List of Cognitive Biases
List of Cognitive Biases
List of Cognitive Biases
Although this research overwhelmingly involves human subjects, some findings that demonstrate bias have been found in non-human
animals as well. For example,hyperbolic discountinghas been observed in rats, pigeons, and monkeys.[8]
Contents
Decision-making, belief, and behavioral biases
Social biases
Memory errors and biases
Common theoretical causes of some cognitive biases
Individual differences in decision making biases
Debiasing
See also
Notes
References
Anthropomorphism or The tendency to characterize animals, objects, and abstract concepts as possessing
personification human-like traits, emotions, and intentions.[13]
Base rate fallacy or Base The tendency to ignore base rate information (generic, general information) and focus
rate neglect [20]
on specific information (information only pertaining to a certain case).
An effect where someone's evaluation of thelogical strength of an argument is biased
Belief bias
by the believability of the conclusion.[21]
A person who has performed a favor for someone is more likely to do another favor for
Ben Franklin effect
that person than they would be if they hadreceived a favor from that person.
Berkson's paradox The tendency to misinterpret statistical experiments involving conditional probabilities.
The tendency to see oneself as less biased than other people, or to be able to identify
Bias blind spot [22]
more cognitive biases in others than in oneself.
Bystander effect The tendency to think that others will act in an emergency situation.
Conservatism (belief The tendency to revise one's belief insufficiently when presented with new
revision) evidence.[4][26][27]
The tendency to believe previously learned misinformation even after it has been
Continued influence effect corrected. Misinformation can still influence inferences one generates after a correction
has occurred.[28] cf. Backfire effect
The enhancement or reduction of a certain stimulus' perception when compared with a
Contrast effect
recently observed, contrasting object.[29]
Courtesy bias The tendency to give an opinion that is more socially correct than one's true opinion, so
as to avoid offending anyone.[30]
When better-informed people find it extremely dif
ficult to think about problems from the
Curse of knowledge
perspective of lesser-informed people.[31]
The predisposition to view the past favourably rosy
( retrospection) and future
Declinism
negatively.[32]
Preferences for either option A or B change in favor of option B when option C is
Decoy effect presented, which is completely dominated by option B (inferior in all respects) and
partially dominated by option A.
Default effect When given a choice between several options, the tendency to favor the default one.
The tendency to spend more money when it is denominated in small amounts (e.g.,
Denomination effect
coins) rather than large amounts (e.g., bills).[33]
The tendency to sell an asset that has accumulated in value and resist selling an asset
Disposition effect
that has declined in value.
The tendency to view two options as more dissimilar when evaluating them
Distinction bias
simultaneously than when evaluating them separately.[34]
The tendency for unskilled individuals to overestimate their own ability and the
Dunning–Kruger effect
.[35]
tendency for experts to underestimate their own ability
Duration neglect The neglect of the duration of an episode in determining its value
The tendency to underestimate the influence or strength of feelings, in either oneself or
Empathy gap
others.
The tendency for people to demand much more to give up an object than they would be
Endowment effect
willing to pay to acquire it.[36]
Based on the estimates,real-world evidence turns out to be less extreme than our
Exaggerated expectation [4][37]
expectations (conditionally inverse of the conservatism bias).
The tendency for experimenters to believe, certify
, and publish data that agree with their
Experimenter's or expectations for the outcome of an experiment, and to disbelieve, discard, or
expectation bias downgrade the corresponding weightings for data that appear to conflict with those
expectations.[38]
Functional fixedness Limits a person to using an object only in the way it is traditionally used.
The tendency to think that future probabilities are altered by past events, when in reality
they are unchanged. The fallacy arises from an erroneous conceptualization of the law
Gambler's fallacy of large numbers. For example, "I've flipped heads with this coin five times
consecutively, so the chance of tails coming out on the sixth flip is much greater than
heads."
Hard–easy effect Based on a specific level of task difficulty, the confidence in judgments is too
[4][43][44][45]
conservative and not extreme enough[4][43][44][45]
Sometimes called the "I-knew-it-all-along" ef
fect, the tendency to see past events as
Hindsight bias
being predictable[46] at the time those events happened.
The "hostile attribution bias" is the tendency to interpret others' behaviors as having
Hostile attribution bias
hostile intent, even when the behavior is ambiguous or benign.
The "hot-hand fallacy" (also known as the "hot hand phenomenon" or "hot hand") is the
Hot-hand fallacy fallacious belief that a person who has experienced success with a random event has a
greater chance of further success in additional attempts.
Discounting is the tendency for people to have a stronger preference for more
immediate payoffs relative to later payoffs. Hyperbolic discounting leads to choices that
are inconsistent over time – people make choices today that their future selves would
prefer not to have made, despite using the same reasoning. [47] Also known as current
Hyperbolic discounting
moment bias, present-bias, and related toDynamic inconsistency. A good example of
this: a study showed that when making food choices for the coming week, 74% of
participants chose fruit, whereas when the food choice was for the current day , 70%
chose chocolate.
The tendency to respond more strongly to a single identified person at risk than to a
Identifiable victim effect
large group of people at risk.[48]
The tendency for people to place a disproportionately high value on objects that they
IKEA effect partially assembled themselves, such as furniture fromIKEA, regardless of the quality
of the end result.
Occurs when a term in the distributive (referring to every member of a class) and
Illicit transference collective (referring to the class itself as a whole) sense are treated is equivalent. The
two variants of this fallacy are thefallacy of composition and the fallacy of division.
Social biases
Most of these biases are labeled asattributional biases.
Name Description
The tendency for explanations of other individuals' behaviors to overemphasize the
influence of their personality and underemphasize the influence of their situation (see
Actor-observer bias also Fundamental attribution error), and for explanations of one's own behaviors to do
the opposite (that is, to overemphasize the influence of our situation and
underemphasize the influence of our own personality).
The tendency to attribute greater accuracy to the opinion of an authority figure
Authority bias [80]
(unrelated to its content) and be more influenced by that opinion.
Illusion of asymmetric People perceive their knowledge of their peers to surpass their peers' knowledge of
insight them.[84]
When people view self-generated preferences as instead being caused by insightful,
Illusion of external agency
effective and benevolent agents
People overestimate others' ability to know them, and they also overestimate their
Illusion of transparency
ability to know others.
Overestimating one's desirable qualities, and underestimating undesirable qualities,
Illusory superiority relative to other people. (Also known as "Lake Wobegon effect", "better-than-average
effect", or "superiority bias".)[85]
The tendency for people to give preferential treatment to others they perceive to be
Ingroup bias
members of their own groups.
The tendency for people to want to believe that the world is fundamentally just, causing
Just-world hypothesis
them to rationalize an otherwise inexplicable injustice as deserved by the victim(s).
The tendency for people to ascribe greater or lesser moral standing based on the
Moral luck
outcome of an event.
Naïve cynicism Expecting more egocentric bias in others than in oneself.
The belief that we see reality as it really is – objectively and without bias; that the facts
Naïve realism are plain for all to see; that rational people will agree with us; and that those who don't
are either uninformed, lazy, irrational, or biased.
Outgroup homogeneity Individuals see members of their own group as being relatively more varied than
bias members of other groups.[86]
Self-serving bias The tendency to claim more responsibility for successes than failures. It may also
manifest itself as a tendency for people to evaluate ambiguous information in a way
beneficial to their interests (see alsogroup-serving bias).[87]
Known as the tendency for group members to spend more time and energy discussing
information that all members are already familiar with (i.e., shared information), and
Shared information bias
less time and energy discussing information that only some members are aware of (i.e.,
unshared information).[88]
The disproportionally higher representation of words related to social interactions, in
comparison to words related to physical or mental aspects of behavior , in most
languages. This bias attributed to nature of language as a tool facilitating human
Sociability bias of
interactions. When verbal descriptors of human behavior are used as a source of
language
information, sociability bias of such descriptors emerges in factor-analytic studies as a
factor related to pro-social behavior (for example, of Extraversion factor in the
Big Five
personality traits [59]
The tendency to defend and bolster the status quo. Existing social, economic, and
System justification political arrangements tend to be preferred, and alternatives disparaged, sometimes
even at the expense of individual and collective self-interest. (See also status quo bias.)
The tendency for people to view themselves as relatively variable in terms of
Trait ascription bias
personality, behavior, and mood while viewing others as much more predictable.
Similar to the fundamental attribution error, in this error a person is likely to make an
Ultimate attribution error
internal attribution to an entire group instead of the individuals within the group.
Primacy effect, recency That items near the end of a sequence are the easiest to recall, followed by the items at
effect & serial position the beginning of a sequence; items in the middle are the least likely to be
effect remembered.[106]
That information that takes longer to read and is thought about more (processed with
Processing difficulty effect
more difficulty) is more easily remembered.[107]
The recalling of more personal events from adolescence and early adulthood than
Reminiscence bump
personal events from other lifetime periods[108]
Rosy retrospection The remembering of the past as having been better than it really was.
That memories relating to the self are better recalled than similar information relating to
Self-relevance effect
others.
Prospect theory
Mental accounting
Adaptive bias – basing decisions on limited information and biasing them based on the costs of being wrong.
Attribute substitution – making a complex, difficult judgment by unconsciously substituting it by an easier
judgment[117]
Attribution theory
Salience
Naïve realism
Cognitive dissonance, and related:
Impression management
Self-perception theory
Heuristics in judgment and decision making, including:
Availability heuristic – estimating what is more likely by what is more available in memory
, which is biased toward
vivid, unusual, or emotionally charged examples [51]
[51]
Representativeness heuristic– judging probabilities on the basis of resemblance
[118]
Affect heuristic – basing a decision on an emotional reaction rather than a calculation of risks and benefits
Some theories of emotion such as:
Debiasing
Debiasing is the reduction of biases in judgment and decision making through incentives, nudges, and training. Cognitive bias
mitigation and cognitive bias modificationare forms of debiasing specifically applicable to cognitive biases and their fects.
ef
See also
Affective forecasting
Anecdotal evidence
Apophenia
Attribution (psychology)
Black swan theory
Chronostasis
Cognitive distortion
Defence mechanisms
Dysrationalia
Fear, uncertainty, and doubt
Feedback
Impostor syndrome
List of common misconceptions
List of fallacies
List of maladaptive schemas
List of memory biases
List of psychological effects
List of topics related to public relations and propaganda
Lists of thinking-related topics
Media bias
Mind projection fallacy
Motivated reasoning
Pollyanna principle
Positive feedback
Prevalence effect
Propaganda
Publication bias
Recall bias
Self-handicapping
Systematic bias
Notes
1. "Cognitive bias cheat sheet"(https://betterhumans.coach.me/cognitive-bias-cheat-sheet-55a472476b18)
. 2016-09-
01. Retrieved 2017-05-27.
2. Haselton, M. G.; Nettle, D. & Andrews, P
. W. (2005). The evolution of cognitive bias(http://www.sscnet.ucla.edu/com
m/haselton/papers/downloads/handbookevpsych.pdf)(PDF). In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The Handbook of Evolutionary
Psychology: Hoboken, NJ, US: John Wiley & Sons Inc. pp. 724–746.
3. Dougherty, M. R. P.; Gettys, C. F.; Ogden, E. E. (1999)."MINERVA-DM: A memory processes model for judgments
of likelihood" (http://www.bsos.umd.edu/psyc/dougherty/PDF%20articles/Dougherty,Gettys&Ogden,1999.pdf)(PDF).
Psychological Review. 106 (1): 180–209. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.106.1.180(https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.106.
1.180).
4. Martin Hilbert (2012). "Toward a synthesis of cognitive biases: Hownoisy information processing can bias human
decision making" (http://www.martinhilbert.net/HilbertPsychBull.pdf) (PDF). Psychological Bulletin. 138 (2): 211–237.
doi:10.1037/a0025940 (https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025940). PMID 22122235 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2
2122235). Lay summary (http://psycnet.apa.org/psycinfo/2011-27261-001) .
5. Maccoun, Robert J. (1998)."Biases in the interpretation and use of research results"(http://socrates.berkeley.edu/~
maccoun/MacCoun_AnnualReview98.pdf)(PDF). Annual Review of Psychology. 49 (1): 259–87.
doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.259 (https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.259). PMID 15012470 (https://ww
w.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15012470).
6. Nickerson, Raymond S.(1998). "Confirmation Bias: A Ubiquitous Phenomenon in Many Guises"(http://psy2.ucsd.ed
u/~mckenzie/nickersonConfirmationBias.pdf)(PDF). Review of General Psychology. Educational Publishing
Foundation. 2 (2): 175–220 [198]. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.2.2.175(https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.2.175).
ISSN 1089-2680 (https://www.worldcat.org/issn/1089-2680).
7. Dardenne, Benoit; Leyens, Jacques-Philippe (1995). "Confirmation Bias as a Social Skill".
Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin. Society for Personality and Social Psychology
. 21 (11): 1229–1239.
doi:10.1177/01461672952111011(https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672952111011) . ISSN 1552-7433 (https://www.worl
dcat.org/issn/1552-7433).
8. Alexander, William H.; Brown, Joshua W. (1 June 2010). "Hyperbolically Discounted Temporal Difference Learning"
(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3005720). Neural Computation. 22 (6): 1511–1527.
doi:10.1162/neco.2010.08-09-1080(https://doi.org/10.1162/neco.2010.08-09-1080) . PMC 3005720 (https://www.ncb
i.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3005720) . PMID 20100071 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20100071).
9. Baron 1994, p. 372
10. Zhang, Yu; Lewis, Mark; Pellon, Michael; Coleman, Phillip (2007). "A Preliminary Research on Modeling Cognitive
Agents for Social Environments in Multi-Agent Systems"(http://www.aaai.org/Papers/Symposia/Fall/2007/FS-07-04/
FS07-04-017.pdf) (PDF): 116–123.
11. Iverson, Grant; Brooks, Brian; Holdnack, James (2008). "Misdiagnosis of Cognitive Impairment in Forensic
Neuropsychology". In Heilbronner, Robert L. Neuropsychology in the Courtroom: Expert Analysis of Reports and
Testimony. New York: Guilford Press. p. 248.ISBN 9781593856342.
12. Coley, John D; Tanner, Kimberly D (2012). "Common Origins of Diverse Misconceptions: Cognitive Principles and
the Development of Biology Thinking"(http://www.lifescied.org/content/11/3/209.full). CBE-Life Sciences Education.
11 (3): 209–215. doi:10.1187/cbe.12-06-0074(https://doi.org/10.1187/cbe.12-06-0074). ISSN 1931-7913 (https://ww
w.worldcat.org/issn/1931-7913). PMC 3433289 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3433289) .
13. "The Real Reason We Dress Pets Like People" (http://www.livescience.com/6141-real-reason-dress-pets-people.ht
ml). LiveScience.com. Retrieved 2015-11-16.
14. Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., & van IJzendoorn, M.H. (2007). "Threat-related
attentional bias in anxious and non-anxious individuals: A meta-analytic study
." Psychological Bulletin.
15. Goddard, Kate; Roudsari, Abdul; Wyatt, Jeremy C. (2011). "Automation Bias – A Hidden Issue for Clinical Decision
Support System Use (https://books.google.com/books?id=NsbaN_fXRe4C&pg=P A17)." International Perspectives in
Health Informatics. Studies in Health Technology and Informatics. IOS Press. doi:10.3233/978-1-60750-709-3-17(htt
ps://doi.org/10.3233%2F978-1-60750-709-3-17)
16. Schwarz, N.; Bless, Herbert; Strack, Fritz; Klumpp, G.; Rittenauer-Schatka, Helga; Simons, Annette (1991). "Ease of
Retrieval as Information: Another Look at the A vailability Heuristic" (https://web.archive.org/web/20140209175640/htt
p://osil.psy.ua.edu:16080/~Rosanna/Soc_Inf/week4/availability.pdf) (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology. 61 (2): 195–202. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.61.2.195(https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.61.2.195).
Archived from the original (http://osil.psy.ua.edu:16080/~Rosanna/Soc_Inf/week4/availability.pdf) (PDF) on 9
February 2014. Retrieved 19 Oct 2014.
17. Kuran, Timur; Cass R Sunstein (1998). "Availability Cascades and Risk Regulation".Stanford Law Review. 51: 683.
doi:10.2307/1229439 (https://doi.org/10.2307/1229439).
18. Sanna, Lawrence J.; Schwarz, Norbert; Stocker , Shevaun L. (2002). "When debiasing backfires: Accessible content
and accessibility experiences in debiasing hindsight"(http://www.nifc.gov/PUBLICATIONS/acc_invest_march2010/sp
eakers/4DebiasBackfires.pdf)(PDF). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory , and Cognition. 28 (3):
497–502. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.28.3.497(https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.28.3.497). ISSN 0278-7393 (https://w
ww.worldcat.org/issn/0278-7393).
19. Colman, Andrew (2003).Oxford Dictionary of Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press. p. 77.ISBN 0-19-
280632-7.
20. Baron 1994, pp. 224–228
21. Klauer, K. C.; Musch, J; Naumer, B (2000). "On belief bias in syllogistic reasoning".Psychological Review. 107 (4):
852–884. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.107.4.852(https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.107.4.852) . PMID 11089409 (http
s://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11089409).
22. Pronin, Emily; Matthew B. Kugler (July 2007). "Valuing thoughts, ignoring behavior: The introspection illusion as a
source of the bias blind spot".Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. Elsevier. 43 (4): 565–578.
doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2006.05.011(https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2006.05.011). ISSN 0022-1031 (https://www.worldcat.o
rg/issn/0022-1031).
23. Mather, M.; Shafir, E.; Johnson, M.K. (2000)."Misrememberance of options past: Source monitoring and choice"(htt
p://www.matherlab.com/s/Matheretal2000.pdf) (PDF). Psychological Science. 11 (2): 132–138. doi:10.1111/1467-
9280.00228 (https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00228). PMID 11273420 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/112
73420). Archived (https://web.archive.org/web/20090117084058/http://www.usc.edu/projects/matherlab/pdfs/Mather
etal2000.pdf) (PDF) from the original on 2009-01-17.
24. Oswald, Margit E.; Grosjean, Stefan (2004). "Confirmation Bias".In Pohl, Rüdiger F. Cognitive Illusions: A Handbook
on Fallacies and Biases in Thinking, Judgement and Memory . Hove, UK: Psychology Press. pp. 79–96.ISBN 978-1-
84169-351-4. OCLC 55124398 (https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/55124398).
25. Fisk, John E. (2004). "Conjunction fallacy".In Pohl, Rüdiger F. Cognitive Illusions: A Handbook on Fallacies and
Biases in Thinking, Judgement and Memory. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. pp. 23–42.ISBN 978-1-84169-351-4.
OCLC 55124398 (https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/55124398).
26. DuCharme, W. M. (1970). "Response bias explanation of conservative human inference".Journal of Experimental
Psychology. 85 (1): 66–74. doi:10.1037/h0029546 (https://doi.org/10.1037/h0029546).
27. Edwards, W. (1968). "Conservatism in humaninformation processing".In Kleinmuntz, B. Formal representation of
human judgment. New York: Wiley. pp. 17–52.
28. Johnson, Hollyn M.; Colleen M. Seifert (November 1994). "Sources of the continued influence fect:
ef When
misinformation in memory affects later inferences". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory
, and
Cognition. 20 (6): 1420–1436. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.20.6.1420(https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.20.6.1420) .
29. Plous 1993, pp. 38–41
30. Ciccarelli, Saundra; White, J. (2014).Psychology (4th ed.). Pearson Education, Inc. p. 62.ISBN 0205973353.
31. Ackerman, Mark S., ed. (2003).Sharing expertise beyond knowledge management(online ed.). Cambridge,
Massachusetts: MIT Press. p. 7.ISBN 9780262011952.
32. Steven R. Quartz, The State Of The World Isn’t Nearly As Bad As Y
ou Think (https://edge.org/response-detail/2666
9), Edge Foundation, Inc., retrieved 2016-02-17
33. Why We Spend Coins Faster Than Bills(https://www.npr.org/templates/story/story.php?storyId=104063298)by
Chana Joffe-Walt. All Things Considered, 12 May 2009.
34. Hsee, Christopher K.; Zhang, Jiao (2004). "Distinction bias: Misprediction and mischoice due to joint evaluation".
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 86 (5): 680–695. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.5.680(https://doi.org/10.1
037/0022-3514.86.5.680). PMID 15161394 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15161394).
35. Kruger, Justin; Dunning, David (1999). "Unskilled and Unaware of It: How Difficulties in Recognizing One's Own
Incompetence Lead to Inflated Self-Assessments".Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 77 (6): 1121–34.
CiteSeerX 10.1.1.64.2655 (https://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.64.2655) . doi:10.1037/0022-
3514.77.6.1121 (https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1121) . PMID 10626367 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubm
ed/10626367).
36. (Kahneman, Knetsch & Thaler 1991, p. 193) Richard Thaler coined the term "endowment ef
fect."
37. Wagenaar, W. A.; Keren, G. B. (1985). "Calibration of probability assessments by professional blackjack dealers,
statistical experts, and lay people".Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes . 36 (3): 406–416.
doi:10.1016/0749-5978(85)90008-1(https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978%2885%2990008-1) .
38. Jeng, M. (2006). "A selected history of expectation bias in physics".American Journal of Physics. 74 (7): 578–583.
arXiv:physics/0508199 (https://arxiv.org/abs/physics/0508199) . Bibcode:2006AmJPh..74..578J (http://adsabs.harv
ard.edu/abs/2006AmJPh..74..578J). doi:10.1119/1.2186333 (https://doi.org/10.1119/1.2186333).
39. Kahneman, Daniel; Alan B. Krueger; David Schkade; Norbert Schwarz; Arthur A. Stone (2006-06-30). "Would you be
happier if you were richer? A focusing illusion"(http://www.morgenkommichspaeterrein.de/ressources/download/125
krueger.pdf) (PDF). Science. 312 (5782): 1908–10. Bibcode:2006Sci...312.1908K (http://adsabs.harvard.edu/abs/20
06Sci...312.1908K). doi:10.1126/science.1129688(https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1129688). PMID 16809528 (http
s://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16809528).
40. Haring, Kerstin; Katsumi Watanabe; Mari Velonaki,; Chad C. Tossell; Victor Finomore. "FFAB-The Form Function
Attribution Bias in Human Robot Interaction"(http://ieeexplore.ieee.org/stamp/stamp.jsp?tp=&arnumber=8400493&is
number=7422051). IEEE Transactions on Cognitive and Developmental Systems. doi:10.1109/TCDS.2018.2851569
(https://doi.org/10.1109/TCDS.2018.2851569) .
41. Zwicky, Arnold (2005-08-07). "Just Between Dr. Language and I" (http://itre.cis.upenn.edu/~myl/languagelog/archive
s/002386.html). Language Log.
42. "What's the Baader-Meinhof phenomenon?"(https://science.howstuffworks.com/life/inside-the-mind/human-brain/ba
ader-meinhof-phenomenon.htm). 20 March 2015. Retrieved 15 April 2018.
43. Lichtenstein, S.; Fischhoff, B. (1977). "Do those who know more also know more about how much they know?".
Organizational Behavior and Human Performance . 20 (2): 159–183. doi:10.1016/0030-5073(77)90001-0(https://doi.
org/10.1016/0030-5073%2877%2990001-0).
44. Merkle, E. C. (2009). "The disutility of the hard-easy ef
fect in choice confidence".Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 16
(1): 204–213. doi:10.3758/PBR.16.1.204(https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.16.1.204).
45. Juslin, P; Winman, A.; Olsson, H. (2000). "Naive empiricism and dogmatism in confidence research: a critical
examination of the hard-easy effect". Psychological Review. 107 (2): 384–396. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.107.2.384(htt
ps://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.107.2.384).
46. Pohl, Rüdiger F. (2004). "Hindsight Bias".In Pohl, Rüdiger F. Cognitive Illusions: A Handbook on Fallacies and
Biases in Thinking, Judgement and Memory. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. pp. 363–378.ISBN 978-1-84169-351-4.
OCLC 55124398 (https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/55124398).
47. Laibson, David (1997). "Golden Eggs and Hyperbolic Discounting".Quarterly Journal of Economics. 112 (2): 443–
477. doi:10.1162/003355397555253(https://doi.org/10.1162/003355397555253).
48. Kogut, Tehila; Ritov, Ilana (2005). "The 'Identified Victim' Effect: An Identified Group, or Just a Single Individual?"(htt
p://pluto.mscc.huji.ac.il/~msiritov/KogutRitovIdentified.pdf)(PDF). Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. Wiley
InterScience. 18: 157–167. doi:10.1002/bdm.492 (https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.492). Retrieved August 15, 2013.
49. Thompson, Suzanne C. (1999). "Illusions of Control: How W e Overestimate Our Personal Influence".Current
Directions in Psychological Science. Association for Psychological Science.8 (6): 187–190. doi:10.1111/1467-
8721.00044 (https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00044). ISSN 0963-7214 (https://www.worldcat.org/issn/0963-7214).
JSTOR 20182602 (https://www.jstor.org/stable/20182602).
50. Dierkes, Meinolf; Antal, Ariane Berthoin; Child, John; Ikujiro Nonaka (2003).
Handbook of Organizational Learning
and Knowledge (https://books.google.com/books?id=JRd7RZzzw_wC&pg=P A22). Oxford University Press. p. 22.
ISBN 978-0-19-829582-2. Retrieved 9 September 2013.
51. Tversky, Amos; Daniel Kahneman (September 27, 1974). "Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases".
Science. American Association for the Advancement of Science.185 (4157): 1124–1131.
Bibcode:1974Sci...185.1124T (http://adsabs.harvard.edu/abs/1974Sci...185.1124T)
.
doi:10.1126/science.185.4157.1124(https://doi.org/10.1126/science.185.4157.1124). PMID 17835457 (https://www.
ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17835457).
52. Fiedler, K. (1991). "The tricky nature of skewed frequency tables: An information loss account of distinctiveness-
based illusory correlations".Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 60 (1): 24–36. doi:10.1037/0022-
3514.60.1.24 (https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.60.1.24).
53. Sanna, Lawrence J.; Schwarz, Norbert (2004). "Integrating emporal
T Biases: The Interplay of Focal Thoughts and
Accessibility Experiences".Psychological Science. American Psychological Society. 15 (7): 474–481.
doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00704.x(https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00704.x) . PMID 15200632 (https://
www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15200632).
54. Baron 1994, pp. 258–259
55. (Kahneman, Knetsch & Thaler 1991, p. 193) Daniel Kahneman, together with Amos Tversky
, coined the term "loss
aversion."
56. Bornstein, Robert F.; Crave-Lemley, Catherine (2004). "Mere exposure effect". In Pohl, Rüdiger F. Cognitive Illusions:
A Handbook on Fallacies and Biases in Thinking, Judgement and Memory . Hove, UK: Psychology Press. pp. 215–
234. ISBN 978-1-84169-351-4. OCLC 55124398 (https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/55124398).
57. Shafir, Eldar; Diamond, Peter; Tversky, Amos (2000). "Money Illusion".In Kahneman, Daniel; Tversky, Amos.
Choices, values, and frames. Cambridge University Press. pp. 335–355.ISBN 978-0-521-62749-8.
58. Haizlip, Julie; et al. "Perspective: The Negativity Bias, Medical Education, and the Culture of Academic Medicine:
Why Culture Change Is Hard"(http://journals.lww.com/academicmedicine/Fulltext/2012/09000/Perspective___The_N
egativity_Bias,_Medical.19.aspx). Retrieved October 3, 2012.
59. Trofimova, IN (2014). "Observer bias: an interaction of temperament traits with biases in the semantic perception of
lexical material" (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3903487). PLoS ONE. 9 (1): e85677.
Bibcode:2014PLoSO...985677T(http://adsabs.harvard.edu/abs/2014PLoSO...985677T) .
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0085677(https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0085677) . PMC 3903487 (https://www.ncbi.
nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3903487) . PMID 24475048 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24475048).
60. Sutherland 2007, pp. 138–139
61. Baron 1994, p. 353
62. Baron 1994, p. 386
63. Baron 1994, p. 44
64. Hardman 2009, p. 104
65. Adams, P. A.; Adams, J. K. (1960). "Confidence in the recognition and reproduction of words dif
ficult to spell". The
American Journal of Psychology. 73 (4): 544–552. doi:10.2307/1419942 (https://doi.org/10.2307/1419942).
PMID 13681411 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/13681411).
66. Hoffrage, Ulrich (2004). "Overconfidence".In Rüdiger Pohl. Cognitive Illusions: a handbook on fallacies and biases
in thinking, judgement and memory. Psychology Press. ISBN 978-1-84169-351-4.
67. Sutherland 2007, pp. 172–178
68. Hsee, Christopher K.; Hastie, Reid (2006)."Decision and experience: why don't we choose what makes us happy?"
(https://web.archive.org/web/20150420205315/http://maelko.typepad.com/DecisionAndExperience.pdf) (PDF).
Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 10 (1): 31–37. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.11.007(https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.11.00
7). PMID 16318925 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16318925). Archived from the original on 2015-04-20.
69. Trofimova, IN (1999). "How people of different age sex and temperament estimate the wo
rld". Psychological
Reports. 85/2: 533–552. doi:10.2466/pr0.1999.85.2.533(https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1999.85.2.533).
70. Hardman 2009, p. 137
71. Attneave, F. (1953). "Psychological probability as a function of experienced frequency".Journal of Experimental
Psychology. 46 (2): 81–86. doi:10.1037/h0057955 (https://doi.org/10.1037/h0057955). PMID 13084849 (https://www.
ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/13084849).
72. Fischhoff, B.; Slovic, P.; Lichtenstein, S. (1977). "Knowing with certainty: The appropriateness of extreme
confidence". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance . 3 (4): 552–564.
doi:10.1037/0096-1523.3.4.552(https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.3.4.552).
73. Garcia, Stephen M.; Song, Hyunjin; T
esser, Abraham (November 2010). "Tainted recommendations: The social
comparison bias". Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes . 113 (2): 97–101.
doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2010.06.002(https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2010.06.002) . ISSN 0749-5978 (https://www.world
cat.org/issn/0749-5978). Lay summary (http://bps-research-digest.blogspot.com/2010/10/social-comparison-bias-or-
why-we.html) – BPS Research Digest (2010-10-30).
74. Dalton, D. & Ortegren, M. (2011). "Gender differences in ethics research: The importance of controlling for the social
desirability response bias".Journal of Business Ethics. 103 (1): 73–93. doi:10.1007/s10551-011-0843-8(https://doi.o
rg/10.1007/s10551-011-0843-8).
75. Kahneman, Knetsch & Thaler 1991, p. 193
76. Baron 1994, p. 382
77. Baron, J. (in preparation).Thinking and Deciding, 4th edition. New York: Cambridge UniversityPress.
78. Forsyth, Donelson R (2009).Group Dynamics (https://books.google.com/books?id=RsMNiobZojIC&pg=P
A317) (5th
ed.). Cengage Learning. p. 317.ISBN 978-0-495-59952-4.
79. "Penn Psychologists Believe 'Unit Bias' Determines The Acceptable AmountoTEat" (https://www.sciencedaily.com/r
eleases/2005/11/051121163748.htm). ScienceDaily (Nov. 21, 2005)
80. Milgram, Stanley (Oct 1963). "Behavioral Study of obedience".The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology
. 67
(4).
81. Walker, Drew; Vul, Edward (2013-10-25)."Hierarchical Encoding Makes Individuals in a Group Seem More
Attractive" (http://pss.sagepub.com/content/early/2013/10/25/0956797613497969)
. Psychological Science. 25 (11):
230–235. doi:10.1177/0956797613497969(https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613497969). PMID 24163333 (https://w
ww.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24163333).
82. Marks, Gary; Miller, Norman (1987). "Ten years of research on the false-consensus ef fect: An empirical and
theoretical review". Psychological Bulletin. American Psychological Association.102 (1): 72–90. doi:10.1037/0033-
2909.102.1.72 (https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.102.1.72).
83. Baron 1994, p. 275
84. Pronin, E.; Kruger, J.; Savitsky, K.; Ross, L. (2001). "You don't know me, but I know you: the illusion of asymmetric
insight". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 81 (4): 639–656. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.4.639(https://doi.
org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.4.639). PMID 11642351 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11642351).
85. Hoorens, Vera (1993). "Self-enhancement and Superiority Biases in Social Comparison".European Review of Social
Psychology. Psychology Press. 4 (1): 113–139. doi:10.1080/14792779343000040(https://doi.org/10.1080/14792779
343000040).
86. Plous 2006, p. 206
87. Plous 2006, p. 185
88. Forsyth, D. R. (2009). Group Dynamics (5th ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.
89. Kruger, J. (1999). "Lake Wobegon be gone! The "below-average effect" and the egocentric nature of comparative
ability judgments". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 77 (2): 221–32. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.2.221(h
ttps://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.77.2.221). PMID 10474208 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10474208).
90. Schacter, Daniel L. (1999). "The Seven Sinsof Memory: Insights From Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience".
American Psychologist. 54 (3): 182–203. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.3.182(https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.54.3.
182). PMID 10199218 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10199218).
91. Cacioppo, John (2002).Foundations in social neuroscience. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. pp. 130–132.
ISBN 026253195X.
92. Walker, W. Richard; John J. Skowronski; Charles P. Thompson (1994). "Effects of Humor on Sentence Memory"(htt
p://facstaff.uww.edu/eamond/road/Research/GenderJokes%28DK1%29/References%20and%20info/Ef fects%20of%
20Humor%20on%20Sentence%20Memory .pdf) (PDF). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory , and
Cognition. American Psychological Association, Inc.20 (4): 953–967. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.20.4.953(https://doi.or
g/10.1037/0278-7393.20.4.953). Retrieved 2015-04-19.
93. Schmidt, Stephen R. (2003)."Life Is Pleasant—and Memory Helps to Keep It That W ay!" (http://www.niu.edu/jskowro
nski/publications/WalkerSkowronskiThompson2003.pdf) (PDF). Review of General Psychology. Educational
Publishing Foundation.7 (2): 203–210. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.7.2.203(https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.7.2.203).
94. Koriat, A.; M. Goldsmith; A. Pansky (2000). "T
oward a Psychology of Memory Accuracy".Annual Review of
Psychology. 51 (1): 481–537. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.481 (https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.481).
PMID 10751979 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10751979).
95. Craik & Lockhart, 1972
96. Kinnell, Angela; Dennis, S. (2011). "The list length ef
fect in recognition memory: an analysis of potential confounds".
Memory & Cognition. Adelaide, Australia: School of Psychology, University of Adelaide. 39 (2): 348–63.
doi:10.3758/s13421-010-0007-6(https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-010-0007-6).
97. Wayne Weiten (2010). Psychology: Themes and Variations: Themes and Variations (https://books.google.com/book
s?id=sILajOhJpOsC&pg=PA338). Cengage Learning. p. 338.ISBN 978-0-495-60197-5.
98. Wayne Weiten (2007). Psychology: Themes and Variations: Themes And Variations (https://books.google.com/book
s?id=Vv1vvlIEXG0C&pg=PA260). Cengage Learning. p. 260.ISBN 978-0-495-09303-9.
99. Slamecka NJ (1968). "An examination of trace storage in free recall".J Exp Psychol. 76 (4): 504–13.
doi:10.1037/h0025695 (https://doi.org/10.1037/h0025695). PMID 5650563 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/56
50563).
100. Shepard, R.N. (1967). "Recognition memory for words, sentences, and pictures".
Journal of Learning and Verbal
Behavior. 6: 156–163. doi:10.1016/s0022-5371(67)80067-7(https://doi.org/10.1016/s0022-5371%2867%2980067-
7).
101. McBride, D. M.; Dosher, B.A. (2002). "A comparison of conscious and automatic memory processes for picture and
word stimuli: a process dissociation analysis".Consciousness and Cognition. 11: 423–460. doi:10.1016/s1053-
8100(02)00007-7 (https://doi.org/10.1016/s1053-8100%2802%2900007-7) .
102. Defetyer, M. A.; Russo, R.; McPartlin, P. L. (2009). "The picture superiority effect in recognition memory: a
developmental study using the response signal procedure".Cognitive Development. 24: 265–273.
doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2009.05.002 (https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2009.05.002).
103. Whitehouse, A. J.; Maybery, M.T.; Durkin, K. (2006). "The development of the picture-supe
riority effect". British
Journal of Developmental Psychology. 24: 767–773. doi:10.1348/026151005X74153(https://doi.org/10.1348/026151
005X74153).
104. Ally, B. A.; Gold, C. A.; Budson, A. E. (2009)."The picture superiority effect in patients with Alzheimer's disease and
mild cognitive impairment"(https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2763351). Neuropsychologia. 47: 595–
598. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.10.010(https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.10.010) .
PMC 2763351 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2763351) .
105. Curran, T.; Doyle, J. (2011). "Picture superiority doubly dissociates the ERP correlates of recollection and familiarity".
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 23 (5): 1247–1262. doi:10.1162/jocn.2010.21464(https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.20
10.21464). PMID 20350169 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20350169).
106. Martin, G. Neil; Neil R. Carlson; William Buskist (2007).Psychology (3rd ed.). Pearson Education. pp. 309–310.
ISBN 978-0-273-71086-8.
107. O'Brien, Edward J.; Myers, Jerome L. (1985). "When comprehension dif ficulty improves memory for text".Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory , and Cognition. 11 (1): 12–21. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.11.1.12(https://d
oi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.11.1.12).
108. Rubin, Wetzler & Nebes, 1986; Rubin, Rahhal & Poon, 1998
109. David A. Lieberman (8 December 2011).Human Learning and Memory(https://books.google.com/books?id=mJsV
-V
r8Q6sC&pg=PA432). Cambridge University Press. p. 432.ISBN 978-1-139-50253-5.
110. Morton, Crowder & Prussin, 1971
111. Ian Pitt; Alistair D. N. Edwards (2003).Design of Speech-Based Devices: A Practical Guide(https://books.google.co
m/books?id=zQ10cPSz1lMC&pg=PA26). Springer. p. 26. ISBN 978-1-85233-436-9.
112. Steton, Chess; et al. (12 December 2017)."Does Time Really Slow Down during a Frightening Event?" (http://journal
s.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0001295). PLoS ONE. 2 (12). Bibcode:2007PLoSO...2.1295S(ht
tp://adsabs.harvard.edu/abs/2007PLoSO...2.1295S) . doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0001295(https://doi.org/10.1371/jour
nal.pone.0001295).
113. E. Bruce Goldstein. Cognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research and Everyday Experience
(https://books.goo
gle.com/books?id=9TUIAAAAQBAJ&pg=P A231). Cengage Learning. p. 231.ISBN 978-1-133-00912-2.
114. "Not everyone is in such awe of the internet"(https://www.standard.co.uk/news/not-everyone-is-in-such-awe-of-the-i
nternet-6383970.html). Evening Standard. Evening Standard. Retrieved 28 October 2015.
115. Poppenk, Walia, Joanisse, Danckert, & Köhler, 2006
116. Von Restorff, H (1933). "Über die Wirkung von Bereichsbildungen im Spurenfeld (The fectsef of field formation in the
trace field)" ". Psychological Research. 18 (1): 299–342. doi:10.1007/bf02409636 (https://doi.org/10.1007/bf0240963
6).
117. Kahneman, Daniel; Shane Frederick (2002). "Representativeness Revisited: Attribute Substitution in Intuitive
Judgment". In Thomas Gilovich; Dale Griffin; Daniel Kahneman. Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive
Judgment. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 49–81.ISBN 978-0-521-79679-8. OCLC 47364085 (https://
www.worldcat.org/oclc/47364085).
118. Slovic, Paul; Melissa Finucane; Ellen Peters; Donald G. MacGregor (2002). "The Af
fect Heuristic". In Thomas
Gilovich; Dale Griffin; Daniel Kahneman.Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment. Cambridge
University Press. pp. 397–420.ISBN 0-521-79679-2.
119. Scopelliti, Irene; Morewedge, Carey K.; McCormick, Erin; Min, H. Lauren; Lebrecht, Sophie; Kassam, Karim S.
(2015-04-24). "Bias Blind Spot: Structure, Measurement, and Consequences"(http://pubsonline.informs.org/doi/10.1
287/mnsc.2014.2096). Management Science. 61: 2468–2486. doi:10.1287/mnsc.2014.2096(https://doi.org/10.1287/
mnsc.2014.2096).
120. Morewedge, Carey K.; Yoon, Haewon; Scopelliti, Irene; Symborski, Carl W.; Korris, James H.; Kassam, Karim S.
(2015-10-01). "Debiasing Decisions Improved Decision Making With a Single raining
T Intervention" (http://bbs.sagep
ub.com/content/2/1/129). Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences
. 2 (1): 129–140.
doi:10.1177/2372732215600886(https://doi.org/10.1177/2372732215600886). ISSN 2372-7322 (https://www.worldc
at.org/issn/2372-7322).
References
Baron, Jonathan (1994).Thinking and deciding (2nd ed.). Cambridge University Press.ISBN 0-521-43732-6.
Baron, Jonathan (2000).Thinking and deciding (3rd ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-
65030-5.
Bishop, Michael A.; Trout, J. D. (2004). Epistemology and the Psychology of Human Judgment
. New York: Oxford
University Press. ISBN 0-19-516229-3.
Gilovich, Thomas (1993). "How We Know What Isn't So: The Fallibility of Human Reason in Everyday Life". New
York: The Free Press. ISBN 0-02-911706-2.
Gilovich, Thomas; Griffin, Dale; Kahneman, Daniel (2002). Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive
judgment. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.ISBN 0-521-79679-2.
Greenwald, Anthony G.(1980). "The Totalitarian Ego: Fabrication and Revision of Personal History"(PDF).
American Psychologist. American Psychological Association.35 (7): 603–618. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.35.7.603.
ISSN 0003-066X.
Hardman, David (2009).Judgment and decision making: psychological perspectives
. Wiley-Blackwell. ISBN 978-1-
4051-2398-3.
Kahneman, Daniel; Slovic, Paul; Tversky
, Amos (1982). Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases
.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.ISBN 0-521-28414-7.
Kahneman, Daniel; Knetsch, Jack L.; Thaler , Richard H. (1991). "Anomalies: The Endowment Effect, Loss Aversion,
and Status Quo Bias" (PDF). The Journal of Economic Perspectives. American Economic Association.5 (1): 193–
206. doi:10.1257/jep.5.1.193. Archived from the original (PDF) on November 24, 2012.
Plous, Scott (1993). The Psychology of Judgment and Decision Making
. New York: McGraw-Hill. ISBN 0-07-050477-
6.
Schacter, Daniel L. (1999). "The Seven Sins of Memory: Insights From Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience"
(PDF). American Psychologist. American Psychological Association.54 (3): 182–203. doi:10.1037/0003-
066X.54.3.182. ISSN 0003-066X. PMID 10199218. Archived from the original (PDF) on May 13, 2013.
Sutherland, Stuart (2007).Irrationality. Pinter & Martin. ISBN 978-1-905177-07-3.
Tetlock, Philip E. (2005).Expert Political Judgment: how good is it? how can we know?
. Princeton: Princeton
University Press. ISBN 978-0-691-12302-8.
Virine, L.; Trumper, M. (2007). Project Decisions: The Art and Science. Vienna, VA: Management Concepts.
ISBN 978-1-56726-217-9.
Text is available under theCreative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License ; additional terms may apply. By using this
site, you agree to the Terms of Use and Privacy Policy. Wikipedia® is a registered trademark of theWikimedia
Foundation, Inc., a non-profit organization.