The Effect of Handwriting Without Tears On Montessori Four-Year-O

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 53

St.

Catherine University
SOPHIA
Masters of Arts in Education Action Research
Education
Papers

12-2017

The Effect of Handwriting Without Tears on


Montessori Four-year-olds' Handwriting Ability
Shelley B. Valdez
St. Catherine University, [email protected]

Follow this and additional works at: https://sophia.stkate.edu/maed


Part of the Early Childhood Education Commons

Recommended Citation
Valdez, Shelley B.. (2017). The Effect of Handwriting Without Tears on Montessori Four-year-olds' Handwriting Ability. Retrieved
from Sophia, the St. Catherine University repository website: https://sophia.stkate.edu/maed/237

This Action Research Project is brought to you for free and open access by the Education at SOPHIA. It has been accepted for inclusion in Masters of
Arts in Education Action Research Papers by an authorized administrator of SOPHIA. For more information, please contact [email protected].
Running head: THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 1

The Effect of Handwriting Without Tears on Montessori Four-year-olds'

Handwriting Ability

An Action Research Report


By Shelley B. Valdez
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 2

The Effect of Handwriting Without Tears on Montessori Four-year-olds' Handwriting Ability

Submitted on November 12th, 2017

in fulfillment of final requirements for the MAED degree

Shelley B. Valdez

Saint Catherine University

St. Paul, Minnesota

Advisor______________________________ Date_______________
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 3

Abstract

The action research question for this study was, “What effect would the addition of a

program by Handwriting Without Tears (HWT) called, Transition to Kindergarten, in a block

everyday have on the development of handwriting skills of my four-year-old students?” Beyond

utilizing the HWT program, the reviewed literature on handwriting instruction expounded on six

major themes: brain benefits of handwriting, blocks of handwriting instruction (speed and

legibility), name writing (gross & fine motor skills), the connection between writing and reading,

and phonological awareness. This project was conducted in a private Montessori school in South

Florida, where six preschool participants (age four) were studied and evaluated for six weeks.

Data was compiled by using a presentation log, attitude scale, and a self-generated rubric to track

the following: pencil firmness on paper, directionality, letter formation, spacing, line usage,

circle closure, name writing, and copying a sentence to a line. The researcher's overall results

were positive when assessing handwriting attributes of formation, size, neatness, speed, posture,

pencil grip and helping hand position. Future implementation of the action research project will

be introduced to the entire preschool as a new addition to the handwriting curriculum.

Keywords: Handwriting without Tears, Transition to Kindergarten, brain benefits, speed and
legibility, academic success, name writing, and phonological awareness.
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 4

Introduction

Guiding young students by use of Maria Montessori’s method is my life’s work. The

spontaneous energy observed in a four-year-old when picking up a pencil is exhilarating.

Children have a sensitive period for handwriting (Lillard, 2007). Currently, the Common Core

does not recognize this sensitive period, or innate desire within that a child radiates when writing

their first phonemic sounds. In the last ten plus years, educational researchers have worked to

identify solutions to the problem of early childhood students having difficulty with basic letter

formation and writing their names. Many four-year-old students are expected to print their name

and copy a full sentence including punctuation on a line before transitioning to kindergarten.

The problem I am trying to solve at my school is the four-year-old children leaving the

Montessori preschool program are unable to write their name and/or copy a full sentence

(uppercase and lowercase letters) including punctuation on a line. This skill-set is required as

part of the learning objectives tested on the Metropolitan Readiness Assessment or MRT for

kindergarten. Kindergarten teachers have informed me that lowercase handwriting skills and

beginning initial sound knowledge is necessary for success when children enter kindergarten.

A Private Montessori school in South Florida, enrolling approximately 315 children aged

two to the eighth grade was the location of this action research study: specifically, an early

childhood classroom with ages three to five which offers a ratio of eight students to one teacher.

Here, thirty children are in the classroom; 82% of these children are Caucasian, 6% are of Asian

descent, 4% are of Eastern European background, 2% are African American and 6% are

Hispanic. All four teachers hold a form of teacher training for ages three to six. Our classroom is

a two-year cycle before moving onto an innovative kindergarten program utilizing

differentiation. Classroom physical dimensions create a capacity to hold thirty-two students with
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 5

four Montessori trained guides. Each teacher works with eight children in a learning group that

rotates in the following fashion: Monday: Practical Life, Tuesday: Language, Wednesday: Math,

Thursday: Sensorial, and Friday: Outdoor Learning Environment. This work cycle is considered

our Montessori morning work time; work cycle length is around ninety minutes.

My goal is to arrange a block of time for my action research project within our

classroom. I will then be able to arrange my role of Montessori Director around this block of

teaching hours. Children spend two years in this Montessori prepared classroom environment

before an evaluation is conducted by use of the Metropolitan Readiness Test (MRT) for

acceptance into a kindergarten classroom. The kindergarten class is not a Montessori based

program. Instead, it utilizes differentiation and first-grade textbooks and workbooks. Typically,

there are twelve children in a kindergarten classroom with a floating assistant. Our students are

challenged in a supportive environment; our families given the opportunity to get involved on

campus, and our faculty and staff are encouraged to be innovative and continually go above and

beyond the call of duty.

I am hoping that the addition of Transition to Kindergarten by Handwriting Without

Tears (HWT) will provide added initial letter sound knowledge and fine motor skill-building. I

plan to look for research on the effects of the actual program by HWT. Purposefully, this action

research study is to determine what effect utilizing a program by Handwriting Without Tears

called Transition to Kindergarten in a block every day had on the development of handwriting

skills of my four-year-old students.

Literature Review

Graham et al. (2008), discovered that 23% of children have difficulty in handwriting, an

identifiable concern. Issues of legibility, size of letter, space between, and reversals are of
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 6

interest when assessing handwriting skills (Dinehart, 2015). Evidence from research data

communicated that when children write less they are more likely not to read at grade level or

write at grade level. Composition still takes place, but it is not inventive. Children have to

worry about rules within the English language before cultivating creativity in writing (Berninger

et al., 1997; Graham, 2010; Puranik & Al Otaiba, 2012).

Current literature on educational best practices shows a variety of approaches to solving

this problem through direct instruction on the skill of handwriting. An initial benefit of direct

instruction is that handwriting will potentially become more and more legible (Graham, 2010).

Proper letter formation is significant because it has been studied to be a contributing factor to

future academic success. Growth in the skills associated with handwriting, including letter

formation directionality (Lifshitz & Har-Zvi, 2015), have been related to practicing tracing

letters and writing one’s name. The implementation of the Handwriting Without Tears (HWT)

program has proven to be effective in facilitating the development of handwriting skills

(Handwriting Without Tears, 2015).

The HWT curriculum teaches students uppercase letters before lowercase letters, then

clusters of letters with similar strokes or the same starting points are taught together. The HWT

intervention is designed to be implemented daily (Handwriting Without Tears, 2015). When

used daily, this explicit handwriting instruction has been attributed to academic success. In a

three-year study conducted by HWT involving over 14,000 kindergarten students who utilized

the program, the children demonstrated an improvement of 27 percent from the beginning of the

academic school year (Handwriting Without Tears, 2014). This study also found that 83 percent

of these students were able to demonstrate innate memory of letter placement on a line

(Handwriting Without Tears, 2015).


THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 7

Many traditional schools implement blocks of handwriting instruction for kindergarten

aged children. Handwriting curricula, such as HWT, have been utilized in many applications

like assisting the child in writing his or her name. The latest research solution came from

LeBrun, McLaughlin, Derby, and McKenzie (2012) in a classroom of thirty-one early childhood

students, aged three to five; some children did not know how to write initial letters and some

children in the study needed to improve on letter formation. The adding of instruction through

HWT resulted in growth for each student involved. The students who were performing worst

with the lowest letter formation made the greatest improvements in handwriting. The researchers,

LeBrun et al. (2012), were able to demonstrate that all students showed a marked improvement

in handwriting using the HWT rubric by referring to the program as a “...well-developed,

interactive means of teaching primary writing skills for students who are typically developing…”

(p. 376). However, the researchers did find a gap in the effectiveness of HWT. This program

was not beneficial to the highest learning group in that it lacked challenge as they “did not

improve at the rapid rates as the two lower achieving groups” (LeBrun et al., 2012, p. 376). In

fact, their findings showed the lowest handwriting group made the biggest gains. The work

suggests that most young children, particularly those who are having the greatest difficulty with

letter formation, soar in letter production with the use of HWT as supplemented curriculum. A

similar study by Steele et al. (2015) tracked a four-year-old preschool student with

developmental delays who showed improvement when asked to “write your name” after using

HWT. Offering daily handwriting practice, allows young children to commit to a using a high

degree and amount of cognitive functioning when forming letters (Graham, 2010). Therefore,

explicit handwriting instruction needs to be conducted daily to provide this innate and automatic

connection (Handwriting Without Tears, 2015).


THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 8

Handwriting instruction is not just incorporated into traditional classrooms but also into

Montessori learning environments. The philosophy emphasizes an innate desire of order,

coordination, and independence (Montessori, 1936/1966; Montessori, 1949/1995). Montessori’s

pedagogical foundation came from research she did in the development of materials and

exercises. Lessons promoting self-care offered an indirect preparation for holding a pencil.

Maria Montessori observed that signs of handwriting development were found in infancy as she

witnessed young children imitating the adult by grasping a writing utensil. In a Montessori,

prepared classroom, by the age of four, children are developmentally ready and eager to begin

the preparation for handwriting (Montessori 1949/1995). Letter-sound knowledge is absorbed by

the fingers tracing lowercase letters; students simultaneously learn to hold a pencil using their

three-finger pincer grip to control their handwriting. Children experiencing a sensitive period for

writing gravitate towards lessons that involve penmanship. This purposeful movement

spontaneously occurs in the preparation for handwriting. The added curriculum of HWT

supports this pattern by its guided lesson plans (Handwriting Without Tears, 2015).

Handwriting Development

Children enter an early childhood classroom already knowing about writing before they begin

school. Yet, there is little research on handwriting before kindergarten (Dinehart, 2015; Puranik

& Al Otaiba, 2012). Handwriting in preschool is typically composed of universal characteristics.

Young children begin literacy work by drawing or scribbling, with space between lines offering

the suggestion of handwriting. Previous studies on handwriting indicate that children in early

childhood begin scribbling letter-like forms around age two. The act of scribbling acquires

characteristics of writing in directions such as left to right, top to bottom, and diagonally

(Dinehart, 2015; Puranik & Lonigan, 2011). An introduction to handwriting is beneficial to the
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 9

four-year-old. Puranik and Al Otaiba (2012) contributed that as students approach their

kindergarten year, their handwriting skill sets have developed to writing sentence structures

which lead to a composition.

The act of handwriting is kinesthetic (Hart, Fitzpatrick, & Cortesa, 2010). Independently

creating letters and words by hand is an intricate skill which requires children to combine their

knowledge of letter formation with fine motor skills. In the Montessori classroom, daily life

skills such as strengthening the three-finger pincer grip, are imperative for the child to develop

fine motor skills (Dinehart, 2015; Hoy, Egan, & Feder, 2011) and hand strength in preparation

for holding a pencil (Montessori, 1936/1966) even before kindergarten (Hart et. al., 2010).

Lifshitz and Har-Zvi (2015) defined pencil grip as stable yet dynamic. The wrist is placed on the

writing surface to stabilize it. The fingers enable the dynamic use of the writing tools, such that

it may move in the directions required for formation of a letter. The three-finger-pincer grip hold

is ideal for tracing and copying letters by memory (Steele et al., 2015). When the young hand is

strengthened, the child will be less frustrated in holding a pencil (Lifshitz & Har-Zvi, 2015).

Children must develop “fundamental skills” (Dinehart, 2015) for handwriting, which

Montessorians support by creating lessons that foster fine motor development through intensive

repetition (Hoy et al., 2011). Children in pre-K develop their pencil grip by first creating

scribbles, then straight lines, and finally curved lines (Steele et al., 2015). When assessing fine

motor skills for handwriting there is no gold standard (Piek, Hands, & Licari, 2012), yet there are

a few ways of assessing readiness for handwriting.

Dinehart (2015) conducted a systematic review of standardized handwriting readiness

assessments. In this extensive study, 3000 preschool-aged participants were assessed for fine

motor skills in handwriting instruction. The large participant group sets this study above others
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 10

of its kind. Supporting the statements of Maria Montessori, this study found that handwriting

instruction prepares students for reading success (Dinehart, 2015).

Beyond utilizing the Handwriting Without Tears program, the reviewed literature on

handwriting instruction expounded on six major themes: brain benefits of handwriting, blocks of

handwriting instruction (speed and legibility), name writing (gross & fine motor skills), the

connection between writing and reading, and phonological awareness.

Brain Benefits

Writing is an intellectual process combined with manual dexterity. It is the ability to

express thoughts with graphic symbols. Although there has been relatively little research on

handwriting in early childhood, there is the support that cognitive function serves a role in

handwriting instruction (Dinehart, 2015; Graham, 2010; Hoy et al., 2011; Puranik & Al Otaiba,

2012; Standing, 1998).

Students benefit from the types of handwriting interventions with a cognate focus where

“students received instruction to help them think their way through letter formation and self-

correction” (Hoy et al., 2011, p. 19). Overall, handwriting instruction has not only been

implemented to serve as a foundation of literacy skills (sound recognition), but it also has been

shown to impact neurological processes. Puranik and Al Otaiba (2012) found a connection

between tracing letters with the hand and the section of the brain for memory retrieval.

Cognitive connections must occur to be a fluent writer (Dinehart, 2015). The results

obtained by Graham (2010) in a national survey of first grade to third grade teachers indicated

that over half admitted that handwriting is fundamental to completing a writing assignment as

early as first grade. His findings argued that children who have developed letter formation can

compose quicker on paper, which relates to enriched content.


THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 11

Handwriting needs to be practiced for the connection to become automatic. Prepared

lessons that make this possible were of particular interest to Maria Montessori when she said,

“The hand has now become the instrument of the brain: and it is through the activity of his hands

that he enriches his experience, and develops himself at the same time” (Standing, 1998, p. 112).

Children had prepared their hand, and their mind to be then able to write.

For many years, significant effort has been devoted to the study of the efficacy of HWT

for students from kindergarten to upper elementary. However, very few publications can be

found in the literature that addresses the issue of writing before kindergarten. In a rare study of

two preschool-aged children, with the goal of teaching these children to write their name, each

student was provided handwriting instruction through Handwriting Without Tears worksheets

and practice tracing letters over the teacher's yellow highlighter to form lowercase letters

correctly. Results indicated that the children’s letter formation in name writing improved on the

criteria of “slant, size, and formation” (McBride et al., 2009, p. 22).

Blocks of Time

Maria Montessori called writing a “natural phenomenon” (Montessori, 1936/1966).

Since her time, the literature focuses primarily on “explicit instruction” in handwriting

(Berninger et al., 1997; Graham et al., 2008). An excitement for writing, “recurs annually in a

well-functioning Montessori Primary classroom when the first 4–year-old suddenly realizes,

after months of working with the preparatory materials, that he or she can write” (Lillard, 2007

p. 198). Research suggests blocks of handwriting instruction result in positive outcomes and

academic success (Berninger et al., 1997). Handwriting is a “sequential process” (Puranik &

Lonigan, 2011) which is supported by frequent practice in a Montessori classroom. Hoy et. al.

(2011) advocated for ample handwriting practice, specifically occurring bi-weekly for twenty
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 12

minutes each session. In addition to group instruction, Hart et al. (2010) suggested that

individual instructional time is also beneficial within handwriting practice to promote

independence. Graham (2010) proposed that daily instruction in handwriting could lead to

quality of writing. Offering daily handwriting practice allows for the processes to be less taxing

on thought processes and more innately driven, enabling children to devote a higher amount of

neurological resources to use for letter formation. Dinehart (2015) explored differentiated

writing experiences and instructing on explicit handwriting instruction to innately cultivate the

internal automatic letter formation that will then make room for the brain to gather complex

composing skills. As students practice the skill of handwriting, they are able to sustain attention

for longer periods of time. The lengthened concentration models order, coordination, and

independence.

Handwriting Instruction and Academic Success

The current literature on handwriting abounds with examples to support future academic

performance and success (Berninger et al., 1997, Hart et al., 2010, Hoy et al., 2011). Dinehart

(2015) concludes, “The extent to which these factors are applicable to children before they enter

formal schooling is unclear, but recent work suggests that the influence of handwriting on later

academic performance may have some of its roots in the years before children enter school” (p.

102). In the Montessori classroom, academic success begins in the practical life area with

activities involving tweezing, tonging, and transferring from left to right. Not only do these

lessons indirectly promote left to right tracking, but they also strengthen the three-finger pincer

grip in preparation of holding a pencil. Practical life work prepares for handwriting instruction,

which in turn leads to literacy in early childhood. Handwriting instructional methods are used

with explicit instruction by definition. Many studies have demonstrated that handwriting
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 13

interventions involving teacher modeling succeed in improving handwriting skills in children

while promoting future academic success, especially in composition (Berninger et al., 1997).

In a study, close to 700 children in first grade were screened to determine which children

were at risk for handwriting issues (Berninger et al., 1997). From the study, 144 students in first

grade, 103 boys, and 41 girls, were found to be at risk when assessed in speed and legibility

when recalling from memory. The children met three times a week for 20 minutes each session

for a total of 24 sessions. “Converging evidence across multiple measures showed that

combining numbered arrows and memory retrieval was the most effective treatment for

improving both handwriting and compositional fluency comprising with time limits” (Berninger

et al., 1997). The idea is that when the memory is sparked in writing letters, it is innately driven

to “creation of retrieval routines” (Berninger et al., 1997, p. 654). An example of a retrieval

routine is the teacher guiding the children to “think their way through letter formation and self-

correction” (Hoy et al., 2011, p. 19). Other instructional approaches include motoric imitation,

visual cues, memory retrieval, visual cues and memory, and copying. The research suggests the

duality of visual cues and memory serving as a positive relationship when children assessed for

handwriting success. The memory and visual cue retrieval tactic showed academic success on

fluency of words (Berninger et al., 1997) contributing to composition.

Writing from memory and fluency of writing are crucial to composition and creative

writing. Several publications have appeared in recent years suggesting the terms “speed” (Julius,

Meir, Shechter-Nissim, & Adi-Japha, 2016) and “legibility” (Graham, 2010) to define quality

handwriting (Dinehart, 2015; Hoy et al., 2011). As reported by Graham (2010), quality letter

formation is necessary for the brain to focus more on comprehension versus letter direction. A

young mind is beaming with ideas to articulate through written text. A hindrance to self-esteem
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 14

and productivity is when a child has to first think about letter orientation, printing of letters

followed by remembering what the story is trying to convey to the reader. Graham (2008)

relates that the confidence to perform as a creative writer is taken over by how to construct letter

formations. In fact, Kohn (2000) found that students who turn in a legible essay on the

standardized test have a better chance of attaining a higher grade. If handwriting is legible, the

students will be more successful in postsecondary education as notes are being taken during

lecture only to read them later for study (Dinehart, 2015; Graham, 2010).

Writing One’s Own Name

One of the most important aspects of handwriting for young writers is developing the

skill of writing one’s name. Puranik and Lonigan (2011) conducted a study among 372 three to

four-year-old children in Florida. They found children naturally gravitated toward their name in

print visually because it is attractive to them and, therefore, use it to practice the symbol and

conventions of writing. “Children’s knowledge about their names at 3 and 4 years of age

extends beyond the universal characteristics of all language systems to include specific shapes of

letters” (Puranik & Lonigan, 2011, p. 581). The research study suggests that practice writing

one’s name may be an enticing way for students to engage in handwriting practice.

Writing in Early Childhood Leads to Reading

Montessori (1936/1966) discovered that children discover writing before reading; writing

naturally precedes reading. As handwriting develops, children with healthy and confident fine

motor skills do better not only in composition but also in reading assessments as late as age

eleven (Dinehart, 2015). Writing by hand not only teaches letter formation but aids in sound-

letter correspondences, a skill necessary for decoding in reading (Puranik & Lonigan, 2011;

Puranik & Al Otaiba, 2012; Wolf, 2016).


THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 15

Phonological Awareness

Letter writing skills are naturally a part of early childhood development (Puranik & Al

Otaiba, 2012). Guided practice, where the teacher models the connection between letters and

sounds, develops phonological awareness (Berninger et al., 1997). Handwriting before

kindergarten results in letter-name recognition. Puranik and Al Otaiba, (2012) found that “Being

able to spell words is a major step in the writing process where children demonstrate knowledge

of phonological awareness skills and knowledge of letters and letter-sound correspondences to

mimic conventional writing” (p. 1525). The research demonstrated the feasibility of innate

connection with letter-sound knowledge or “cognitive focus” (Hoy et al., 2011, p. 19).

“Alphabet knowledge” (Jones, Clark, & Reutzel, 2013) or letter-name knowledge is the “most

durable” indicator of future academic achievement in literacy. It is recommended that pairing a

letter’s name with its respective sound leads to academic success (Berninger et al., 1997).

Conclusion

The addition of handwriting instruction among four-year-olds offers a guideline for the

best practices for students in pre-Kindergarten. Given the importance of handwriting for

developing students’ writing and reading skills, there is little research on how to best support

preschool-aged students in this area. Handwriting Without Tears has been established as a

program that has resulted in student growth in letter formation, yet it has not been studied

enough in early childhood learning environments and has not been studied in Montessori

classrooms. Given the necessity of handwriting instruction for student outcomes, additional

research on the implementation HWT in a Montessori classroom could offer a framework for

teachers for how to best add a curriculum to instruct using quality handwriting instruction. The
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 16

literature review could serve as future improvements of Montessori teacher training in

preparation for children going to kindergarten.

The key to an authentic Montessori language arts curriculum is the quality of the

materials a teacher provides for children's introduction to writing. This sensitive period for

language acquisition innately guides the children to investigate their environment. Children are

attracted to handwriting, such as how to make the letters, the order in sounds represented by

letters, and the order that follows with phonological awareness instruction is the ability to hear

and identify, individual sounds-phonemes in spoken words. Thus, it is a natural conclusion that

HWT may be an instinctive way to supplement the Montessori curriculum in the kindergarten

year to support students’ growth in handwriting. Again, the literature provides support for a

potentially successful handwriting implementation for teaching students age four. Proving the

importance of handwriting instruction for four-year-old’s could offer an opportunity for teachers

on how to best supplement the curriculum to provide quality handwriting instruction.

Methodology

Over the past four years, I observed my students falling behind on literacy skills,

particularly those of writing initial letters, name writing, and copying a sentence onto a line. I

am often asked, “Miss Shelley, how do I make a(n) [insert letter]?” I feel there is a learning gap

and the kindergarten teachers have informed me that lowercase handwriting skills and beginning

initial sound knowledge are necessary for success when children enter kindergarten. Therefore,

the problem I am trying to solve is the four-year-old children leaving the Montessori preschool

are unable to write their name and/or copy a full sentence (uppercase and lowercase letters)

including punctuation on a line. This skill-set is required as part of the learning objectives theses

on the Metropolitan Readiness Assessment or MRT for Kindergarten.


THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 17

The methodology for my study focused on differentiated instruction utilizing HWT.

Every day, during the morning work time, I provided explicit handwriting instruction from the

HWT Transition to Kindergarten curriculum to my six students in blocks of 20 minutes. I pulled

all six children for twenty minutes each morning for explicit instruction. I collected data for six

weeks from September 4th to October 13th, 2017. Daily handwriting instruction was modeled on

a chalkboard using verbiage from the HWT lesson plan, to my group of six students and

individually within the following four skill-sets:

1) Imitation (Demonstration)

2) Copying a Sentence

3) Independent Writing (Journal Artifact Collection)

4) Name Writing

It is normal classroom practice for my students to practice writing their name and

copying a sentence onto a line one day a week while in the language area. This study added the

HWT curriculum inviting children to handwriting instruction and independent writing time.

My first form of data was an anecdotal tool collection by self-generated Presentation Log

and Reflection Journal [Appendix A]. I utilized the Presentation Log and Reflection Journal as a

way to track what I am teaching each day. The data tool collection was also used for logging any

events/situations that may have an effect on my action research study; to see if any factor could

impact a child's ability or progress. I reflected on questions like the following:

1. Did I have to repeat lessons?

2. How did the children respond to the lessons?

3. Did anything outside of the norm affect the lessons?


THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 18

My second data collection was in the form of an artifact collection of samples of CVC

(consonant-vowel-consonant) written work in the student journals and/or other normal classroom

work. Student journal work is a part of the students’ normal classroom routine and work

samples were given by use of the Handwriting Without Tears journal pages for the purpose of

this study. Student journal work was scored according to the Printing Concerns Checklist

[Appendix B] from HWT. Every Tuesday of my action research project I collected written work

demonstrating CVC knowledge for the purpose of handwriting analysis. To keep student

identity confidential, I did not include the student name, but instead a color word. Also, I used a

1-4 scale as follows:

1 = Does not meet

2 = Approaches

3 = Meets

4 = Exceeds

This number system is more quantifiable and correlates better with my other data

collection tools. The purpose of this data was to see if the work during the HWT intervention is

carried into their normal classroom work. Baseline data was collected (the first day of the study)

using data tools number two [Appendix B], three [Appendix C] and four [Appendix D] described

below. All data tools were used again at the end (concluding data) during the study.

The third form of data collection tool was an assessment of “write your name” and “copy

a full sentence to a line”. As a part of our normal classroom work, students were asked weekly

to write their name and copy a sentence on a line. For the purposes of this project, students were

provided a HWT journal in which to complete these tasks. Samples of these writing tasks were

collected weekly (Monday) and used for analysis. These journal samples were then evaluated
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 19

against the rubric I created and titled, “Handwriting-Pre-K-Assessment: Writing Name &

Sentence Rubric”, [Appendix C].

A fourth form of data tool collection was implementing a Student Attitude Scale

[Appendix D], a quantitative data collection that used closed-ended self-assessment. The inquiry

form was a visual scale presented to my students on the iPad (point of interest). Three choices

were offered in “thumbs.” The first option is a “thumbs-up” with the word, “Yes”; the second is

a “thumb-sideways” with the words “in between” and the third is a “thumbs-down” showing the

word, “No.”

My students were given the opportunity to share their feelings about their handwriting,

both at the beginning of my action research (for baseline data) and again at the end. I read the

questions to the students. This feedback form allowed my four-year-old students to

communicate how they felt about their own handwriting.

My fifth data collection tool was using the HWT workbook/work/writing that is produced

as a part of the HWT lessons and project interventions. This was then collected weekly and

scored with the Printing Concerns Checklist [Appendix B] by HWT. I did not include the

student name, but instead a color word. Instead of the checkmark suggestion, I used a 1-4 scale

as follows; this number system is more quantifiable and correlated better with my other data

collection tools:

1= Does not meet

2 = Approaches

3 = Meets

4 = Exceeds
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 20

Analysis of Data

While the purpose of this study was to improve the handprint of writing one’s name and

copying a sentence to a line, it is important to note the action research project was conducted in a

two-year Montessori cycle, ages three to five, prepared environment. Not all calendar weeks

were five consistent days due to Hurricane Irma in South Florida. The school was closed by our

Head of School for one day and by Florida’s Governor Rick Scott for six days. The Labor Day

holiday and Rosh Hashanah holiday took away two of our school days; resulting in a total of

nine missed days of data collection.

The classroom was not an authentically Montessori multi-aged classroom. Instead, the

majority of children were age three which indirectly caused an unfavorable indoor work

environment that presented a shift of my participating student’s (age four to five) work cycles.

Day two of my data collection I committed to conducting my action research project in our

outdoor learning space which indirectly created an environment for some of the lessons being

longer than the twenty minutes initially anticipated.

The overall results of this action research project study were positive! Qualitative data

from the Attitude Scale [Appendix D] taken at the beginning and again at the end were coded by

a number. I used number one to represent a “thumbs up”; number two to represent “in between”

and number three to represent “thumbs down”. Below is my tally sheet with a color word for

each child in the study. Please note that if there is no number of data to show, that means the

child was absent during the data collection.


THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 21

Here is my column chart to show the Attitude Scale [Appendix D] at the beginning of my

action research project with my six participating students. My student coded the color Purple

was not at school for this assessment. At the end of my six-week action research project, I

repeated the Attitude Scale with the children. Below is another column chart to reflect the last

week results. My student color code Yellow was not at school for this assessment. In the end,

the results indicate that overall the children were in positive agreement that their letters were

formed correctly, written on a line, modeled proper spacing, and they thought their writing

looked neat.

Beginning of Action Research Project:


THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 22

End of Action Research Project:

A Presentation Log and Reflection Journal, an anecdotal tool, shows that students were

engaged in writing for a longer length of time after the inclusion of HWT strategies during the

mini-lesson. During this uninterrupted research time frame, students were engaged in the smaller

group instruction, while sitting in our outdoor learning environment. A handwriting lesson was

presented and logged into the Presentation Journal [Appendix A] to track what lesson was

presented each day. I also noted how long the students were engaged after my presentation.

Students were found to work with concentration longer than the originally planned twenty

minutes. The longest work cycle time was 43 minutes. During the smaller group lessons, the

students would chat among themselves while enjoying the wonders of the outdoor environment.

Students lengthened their writing time when there were no interruptions of a special such as

physical education or rehearsals for our Peace Program. My students were able to write for

longer lengths of time when they were given time to work on their writing, after my instructional

lesson. Several students inquired if they could work on their HWT workbook during afternoon

work time. One student consistently asked to work on the HWT workbook during rest.
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 23

When looking at the qualitative data collection tool used in the assessment, “write your

name” and “copy a full sentence to a line,” the results were expected and yet a bit surprising

considering the nine days the school was closed resulting in missed data collection days. For the

purpose of this action research project and as a part of our classroom, students were provided a

journal in which to complete the two writing tasks. Students were asked weekly to write their

name and copy a sentence onto a line; I collected the data from this normal classroom work. The

students used the HWT journal, and I took samples each week to be used for analysis. The

journal samples were used against my self-generated rubric called “Handwriting Pre-K

Assessment: Writing Name & Sentence Rubric” [Appendix C]. The categories and questions

were arranged on my rubric to address attributes within handwriting, especially aims to address

each of the following skill sets: Paper/Pencil; Directionality; Letter Formation; Spacing; Line

Usage; Circle Closure; Name Writing; and Copy Sentence to a Line. My hope was there would

be an improvement in handwriting when my students were asked to write their name and copy a

sentence to a line. The self-generated rubric allowed me to analyze whether the implementation

of my methodology (HWT) correlates with an improvement of handwriting in my classroom.

When assessing my student’s work, I specifically utilized the following:

1) Position/Pencil/Paper: Does the student model the three-finger pincer grip?

2) Directionality: Does the student begin and end letters at correct beginning points?

3) Letter Formation: Are the letters from top to bottom?

4) Spacing: Does the student demonstrate spacing between letter formations?

5) Line Usage: Does the student use the provided line in their journal?

6) Circle Closure: Do circles close in the letter formation?

7) Name Writing: Are the letter formation lines horizontal, diagonal and vertical straight?
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 24

8) Copy a Sentence to a Line: Do the students copy the sentence to a line? Is there punctuation?

I took three photos to show work samples of “copy a sentence on a line” from week four

of my action research study. Each child was given a sentence to copy; it was optional to add an

illustration. I chose work samples from these three-color codes of student: Orange, Green and

Blue.

“I saw a cat”.
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 25

“I am a rat”.

“I found a bug”.

The inference to be drawn from this is that when looking at week four, the first full week

of school since Hurricane Irma, the children were detailed in neatness and letter formations. The

x and y line graph below for the six children representing a trend of improvement for line usage,

circle closure, and name writing persuades me to believe that the children were settling into the

weeks after Hurricane Irma. Again, when this data collection was gathered, the children had

already missed a total of nine days of school. Week four is significant because it is only the
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 26

second full week of school since the first day of school. See the x and y line graph summaries of

averages below for each student. Again, I used a color word to represent each of my six

students. Name writing was consistently higher than the other averages leading to believe that

there was an improvement over the six-week intervention. The scale is as follows:

2.5 = Does not meet

5 = Approaches

7.5 = Meets

10 = Exceeds

RED:
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 27

ORANGE:
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 28

YELLOW:
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 29

GREEN:
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 30

BLUE:
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 31

PURPLE:

Every Tuesday of my action research project I collected written work demonstrating

CVC knowledge for the purpose of handwriting analysis. A quantifiable number system was

used as it correlates better and compliments my other data collection tools. The purpose of this

data is to see if the work during the HWT intervention is carried into their normal classroom

work.

To answer question whether or not this implementation was successful is seen in week

six of the x and y line graph. An implication being that children improved in neatness, formation

and pencil grip. This again suggests that children were settling into a school routine after being
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 32

away for Hurricane Irma, Labor Day and Rosh Hashanah holiday. When comparing week one

scores and week six scores, it noticeably shows that students grew in handwriting knowledge; the

children really seemed to enjoy their journals! Below are the handwriting intervention results.

Red Student:
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 33

Orange Student:

Yellow Student:
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 34

Green Student:

Blue Student:
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 35

Purple Student:

My next data tool looked at my student work choices during the Montessori afternoon

work cycle for weeks four, five, and six. Data collection was only conducted for weeks four,

five, and six; due to Hurricane Irma. The data shows the skill sets students' writing was assessed

on after turning in their Kickstart to Kindergarten books each week; this writing involved a lot of

tracing and coloring of letters.

I observed skill-sets of circle closure and formation modeled as handwriting lessons were

being added to the student journals. The students used skills of the “helping hand” which they

learned in the HWT lessons in their journals while completing work. I included examples from

the handwriting intervention when working with the Montessori Pink Series or consonant-vowel-

consonant work. Data collection took place when my students were asked to write the CVC

word that they spell or read into their HWT journal. This action is a part of normal classroom

practice for a four-year-old. I used the Printing Concerns Checklist [Appendix B]. I took
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 36

photographs of spontaneous handwriting of the following three students: Red, Yellow, and

Purple.

Red Student:

Yellow Student:
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 37

Purple Student:

Informative charts below show where students were placed; an example of this rating

scale can be found in the [Appendix B] called Printing Concerns Checklist. Writing in the

Transition to Kindergarten workbook samples were collected in late August to serve as a

baseline of students writing, and another sample was collected in mid-October as a final sample.
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 38
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 39

Another element of the students' writing I observed was the length of time spent writing

after lesson each day. While reviewing all the data, I noticed that the time spent writing varied

from day to day. Throughout the project, while students were working on their writing, I allowed

them to continue their work until I observed that some students became unfocused on the

intervention of writing task. I went back to my Presentation Log to review my observational

notes and I noticed that on days that were humid outside, their writing concentration was less

than on days that were cooler in temperature outside.

My literature review explains the importance of tracing letters. What I like about HWT is
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 40

that they know to produce proper pencil holding, forming lowercase and uppercase letter,

and writing simple words and sentences. That in itself was my problem!

After reviewing all the data sources, my conclusion is the addition of Transition to

Kindergarten by HWT did provide added initial letter sound knowledge and fine motor skill-

building for my six participants. I looked for research on the effects of the actual program by

Handwriting Without Tears. The purpose of my action research study was to determine what

effect utilizing a program by Handwriting Without Tears called “Transition to Kindergarten” in a

block every day would have on the development of handwriting skills of my four-year-old

students.

Action Plan

The results of this action research project sparked change in my classroom practice as I

used the addition of Transition to Kindergarten by Handwriting Without Tears to provide added

initial letter sound knowledge and fine motor skill-building of my four-year-old students. How

would it serve them in name writing and copying a sentence onto a line? My young students

enjoyed these "workbooks" which involved a lot of tracing and coloring of uppercase and

lowercase letters. My literature review explains the importance of tracing letters. What I like

about HWT is that it is a program known to produce accurate formation of lowercase and

uppercase letters, and writing simple words and sentences. That in itself was my problem I

initially identified in my classroom. The program is designed as a fundamental strategy of

imitation, tracing, and copying. Before my intervention, I never used the HWT program.

In addition to the workbooks, I provided journals to the children. Here, they practiced

writing words or letters on a line. I created my own rubric to assess each week how the

following rated: pencil firmness on paper, directionality, letter formation, spacing, line usage,
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 41

circle closure, name writing, and copying a sentence to a line. And, to assess CVC handwriting,

I used the rubric from HWT which looked at the following: formation, size, neatness, speed,

posture, pencil grip and "helping hand." I am pleased that I can witness progression and

improvement when I look at the "x" and "y" on a line graph. I am now comfortable with the

addition of this handwriting intervention in my Montessori prepared environment. As the

Director of Montessori at this preschool, I can easily see myself training the twelve teachers

under my guidance.

Student learning has been affected by this academic addition to the classroom. A

possible impact I think my action research will have on student learning is now, the students

have a workbook and an indirect introduction to print in a book. Next year the kids in my Action

Research study will be expected to innately model the skill set of proper pencil holding by use of

the three-finger pincer grip and be comfortable writing in a workbook. The children were

thrilled to have their own workbook and journal to take out and write, especially in the afternoon

work cycle. As a Montessorian, I am skilled at observation. My students have more extended

concentration and an innate desire to write!

The parents who signed my consent form were excited to get a glimpse into the

handwriting intervention from my action research project. I have been asked by one of the

parents what my future plans are within the classroom for the next school year. I guess my work

has been even more critical than I initially thought. A thorough analysis of the data indicates that

my students gained in name writing and sentence writing which suggests possible methods to

change current education practices, which leads questions for future research. Eleven of the

thirty children in my class will have a sibling in my room in the next two years. I feel that I want

to continue my investigation at my school, but this time for an entire school semester.
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 42

The act of handwriting on a line is essential. The space, size, letter formation, and

orientation of the letter is vital. I know why I chose this topic! Handwriting of a four-year-old is

an assessment my school gives before a child may enter our Kindergarten; a first-grade academic

skill set.

I am confident in articulating my findings, it is crucial because the children have an

assessment for letter size, shape, formation, closure, and writing on a line. My students

cultivated knowledge about the attributes of handwriting development within the following

areas: closure, configuration, spacing and size. I am very proud of their accomplishments! Now,

our school has an actual handwriting program because of my action research project.
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 43

References

Berninger, V. W., Vaughan, K. B., Abbott, R. D., Abbott, S. P., Rogan, L. W., Brooks, A., &

Graham, S. (1997). Treatment of handwriting problems in beginning writers: Transfer

from handwriting to composition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(4), 652-666.

Dinehart, L. H. (2015). Handwriting in early childhood education: Current research and future

implications. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 15(1), 97-118.

Graham, S. (2010). Want to improve children's writing? Don't neglect their handwriting.

Educational Digest, 76(1), 49-55.

Graham, S., Harris, K. R., Mason, L., Fink-Chorzempa, B., Moran, S., & Saddler, B. (2008).

How do primary grade teachers teach handwriting? A national survey. Reading and

Writing, 21, 49-69.

Handwriting without Tears. (2014). Handwriting without tears delivers success. Retrieved from

https://www.hwtears.com/files/HWT_Screener_Efficacy.pdf

Handwriting without Tears. (2015). Research Review: Handwriting without Tears, K-5.

Retrieved from https://www.hwtears.com/files/HWT%20Research%20Review.pdf

Hart, N. V., Fitzpatrick, P., & Cortesa, C. (2010). In-depth analysis of handwriting curriculum

and instruction in four kindergarten classrooms. Reading and Writing, 23, 673-699.

Hoy, M. M., Egan, M. Y., & Feder, K. P. (2011, February). A systematic review of interventions

to improve handwriting. Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy, 78(1), 13-25.

Jones, C. D., Clark, S. K., & Reutzel, D. R. (2013). Enhancing alphabet knowledge instruction:

Research implications and practical strategies for early childhood educators. Early

Childhood Education Journal, 41, 81-89.


THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 44

Julius, M. S., Meir, R., Shechter-Nissim, Z., & Adi-Japha, E. (2016). Children's ability to learn a

motor skill is related to handwriting and reading proficiency. Learning and Individual

Differences, 51, 265-272.

Kohn, A. (2000). The case against standardized testing: Raising the scores, ruining the schools.

Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

LeBrun, M., McLaughlin, T. F., Derby, K. M., & McKenzie, M. (2012, March/April). The

effects of using Handwriting Without Tears to teach thirty-one integrated preschoolers of

varying academic ability to write their name. Academic Research International, 2(2),

373-378.

Lifshitz, N., & Har-Zvi, S. (2015). A comparison between students who receive and who do not

receive a writing readiness interventions on handwriting quality, speed and positive

reactions. Early Childhood Education Journal, 43, 47-55.

Lillard, A. S. (2007). Montessori: the science behind the genius. New York, NY: Oxford

University Press.

McBride, M., Pelto, M., McLaughlin, T., Barretto, A., Robison, M., & Mortenson, S. (2009).

The effects of using Handwriting Without Tears procedures and worksheets to teach two

preschool students with severe disabilities to write their first names. The Open Education

Journal, 2, 21-24.

Montessori, M. (1966). The secret of childhood. New York, NY: Ballantine Books. (Original

work published 1936).

Montessori, M. (1995). The absorbent mind. New York, NY: Henry Holt and Company.

(Original work published 1949).


THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 45

Peik, J. P., Hands, B., & Licari, M. K. (2012). Assessment of motor functioning in the preschool

period. Neuropsychology Review, 22(4), 402-413. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11065-012-

9211-4

Puranik, C. S., & Al Otaiba, S. (2012). Examining the contribution of handwriting and spelling

to written expression in kindergarten children. Reading and Writing, 25, 1523-1546.

Puranik, C. S., & Lonigan, C. J. (2011). From scribbles to scrabble: Preschool children's

developing knowledge of written language. Reading and Writing, 24, 567-589.

Standing, E. M. (1998). Maria Montessori: Her life and work. New York, NY: Plume.

(Original work published 1957).

Steele, E. C., Weber, K. P., McLaughlin, T. F., Donica, D., Derby, K. M., & McKenzie, M.

(2015, February-April). Employing Handwriting Without Tears to teach a 4-year-old

preschool student to write his name with the appropriate size, shape, and form combined

with an imitate/trace/copy/memory procedure. Journal of Educational Psychology, 8(4),

16-25.
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 46

Appendix A: Presentation Log and Reflection Journal


THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 47

Appendix B: Printing Concerns Checklist

Appendix C: Handwriting Pre-K Assessment: Writing Name & Sentence Rubric


THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 48
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 49

Appendix D: Student Attitude Scale


THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 50
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 51
THE EFFECT OF HANDWRITING INTERVENTION 52

You might also like