optimal developmental outcomes and insecure attachment predicting behaviour and
relationship difficulties. We have also found a secure attachment to be a major protective
factor for children who function in a competent fashion even in the face of adversity.13 In
addition, attachment relationships may have long-term effects on functioning by
influencing the course of biological development, including brain development. 14
Inner working models are carried forward from infancy throughout the life course and, as
noted above, they influence the individual’s expectations and behaviour in relationships,
including parenting in the next generation. Using the Adult Attachment Interview
(AAI),15 a number of studies have demonstrated that parent attachment organization is
related to infant attachment patterns. Parents with secure organizations are likely to have
infants who are securely attached with them, and parents with insecure organizations are
likely to have infants who are insecurely attached with them.16,17
Because of the many positive outcomes associated with a secure attachment, the
implications are clear. Design (and evaluate) prevention and intervention programs to
promote a secure parent-infant attachment relationship in order to improve developmental
outcomes of infants and children who are at risk for poor developmental outcomes and
prevent behaviour problems and psychopathology.
Problems
Attachment relationships, like all other aspects of development, do not exist in isolation
from their context. As noted above, caregivers who respond to their infant’s needs and
cues in a sensitive fashion are likely to develop a secure attachment relationship with
their infant. There are many personal (e.g. mother’s depression) and interpersonal (e.g.
violent relationship with spouse) factors that may make it more difficult for the caregiver
to respond to the infant in a sensitive and emotionally responsive fashion. In addition, a
host of environmental factors, such as chaotic living conditions, may interfere with the
developing attachment relationship, particularly when intervening with families from
high-risk populations who face multiple personal and environmental challenges. Many
programs were not equipped to deal with the problems of high-risk families.
Research Context
In 1995, van Ijzendoorn et al.18 conducted a review of 12 attachment interventions, and in
2000, Egeland and colleagues19 found a few more programs that had been implemented
and evaluated. In 2003, the Dutch investigators conducted another meta-analysis that
included 29 investigations designed to enhance attachment security. More recently, there
has been an increase of attachment-based prevention and intervention programs20.
There are basically two broad types of intervention programs designed to enhance the
quality of mother-infant attachment: (1) those that endeavour to help the parents become
more sensitive to infant cues; and (2) those that attempt to change parents’
representations of how they were cared for by their own parents. Many of the attachment
interventions fall into one of these two categories, while others combine the two
approaches and still others, such as Beckwith’s21 program emphasizing social support.
In a more recent investigation, van Zeijl and colleagues25 used the video feedback
procedure with a group of 1- to 3-year-old children who had high levels of externalizing
behaviour. The intervention was effective in decreasing overactive, oppositional, and
aggressive behaviour compared to the control group. Later analyses of these data by
Bakermans-Kranenburg and colleagues26 indicated that genetic differences moderated the
effects of intervention. Children with a certain genotype on the dopamine receptor gene
showed the largest decrease of externalizing behaviour in the cases where parents showed
the largest increase in the use of positive discipline. Findings that children’s susceptibility
to changes in their environment depends in part on genetic differences are very
provocative and hopefully will lead to more gene by environment studies in the area of
prevention and intervention in the early years.
Conclusions
The positive long-term developmental outcome associated with a secure parent-infant
attachment relationship provides an excellent rationale for implementing attachment-
based prevention programs early in life. Recognizing the significance of this early
relationship, however, has not resulted in a large number of attachment-based
interventions. A variety of early parent education and home visitation programs exist, but
very few have as their primary goal facilitating the development of a secure attachment
relationship. The results of the evaluation of existing attachment-based interventions are
encouraging, particularly the Dutch studies involving relatively low-risk samples. Based
on the findings of the Dutch studies, it appears that attachment-based interventions that
focus on enhancing sensitivity are likely to be successful with parents who are motivated
to learn ways of responding with their difficult infants. For more high-risk families, it
appears that more comprehensive, long-term interventions are necessary.
Implications
Based on attachment theory and research, as well as results from evaluations of existing
attachment-based interventions, it would be recommended to incorporate attachment-
based intervention/prevention programs into existing home visitation and parent
education programs for high-risk families of young children, as well as investigate new
approaches for changing parents’ cognitive representation of their attachment with their
parents. Much is known about parent-child interaction, parental characteristics and
beliefs, and contextual factors that are antecedents of a secure attachment relationship.
This knowledge needs to be applied in the development of the next generation of
attachment interventions. The needs and strengths of high-risk families are highly varied.
Intervention programs must be designed to meet the unique needs of each family as well
as to take advantage of their strengths.
REFERENCES
1. Bowlby J. Attachment. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Basic Books; 1982. Attachment
and loss; vol 1.
2. Ainsworth MDS, Blehar M, Waters E, Wall S. Patterns of attachment: A
Psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates; 1978.
3. Kestenbaum R, Farber E, Ellen A, Sroufe LA. Individual differences in empathy
among preschoolers: Relation to attachment history. New Directions for Child
Development 1989;44:51-64.
4. Oppenheim D, Sagi A, Lamb ME. Infant-adult attachments on the kibbutz and
their relation to socioemotional development four years later. In: Chess S, Hertzig
ME, eds. Annual progress in child psychiatry and child development, 1989.
Philadelphia, Pa: Brunner/Mazel Inc.; 1990:92-106.
5. Sroufe LA. Infant-caregiver attachment and patterns of adaptation in preschool:
The roots of maladaptation and competence. Minnesota Symposia on Child
Psychology 1983;16:41-83.
6. Elicker J, Englund M, Sroufe LA. Predicting peer competence and peer
relationships in childhood from early parent-child relationships. In: Parke RD,
Ladd GW, eds. Family-Peer Relationships: Modes of Linkage. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1992:77-106.
7. Sroufe LA, Egeland B, Carlson EA, Collins WA. The development of the person:
The Minnesota study of risk and adaptation from birth to adulthood. New York,
NY: Guilford Press; 2005.
8. Sroufe LA, Egeland B, Carlson EA. One social world: The integrated
development of parent-child and peer relationships. In: Collins WA, Laursen B,
eds. Relationships as developmental contexts. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates; 1999: 241-262. Minnesota symposium on child psychology; vol 30.
9. Thompson RA. Early attachment and later development: Familiar questions, new
answers. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, eds. Handbook of attachment: Theory,
research, and clinical applications. 2nd Ed. New York: Guilford Press; 2008: 348-
365.
10. Egeland B, Carlson B. Attachment and psychopathology. In: Atkinson L,
Goldberg S, eds. Attachment issues in psychopathology and intervention.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2004:27-48.
11. Erickson MF, Sroufe LA, Egeland B. The relationship between quality of
attachment and behavior problems in preschool in a high-risk sample.
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 1985;50(1-2):147-
166.
12. Lyons-Ruth K, Easterbrooks MA, Cibelli CD. Infant attachment strategies, infant
mental lag, and maternal depressive symptoms: Predictors of internalizing and
externalizing problems at age 7. Developmental Psychology 1997;33(4):681-692.
13. Yates TM, Egeland B, Sroufe LA. Rethinking resilience: A developmental
process perspective. In: Luthar SS, eds. Resilience and vulnerability: Adaptation
in the context of childhood Adversities. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
Press; 2003: 243-266.
Egeland B. Attachment-based intervention and prevention programs for young children. Rev ed. In:
Tremblay RE, Barr RG, Peters RDeV, Boivin M, eds. Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development
[online]. Montreal, Quebec: Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development; 2009:1-8. Available
at: http://www.child-encyclopedia.com/documents/EgelandANGxp_rev.pdf. Accessed [insert date].
Copyright © 2004-2009
This article is funded by the Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development
(CEECD), the Strategic Knowledge Cluster on ECD (SKC-ECD) and the Alberta
Centre for Child, Family and Community Research
Topic
Attachment
Introduction
There is growing prospective and retrospective evidence linking the quality of early
infant-caregiver attachment relationships with later social and emotional outcomes.1,2
Four types of infant attachment have been described. Secure infant-caregiver attachment
is believed to develop when the caregiver responds to the child’s distress in a sensitive
manner. Insecure-avoidant infant-caregiver attachment presumably results from the
caregiver consistently responding to the child’s distress in ways that are rejecting.
Insecure-resistant infant-caregiver attachment is thought to occur when the caregiver
responds to the child’s distress in ways that are inconsistent and unpredictable. Insecure-
disorganized infant-caregiver attachment evidently develops when the caregiver displays
unusual and ultimately frightening behaviours in the presence of the child. Of the four
patterns of infant-caregiver attachment (secure, avoidant, resistant, disorganized), the
disorganized classification has been identified as a powerful childhood risk for later
socio-emotional maladjustment and psychopathology.2,3
Subject
Children who have disorganized attachment with their primary attachment figure have
been shown to be vulnerable to stress, have problems with regulation and control of
negative emotions, and display oppositional, hostile-aggressive behaviours, and coercive
styles of interaction.2,3 They may exhibit low self-esteem, internalizing and externalizing
problems in the early school years, poor peer interactions, unusual or bizarre behaviour in
the classroom, high teacher ratings of dissociative behaviour and internalizing symptoms
in middle childhood, high levels of teacher-rated social and behavioural difficulties in
class, low mathematics attainment, and impaired formal operational skills.3 They may
show high levels of overall psychopathology at 17 years.3 Disorganized attachment with
a primary attachment figure is over-represented in groups of children with clinical
problems and those who are victims of maltreatment.1,2,3 A majority of children with
early disorganized attachment with their primary attachment figure during infancy go on
to develop significant social and emotional maladjustment and psychopathology.3,4 Thus,
an attachment-based intervention should focus on preventing and/or reducing
disorganized attachment.
more effective than interventions without such characteristics. One shortcoming of the
Bakermans-Kranenburg et al.8 meta-analysis is that it did not address the question of
whether attachment-based interventions focusing on caregiver sensitivity have a
significant impact on preventing disorganized attachment.
Conclusions
In summary, attachment-based interventions to date have focused mainly on precursors of
organized types of attachment rather than on precursors of disorganized attachment,
reflecting the fact that the extent of negative sequelae of disorganized child-caregiver
attachment has only recently been identified, as have precursors of disorganized
attachment. Given the high base rate of organized but insecure (avoidant or resistant)
attachment in the general population, it might not be realistic or even necessary to focus
interventions on preventing or eliminating avoidant or resistant attachment, unless the
infant is symptomatic. On the other hand, a large proportion of infants who develop
insecure-disorganized attachment with their primary caregiver go on to develop
significant social and emotional maladjustment and psychopathology. Thus, clinically,
insecure-disorganized child-caregiver attachment appears to be the most significant type
of attachment that requires intervention. The direct focus on antecedents of disorganized
attachment, such as atypical caregiver behaviours, represents a promising direction for
future research.
Implications
Research findings suggest that an attachment-based intervention should focus on
improving caregiver sensitivity to promote secure child-caregiver attachment and the
positive social and emotional outcomes associated with secure attachment. However, an
exclusive focus on improving caregiver sensitivity may be neither sufficient nor effective
in preventing or reducing the most clinically relevant type of insecure attachment, i.e.
disorganized attachment. Recent research findings suggest that a focus on reducing
atypical caregiver behaviours might be a promising direction to reduce disorganized
REFERENCES
Benoit D. Efficacy of attachment-based interventions. Rev ed. In: Tremblay RE, Barr RG, Peters RDeV,
eds. Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development [online]. Montreal, Quebec: Centre of Excellence for
Early Childhood Development; 2009:1-5. Available at: http://www.child-
encyclopedia.com/documents/BenoitANGxp_rev-Attachment.pdf. Accessed [insert date].
Copyright © 2005-2009
This article is funded by the Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development
(CEECD), the Strategic Knowledge Cluster on ECD (SKC-ECD) and the Alberta
Centre for Child, Family and Community Research
Topic
Attachment
Introduction
Attachment theory provides a powerful perspective for investigating the nature of the
relationship between experiences of caregiving and developmental outcome.1,2,3
Attachment organization in infants and toddlers has been linked with future adaptation.4,3
Therefore, it is important to promote secure attachment relationships between caregivers
and their offspring. In the last decade, investigators have increasingly directed their
efforts toward understanding and modifying attachment relationships in high-risk and
psychiatric populations.5,6,7 Dozier, Egeland, and Benoit have been at the forefront of
theoretical and research initiatives designed to prevent insecure relationships and
promote secure attachment relationships in young children. In these articles, the authors
review the literature on attachment-based interventions and highlight key empirical
findings regarding the efficacy of prevention and intervention initiatives.
1
Comments on original paper published by Mary Dozier in 2004. To have access to this article, contact us
at [email protected].
2
Comments on original paper published by Byron Egeland in 2004. To have access to this article, contact
us at [email protected].
3
Comments on original paper published by Diane Benoit in 2005. To have access to this article, contact us
at [email protected].
Egeland emphasizes that security of attachment during infancy has been consistently
shown to predict aspects of social development during childhood, with secure attachment
relating to more optimal developmental outcomes and insecure attachment predicting
socioemotional maladaptation. Egeland further states that attachment relationships may
have long-term effects on the course of biological development. Consistent with Dozier,
Egeland therefore concludes that it is critical to design and evaluate programs to promote
a secure parent-infant attachment relationship. Like Dozier, Egeland also discusses two
broad types of intervention strategies designed to foster secure attachment relationships:
1) strategies that target parental sensitivity; and 2) strategies that strive to alter parental
representation with respect to their own histories of caregiving. A central tenet of
attachment theory is that the early relationships between infants and their caregivers lead
to the formation of mental representations of the self, others, and of the self in relation to
others. Therefore, the focus of interventions on modifying these mental representations or
targeting caregiver behaviour assumes importance. Egeland proffers an important caveat
to findings that support the utilization of short-term interventions that target modifying
parental sensitivity. Specifically, Egeland cautions that although these programs are
successful with relatively low-risk samples, more comprehensive and long-term
interventions are likely to be necessary with high-risk families.
do not have evidence that they would be as effective, or effective at all, with higher-risk
populations.
In fact, studies recently conducted at Mt. Hope Family Center have offered compelling
evidence that preventive interventions that target maternal representations of relationships
are very effective in promoting attachment security. In the first investigation, toddler
offspring of mothers who had experienced a major depressive disorder since the birth of
the child were randomly assigned to an attachment-theory informed intervention or to a
community standard condition. A group of non-depressed mothers served as a normative
comparison group. Although at baseline toddlers with depressed mothers evidenced
higher rates of insecurity than did toddlers with non-depressed mothers, at the completion
of the intervention the group that received the attachment-theory informed intervention
had significantly higher rates of security than did participants who received the
community standard intervention. Importantly, rates of security in the mother-child dyads
that received the attachment-theory informed intervention did not differ from those
present in the dyads where mothers were not depressed.5 For toddlers who participated in
the attachment intervention, there was also a greater maintenance of secure attachment
organization among those who were initially secure, as well as a greater shift from
insecure to secure attachment groupings. Similarly compelling results have been obtained
with maltreated infants, where baseline rates of insecurity were over 90% and where
post-intervention attachment security did not differ from that of non-maltreated infants.
Maltreated infants randomized to the community standard condition continued to
evidence extremely high rates of insecure attachment consistent with that present at
baseline.9 Interestingly, in the latter preventive intervention, a didactic and more
behaviourally focused intervention was just as effective as one dealing with maternal
representations in promoting secure attachment. Conversely, in the evaluation of a
preventive intervention for maltreated preschool-aged children, only an intervention that
targeted maternal representations resulted in improvement in child representations of
caregivers and of self.10 Thus, the issue of preferred intervention strategy appears to be
far from resolved and caution must be exercised in bringing premature closure to this
issue.
effectiveness. Only then will we truly know how best to promote secure attachment and
what approaches may be most effective for a given population.
REFERENCES
Toth SL. Attachment-based interventions: Comments on Dozier, Egeland, and Benoit. In: Tremblay RE,
Barr RG, Peters RDeV, eds. Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development [online]. Montreal, Quebec:
Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development; 2005:1-4. Available at: http://www.child-
encyclopedia.com/documents/TothANGxp.pdf. Accessed [insert date].
Copyright © 2005
Topic
Attachment
Introduction
Since Bowlby and Ainsworth formulated attachment theory,1,2 many early intervention
programs have been launched that aim to promote secure child-parent attachment
relationships. Usually, these intervention programs are designed to enhance parental
sensitivity, the ability to accurately perceive children’s attachment signals, and the ability
to respond to these signals in a prompt and appropriate manner.2 The ultimate goal of
these interventions is to turn insecure-avoidant (A) and insecure-resistant (C) attachment
relationships into secure (B) child-parent attachment relationships.2 In a few programs,
the intervention is not only directed at sensitive parental behaviour but also at maternal
mental attachment representations, as in the STEEP (Steps Toward Effective Enjoyable
Parenting) program described by Egeland. According to Benoit, with the discovery of a
new insecure attachment category, disorganized attachment (D),3 new challenges arose
for attachment-based interventions. Because of the negative impact of, in particular,
disorganized attachment on child outcomes, attachment-based interventions should not,
or not only, focus on the empirically derived determinants of organized (A, B, and C)
attachment, such as parental (in)secure mental attachment representations and sensitive
behaviour (see Dozier), but also on the determinants of disorganized (D) attachment.
Empirical studies have found evidence for Main and Hesse’s4 model that parents’
unresolved loss or trauma is linked to children’s insecure-disorganized attachment
through frightening or frightened parental behaviour. However, there are as yet no
reported outcomes from interventions that have directly targeted frightening behaviours.
As a first step, it is important to evaluate the effects of attachment-based interventions
that include infant attachment disorganization as an outcome measure (see below), but in
1
Comments on original paper published by Diane Benoit in 2005. To have access to this article, contact us
at [email protected].
2
Comments on original paper published by Mary Dozier in 2004. To have access to this article, contact us
at [email protected].
3
Comments on original paper published by Byron Egeland in 2004. To have access to this article, contact
us at [email protected].
the next step interventions that are specifically designed to prevent insecure disorganized
attachment should be tested.
Egeland does not address the follow-up of this first meta-analysis on parental sensitivity
and attachment, nor does he cover the question of how insecure disorganized attachments
might be prevented. Recently, 88 interventions on maternal sensitivity and infant security
in 70 studies were included in a thoroughly extended and updated quantitative meta-
analysis.6 This meta-analysis showed that interventions that specifically focused on
promoting sensitive parental behaviour appeared to be rather effective in changing
insensitive parenting as well as infant attachment insecurity. One of the conclusions of
this series of meta-analyses, also illustrated in the title of the paper “Less is more,” was
that interventions with a modest number of intervention sessions (up to 16) appeared to
be more effective than interventions with larger numbers of sessions, and this was true for
clinical as well as for non-clinical groups.6 This diverges from Egeland’s conclusion that
more comprehensive, long-term interventions are necessary for high-risk families.
Although this might be true for other intervention goals, such as helping high-risk
mothers to cope with adversity or the daily hassles surrounding the birth of a child, the
recent meta-analysis shows that for sensitivity and attachment, the most effective way is
to provide attachment-based interventions in a modest number of sensitivity-focused
sessions.
Dozier elaborates on parental state of mind as one of the strongest predictors of infant
attachment. Parents who are able to reflect on their own childhood experiences in a
coherent way are said to have autonomous states of mind. When parents are not coherent
in discussing their own attachment experiences, they are said to have non-autonomous
states of mind. Here, the work of Main comes to the fore: the Adult Attachment
Interview7 enables coders to distinguish reliably between parents with insecure
(dismissing, preoccupied or unresolved) states of mind and parents with secure
(autonomous) attachment representations. Several empirical studies and a meta-analysis8
have found that insecure parents usually have insecurely attached infants and secure
parents tend to have secure children. Dozier remarks that some attachment-based
interventions are designed to target parent state of mind as a means of changing infant
attachment, although many other interventions try to change parental sensitivity alone.
In contrast to the first two authors, Benoit explicitly describes the challenge of the
discovery of insecure-disorganized attachment for the field of attachment-based
interventions. At the beginning of her paper, she notices that of the four patterns of infant
attachment (secure, avoidant, resistant, disorganized), the disorganized classification has
been identified as a powerful childhood risk for later psychopathology. She continues
with the observation that for disorganized attachment the focus of the intervention should
not be parental sensitivity, as she notes that sensitivity is not linked to disorganized
attachment. Nevertheless, a meta-analysis showed that interventions with a focus on
sensitivity were successful in reducing or preventing attachment disorganization9 (see
below), and we noted that the explanation for this finding might be that parents become
more focused in the interaction with their child, and thereby less prone to dissociative
processes in the presence of the child. According to Benoit, one recently identified
pathway to disorganized attachment is children’s exposure to specific forms of aberrant
caregiving behaviours that are referred to as “atypical.” Therefore, Benoit concludes that
attachment-based interventions should focus both on improving parental sensitivity (to
promote secure attachment) and on reducing or eliminating atypical parental behaviours
(to prevent or reduce disorganized attachment). Benoit’s own study, which demonstrated
the effects of a brief, focused, behavioural parent training intervention in reducing
atypical caregiver behaviours, is a first example of much needed studies designed to
reduce frightening/frightened or atypical parental behaviours. It would be exciting to
learn whether this type of intervention was indeed successful in preventing or reducing
disorganized attachment.
several conclusions for clinical practice and services can be drawn. It has been
empirically proven that interventions can successfully enhance parental sensitivity and
promote secure attachment in children, in particular when the intervention is relatively
brief (up to 16 sessions), behaviourally oriented, focuses on sensitivity only (instead of
broader interventions including social support, etc.), and starts after the infant’s age of six
months. However, long-term and broadly-focused support of multi-problem families in
coping with their daily hassles may be needed in order to enable them to focus on
sensitivity subsequently.6 The 2003 meta-analysis also found an important dose-response
relation between the success of the intervention on parental sensitivity and its impact on
children’s attachment security: only interventions that brought about substantial effects
on sensitivity succeeded in changing attachment insecurity.6
Recently, a narrative review and quantitative meta-analysis has been completed including
15 preventive interventions that included infant disorganized attachment as an outcome
measure.9 Although the overall effect of all interventions combined was not significant,
some interventions did succeed in preventing disorganized attachment in children. These
interventions shared the following characteristics: They started after six months of the
infant’s age rather than before six months; they were sensitivity-focused; and they
involved samples with children at risk rather than at-risk parents.9
REFERENCES
1. Bowlby J. Attachment. New York, NY: Basic Books; 1982. Attachment and loss.
2nd ed; vol 1.
2. Ainsworth MDS, Blehar MC, Waters E, Wall S. Patterns of attachment: a
psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates; 1978.
3. Main M, Solomon J. Procedures for identifying infants as
disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth Strange Situation. In: Greenberg
MT, Cicchetti D, Cummings EM, eds. Attachment in the preschool years: Theory,
research, and intervention. Chicago, Ill: University of Chicago Press; 1990:121-
160.
4. Main M, Hesse E. Parents’ unresolved traumatic experiences are related to infant
disorganized attachment status: Is frightened and/or frightening parental behavior
the linking mechanism? In: Greenberg MT, Cichetti D, Cummings E, eds.
Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention. Chicago,
Ill: University of Chicago Press; 1990:161-182.
5. Van IJzendoorn MH, Juffer F, Duyvesteyn MGC. Breaking the intergenerational
cycle of insecure attachment: A review of the effects of attachment-based
interventions on maternal sensitivity and infant security. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry 1995;36(2):225-248.
6. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH, Juffer F. Less is more: Meta-
analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood.
Psychological Bulletin 2003;129(2):195-215.
7. Main M, Goldwyn R. Adult attachment rating and classification system.
Berkeley, Calif: University of California. Unpublished manuscript.
8. Van IJzendoorn MH. Adult attachment representations, parental responsiveness,
and infant attachment: A meta-analysis on the predictive validity of the Adult
Attachment Interview. Psychological Bulletin 1995;117(3):387-403.
9. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH, Juffer F. Disorganized infant
attachment and preventive interventions: A review and meta-analysis. Infant
Mental Health Journal. In press.
10. Solomon J, George C. The place of disorganization in attachment theory: Linking
classic observations with contemporary findings. In: Solomon J, George C, eds.
Attachment disorganization. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1999:3-32.
11. Juffer F, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH. The importance of
parenting in the development of disorganized attachment: evidence from a
preventive intervention study in adoptive families. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry 2005;46(3):263-274.
12. Juffer F, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH. Enhancing children’s
socioemotional development: A review of intervention studies. In: Teti DM, ed.
Handbook of Research Methods in Developmental Science. Oxford, United
Kingdom: Blackwell Publishers; 2004:213-232.
13. Juffer F, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH. Introduction and
outline of the VIPP and VIPP-R program. In: Juffer F, Bakermans-Kranenburg
MJ, van IJzendoorn MH, eds. Attachment-based intervention with video-feedback
and biographical discussion: The Leiden VIPP and VIPP-R program. Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. In press.
14. Juffer F, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH, eds. Attachment-
based intervention with video-feedback and biographical discussion: The Leiden
VIPP and VIPP-R program. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. In press.
Juffer F, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH. Supporting families to build secure attachment
relationships: Comments on Benoit, Dozier, and Egeland. In: Tremblay RE, Barr RG, Peters RDeV, eds.
Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development [online]. Montreal, Quebec: Centre of Excellence for
Early Childhood Development; 2005:1-7. Available at: http://www.child-
encyclopedia.com/documents/JufferANGxp.pdf. Accessed [insert date].
Copyright © 2005
Topic
Attachment
Child care (0-5 years)
Introduction
Whether and how non-maternal child-care experience affects children’s development
have been of long-standing interest to parents, policymakers and developmental scholars.
Ever since Bowlby1 promulgated attachment theory, thinking derived from it has led
some to expect day care, especially when initiated in the earliest years of life, to
undermine the security of infant-parent attachment relationships. To some, this was
because day care involved the infant’s separation from mother (or other principle
caregiver), as separation from the attachment figure was inherently stressful. Separation
could also undermine the mother’s own capacity to provide sensitive care, the primary
determinant of security, thereby fostering insecurity indirectly (i.e.,
separationÆinsensitivityÆinsecurity). A final reason for anticipating a link between day
care and attachment security was because security reflected general emotional well-being,
so adverse effects of day care in infancy would manifest themselves as insecure
attachment.
Background
Early research on the link between day care and attachment, often carried out on children
3-5 years of age, provided no compelling evidence to support the claim that day care
undermined security.2 But by the mid-1980s, studies carried out on much younger
children began to chronicle links between day care and insecurity as measured in the
Strange Situation Procedure (SSP) (e.g., Barglow, Vaughn & Molitar3). This led
Belsky4,5,6 to conclude that infant day care, especially that initiated on a full- or near full-
time basis beginning in the first year of life,7 was a “risk factor” in the development of
insecure attachment in infancy (and of aggression and disobedience in 3-8 year olds).
This conclusion did not go unchallenged. One criticism was that the apparent influence of
early and extensive day care on insecurity was the result of other explanatory factors
(e.g., family income) not adequately accounted for in existing research.8 Another was that
(unmeasured) poor quality care and not timing and quantity of care was the influential
factor.9 And a third was that independent behavior displayed by day care children not
particularly stressed by the SSP ̶ due to their familiarity with separation ̶ was
misconstrued as avoidant behavior, leading to erroneous assessments of children as
insecure-avoidant.10
Research Questions
All agreed, however, that more research was needed to illuminate the conditions under
which early day care did and did not undermine ̶ or enhance ̶ attachment security.
Considered especially important was (a) taking into account confounding child, parent
and family background factors that could be responsible for any putative child care
effects; (a) distinguishing and disentangling potential effects of distinctive features of the
child-care experience, particularly quality, quantity and type of care (e.g., center-based
vs. home-based); and (b) determining whether day care was associated with less
separation distress in the SSP or independent behavior was mischaracterized as avoidant
behavior.
Recent Research
The NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (SECCYD), launched in
1991 in the US, sought to address these issues and many others.11 It followed more than
1300 children from birth through the primary-school years12 and into adolescence,13
while administering SSP assessments at 15 and 36 months.
After taking into account a host of potentially confounding background factors, results
proved strikingly consistent with the risk-factor conclusion14 ̶ even though the opposite is
implied by many writers.15,16 Typically emphasized is that no single feature of the day
care experience in and of itself ̶ quantity, type or quality of care ̶ predicted attachment
security, seeming to suggest no effect of day care on attachment security. Yet what the
findings actually revealed was a “dual-risk” phenomenon.17 Although the strongest
predictor of insecurity at 15 months of age was, as expected, insensitive mothering
(observed at ages 6 and 15 months), this effect was amplified if any one of three distinct
child-care conditions characterized the child’s experience across the first 15 months of
life: (a) averaging more than 10 hours per week in any type of care, irrespective of
quality; (b) enrolment in more than a single child-care arrangement; and (c) exposure to
low quality care. The first two amplifying conditions applied to most children being
studied. But only the first, quantity of care, also contributed to the prediction of
attachment insecurity at 36 months,18 again in interaction with insensitive mothering. Just
as important was evidence that infants with extensive day care experience (a) were not
less stressed in the SSP than other infants (see also19) and that (b) putatively independent
behavior was not misconstrued as avoidant behavior.14
Two other reasonably large-sample studies yield results that are at odds with those of the
US study. In one investigation of more than 700 Israeli infants, Sagi and associates20
found that “center-care, in and of itself, adversely increased the likelihood of infants
developing insecure attachment to their mothers as compared with infants who were
either in maternal care, individual nonparental care with a relative, individual nonparental
care with a paid caregiver, or family day-care.” Additional results suggested it was “the
poor quality of center-care and the high infant-caregiver ratio that accounted for this
Research Gaps
It remains unclear why results from different locales produce variable findings. It could
well involve the broader, national child care systems in which day care is embedded.
Cross-national research seems called for.
Characteristics of children themselves, perhaps especially their genetic make up, also
merits further consideration. After all, ever more evidence indicates that children vary
substantially in their susceptibility to environmental influences,22 including day care23
with some proving more developmentally malleable than others.
Conclusions
After decades of debate and study, findings from the largest studies of day care and
attachment compellingly discredit any claim that “no relation exists between day care and
attachment.” Also disconfirmed are assertions that the SSP is methodologically unsuited
for evaluating effects of day care or that, at least in the US, adverse effects of day care are
simply a function of poor quality care. Nevertheless, the fact that results of three large-
scale studies carried out in different locales vary substantially should make it clear that
there are probably no inevitable effects of day care on attachment. Effects appear
contingent on the societal context in which day care is experienced.
Implications
The fact that detected effects of day care on attachment security vary substantially by
national context means that it is precarious to draw strong inferences from attachment
theory as to what the effect of day care will be. Ultimately, day care is a multi-
dimensional phenomenon, so questions such as “is day care good for infants (or young
children)?” are too simplistic. Quality, type, timing and quantity of care must be
distinguished and effects of these features of the child care may vary as a function of the
larger familial, community, societal and cultural context in which child care occurs. Not
to be forgotten in any evaluation of the effects of day care are humanitarian
considerations: What, not only, do mothers, fathers, policymakers and society more
generally want, but what do children want?
REFERENCES
1. Bowlby J. Attachment and Loss. New York, NY: Basic Books; 1969. Attachment.
Vol 1.
2. Belsky J, Steinberg L. The effects of day care: A critical review. Child
Development 1978;49(4):929-949.
3. Barglow P, Vaughn B, Molitor N. Effects of maternal absence due to employment
on the quality of infant-mother attachment in a low risk sample. Child
Development 1987;58(4):945-954.
4. Belsky J. Infant day care: A cause for concern? Zero to Three 1986;6(4):1-7.
5. Belsky J. The “effects” of infant day care reconsidered. Early Childhood
Research Quarterly 1988;3(3):235-272.
6. Belsky J. Emanuel Miller Lecture: Developmental risks (still) associated with
early child care. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2001;42(7):845-859.
7. Belsky J, Rovine M. Nonmaternal care in the first year of life and the security of
infant-parent attachment. Child Development 1988;59(1):157-167.
8. Scarr S, Phillips D, McCartney K. Facts, fantasies, and the future of child care in
the United States. Psychological Science 1990;1(1):26-35.
9. Phillips D, McCartney K, Scarr S, Howes C. Selective review of infant day care
research: A cause for concern. Zero to Three 1987;7:18-21.
10. Clarke-Stewart KA. Infant day care: Maligned or malignant? American
Psychologist 1989;44(2):266-273.
11. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, ed. Child Care and Child
Development: Results of the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth
Development. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2005.
12. Belsky J, Vandell D, Burchinal M, Clarke-Stewart KA, McCartney K, Owen MT
and The NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. Are There Long-term
Effects of Early Child Care? Child Development 2007;78(2):681-701.
13. Vandell DL, Belsky J, Burchinal M, Steinberg L, Vandergrift N, the NICHD
Early Child Care Research Network. Do Effects of Early Child Care Extend to
Age 15 Years? Child Development. In press.
14. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network The effects of infant child care on
infant-mother attachment security: Results of the NICHD Study of Early Child
Care. Child Development 1997;68(5):860-879.
15. Allhusen VD, Clarke-Stewart KA, Miner JL. Childcare in the United States:
Characteristics and consequences. In: Melhuish E, Petrogiannis K, eds. Early
Childhood Care and Education: International Perspectives. London, UK:
Routledge; 2008:7-26.
16. Love M, Harrison L, Sagi-Schwartz A, van Ijzendoorn MH, Ross C, Ungerer JA,
Raikes H, Brady-Smith C, Boller K, Brooks-Gun JC, Jill K, Ellen E, Paulsell D,
Chazen-Cohen R. Child Care Quality Matters: How Conclusions May Vary with
Context. Child Development 2003;74(4):1021-1033.
17. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network The effects of infant child care on
infant-mother attachment security: Results of the NICHD Study of Early Child
Care. Child Development 1997;68(5):876.
18. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. Child care and family predictors of
preschool attachment and stability from infancy. Developmental Psychology
2001;37(6):847-862.
19. Belsky J, Braungart J. Are insecure-avoidant infants with extensive day-care
experience less stressed by and more independent in the strange situation? Child
Development 1991;62(3):567-571.
20. Sagi A, Koren-Karie N, Gini M, Ziv Y, Joels T. (2002). Shedding further light on
the effects of various types and quality of early child care on infant-mother
attachment relationship: The Haifa Study of Early Child Care. Child Development
2002;73(4):1166.
21. Harrison LJ, Ungerer JA. Maternal employment and infant-mother attachment
security at 12 months postpartum. Developmental Psychology 2002;38(5):758-
773.
22. Belsky J, Bakermans-Kranenburg M, van Ijzendoorn M. For Better and for worse:
Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Current Directions in
Psychological Science 2007;16(6):305-309.
23. Pluess M, Belsky J. Differential susceptibility to rearing experience: the case of
childcare. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2009:50(4):396-404.
Belsky J. Early day care and infant-mother attachment security. In: Tremblay RE, Barr RG, Peters RDeV,
Boivin M, eds. Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development [online]. Montreal, Quebec: Centre of
Excellence for Early Childhood Development; 2009:1-6. Available at: http://www.child-
encyclopedia.com/documents/BelskyANGxp-Attachment.pdf. Accessed [insert date].
Copyright © 2009
This article is funded by the Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development
(CEECD), the Strategic Knowledge Cluster on ECD (SKC-ECD) and the Alberta
Centre for Child, Family and Community Research