A Framework For The Implementation of Task-Based Instruction
A Framework For The Implementation of Task-Based Instruction
A Framework For The Implementation of Task-Based Instruction
CITATIONS READS
506 4,154
1 author:
Peter Skehan
St Mary's University, Twickenham
56 PUBLICATIONS 5,202 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Peter Skehan on 24 April 2015.
components of definition that are offered here (task completion and task
outcome assessment), but the point is that such a concept of task has sufficient
focus to enable it to be a viable component for what goes on in classrooms '
Long and Crookes (1991) discuss a further quality of tasks that they have a
clear pedagogic relationship to out-of-class language use, in that needs analysis
should clarify how students will need to use language in real-life, and task design
should ensure that classroom tasks bear a developmental relationship to such
non-classroom activity For this article, such a quality is regarded as desirable,
but difficult to obtain, and the more restrictive approach to characterizing tasks
will be used Similarly, there will be no discussion of the interesting possibility
that students could be involved in the negotiation of which tasks are used, and
how they are used (Breen 1987) The approach taken here is to try to develop a
framework which will help the teacher to better understand the tasks s/he is
using, and to sequence and implement them more effectively
One can, on the basis of these task characteristics, identify strong and weak
forms of the task-based approach A strong form would argue that tasks should
be the unit of language teaching, and that everything else should be subsidiary
In this view, the need to transact tasks is seen as adequate to drive forward
language development, as though second language acquisition is the result of the
same process of interaction as first language acquisition (Wells 1985) A weak
form of task-based instruction would claim that tasks are a vital part of language
instruction, but that they are embedded in a more complex pedagogic context
They are necessary, but may be preceded by focused instruction, and after use,
may be followed by focused instruction which is contingent on task per-
formance This version of task-based instruction is clearly very close to general
communicative language teaching It could also be compatible with a traditional
presentation, practice, production sequence, only with production based on
tasks (as defined above), rather than more stilted and guided production
activities (Littlewood 1981)
If we turn next to research studies into task-based learning, a range of
approaches are evident Candhn (1987) discusses criteria by which tasks may
be analysed, basing the categones used on an essentially data-free account of
task properties, an approach developed by Nunan (1989) and Skehan (1992)
Pica, Kanagy, and Falodun (1993) take a slightly different perspective, relying
on research studies more directly They analyse tasks in terms of interactional
patterns and requirements, (i e how involved each participant needs to be), as
well as the sorts of goals that underlie the tasks-to-be-transacted (e g one-way
vs two-way)
A number of studies have been more specific in their analyses of tasks,
positing particular contrasts or dimensions as the basis for characterizing tasks
Prabhu (1987) argues in support of reasoning-gap tasks, Duff (1986) contrasts
divergent and convergent tasks, arguing that the latter engage acquisitional
processes more effectively, and Berwick (1993) contrasts two dimensions
experiential-expository, and didactive-collaborative There have also been
studies of the participants within tasks such as Yule, Powers, and Macdonald
40 TASK-BASED INSTRUCTION
1
(1992) on 'hearer effects and Plough and Gass (1993) on participant (and task)
familiarity Brown, Anderson, Shilcock, and Yule (1984) have also investigated
various task design features, in an attempt to establish task difficulty on an
empirical basis They propose that static tasks (e g description) are easier than
dynamic tasks (e g narration), which in turn are easier than abstract tasks (e g
opinion giving), and that the number of elements, participants, and relationships
in a task makes it more difficult There have also been studies of processing
influences on tasks Tarone (1985) has shown that attention to form has a clear
effect on accuracy of performance Ellis (1987) reported an interaction between
the engagement of planned discourse and different forms of the past tense under
different task conditions Crookes (1989) reported greater complexity and
lexical variety for tasks done under a planning time condition, but, interestingly,
no greater accuracy Foster and Skehan (1994) report an interaction between
opportunity to plan and task type These research studies are individually
revealing, but do not currently provide the basis for more general perspectives
on task-based instruction They do, though, play a part in the wider framework
which is proposed below
benefit from 'input to intake' hints from a coach, along the lines of 'Think about
your footwork' In language, too, attention can be directed without this direction
necessarily involving detailed explanation of rules The issue, centrally, is that
limited capacity is being focused in the area which is most helpful, with the
possibility that what then happens triggers implicit processes Generalizations
in data which might otherwise be missed become more accessible with the
structuring of the learning experience that is involved
inaccurate on this basis relates to how well established the particular part of the
lnterlanguage system is But it is also possible that inaccuracy is the result of the
competence-performance relationship, and of the way in which communicative
pressure has led to an error being made which, under other circumstances,
would not be a lapse, in Corder's (1981) terms And, of course, we cannot
ignore the possibility that 'inaccuracy' may itself be targeted as desirable, for
whatever reasons the learner is motivated by (Trevise and Noyau 1984)'
The reverse side of this coin is to consider what promotes accurate language
use Clearly, the use of well-integrated aspects of the lnterlanguage system will
be helpful here, I e a sort of conservative communication strategy, in which
what is well-known is used, and what is not is avoided (Schachter 1974)
Learners who dislike risk-taking will, presumably, be drawn to accuracy
because of a reluctance to use language they are not sure of A similar effect will
result from a greater concern, on the part of the learner, to be correct, to
conform to target language norms, and to value them as important But other
factors are also likely to have an impact, such as the effectiveness with which
attentional resources are mobilized, and the processing capacity which is
available given other aspects of communicative pressure, I e the more attention
is diverted elsewhere, the less attention is available for form and accuracy (Van
Patten 1990, Van Patten and Cadierno 1993)
In this regard, we need to turn to the next goal that has been proposed for
language learningcomplexity/restructuring Restructunng is concerned with
the process by which the lnterlanguage system becomes more complex,
elaborate, and structured (McLaughhn 1990), maybe more efficient and less
circumlocuitous in communication (Cheng 1985), more consistent with input
data, and more native-like (Cook 1994) It requires a learner who explicitly
accepts such developments as goals and who is driven, by whatever means, to
achieve them More complex lnterlanguage systems are desirable since they
reflect acquisition having taken place, and will enable a greater degree of
acceptance as a speaker of the language concerned Equally important, such IL
systems should enable greater precision in communication (Swain 1985), and
greater communicative efficiency in the face of difficult performance circum-
stances They should also make it more likely that more complex ideas will be
expressed effectively (Swain, in press)
If we examine why restructuring may to some extent fail to occur a number of
reasons present themselves There may be a lack of interest on the part of the
learner either in the goal of becoming more native-like or simply in making the
effort to change and re-organize an lnterlanguage system There may also be
impoverished input (or instruction), such that necessary conditions for restruc-
tunng are impaired But there might also be problems even if the above two
influences are not present For example, learners may prefer not to take risks,
relying on less elaborate lnterlanguage systems which are adapted to
communicate meanings in such a way that lnterlanguage is not pressured for
change (Schachter 1974) Such a conservative strategy would promote accuracy
at the expense of complexity Equally, there may be pressure to communicate
48 TASK-BASED INSTRUCTION
which does not provide adequate time for restructuring to occur, since
processing resources have to be excessively committed to achieve certain
communicative outcomes In such cases, it is likely that 'solutions" to com-
municative problems will be procedurahzed, and exemplar-based learning will
occur (Schmidt 1983,1992)
Reversing the above analysis makes it clear how restructuring can be more
likely to be achieved There needs to be an interest in achievmg native-like
performance, and possibly an interest in change more generally There also
needs to be helpful input, both explicit and implicit It is also important that
interactive opportunities, e g tasks which need to be transacted, will have a
stretching influence on mterlanguage, in that precision of expression should be
integral to their completion (Swain, in press) It would also be helpful if there
were time to engage restructuring processes, and to attend to things other than
immediate communication pressures Finally, it would help if there were
support for restructuring, through sequencing of teaching activities, through
relevant preparation (Crookes 1989), and through appropriate post-task
activities (Willis and Willis 1988, Skehan 1992)
We can turn finally to fluency, the last of the three goals outlined earlier
Fundamentally, this consists of the capacity to mobilize one's linguistic
resources in the service of real-time communication, l e to produce (and
comprehend) speech at relatively normal rates, approaching (but not neces-
sarily identical to) one's own native-language speech rates In particular, one
would look at features such as rate, pausing, reformulation, hesitation,
redundancy, and the use of lexical units (Bygate 1987) to establish the level of
fluency which has been achieved It is assumed that to achieve this goal requires
a capacity to use implicit knowledge systems in actual performance (Schmidt
1992)
Adequate levels of fluency are desirable if one wants to be acceptable as a
worthwhile interlocutor (Schmidt 1983) Poor fluency will lead to difficult (and
less frequent) patterns of interaction and further opportunities for learning
(Larsen-Freeman and Long 1991) It will also lead to dissatisfaction, as it
becomes difficult to express interesting ideas in real-time and the normal
orchestration of conceptualization, planning, and execution (Levelt 1989)
From a learning perspective, fluency is also desirable to the extent that it
integrates, as implicit knowledge, the results of emerging and developing
restructuring, and makes accessible the lexicahzed products of the operations of
the restructuring in actual communication (Carr and Curren 1994) More
questionably, the sort of fluency which represents the procedurahzation or
lexicahzation of transitional forms which are incorrect, and whose consolida-
tion may compromise future development, may be undesirable It may
represent communicative progress, but of a type which makes later restructur-
ing more difficult
Given these perspectives, we next need to examine what promotes (a) lack of
fluency, (b) undesirable fluency, and (c) effective fluency Lack of fluency is
clearly more likely when the speaker does not valuefluency,but instead is more
PETER SKEHAN 49
on accuracy makes it less likely that interlanguage change will occur, more likely
that speech will be slow, and probably consumes a considerable portion of
attentional resources A focus on complexity and the process of restructuring
increases the chances that new forms will be incorporated into interlanguage
systems, promotes nsk-taking, and requires attention being devoted to the new
forms of language which are being assembled (Foster and Skehan 1994)
Finally, a focus on fluency will lead to language being produced more quickly, to
an emphasis on accessibility (Bygate 1988), and with lower priority being
attached to getting language right, or to the use of new forms
If we consider the processing implications of having these three goals, it is
clear that there is not sufficient capacity for learners to devote resources to each
of them so that they can be met simultaneously As a result, decisions about the
pnontization of attentional resources have to be made during communication
and learning, leading us to need to explore the consequences of allocating
attention in one direction, and not another (Van Patten 1994) Performance is
likely to prioritize fluency, and relegate restructuring and accuracy to lesser
importance A focus on development, on the other hand, is likely to prioritize
restructuring, with accuracy and fluency being more secondary Further, to the
extent that such learning is cumulative, cycles of restructuring, followed by an
emphasis on accuracy and fluency, may be followed by contingent, develop-
mental cycles of further restructuring, as a particular interlanguage sub-system
is progressively complexified
We now need to consider how the three goals of accuracy, complexity-
restructuring, and fluency have been addressed in language teaching method-
ology, i e what decisions are explicit or implicit in existing approaches to
pedagogy A conventional presentation, practice, production sequency tacitly
assumes that change will come about through the presentation phase, and this
will be translated into accuracy and fluency through the succeeding practice and
production stages (Rivers 1981) Following the earlier discussion, we have seen
that there are problems with this approach It assumes that 'restructuring' can be
equated with whatever the teacher (or the syllabus writer) deems to be worth
presenting, and ignores thefindingsof systematicity that have emerged through
second language acquisition research (Ellis 1994) It similarly assumes that the
teacher's plan can be cumulative, with units chosen and sequenced on non-
acquisitional grounds (Breen 1984) Further, it generally assumes that there is a
linear sequence to learning the units of language when they are covered, they
are learned, and do not require cyclical revisiting and extension Finally, there is
the problem that the practice model which underlies the development of habits
in such a *3Ps' approach, has been itself widely discredited (Stern 1983) So,
although there is a sense in which the three terms of restructuring, accuracy, and
fluency could be associated with presentation, practice, and production, the
match is, on examination, clearly inappropriate
A task-based approach, in contrast, may achieve the goal of restructuring, if it
is assumed that interaction-opportunities have an 'extending' influence on
interlanguage development, and engage acquisitional processes But we have
PETER SKEHAN 51
seen that there is also the possibility of over-pnontization of attentional
resources towards fluency, so that the proceduralization of a lexicahzed
competence emphasizing accessibility is at the expense of complexity-1
restructuring and of accuracy To overcome the difficulties of achieving such
conflicting goals, task-based instruction needs to find ways of balancing
attention allocation The next section addresses this issue
Code complexity
Cognitive complexity
Cognitive processing
Cognitive familiarity
Communicative stress
Time pressure
Modality
Scale
Stakes
Control
the task (Willis and Willis 1988,Tarone 1983) They achieve this by reminding
learners that fluency is not the only goal during task completion, and that
restructuring and accuracy also have importance
Drawing on Table 2, two phases of post-task activities can be used In 'Post 1",
the more immediately linked to the teaching which has just occurred, three
general post-task activities can be mentioned public performance, analysis, and
tests With the first of these, public performance, learners will be asked, after
they have completed a task 'in the privacy of their own group' to repeat their
performance, publicly, in front of some sort of audience The audience could be
the rest of a learning group, (who themselves may also have been doing the same
or a similar task, and who could equally well be asked to engage in the public
performance), the teacher, or even a video camera, so that the performance
could be played back later, with even the participants themselves required to
watch In this way, the knowledge while the task is being done that a task may
have to be re-done publicly will cause learners to allocate attention to the goals
of restructuring and accuracy where otherwise they would not In this way, a
concern with syntax and analysis can be infiltrated into the task work without
the heavy-handedness of teacher intervention and error correction
There are also other post-task aspects of task-based learning which are
important, as shown in 'Post 2' from Table 2 One must examine task sequences,
task progression, and generally how sets of tasks relate to one another, and to
the underlying and more important goals which are driving forward instruction
For example, there may be reasons to repeat tasks, with the idea that learners
will be more effective with the analysis and synthesis goals and that the task was
meant to embody Similarly, there may be parallel tasks Such tasks are likely to
be similar to one another in some important respect, but at the same time
contain new elements which are sufficient to engage the interest of the learner
(Plough and Gass 1993) Perhaps most generally of all, it is useful to think in
terms of 'task families', where a group of tasks resemble one another and may
well have similar language or cognitive demands (Candhn 1987) In this way,
learners will be clearer about the goals of such task groups, and there will be less
tendency for discrepancies to arise between teachers' and learners' views about
task requirements
These various methods of analysing tasks, in terms of syllabus and
methodology, are brought together in Table 3 This table shows how the three
major stages of task implementation, together with the associated goals in each
case, cross-reference with syllabus design factors In this way, it is possible to see
how systematic decisions can be made regarding the change which it is intended
should occur in learners' interlanguage systems One aspect of this table which
should be noted in passing is that it suggests that the syllabus-methodology
distinction is still relevant, even for task-based learning Nunan (1993) argues
that this is not so because we learn to communicate by communicating, we
cannot so easily separate the target from the means of achieving it The
discussion m the last few pages, however, suggests that while we cannot pretend
to offer a comprehensive sequence of tasks, there are methods of analysing
PETER SKEHAN 57
Pre-emptive
Restructuring Pre-teach Processing
establish target Consciousness-raising i
) Observe
language Practice Solve similar tasks
reduce cognitive conventional Plan
load parallel tasks cognmvely
rehearsal of linguistically
elements Familiantv
Activate
During TASK C H O I C E
Mediating accuracy Accuracv focus Time Support available
and fluencv Conformity pressure Modalitv Surprise elements
Scale additional
Stakes conflicting
Control
Postl
Increases accuracy Public performance
Encourages teacher
restructuring - group
Discourages excessive camera
synthesis Degree of analysis
Testing
Post 2 The task sequence
Cycle of synthesis and repeating
analysis parallel tasks
Task families
tasks, both for difficulty and for type, and that as a result, we can try to work with
syllabus units in a well-defined and principled way Similarly, viewing task
implementation in terms of the three phases of pre, during, and post clearly
indicates where methodological choices are relevant in task-based learning, that
these choices take as input the units from a syllabus specification, and that the
choices themselves are methodologically motivated So it is argued that the
syllabus-methodology distinction can still be relevant, provided that a
framework such as that advocated here is used
CONCLUSIONS
Task-based learning is an area which has grown in importance enormously
during the last ten years, and can now be approached from a number of perspec-
tives The present paper has taken a processing-pedagogic viewpoint and its
main ideas are
58 TASK-BASED INSTRUCTION
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
The author would like to thank Antony Bruton, Graham Crookes, Pauline Foster, and
three anonymous reviewers, who read earlier versions of this article Needless to say, any
remaining errors or shortcomings are the responsibility of the author
PETERSKEHAN 59
NOTES
1
As an Applied Linguistics reviewer pointed out, this represents a narrow
interpretation of what a task is The justification for this, which should become clearer as
the argument develops, is that the narrower approach draws upon acquisition research
more directly, assuming thai mterlanguage development is a key goal for pedagogy,
particularly in relation to the development of spoken language ability
2
Although it is recognized that to accept this goal makes a number of assumptions, it is
accepted here for expository purposes, but is challengable on a number of grounds First
of all, there is the issue of what native-like means There is also the issue that many
language learners may have other models of competence that they aspire to, rather than a
particular native-speaker version Finally, there are learners who reject a native-speaker
model completely or partially, which complicates the picture considerably
1
Again, for expository purposes, the concentration here is on what Bachman and
Palmer (in press) would term Organizational Competence, and does not concern their
Pragmatic Competence
REFERENCES
Anderson, A andT Lynch 1987 Listening Oxford Oxford University Press
Anderson, J R. 1989 'Practice, working memory, and the ACT theory of skills
acquisition A comment on Carlson, Sullivan, and Schieder" Journal of Learning,
Memory, and Cognition 15 527-30
Bachman, L. and A Palmer, (in press) Language Jesting in Practice Oxford Oxford
University Press
Bates, E , I. Bretherton, and L Snyder 1988 From First Words to Grammar
Cambridge Cambridge University Press
Berwick, R. 1993 "Towards an educational framework for teacher-led tasks' in S Gass
and G Crookes (eds ) 1993a
Bohnger, D. 1975 "Meaningand memory Forum Ltnguisticum 1 2-14
Breen, M. 1984 "Process in syllabus design' in C Brumfit (ed ) 1984 General English
Syllabus Design Oxford Pergamon
Breen, M. 1987 "Contemporary paradigms in syllabus design (Parts 1 and 2)' Language
reaching2Q 91-2 and 157-74
Brown, G , A Anderson, R. Shilcock, and G Yule. 1984 Teaching Talk Strategies for
Production and Assessment Cambndge Cambridge University Press
Brumfit, C 1984 Communicative Methodology in Language Teaching Cambndge
Cambndge University Press
Bygate, M. 1987 Speaking Oxford Oxford University Press
Bygate, M. 1988 'Units of oral expression and language learning in small group
interaction' Applied Linguistics 9/1 59-82
Candlin, C 1987 Towards task based language learning" in C Candlin and D Murphy
(eds) 1987 Language Learning Tasks Englewood Cliffs, NJ Prentice Hall
Carr, T. and T Curren. 1994 'Cognitive factors in learning about structured sequences
Applications to syntax,' Studies in Second Language Acquisition 16/2 205-30
Cheng, P W. 1985 'Restructuring versus automaticity Alternative accounts of skill
acquisition' Psychological Review 92 414-23
Clark, H. H. and E. Clark 1977 Psychology and Language Harcourt, Brace,
Jovanovich
Cook, V. J. 1994 Linguistics and Second Language Acquisition London Macmillan
60 TASK-BASED INSTRUCTION
Corder, S Pit 1981 Error Analysis and Interlanguage Oxford Oxford University
Press
Crookes, G. 1989 'Planning and interlanguage variation' Studies in Second Language
Acquisition 11 367-83
Duff, P. 1986 'Another look at interlanguage talk Taking task to task' in R Day (ed )
1986 Talking to Learn Rowley, MA Newbury House
Ellis, R 1987 'Interlanguage variability in narrative discourse Style shifting in the use of
the past tense' Studies in Second Language Acquisition 9 12-20
Ellis, R. 1994 The Study of Second Language Acquisition Oxford Oxford University
Press
Faerch, C and G. Kasper. 1983 Strategies in Interlanguage Communication London
Longman
Foster,P.andP Skehan 1994 'Themfluenceofplanmngonperformanceintask-based
learning' Paper presented at the British Association of Applied Linguistics, Leeds
Gass, S and G. Crookes (eds) 1993a Tasks in a Pedagogical Context Integrating
Theory and Practice Clevedon, Avon Multilingual Matters
Cass, S and G. Crookes (eds) 1993b Tasks and Languge Learning Integrating Theory
and Practice Clevedon, Avon Multilingual Matters
Gnce, H P 1975 'Logic and conversation'in P Cole and J Morgan (eds) 1975 Syntax
and Semantics 3 Speech Acts, New York Academic Press
Karmilloff-Smith, A. 1986 'From meta-processes to conscious access Evidence from
children's metalinguistic and repair data" Cognition 23 95-147
Kellerman, E. 1991 'Compensatory strategies in second language research A critique, a
revision, and some (non-)implications for the classroom' in R Phillipson, E
Kellerman, L Sehnker, M Sharwood-Smith, and M Swain (eds) 1991 Foreign/
Second Language Pedagogy Research Clevedon, Avon Multilingual Matters
Kess, J E. 1992 Psycholinguistics, Amsterdam John Benjamins
Klein, W. 1986 Second Language Acquisition Cambridge Cambridge University Press
Larsen-Freeman, D andM. Long. 1991 An Introduction to Second Language Acquisi-
tion Research London Longman
Levelt, W. J. M. 1989 Speaking From Intention to Articulation Cambridge, MA MIT
Press
Littlewood, W. 1981 Communicative Language Teaching An Introduction Cambridge
Cambridge University Press
Logan, G. D. 1988 'Towards an instance theory of automatisation' Psychological
Review 95 492-527
Long, M. and G. Crookes. 1991 "Three approaches to task-based syllabus design'
TESOL Quarterly 26/1 27-55
Loschky, L and R. Bley-Vroman. 1993 'Grammar and task-based methodology' in S
Gass and G Crookes (eds) 1993b
Matthews, R C , R. R Buss, W. B. Stanley, F Blachard-Fields, J R. Cho, and B
Druhan. 1989 'Role of implicit and explicit processes in learning from examples A
synergistic effect' Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, and
Cognition 15 1083-100
McLaughlin, B. 1990 'Restructuring' Applied Linguistics 1 1 1 1 3 - 2 8
Nattinger, J. R. and J. S. DeCarrico. 1992 Lexical Phrases and Language Teaching
Oxford Oxford University Press
Nelson, K. 1981 'Individual differences in language development Implications for
development and language' Developmental Psychology 17 170-87
PETERSKEHAN 61
activities on accuracy and complexity in task based learning' Thames Valley University
Working Papers in English Language Teaching, Vol 3
Stern, H. H. 1983 Fundamental Concepts of Language Teaching Oxford Oxford
University Press
Swain, M. 1985 'Communicative competence Some roles of comprehensible input and
comprehensible output in its development' m S Gass and C Madden (eds) 1985
input in Second Language Acquisition, Rowley, MA Newbury House
Swain, M. 1995 Three functions of the output m second language learning' in G Cook
and B Seidlhofer (eds) Principle and Practice in Applied Linguistics Studies in
Honour of H G Wtddowson
Tarone, E 1983 'On the variability of mterlanguage systems' Applied Linguistics 4
143-63
Tarone, E. 1985 "Variability in mterlanguage use A study of style-shifting in
morphology and syntax' Language Learning 35 373-403
Trevise, A. and C. Noyau. 1984 'Adult Spanish speakers and the acquisition of French
negation forms Individual variation and linguistic awareness' in R W Andersen (ed)
1984 Second Languages Rowley, MA Newbury House
Van Patten, B. 1990 'Attending to content and form m the input An experiment in
consciousness' Studies in Second Language Acquisition 12 287-301
Van Patten, B. 1994 'Evaluating the role of consciousness in SLA Terms, linguistic
features, and research methodology' AILA Review 11 27-36
Van Patten, B. and T Cadierno. 1993 'Explicit instruction and input processing'
Studies in Second Language Acquisition 15 225-43
Wells, G. 1985 Language Development in the Preschool years Cambridge Cambridge
University Press
Widdowson, H. G 1983 Learning Purpose and Language Use Oxford Oxford
University Press
Widdowson, H. G 1989 'Knowledge of language and ability for u s e ' Applied
Linguistics 10/2 128-37
Willis, D. 1993 'Syllabus, corpus, and data-driven learning' in C Kennedy (ed) 1993
Plenanesfrom the 1993IA TEFL Conference Swansea, Wales, IATEFL
Willis, D. and J.Willis. 1988 COBUILD Book 1 London Collins
Willis, D. and J. Willis, in press Language Teaching Alternatives Methodological
Innovation in English Language Teaching Oxford Heinemann
Wilson, D. 1994 'Context and relevance' in G Brown, K Malmkjaer, A Pollm, and J
Williams (eds) 1994 Language and Understanding Oxford Oxford University Press
Yule, G., M. Powers, and D. Macdonald. 1992 The variable effects of some task-based
learning procedures on L2 communicative effectiveness' Language Learning 42/2
249-77