Text And/versus Discourse: January 2015
Text And/versus Discourse: January 2015
Text And/versus Discourse: January 2015
net/publication/331286451
CITATIONS READS
0 6,696
1 author:
Nelly Tincheva
Sofia University "St. Kliment Ohridski"
27 PUBLICATIONS 19 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
Linguistic signalling of text world and discourse world construction View project
All content following this page was uploaded by Nelly Tincheva on 22 February 2019.
12
Chapter Two. Text and/versus Discourse
Prior to the onset of such views, anything beyond the sentence was
assigned to other domains, most often Literature and Stylistics. Prior to
that point, the science which, logically, should have been most closely
associated with the study of texts had practically divorced itself from the
very concept it had created.
Non-linguistic approaches to text (such as Sociology, Anthropol-
ogy, Philosophy and Pragmatics), however, did not limit themselves and
13
TEXT STRUCTURE: A WINDOW INTO DISCOURSE, CONTEXT AND MIND
14
Chapter Two. Text and/versus Discourse
way in which we talk about our subject” and “In linguistics up till now
we have been relying very heavily on speculation” (ibid.).
However, it should be duly noted that, in those times, interest in
text developed in parallel with and most probably in dependence on
some other trends in the communication-oriented Humanities and espe-
cially in Linguistics. If the transition from sentence to text represented
evolution in terms of, let’s call it, scope of object of analysis, then that
happened simultaneously with two other major tendencies. The first one
can be said to have to do with the basic method of analysis of language
data. That method can be summarized to have moved from prescrip-
tive to descriptive. In other words, analysts stopped prescribing how
language should be used; they moved on to describing, and, more im-
portantly, analyzing how language is actually used on a daily basis. The
second tendency can be said to have to do with, let’s call it, the na-
ture, or essence, of the object of analysis. In that respect, studies can
freely be accepted to have evolved from a, practically, exclusive accep-
tance of highly-idealized, standard-only data (for a discussion see, e.g.,
Coulthard, 1994) to naturally-occurring language uses.
Clearly, there is a strong, and very logical, if not natural, link be-
tween those two trends. If a scientist sees the need to approach language
(and text specifically) in its true nature and reality, then s/ he does need
to describe and analyze it the way it is and the way it happens, and not
use their own (invariably privileged) knowledge of language as a means
of sanctioning what classifies as language and what does not. And here
comes the very logical third trend which emerged in the process of ac-
cepting text as part of language (and communication) studies – if we
study language as it happens and we do not only prescribe for how it
should happen, then no-one could ever stand behind the sentence as be-
ing the actual and ultimate ‘unit’ of communication. As a consequence
to all three trends, a fourth shift occurred – one in emphasis from com-
positionality (i.e. determining minimal units and the strict rules for their
composition into the next-level basic units) to integration (i.e. incorpo-
rating various entities from across ‘levels’ and sometimes doing that
strictly against principles of compositionality).
That fourth – and crucial to text analysis – shift prompted text stud-
ies (e.g. Halliday, 1973, 1975, 2003; Halliday and Hasan, 1976, 1985;
van Dijk and Kintsch, 1978, 1983; de Beaugrande, 1980; van Dijk,
1972, 1977, 2000, 2008, 2009, 2015) to focus on the joint and simulta-
15
TEXT STRUCTURE: A WINDOW INTO DISCOURSE, CONTEXT AND MIND
16
Chapter Two. Text and/versus Discourse
17
TEXT STRUCTURE: A WINDOW INTO DISCOURSE, CONTEXT AND MIND
dinate sentences are very few in it. Spoken language relies predomi-
nantly on the use of active declarative forms and avoids passives,
it- and wh-cleft sentences. While written language sentences are
generally structured in subject-predicate form, in spoken language
topic-comment structure is more frequent as, e.g., in ‘the cats + did
you let them out’ (Brown and Yule, 1983: 17).
(b) Parataxis in spoken language employs almost exclusively and,
but, and then while in written language an elaborate set of discourse
markers (e.g. firstly, more importantly, in conclusion) are used to
make relationships between clauses overt. In general, ‘the speaker
is typically less explicit than the writer’ (ibid.: 16)
(c) Heavily pre-modified noun phrases characterize written lan-
guage and are quite rare in spoken language (ibid.: 17).
(d) In terms of vocabulary, ‘spoken language is certainly more flex-
ible. A speaker may go back and replace or repair a first choice
of expression, e.g. ‘this man + this chap she was going out with’
(ibid.).
As I have argued elsewhere (Tincheva, 2012c: 14), other research
in the field (e.g. Chafe, 1982; Schlesinger, 1989) also focuses on the
isolation of parameters along which the two channels diverge. A sum-
mation of the very basic lines of differences between spoken and written
texts would need to include:
(a) speed of production (oral delivery is characterized by a much
higher rate than written presentation – a fact which imposes certain
restrictions on the latter);
(b) degree of planning (the literacy end of the scale features careful-
ly pre-planned texts of high semantic density/ informational load,
more complex syntax, etc.),
(c) spatio-temporal distance/ proximity (oral delivery presupposes
a greater physical closeness between the participants than written
presentation does, which may involve social status distance/ prox-
imity effects),
(d) participants’ involvement (personal, often emotional, engage-
ment is considered more characteristic of oral discourse),
(e) the role of non-verbal behaviour (body language specificities,
voice quality, dress code, etc. play a greater role in face-to-face
contact, and, consequently, in oral text production).
18
Chapter Two. Text and/versus Discourse
19
TEXT STRUCTURE: A WINDOW INTO DISCOURSE, CONTEXT AND MIND
20
Chapter Two. Text and/versus Discourse
21
TEXT STRUCTURE: A WINDOW INTO DISCOURSE, CONTEXT AND MIND
II.2.3. Size
The structuralist preoccupation with size (see II.1. above) neces-
sitates a note on yet another characteristic feature of text – how long a
stretch of language has to be in order for it to be called a text. Clearly,
a whole novel, or a series of volumes would readily classify as texts.
But does a one-word or a one-sound communicative chunk also rate as
such?
At present, the answer to that question is a rarity in that there is,
practically, unanimous agreement throughout the literature on its an-
swer. As Semiotic studies began to gain speed (through the works of,
e.g., Lotman, 1976, 1990; Eco, 1966, 1976), the issue of the ‘lower lim-
it’ of text ceased to exist. The STOP sign, for instance, has become a
most salient example (e.g. Peirce, 1931; Atkin, 2010). The most recent
22
Chapter Two. Text and/versus Discourse
23
TEXT STRUCTURE: A WINDOW INTO DISCOURSE, CONTEXT AND MIND
24
Chapter Two. Text and/versus Discourse
What is more, within this second type, there may be argued to exist
two subtypes, according to which, as Labocha maintains, discourse can
be used either in the singular or in the plural form, each option express-
ing different content:
The term discourse when used only in the singular number is understood
as a cultural norm which regulates linguistic (communicative) behaviour,
whereas the term discourse when used also in the plural number, i.e. in the
form discourses, may be understood […] as an expression synonymous
with the term utterance. (2011: 62)
Importantly, the two interpretations, or, rather, the two major ap-
proaches to the notion of discourse cannot and should not be seen as
standing in opposition. On the contrary, it is more accurate (as well as
more practical) to see them as representing the two sides of the same
coin. Which one to flip on is rather a matter of theoretical viewpoint
and analytical need, and not that much of exclusion and rejection of
the alternative theoretical strand. Such a neat and logical line between
the general approaches to discourse and the general approaches to text,
however, is way more difficult to draw.
If a study offers an overt formulation of the distinction between text
and discourse, that study would be an exception. Even de Beaugrande
25
TEXT STRUCTURE: A WINDOW INTO DISCOURSE, CONTEXT AND MIND
and Dressler (1981), famous for their exhaustive reviews of the literature
and their (re-)definitions of existing concepts, focus exclusively on text,
and when they mention discourse (only in the last parts of their ground-
breaking book of 1981), they do so almost out of the blue and without
providing any definition. Moreover, de Beaugrande and Dressler’s treat-
ment is typical of yet another peculiarity of investigations on text and
discourse.
As I have argued elsewhere (Tincheva, 2012c: 12), there can be
summarized to exist two generally-employed ways of dealing with the
theoretical and terminological jumble around the two concepts. The first
one is to approach them as opposites. In such cases, discourse is often
postulated as long, while text – as short. If text is taken to be written,
discourse tends to be defined as its oral counterpart. If text is seen as
relating to monologues, then discourse is defined as controlling inter-
active forms such as dialogues. Frequently, those lines of dissimilarity
are taken to run in parallel – text is both written and monologic, while
discourse is both oral and dialogic (for a discussion of the distinctions
along those three lines see, e.g., Stubbs, 1983; Faiclough, 1989; Vir-
tanen, 1990). However, as already argued earlier here (II.2.1.), real lan-
guage uses do not support the existence of an Oral-Written dichotomy.
The monologic-dialogic distinction should similarly be seen as a scale
rather than a clear-cut division. The fact that the two are believed to of-
ten run in parallel also lends support to the latter claim. In other words,
this fact can be seen as support to the hypothesis of the existence of, let’s
call it, the monologue-dialogue continuum. Basically, opposing text and
discourse does not seem an alternative which would allow one to ap-
proach either notion successfully.
Opposing text and discourse, as mentioned above, is not the only
possible way of tackling the issue of their nature and differences. De
Beaugrande and Dressler, for example, opt for the second possible way
of dealing with the terminological and theoretical puzzle surrounding
text and discourse – they select and use one of the two terms exclu-
sively. Halliday (e.g. 1976; 1985), similarly, focuses only on text and
Sinclair and Coulthard (1975; 1977) use the term discourse only. That,
as Virtanen claims (ibid.), is a rather typical choice – most frequently,
analysts prefer one term to cover both oral and written channel, mono-
logic and dialogic form. That could happen for several analytical and/
or theoretical reasons.
26
Chapter Two. Text and/versus Discourse
27
TEXT STRUCTURE: A WINDOW INTO DISCOURSE, CONTEXT AND MIND
28
Chapter Two. Text and/versus Discourse
29
TEXT STRUCTURE: A WINDOW INTO DISCOURSE, CONTEXT AND MIND
and Brown and Yule’s definitions (above) on the one hand, and Hal-
liday and Hasan’s, on the other, will reveal no differences in principle.
Rather, the major dissimilarity lies in the choice to use only the term text
that Halliday and Hasan, Neubert and Dirven and Verspoor have made.
Thus, what Fairclough and Brown and Yule suggest to be discourse-as-
process can easily be argued to be basically the same as what Halliday
and Hasan propose as text-as-process. What Fairclough and Brown and
Yule suggest to be text-as-product is basically the same as what Hal-
liday and Hasan also see as a product. In other words, there seems to
be consensus in the literature on text as the product of communication.
Moreover, there also seems to be consensus on the fact that there exist
‘processes’ – social or individual – from which the product-text results.
The only theoretical differences ensue from the diverse viewpoints on
how to term those processes and, crucially, on how far to broaden analy-
sis of those processes.
True to fact, with the advance of cognitive studies, the linguistic
view of text has gradually expanded to equal the discourse-as-a-process
one – a development most plainly described as moving from a static
analytical stance to a dynamic approach with an increasing emphasis on
text reception (see Virtanen, 1990; Dobrzynska, 2005). As Dirven and
Verspoor put it, at present,
Text linguistics is the study of how S (speaker, writer) and H (hearer, read-
er) manage to communicate via texts, that is how they go beyond the text
(words) they produce or have in front of them to see the relations between
the sentences, the paragraphs, the sections, etc. (2004: 180)
Furthermore,
Text can consequently be defined as the linguistic expressions used in
communication between people and the interpretation the hearer or reader
makes of them. This definition applies to both oral and written commu-
nication, but has the additional condition that text here only denotes the
verbal part of the communication, excluding the paralinguistic and non-
verbal aspects. This text definition also presupposes the cultural or world
knowledge on which the text interpretation is based. (ibid.)
30
Chapter Two. Text and/versus Discourse
what extent has it broadened? How far ‘around’ the text-product does it
stretch? (Whether it, at all, should be seen as ‘surrounding’ the text will
be the subject of our discussion in Chapter IV.) And, as all the defini-
tions cited above include ideas of processes, processing and participants’
interpretations, it is only logical to ask: where exactly does text reside?
Is its main locus ‘out there’ on paper? Is it ‘out there’ ‘in’ society? Or
does it pertain to the individual participant’s (mental) processes?
31
TEXT STRUCTURE: A WINDOW INTO DISCOURSE, CONTEXT AND MIND
32
Chapter Two. Text and/versus Discourse
33
TEXT STRUCTURE: A WINDOW INTO DISCOURSE, CONTEXT AND MIND
Figure 1
34
Chapter Two. Text and/versus Discourse
35
TEXT STRUCTURE: A WINDOW INTO DISCOURSE, CONTEXT AND MIND
36
Chapter Two. Text and/versus Discourse
Dressler’s position – is seen from the vantage point of the human mind
(i.e. it is decoded through contemporary knowledge of the brain’s pro-
cessing capacities) and those processes and capacities are interpreted as
indubitably bound to their language-signal results.
As far as the main objectives of the present investigation are con-
cerned, the most important conclusion emerging from the discussion of
the notions of text and discourse so far is that they are easily ‘confused’
and extremely hard to delineate, even for analytical purposes. Second,
and no less important, even when the two are delineated for analytical
purposes, scientists can switch the places of the two and justify the posi-
tion of either from various perspectives. Both facts will be positioned
here as crucial parts of my rationale behind the argument that overlap-
ping and blending cognitive techniqiues control the joint operation of
the concepts of text and discourse (IV.3.3. and V.1.5.2.). In a way, the
theories and approaches discussed so far will be used as a kind of a cor-
pus employed to confirm the hypothesized possibility for text-discourse
overlap and blending to exist.
To aid and defend that kind of understanding, the next Chapter Three
will be dedicated to the clarification of some basic cognitive tenets and
terms. Those tenets and terms are also expected to be indispensable for
our further discussion of the distinction between text and discourse, the
way it will be presented in Chapter Four.
37