Social Science and Its Methods

Download as doc, pdf, or txt
Download as doc, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1of 31

Part I Introduction

Social Science
and Its Methods
chapter 1
Afterreading this chapter,you should be able to:
• Define social science and explain why it is important
• List the various social sciences
• State the nine steps that make up the scientific method Theories should be as simple
• Discuss some reasonable approaches to problems in social science as possible, but not more so.
• Differentiate the historical method from the case method —Albert Einstein
and the comparative method
• Distinguish educated common sense from common sense
• Explain why a good scientist is always open to new ways of
looking at issues

On September 11, 2001, eighteen men boarded airplanes with the intent of crashing them into the
World Trade Center, the Pentagon, and the White House or Capitol. They suc-ceeded with three
of the planes, causing enormous destruction. The fourth plane crashed, but thanks to passengers
who discovered the highjackers’ plans and attacked the highjack-ers, the destruction of the White
House or Capitol was prevented. What forces drove the highjackers to undertake such action?
What forces led the passengers to organize together to thwart them? What might have prevented
the highjackings? Such questions fall under the purview of social science—the scientific study of
social, cultural, psychological, economic, and political forces that guide individuals in their
actions.
Formal social science is relatively new. Nevertheless, a vast amount of information
has been accumulated concerning the social life of human beings. This information has
been used in building a system of knowledge about the nature, growth, and functioning
of human societies. Social science is the name given to that system of knowledge.
All knowledge is (1) knowledge of human beings, including their culture and
products, and (2) knowledge of natural environment. Human culture has been changing,
and knowl-edge about it has been gradually accumulating ever since the far distant time
when humans first assumed their distinctively human character. But until rather recent
times, this knowl-edge was not scientific in the modern sense. Scientific knowledge is
knowledge that has been systematically gathered, classified, related, and interpreted. It
is concerned with learn-ing the concepts and applying those concepts to particulars,
rather than just learning a vast amount of information.
Primitive peoples acquired much of their knowledge unconsciously, just as we today still
begin the use of our native language and acquire many of the basic elements in our cul-ture
unconsciously. For the most part, they accepted the world as they found it, and if any

1
2 chapter 1 Social Science and Its Methods

Social Science versus the Soaps two types have certain drives, and desires, and cer-
tain rules that are passed on to them, either through
Faced with the events that affect our lives, we have two their genes or through society’s mores.
options: We can lose ourselves in a parody of real-ity, The ultimate goal of the game is often unclear,
such as becoming experts on the soaps (is Laura really although its day-to-day objects can be said to consist
sleeping with John’s wife’s brother?), or we can try to of continuing to play the game and to keep the game
understand those events—what actually hap-pens. Some itself alive. What winning or losing the game might be
educators, following the philosophy of Plato, try to argue is clouded. Probably, if we commit suicide, we are
the moral superiority of the latter: Better to be an los-ers. If we make a million dollars, are admired by
unhappy learned person than a happy fool. Others find our acquaintances for it, and are happy, we are
that unconvincing. Following Jeremy Bentham, the social probably winners. Many people even question
philosopher, they prefer happi-ness. The problem they whether we are playing the game of our own free will
have with the soaps is that soaps don’t make you happy; or whether we are merely the pawns of a god who
soaps quickly become boring. You soon play out the has predetermined all our actions.
options in your head and, often, create far better This game is far more diverse and interesting than
scenarios than the televi-sion writer. It’s a bit like tic tac other games. The possibilities are endless and the
toe: one move (if you know what you are doing), and the challenge immediate. It has elements of danger, like
game is done. Pinochle is somewhat more interesting, Russian roulette (if we really do goof, we will blow our-
and the good TV shows approach the complexity of selves up). And it has its peaceful moments. But what
pinochle. But here again, after seven or eight cards have makes it the most interesting game of all is that we are
been played, the possibilities soon become evident. both the players and the played, at times moving
Chess is a step above this, with its infinite number of ourselves as we make stupid or foolish choices and
possibili-ties. But still, after twenty or so moves (and often contrive sophisticated or imaginative solutions, and at
fewer), good chess players can anticipate the outcome other times watching other players as they make their
and choose to call a draw, resign, or declare victory. choices and contrive their solutions. Trying to under-
stand this game is what social science is all about. And
Quite frankly, soaps, tic tac toe, pinochle, and the reason I am a social scientist instead of a TV fan is
chess are not for this author. I prefer a far more that I watch society and try to understand what makes
complicated game—one in which I’m both a player society work. It’s a whole lot more challenging and fun
and a pawn. That game could be called the game of than watching the soaps. Moreover, unlike the soaps,
life, or it could be called the game of society. It is watching society has a purpose—if we can un-derstand
played by some 7.8 billion people, each having a society, we might be able to make it better.
wide variety of possible moves that range from Social science has fascinated enormous numbers
shooting up a playground full of schoolchildren to of people, and a whole set of ponderings about the
trying to travel farther into outer space, construct game has already developed. These ponderings con-
faster computers, or improve humans by modifying cern the nature, growth, and functioning of human
their genes. The players in the game of society are societies. This book introduces you to the past
divided into two types: male and female. These ponderings of social scientists.

explanations seemed called for, they invented supernatural ones. Some primitive peoples
believed that every stream, tree, and rock contained a spirit that controlled its behavior.
In modern times, our emphasis is on the search for scientific knowledge. We have di-vided
human knowledge into a number of areas and fields, and every science represents the systematic
collection and study of data in one of these areas, which can be grouped roughly into two major
fields—social science and natural science. Each of these fields is subdivided into a number of
specialized sciences or disciplines to facilitate more intensive study and deeper understanding.
Social science is the field of human knowledge that deals with all as-pects of the group life of
human beings. Natural science is concerned with the natural envi-ronment in which human
beings exist. It includes such sciences as physics and chemistry, which deal with the laws of
matter, motion, space, mass, and energy; it also includes the biological sciences, which deal with
living things. The third field of study is the humanities,
Social Science 3

which deals with literature, music, art, and philosophy. The humanities are closely
related to social science in that both deal with humans and their culture. Social science,
however, is most concerned with those basic elements of culture that determine the
general patterns of human behavior. The humanities deal with special aspects of human
culture and are prima-rily concerned with our attempts to express spiritual and esthetic
values and to discover the meaning of life. Whereas the social sciences study issues in a
systematic, scientific way, the focus of the humanities is more on the emotions and
feelings themselves than on the sys-tem employed to sharpen that focus.
The importance of social science goes far beyond the specific social sciences. It is
social science thinking that underlies much of the law as well as our understanding of
interna-tional relations and government. All these fields are the natural byproducts of
social science inquiry. Thus, a knowledge of social science is necessary for anyone
trying to understand current world events.

Social Science
No field of study is more important to human beings than the social sciences. To under-stand
society is to learn not only the conditions that limit our lives but also the opportuni-ties open to
us for improving the human condition. Increasing our knowledge of human society is as
important as learning more about mathematics, physics, chemistry, or engi-neering, for unless we
can develop societies in which human beings can live happy, mean-ingful, and satisfying lives,
we cannot reap the benefits from learning how to make better automobiles and skyscrapers,
traveling in space, or constructing faster computers. Albert Einstein summed it up: “Politics is
more difficult than physics and the world is more likely to die from bad politics than from bad
physics.”
Because all expressions of human culture are related and interdependent, to gain a
real understanding of human society we must have some knowledge of all its major
aspects. If we concentrate on some phases and neglect others, we will have a distorted
picture. But so-cial science today is such a vast complex that no one student can hope to
master all of it. Thus, social science itself has been broken up into anthropology,
sociology, history, geogra-phy, economics, political science, and psychology. (The
boxes in this chapter provide a brief introduction to each of these disciplines.)
This list of social science disciplines is both too broad and too narrow. It is too
broad because parts of the fields of history, geography, and psychology should not be
included as social sciences. For instance, parts of history and geography belong in the
humanities, and parts of psychology belong in the natural sciences. The list is too
narrow because new so-cial sciences are emerging, such as cognitive science and
sociobiology, that incorporate new findings and new ways of looking at reality. (See
box on The Evolving Social Sciences.)
Because all knowledge is interrelated, there are inevitable problems in defining and
cat-aloging the social sciences. Often, it is difficult to know where one social science
ends and another begins. Not only are the individual social sciences interrelated, but the
social sci-ences as a whole body are also related to the natural sciences and the
humanities. The strains of the old song, “The hip bone’s connected to the thigh
bone, . . .” are appropriate to the social sciences. To understand history, it is helpful,
even necessary, to understand geog-raphy; to understand economics, it is necessary to
understand psychology. Similar argu-ments can be made for all of the social sciences.
One of the difficulties in presenting definitions and descriptions of the various social
sciences is that social scientists themselves don’t agree on what it is they do, or should be doing.
In preparing this chapter, we met with groups of social scientists specializing in spe-cific fields
and asked them to explain what it was that distinguished their field from others.
4 chapter 1 Social Science and Its Methods

The Evolving Social Sciences philosophy, social anthropology, and molecular biology.
Although it is still in the process of formation, a tenta-tive
The themes of this book are evolution and change. definition of cognitive science is the study of how the
Thus, it would be surprising if the divisions among the mind identifies problems and how it solves those
social sciences that currently exist still remain ten problems. For instance, there are more ways to write the
years from now. Indeed, with the development of new letter s than there are people who know how to write that
technology and technological advances in the letter (all people who write plus the printing press and
physical sciences, the distinction among the various computer software and innumerable type-faces designed
sciences is blurring and new sciences are developing. for them). Let us identify the problem as how to
As these fields develop, the boundaries of the various recognize the letter s when we see it. We know the result
social sciences change. of the exercise: Everyone who knows how to read can
Interaction among the various social sciences is instantly recognize most renditions of the letter s (the
creating new fields, such as economic psychology, handwriting of a few college students and some
psy-chological economics, and sociopolitical physicians excepted). But we do not cur-rently know how
anthropology. In economics and political science, too, we do it. Or, how do you know the face of your roommate
a group of economists is calling for the reintegration from the face of your mother, from the face of the letter
of these two fields into political economy, and some carrier, from the face of Brad Pitt? There has been
schools do have departments of political economy. speculation about how the mind works for almost as long
Change is also occurring in the natural sciences, and as there have been minds, theories, and even
there is interaction between the natural and social experiments, but few specific riddles have been
sciences. New developments in genetic theory, which will conclusively solved.
be discussed in Chapter 2, have caused many to believe Whether these upstart disciplines take hold
it is time for a new social science, called cogni-tive remains to be seen, but that some change will take
science, which combines psychology, linguistics, place is certain.

There was little agreement among specialists in a particular social science, let alone
among all social scientists. A cynic once said, “Economics is what economists do.” If
we replaced “economics” and “economists” with any of the other social sciences and its
practitioners, we would have as good a definition as possible. Unfortunately, it would
not be very helpful to those who do not know what social scientists do.
One important difference among the individual social scientists did come out of these
discussions: Even when two social scientists are considering the same issue, because their
training is different they focus on different aspects of that problem. Geographers fixate on spaces
and spatial relativities, economists on market incentives, and political scientists on group
decision making. Thus, although we might not be able to define, unambiguously, the domains of
the various social sciences, you will get a sense of the various approaches as we consider issues
from various perspectives throughout the book.
The study of social science is more than the study of the individual social sciences.
Although it is true that to be a good social scientist you must know each of those com-
ponents, you must also know how they interrelate. By specializing too early, many
social scientists can lose sight of the interrelationships that are so essential to
understanding modern problems. That’s why it’s necessary to have a course covering all
the social sci-ences. In fact, it wouldn’t surprise me if one day a news story such as the
one in the box on the next page appeared.
To understand how and when social science broke up, you must study the past. Imagine for a
moment that you’re a student in 1062, in the Italian city of Bologna, site of one of the first major
universities in the Western world. The university has no buildings; it consists merely of a few
professors and students. There is no tuition fee. At the end of a professor’s lecture, if you like it,
you pay. And if you don’t like it, the professor finds him-self without students and without
money. If we go back still earlier, say to Greece in the
Social Science 5

Unified Social Systems by John von Neumann in the late 1940s and early
1950s with game theory, also begun by von
Theory Derived Neumann, to form a coherent whole, and captures
Dateline 2050. Researchers today announced the many of the interrelationships that were previously
development of a unified theory of the social sciences. lost in the fragmentation or divisions of social science.
The new theory, which had its early foundations in the That work was extended in the complexity revolution
work of Ludwig von Bertalanffy, is the equivalent in social in science that came into its own in the early 2000s.
science of the unified field theory in physics, which tied By combin-ing these theories with recent advances in
together the various forces of nature into a general the separate social sciences, the resulting new unified
theory. The formulation of the unified field theory in 2020 social science theory provides new insights into how
solved the problem that stymied earlier physicists such society works.
as Albert Einstein. It intensified the efforts of social When asked what set her on this path, the social
scientists to develop their own unified theory. The theory, scientist who developed the theory said it was the
which is also called a unified social systems theory, ties experience in her first social science class, in which
together the various social sci-ences that in the she used the classic Hunt and Colander text, Social
nineteenth and twentieth centuries diverged into Science. In that class, with the aid of the insights her
anthropology, sociology, history, geogra-phy, economics, teacher provided, the scientist grasped the first
political science, and psychology. The theory combines inklings of how these various theories might be put
the work on complex systems begun together, setting the stage for her later achievement.

sixth century B.C., we can see the philosopher Socrates walking around the streets of
Athens, arguing with his companions. He asks them questions, and then other questions,
leading these people to reason the way he wants them to reason (this became known as
the Socratic method).
Times have changed since then; universities sprang up throughout the world and
cre-ated colleges within the universities. Oxford, one of the first universities, now has
thirty col-leges associated with it, and the development and formalization of educational
institutions has changed the roles of both students and faculty. As knowledge
accumulated, it became more and more difficult for one person to learn, let alone retain,
it all. In the sixteenth cen-tury, one could still aspire to know all there was to know, and
the definition of the Renaissance man (people were even more sexist then than they are
now) was one who was expected to know about everything.
Unfortunately, at least for someone who wants to know everything, the amount of
in-formation continues to grow exponentially while the size of the brain has grown only
slightly. The way to deal with the problem is not to try to know everything about every-
thing. Today we must specialize. That is why social science separated from the natural
sci-ences and why social science, in turn, has been broken down into various subfields,
such as anthropology and sociology.
There are advantages and disadvantages to specialization, and many social
problems today are dealt with by teams of various social scientists. Each brings his or
her specialty to the table. For example, one of the authors is an economist but works on
projects with geographers, sociologists, anthropologists, political scientists, and
psychologists. More and more interdisciplinary majors are being created; one of the
authors of this book teaches in both the economics department and the international
politics and economics department at his school. Interdisciplinary graduate schools of
public policy have grown enormously. In these programs, students study all the social
sciences while specializing in one. Figure 1.1 provides a graphic overview of the
evolution of knowledge and the pres-ent social sciences. (The appendix at the end of
this chapter expands on the ideas in this diagram.)
6 chapter 1 Social Science and Its Methods

THE HISTORY OF KNOWLEDGE


Ancient Greece 600 B.C.–A.D. 100

Physics Economics Arts Psychology Geography


and
Humanities

Roman Period
Western Philosophy 100 500

A.D. –A.D.

Preservation of
Greek Learning Fall of Rome
in Eastern Countries
Religion Middle Ages

and the 476–1453


Crusades
International Trade

Renaissance
1453–1690

Physics Metaphysics Arts


Philosophy and Humanities
Natural Sciences

Chemistry Physics Biology Logic Morals Epistemology

Documentation and Proof

Induction Deduction

The Enlightenment
1700–1800

Social Sciences

Political Philosophy

Political Economics Sociology Anthropology Geography Psychology History


Science

Figure 1.1
Knowledge at a glance. The development of knowledge in messy, but assuming that a
picture is worth a thousand words, we offer this sketch of the development of knowledge.
Maybe it’s worth five hundred words.
Social Science 7

Anthropology Social Science as a System ofRules


Today the amount of knowledge is increasing
Anthropology is the study of the relationship between
faster than ever. How, then, can a unified social
biological traits and socially acquired characteristics. science theory ever be formulated? The answer is
Sometimes called the study of humans, it consists of found in abstraction and the ability to discover
two broad fields: rules or relationships (rather than simply facts)
1. Physical anthropology and rules relating rules to other rules.
2. Cultural anthropology To understand the importance of knowing
Some of the concerns of physical anthropology are: rules, think back to grade school when you learned
addition. You didn’t memorize the sum of 127 and
■ Influence of evolution of natural environment on 1,448. Instead you learned an algorithm (a fancy
the physical characteristics of humans name for a rule) about adding (7 + 8 = 15; write
■ Human evolution: how modern homo sapiens down the 5 and carry the 1 . . .). Then you had to
evolved from earlier species memorize only a few relationships. By changing
Some of the concerns of cultural anthropology are: the number system from a base ten system to a
binary system (0 and 1 are the only numbers), you
■ Archaeology, or the remains of extinct civilizations cut substantially the amount of memorization
that left no written records (all you need to know is 0 + 0 = 0; 0 + 1 = 1; and
■ Organization of preliterate societies 1 + 1 = 10) and you could apply the same rule
■ Characteristics of subgroups or subcultures within again and again, adding all possible numbers (an
contemporary society insight that played an important role in the devel-
Among the topics that interest anthropologists are exca- opment of the computer). Knowing the rules
saved you from enormous amounts of memoriza-
vation of formerly inhabited sites, fossils, the gene pool, tion, but nonetheless gave you access to a large
technology and artifacts, linguistics, values, and kinship. amount of information.
Another way to look at the problem is to
think of the library. If you have a small library,
you can know nearly everything in it, but once your library gets larger, you will quickly
find that having more books makes it harder to know what’s in there. However, if you
put in place a filing system, such as the Dewey decimal system or the Library of
Congress system, you can access the books through a filing system. The rules of the
filing system give you the key to great amounts of information, just as the rules of
addition, subtraction, or algebra do. General rules, once learned, can be applied to large
numbers of particulars. The higher you go (rules about rules about rules), the more you
1
can know with less memorization.
All this is relevant to social science and the 2050 dateline because social science, too, is held
together by rules or relationships. If there is to be a unified social science theory, it will be
because some student started thinking about rules and how the rules of the various so-cial
sciences can fit together. If you understand the general concepts, you can apply them in a variety
of circumstances. Thus the future “unified social scientists” will not necessarily know all the
facts of a particular social science. Each of the specialties will retain its identity and will likely
become even more specialized. But as that specialization occurs, it creates the need for a new
specialization that concentrates on tying together the various component parts of social science.
The new unified social scientists will know the general rules of the individual social sciences and
the rules of how one social science interacts with another, but they will not know all the specific
facts of any one of them.
The preceding argument is a heavy one to throw at you in the first pages of a textbook
because it asks you not only to know the lessons of the individual social sciences, but also to

1
It was an architect, Ludwig Mies van der Rohe, who compressed such exposition into a famous statement,
“Less is more.”
8 chapter 1 Social Science and Its Methods

Sociology
go beyond and strive for an understanding of
their synthesis. Going beyond is ultimately what
learning is all about and what makes it so chal-
Sociology is the systematic study of relationships lenging. We would like to be able to say that we
among people. Sociologists assume that behavior is can guide you to a unified social science theory,
influenced by people’s social, political, occupa-tional, but the truth is that all we can do is give you a
and intellectual groupings and by the particu-lar boost and encouragement. After surveying the
settings in which they find themselves at one time or so-cial sciences, you can decide in which one, if
another. Sociologists differ in their approach. Their any, you want to specialize; whether you should
three major choices are: work toward tying them all together; or whether
1. Functionalism you should bag the whole approach and go into
2. Conflict a premed program.
3. Interactionism
Sociology’s vast subject matter can be identified as The ScientificMethod and Its Application
a study of people:
■ Where they collect
The scientific method is a set of rules about how to
establish rules. The use of the scientific method is
■ How they socialize and organize
perhaps the most important tool you can have in
■ Whom they include in and exclude from their groups
studying social science because it enables you not
■ What they do to their environment only to learn the lessons of the individual so-cial
■ When they confront formulas for control, such as sciences, but also to go beyond and strive for an
politics, law, finance, religion, education, and understanding of their synthesis.
social pressures
■ Why they change Conditions Favorable to Scientific Inquiry.
Scientific inquiry is possible only in a society in
which certain attitudes are developed or tolerated.
Successful scientific investigation requires from the
investigator not only intelligence but certain mental
attitudes as well. One of these is curiosity, which
makes people ask two questions: Why? and How?
Another is skepticism, which makes people

Geography reexamine past explanations and reevaluate past


evidence. To reexamine and reevaluate, investigators
need objectivity, which enables them to seek
Geography is the study of the natural environment and
impartially for the truth, to make every effort not to
how it influences social and cultural development. Some
allow personal preconceptions, prejudices, or desires
of the concerns of geography are:
to color the observed facts or influence the
■ Ecology interpretation of those facts. When these three
■ Climate attitudes—curiosity, skepticism, and objectivity—
■ Resources come together, scientific inquiry can flourish.
■ Accessibility In preliterate tribal societies, the obstacles to the
■ Demography development of scientific methods of inquiry are
very great. Such societies are much more bound by
Geography has practical applications manifest in: custom and tradition than are modern societies. The
traditional way of doing things is regarded as the
■ Maps
only right way. Moreover, any seri-ous deviation
■ Trade patterns
from established procedures is likely to be regarded
■ Industrial and agricultural decisions as a danger to the group.
■ Settlement of population We cannot classify Europe in the Middle Ages
■ Aggression and acquisition as either preliterate or tribal. Nevertheless, respect
for tradition, for ancient authorities, and for religious
Social Science 9

History
History is the study of past events. It is a social sci-
ence in the sense that it is a systematic attempt to
learn about and verify past events and to relate them
to one another and to the present. Every event has a
historical context within which we commonly say the
event must be studied. The subject matter of history is
everything that has already happened. The study of
history involves:

■ Identifying
■ Classifying
■ Arranging
■ Patterning

The fruits of the study of history are:

■ Imposition of order
■ Appreciation of variety
■ Possibilities of prediction
■ Realization of limitation

dictates was so strong then that the growth of a sci-


entific spirit was stunted. The free development of
modern science had to wait until such events as the
Crusades, the Renaissance, the great voyages of dis-
covery, and the Reformation had loosened the hold
of tradition.

Nature of the Scientific Method. Modern


science is based on the assumption that this is an
orderly universe, ruled by the law of cause and
effect. Any given set of circumstances always
produces the same result. If seemingly identical
situations have different results, they were not
really alike; some significant difference existed
and was overlooked. Further investigation should
disclose what this difference was.
Science offers no final explanations of the
universe and its phenomena. Time, space, matter,
energy—existence itself—are mysteries the ulti-mate
nature of which are probably forever beyond the
grasp of the human search. But an accepted scientific
theory may be regarded as an explana-tion, up to a
certain point, of a scientific law.
Scientific investigation is seldom simple. Each field of knowledge has its special
problems, and investigators must always adjust their methods to the peculiarities of the
situation they are
dealing with. A method of investigation that is of great importance in some fields is the
set-ting up and carrying out of controlled experiments.
The Experimental Method and Its Limitations. The experimental method is a method
of separating out causal factors. It consists of running an experiment many times with only
one variant. If the results of the experiments are different, that one variant is most likely the
2
cause. In chemistry, physics, and biology, such controlled experiments play an important
role in discovering facts and testing hypotheses. In these sciences, an investigator can create
a situation in which all the significant factors that bear on a problem can be controlled.
But there are limits to the use of the experimental method when a scientist cannot con-trol
the situations that are significant for the solution of problems. In the social sciences, very little
use can be made of the method of controlled experiment except in dealing with certain
relationships that involve rather small groups, because the investigator cannot con-trol the
situations. For example, one way to prove or disprove the proposition that high tar-iffs bring
prosperity would be to apply very heavy duties to all goods entering the United States for a
considerable period of time, while holding constant all other factors affecting business activity. If
a sustained increase in prosperity followed, we would then have sub-stantial evidence to support
the thesis that high tariffs are a cause of prosperity. No investi-gator, let us say an economist, can
control the country’s tariff policy; and even if she could, while the high tariff was in effect many
other social changes would be taking place, such as strikes, the establishment of new industries,
and perhaps even wars. Some of these other changes would doubtless have much more influence
on the state of national prosperity than

2
But it is always possible that some other factor was not “held constant.” If you remember chemistry
experiments in high school, you know how hard it is to keep all other things constant.
10 chapter 1 Social Science and Its Methods

The Saga ofHans, did not know the answer. A social scientist’s skepticism
the Thinking Horse had shown that Hans could not really reason, even
though it seemed as if he could. This true story demon-
The scientific method can be seen in the saga of Hans, strates the important trait of skepticism. The scientific
the Thinking Horse. Around 1900, according to reports community declared that Hans was just a horse.
published in a Berlin, Germany, newspaper, there was a But a quality those scientists did not show was
horse that was good at math, and when his owner asked imagination. Even though Hans could not think and
him math questions, the horse could answer by tapping reason, he had an amazing ability: He could almost read
out the correct number with one of his front hooves. minds. When it came to people who knew the answers to
People who witnessed the horse’s ability were puzzled, the questions they were asking, he could monitor
and they called in a number of social scientists to in- changes in his questioners’ posture, their breathing, their
vestigate the phenomenon. To their amazement, they facial expressions, and their inflec-tions and speech
found that not only could Clever Hans, as he was patterns. He could interpret the sig-nals they were
known, add and subtract when his owner asked him— sending and then provide the responses they wanted.
he could also calculate square roots. The social scien- This is an ability that some humans have—although
tists were convinced that, against all odds, they had generally to a lesser degree than Hans—and it is an
indeed been shown a thinking horse. ability that can supplement think-ing. Yet it was only at
Another social scientist, though, a skeptical young the end of the twentieth century that comparative
psychologist by the name of Oskar Pfungst, had a dif- psychologists showed the imagina-tion to start analyzing
ferent idea. He retested Hans, asking a set of questions this kind of ability in detail.
to which Pfungst himself did not know the answers. He The lack of imagination exhibited by some
discovered that although Hans succeeded on nearly scien-tists in the past limited the scope of the
every question if the questioner knew the answer, the scientific programs they followed. A good scientist
horse failed nearly every question when the questioner must have both skepticism and imagination.

would the high tariff and would make it impossible to separate out the effects of the
high tariff from the effects of all these other events.
Most problems of interest to social scientists involve very large groups of people, often
society as a whole. Controlled experiments cannot be used to solve such problems. When,
however, social scientists can solve a problem by working with small groups, they may be able to
make a limited use of the experimental method if the people involved will cooperate. Also, they
can study natural experiments, which occur when two similar areas or entities choose different
policies, and the effects of the different policies can be systematically stud-ied. With natural
experiments, researchers do not get perfect control, but they get some.
In the future, with further advances in computer technology, social scientists will study
policy issues using virtual social systems in which a computer model of numerous interacting
individuals creates a virtual system that can analogue what occurs in the real world. Because of
the complexity of social systems, such virtual systems remain a hope for the future, not a reality.
Social experiments are sometimes called experiments, but, unless they have a
“control” that followed a different path and hence can be studied as a natural
experiment, they are not what we mean by experiment. A social experiment is simply
the introduction and “try-ing out” of new social policies. For example, Oregon’s change
in the financing of health insurance or Florida’s experiments with vouchers for
financing education might be called social experiments. The distinction involves the
ability to have a control and to be able to replicate the experiment. The less the control,
and the less the ability to repeat the experi-ment, the less sure we are of the results.

Methodology and the Social Sciences


Because it is so difficult to experiment in social science, some people have insisted that it is not
science. Except for the prestige carried by the word, whether we call the study of society
Social Science 11

E
conomics
Economics is the study of the ways
in which men and women make a living, the most
pressing problem most human beings face. It
considers the social organiza-tion through which
people satisfy their wants for scarce goods and
services. Its subject matter is often summarized as:

Production
Distribution

Consumption

Some of the topics


it includes are: ■ Supply and
demand
■ Costs
■ Inflation
■ Unemployment

Economics seeks to explain, guide,


and predict social arrangements by which we
satisfy economic wants.

Politi
cal Science
Political science
is the study of social
arrangements to maintain peace and order within
a given society. It deals with government, and its
interests are:
■ Politics ■
Laws
■ Administration
■ Theory of the nature and functions of the state
■ International relations

It has both a philosophical and a practical base. It


examines the theory of systems of government, but
it also studies actual practices by which
government:
■ Taxes
■ Prohibits
■ Regulates
■ Protects
■ Provides services

a science is not important. It is merely a question of


definition. If we mean by science the natural sci-ences
only, then social science is not true science. If we
mean by science only the so-called exact sci-ences,
then again social science is not included. If, however,
we use the term science broadly, to in-clude all
systematic attempts to expand knowledge by applying
the scientific method, then social sci-ence must
definitely be included in the scientific family. What is
really important is that social sci-entists have
discovered many significant relation-ships that are
sufficiently dependable to add greatly to our
understanding of social behavior and to serve as
useful guides in dealing with some social problems.

There has been much debate about the cor-


■ Monetary and fiscal policy
rect methodology to be used in social science.
Thomas Kuhn, a famous philosopher of science,
defined a paradigm as a scientific theory and
the core of beliefs that surround it. He argued
that scientific progression occurs by paradigm
shifts in which, for a long time, scientists will
resist change and hold on to an old theory even
as evidence mounts up against it, and even when
another theory better fits the data. Eventually,
however, the evidence in favor of the new theory
is so great that suddenly scientists shift their
thinking. The process can be likened to the way
a drop of water forms on a faucet. It grows larger
and larger until it falls. A good example in the
sciences is Einstein’s relativity theory in physics,
which was initially scoffed at but was later
adopted because it was consistent with a wider
range of physical phenomena than was the
earlier gravitational theory of Sir Isaac Newton.
Social scientists have discussed at great
length whether Kuhn’s theory of paradigm shift
is appropriate for the social sciences. If it is, it
gives legitimacy to competing theories. If it is
not, then the generally accepted theory can be
considered the best. The issue has never been
resolved, but our understanding of the relevance
of theories has advanced.
Imre Lakatos, another famous philosopher of
science, has extended Kuhn’s arguments by saying
that in social science there are generally many
competing theories, each being extended through
competing research programs, or groups of scientists working on a particular problem.
For example, in psychology there are the behaviorists and the Freudians. In sociology
there are functionalists, conflict theorists, and interactionists. We could cite different
theories within each social science. Advocates of each of the paradigms compete for
researchers. The group of researchers most successful in competing for followers is the
one most likely to grow.
12 chapter 1 Social Science and Its Methods

Psychology
Psychology deals with the mind and personality of
the individual. It is a social science because humans
are social creatures. It focuses on the individual and
physical processes, such as:

■ Biological structure
■ Development and maturation

Of the various branches of psychology, the most rele-


vant to social science is social psychology. Social
psychology is the study of the individual’s behavior as
it influences and is influenced by the behavior of
others. Some specific topics that interest psychologists
and social psychologists are:

■ Socialization
■ Environment and heredity
■ Adjustment and maladjustment

These social scientists deal with natural phenomena such


as emotion, memory, perception, and intelligence.

Other philosophers of science go further.


Some, like Paul Feurabend, argue that all
methodology is limiting and that the correct
methodology is no methodology. Still others ar-
gue that sociological issues, such as what is
likely to advance a scientist’s career, rather
than the truth of a theory determine what the
scientist believes.
In this book, we emphasize the competition
among various theories. By doing so, we hope
to show how, in social science, controversy
plays an important role in the development of
our knowledge.
Probably the best way to understand the sci-
entific method is to consider a couple of exam-ples
that do not follow the scientific method. For
instance, consider astrology or numerology. These
pseudostudies hold that by analyzing the alignment
of the stars or the position of certain numbers,
individuals can discover or predict events that will
affect them. However, the accu-racy of the
discoveries or the reliability of the predictions has
never been satisfactorily demon-strated to most
social scientists. Even though we might turn to our
horoscopes and say, “Aha!
That seems to fit my character or my experience,” if we critically consider these predic-
tions, often we see that the statements are so broad that they can be applied more or less
appropriately to a wide range of happenings or possibilities. This is not to say that the
so-cial sciences always avoid that. Economics, for instance, often comes up with
predictions from large, highly sophisticated mathematical models (called econometric
models), and some of these predictions are no better for steering a course than back-of-
the-envelope estimates.
A good social scientist generally takes an agnostic (not believing but also not disbe-
lieving) position about claims until they can be tested and retested. Consider, for
example, parapsychology, which argues that people can transmit certain information
independently of all conventional forms of communication. Shirley MacLaine’s best-
selling book Out on a Limb convinced many people that the claim of parapsychology is
true. Most social scien-tists remain unconvinced. They hold that, to date, the theories
have not been sufficiently demonstrated. In stating that these theories have not been
tested, a good social scientist is not dogmatic. It is possible that we social scientists
become so tied to our way of looking at the world that we are unable to consider the
possibilities of other ways. Who is to say that the tests we accept as conclusive are the
“right” tests? Or that our training hasn’t biased the tests?
Ultimately, however, we must make a working judgment about what is and what
isn’t an acceptable test, and social scientists’ methodology is an expression of that
working judgment. It should, however, be presented as a working judgment, not as a set
of definitive criteria of what is true and what is false. That’s why, generally, good social
scientists remain agnostic over a wide range of issues that they just don’t have time to
investigate. Thus, in many ways, what you will get out of a study of social science and
an understanding of its methods is a healthy understanding of the limitations of your
powers to know.
The Methods of Social Science 13

The Methods ofSocial Science


The basic procedures of the scientific method are as important in social science as in
physi-cal science. Social scientists must observe carefully, classify and analyze their
facts, make generalizations, and attempt to develop and test hypotheses to explain their
generalizations. Their problem, however, is often more difficult than that of physical
scientists. The facts gathered by the social scientist—for example, those concerning the
cultures of different peoples—have similarities, but each fact may also be unique in
significant respects. Facts of this kind are difficult to classify and interpret. Further, as
we have already noted, the gener-alizations or laws that the social scientist can make are
likely to be less definite and certain than those of the physical scientist.
The difficulty of discovering relatively exact laws that govern social life results
from sev-eral circumstances. First, the things of greatest importance in our social life—
satisfactions, social progress, democracy—are not really measurable. Second, society is
extremely complex. It is difficult and usually impossible to find and evaluate all the
many causes of a given situation, though often we can discover the factors that were
most important in bring-ing it about. Third, in every social situation there is the human
element. Frequently, the course of social events depends on the reaction of a few
individuals who are leaders, and, except in routine situations, we can seldom predict
individual behavior with complete certainty.
If the social scientist finally does succeed in finding uniformities or “laws” of social
behavior and in setting up hypotheses to explain them, there is still another difficulty—
namely, that investigators can seldom employ controlled experiments to test their hy-
potheses. To a considerable extent, the social scientist must substitute careful
observation and the mental process of abstraction for experi-ments. The investigator
abstracts from a given situa-tion some one factor in order to consider what effect it
would have if acting alone. To do this, the investi-gator imagines that any other factors
present remain constant or inert and asks, for example, a question such as: If other
factors affecting economic life re-mained constant, what would be the economic effect

of raising tariff rates on imports?


A social scientist with a thorough knowledge of a
situation may correctly calculate the effect of a given
causal factor by assuming that all other things remain
equal. However, to reach correct conclusions by this
method, the investigator must be both competent and
painstaking. Even then, the dangers of error are great. If
anything, there is more need for competence in the social
scientist than in the physical scientist. The theo-ries of a
physical scientist often can be proved right or wrong by
experiments, but this is seldom true of those of the social
scientist. An unfortunate result is that it is easier in social
science than in physical science to be needlessly vague, to
perpetuate errors, and to cover up incompetence.
“I’m a social scientist, Michael. That means I can’t
explain electricity or anything like that, but if you
ever want to know about people. I’m your man.”
Social scientists also have more difficulty than
physical scientists in being objective. Because they deal
14 chapter 1 Social Science and Its Methods

with human beings and are human themselves, social scientists find it hard to put aside their own
likes and dislikes, their sympathies, prejudices, and frustrations. As a result, they some-times fall
into the trap of trying to justify their own hopes, beliefs, or biases instead of seeking to discover
the truth. We should always be on guard against those who pose as social scientists but who, in
fact, substitute propaganda and charisma for objectivity and competence.
This does not mean that social science is any less scientific than the natural
sciences, or that it is less objective. It simply means that social scientists must be
continually on guard against such traps and must be as clear and objective as possible.
The differences between physical science and social science lead to slightly
different structures of research. Although there is no ideal structure, a reasonable
approach to a problem in social science is the following:
1. Observe.
2. Define the problem.
3. Review the literature. (Become familiar with what others have observed.)
4. Observe some more.
5. Develop a theoretical framework and formulate a hypothesis.
6. Choose the research design.
7. Collect the necessary data.
8. Analyze the results.
9. Draw conclusions.
Using this outline as a rough guide, and recognizing that the specific project and
each specific social science determine the exact nature of the methodology to be used,
you have a reasonably good method of attack.

Observing. Notice that social science begins with observation. Social science is about
the real world, and the best way to know about the real world is to observe it.

Defining the problem. Of the various research steps listed, this one is probably the most
important. If you’ve carefully defined your terms, you can save an enormous amount of
energy. Put simply, if you don’t know what you’re doing, no matter how well you do it,
you’re not going to end up with much. The topic might be chosen for a variety of
reasons, perhaps because it raises issues of fundamental social science importance,
perhaps because it has suddenly become a focus of controversy, or perhaps because
research funds have become available to investigate it.

Reviewing the literature. Knowledge of the relevant literature is essential because it


provides background, suggests approaches, indicates what has already been covered and
what hasn’t, and saves you from redoing what has already been done. It is a way of
using other people’s observations.

Observing some more. After you have defined your problem and reviewed the literature,
your observation will be sharper. You will know more precisely what you are looking
for and how to look for it.

Developing a theoretical framework and formulating a hypothesis. Make a statement predicting


your results and then clarify what each of the terms in the statement means within the framework
of your research. Suppose your hypothesis is: “High price increases sales of fashionable
magazines.” You should specify how high is high, and compared to what specific price is the
price stated to be high; how much of an increase is significant over the circulation the magazine
enjoyed at the lower price; what sales are included (newsstand,
The Methods of Social Science 15

subscription, or both); and what is “fashionable.” Different researchers may define the
same term differently, which is one of the reasons why the same research subject can
produce different results.

Choosing a research design. Pick a means of gathering data—a survey, an experiment,


an observational study, use of existing sources, or a combination. Weigh this choice
carefully because your plan is the crux of the research process.

Collecting the necessary data. Data are what one collects from careful observation.
Your conclusions will be only as good as your data, so take great care in collecting and,
especially, in recording your data. If you can’t document what you’ve done, you might
as well not have done it.

Analyzing the results. When all the data are in, classify facts, identify trends, recognize
relationships, and tabulate the information so that it can be accurately analyzed and
interpreted. A given set of facts may be interpreted two different ways by two different
analysts, so give your analysis careful, objective attention. After this step has been
taken, your hypothesis can then be confirmed, rejected, or modified.

Drawing conclusions. Now you can prepare a report, summarizing the steps you’ve
followed and discussing what you’ve found. A good report will relate your conclusions
to the existing body of research, suggest where current assumptions may be modified
because of new evidence, and possibly identify unanswered questions for further study.

These steps differ slightly from those used by a natural scientist, but only slightly—
the primary difference comes in testing a hypothesis. In some natural sciences, it is
possible to conduct controlled experiments in which the same experiment can be
repeated again and again under highly regulated conditions. In the social sciences, such
controlled experiments are more difficult to construct.
The line between social science and natural science is not fixed. In some natural sci-ences,
perfectly controlled experiments are impossible. In cosmological physics, for example, one can’t
create the universe again and again. Thus, one must speculate about a hypothesis, draw
conclusions from that hypothesis, and see whether the conclusions match what one observes in
the universe. Alternatively, in the social science of psychology, certain controlled experiments
are possible—for example, individuals can be given specific stimuli under specific conditions
again and again. Thus, the difference between the way one deals with the natural sciences and the
way one deals with the social sciences can be blurry.
Let’s take an example of the use of the social science method—Joseph Holz’s study of the
implications of teen pregnancy. First, he studied all the writing on teen pregnancy. Then he set up
the following hypothesis: Teen motherhood causes the mothers to be economically and socially
worse off than they otherwise would have been. To test this hypothesis, he used data that had
been collected over many years tracking the lives of teenage women. From that he extracted two
groups—a set of teenagers who had become pregnant and borne the child and a set of teenagers
who had become pregnant but had miscarried. He then compared their economic and social
positions when they were in their mid-thirties. If teen mother-hood caused the mother to be worse
off, then the teens who had borne their babies should have been in a worse position than those
who miscarried. They weren’t. He found no signifi-cant difference between the two groups: Both
were low income, significantly dependent on welfare benefits, and had completed the same
number of years of school. The initial hypoth-esis was false. Teen pregnancy did not make
mothers worse off; it was simply a symptom of a larger set of problems. This larger set of
problems was so severe that whether mothers had borne a child in their teens made little
difference to their economic and social positions.
16 chapter 1 Social Science and Its Methods

Holz’s findings were published as the government was conducting a costly


campaign against teen motherhood, and his conclusions were unpopular with both
liberals and conservatives. Liberals did not like them because his study suggested that
much of the family planning advice and sex education developed by liberals was of
little help in im-proving these women’s lives. Conservatives didn’t like them because
his study implied that more substantive changes than simply eliminating teen
motherhood were needed to improve these women’s lives and break the cycle of
poverty. But good social science methodology is not about pleasing anybody—it is
about understanding social issues and social problems.
Although Holz’s experiment was not fully controlled, it was as close as one could come to a
controlled experiment in the social sciences. It selected similar groups to compare in such a way
that no obvious reason existed as to why these two groups should differ.

Social Science Approaches to Problems


As you review the literature about various social science studies, you will see that social
sci-entists can use many different approaches and methods as they study problems. We
first consider alternative approaches; then we consider alternative methods.

Alternative Approaches. The approach one takes when analyzing a problem reflects one’s
worldview—the lens through which one sees the world. Four approaches that social
scientists use are the functionalist theory approach, the exchange theory approach, the
conflict theory approach, and the symbolic interaction theory approach.

The functionalist theory approach. This approach emphasizes the interconnectedness of


social life and the difficulty of affecting only one part of society with a policy.
Followers of the functionalist theory approach are hesitant to make social judgments
because all aspects of society have certain functions.

The exchange theory approach. Closely related to the functionalist approach, the
exchange theory approach emphasizes the voluntary exchanges of individuals as
reflecting individuals’ choices. Thus, the structure of society reflects individuals’
desires. The exchange theory approach lens is one of relative harmony in society,
sometimes upset by dysfunctional elements.

The conflict theory approach. The conflict theory approach sees far less harmony than
the exchange theory approach. Followers of this approach see social behavior in terms
of conflict and tension among competing groups or classes. Whereas the exchange
theory approach sees individuals’ voluntary choices, the conflict theory approach sees
force and power directing individual actions.

The symbolic interaction theory approach. The symbolic interaction theory approach
sees individuals as deriving meaning from the symbols they learn from. Followers of
this approach see reality as reflecting less what people do and more what they think and
feel. Their motives and perceptions, rather than actions, are emphasized.

These approaches are not necessarily independent of one another. Some social
scien-tists use a combination of approaches to study problems, while some use one at
one time and another at another time.

Alternative Methods. In addition to using different approaches, social scientists also use
different methods. These include the historical method, the case method, and the
comparative and cross-cultural methods.
The Methods of Social Science 17

The historical method. Because most social developments—such as the government of


the United States—have unique characteristics, in order to understand them as fully as
possible the social scientist must rely heavily on a study of their historical background.
We can never understand completely how any historical situation came to exist, because
there are limits to our historical knowledge and causes become increasingly complex
and uncertain as we trace them further into the past. We can, however, make both
historical events and present social situations much more intelligibly by using the
historical method—tracing the principal past developments that seem to have been
directly significant in bringing about a social situation. To trace these past
developments, a historian will use many of the same methods as other social scientists
such as collecting birth and marriage certificates and classifying those data.
It has been noted that history never really repeats itself. Nevertheless, present and
past situations often have such striking similarities that a knowledge of the past can give
us in-sights into present situations and sometimes into future trends.

The case method. Writers on the methodology of social research have devoted a great
deal of attention to the case method—its characteristics, its variations, the uses it can
serve, its advantages, and its limitations. Here we only describe its basic nature. The
case method involves making a detailed examination and analysis of a particular issue
or problem situation. This can involve a case study of a single person such as that by a
psychologist of his client, a single area or town such as a sociologist’s study of why a
town changes, or even a study of whole countries such as an economist’s when
comparing various countries.
A case study can be intended to discover how to bring about desirable changes in a par-
ticular problem situation: for example, to find the most effective ways of upgrading or reha-
bilitating a slum area. More often, the chief purpose of a case study is to throw light on many
similar situations that exist in a society. The hope is that an understanding of one or a few cases
will illuminate the others and thus aid in solving the social problems they present. The case or
cases selected should be typical of the group they purport to represent.
The preceding requirement can be a limiting factor in the usefulness of the case
method. Suppose we wanted to make a study of the class structure of U.S. society as a
whole. Obviously, it would be easier to select as cases for study several relatively small
and isolated cities in various sections of the country. But it is questionable whether
these would give us a true picture of the country as a whole, because today a great
proportion of our people live in large metropolitan areas where the class structure is
likely to be much more complex than in smaller and more isolated communities.
However, to study and describe in detail the class structure of such an area may be
prohibitively difficult and expensive, and therefore impractical.

The comparative and cross-cultural methods. The comparative method was formerly
often employed in the hope of discovering evolutionary sequences in the development
of human institutions—that is, patterns of social development or progress that would be
universal. For example, it was sometimes assumed that definite stages existed in the
development of governmental institutions, and it was thought that these stages could be
discovered by comparing a society at one level of development with some other society
at a different level. Today, this attempt to find patterns of social evolution that can be
applied to all societies has been largely abandoned.
However, comparison of different societies still plays an important role in anthropo-logical
studies through what is called the cross-cultural method. This method consists of making
detailed studies of the culture patterns of a number of societies for the purpose of comparing the
different ways in which their people meet similar needs. These studies some-times show
surprising similarities in the cultural traits of widely separated peoples who appear to have had no
direct or indirect contacts with one another.
18 chapter 1 Social Science and Its Methods

Comparison of the characteristics of different societies involves problems. At times,


it is difficult to decide whether two or more societies are independent or should be
treated as one. Or consider definitions: If we are comparing the family institution in
different soci-eties, we must define family broadly enough to cover cultural variations
yet specifically enough to make comparisons meaningful. Sociologists do not always
agree on just what a family is. Again, if we are comparing unemployment in urban-
industrial societies, we must agree on what we mean by unemployment. For example, in
the early 1980s, the unemploy-ment rate in Mexico, computed by U.S. standards, was
approximately 30 percent. Mexican economists, however, argued that this figure was
meaningless because Mexican work habits and culture were different from those in the
United States. Much of what was measured as unemployment, they said, was actually
individuals working at home and not earning money in the marketplace. Thus, although
they had nonmarket jobs, they had been counted as unemployed.

Common Sense in the Social Sciences


Probably the most important lesson to remember when conducting any research is that
you should use what might be called an educated common sense. You can understand
the analytic argument for common sense by considering the mind as a supercomputer
stor-ing enormous amounts of information, not all of which may lie at the surface of
recall. This holds true even with the vast increase in computer power. Processing speeds
of com-puters double every eighteen months, according to Moore’s Law. That increase
has made it possible to do enormous things even with home computers. However,
compared with the capabilities of the human mind, even the most powerful computer
counts by using its fingers and toes. The mind processes trillions of pieces of
information in millinanosec-onds (we don’t know what they are either, but we do know
they are very small). When the results of the models and the minds diverge, it seems
reasonable to rely on the more pow-erful computer—the mind. It makes sense to do so,
however, only if the best information has been input into the mind. Common sense is
not sufficient; we must use educated common sense.
To see the difference between common sense and educated common sense, consider the
problem: Does the earth circle the sun or does the sun circle the earth? Uneducated common
sense tells us that the sun circles the earth, and that commonsense conclusion became built into
society and society’s view of itself throughout the Middle Ages. To believe otherwise was
heresy. In 1540, Copernicus tried to fit that commonsense view with observations that classi-cal
Greeks had made of the heavens. As he went about this task, he discovered that he could get a
good fit of the data with the theory only if he assumed the earth moved around the sun. His was
an educated common sense—rational thought based on observation and the best information
available. It was that kind of educated common sense that ultimately led to the scientific method.
As specialization makes us focus on narrower and narrower issues, it is important to keep in the
back of our minds that scientific analysis has made us look at only part of the problem and that
we must also use our educated common sense to interpret the results reasonably.

The Use ofStatistics


Whenever possible, social scientists rely on quantitative data—data that can be reduced to
numbers—but often quantitative data are not available, so social scientists must rely on
qualitative data such as interviews or heuristic summaries of information in the literature. When
using qualitative data, it is much more difficult to draw specific inferences from the data, because
the “facts” one finds depend on how one interprets the qualitative data. One way to partially
overcome such “interpretive problems” is the “Delphic method” in which
The Methods of Social Science 19

another specialist in the field reviews your interpretation and then you modify your
inter-pretation in response if you see fit, explaining your reasons for accepting or
rejecting the suggested modifications. Another way is to translate the qualitative data
into quantitative data, creating “proxies” (stand-ins) for any missing quantitative data,
although that often simply hides the interpretative issues rather than eliminating them.
If quantitative data are available, social scientists rely on statistical analysis—informa-tion
in numerical form that has been assembled and classified—to provide the social scien-tist with
the information needed to understand social relationships and processes. Statistics do not enable
us to measure directly such basic social values as good citizenship, happiness, or welfare, but
they are useful in measuring other factors that underlie social life, such as the size of the
population of a country, or the number of families whose incomes fall below some level that we
set as the minimum for decent and healthful living. Statistical relationships also give us insights
into social problems. If we find that the proportion of males in juvenile de-tention centers who
come from broken homes is substantially greater than the proportion of males in the population at
large who come from such homes, this suggests that broken homes may be an important factor
contributing to juvenile delinquency. But statistics must always be interpreted with care, for it
can be easy to read into them conclusions they do not justify. Also, it is sometimes possible to
manipulate them so that they appear to show what we want them to show.

Although statistics measure the results of social activity and highlight trends, they have other
useful functions: testing theories and discovering relationships. For example, correlation is the
relationship between two sets of data. A high correlation between sets of data means that if an
element in one set rises, its corresponding element in the other set is also likely to rise. Other
statistics determine how sure we are of a relationship. We do not discuss these statistics because
an introductory social science course is not the place to learn them, but it is the place to learn that
such techniques of testing relationships exist, and they may be worth your while to study at some
point in the future.
If we are going to use statistics, we must have data. Data are the raw numbers
describ-ing an event, occurrence, or situation. Social scientists’ data come from
measuring and counting all occurrences of a particular happening. For example, we
might find, “In 2007, there were x number of murders and y number of suicides.” One
way to get data is to con-duct a survey, a method whereby data are collected from
individuals or institutions by means of questionnaires or interviews. For instance, we
might conduct a survey in which selected people are questioned or polled on such
matters as their incomes, their beliefs on certain issues, or the political candidate for
whom they intend to vote. Figure 1.2 gives an example of such a survey. Statistics can
tell us how large a portion of a group must be sur-veyed before we can be reasonably
sure that the results will reflect the views of the entire group. Such techniques are used
extensively in surveys such as the Gallup or Harris public opinion polls.
The use of statistics has been greatly facilitated, and therefore greatly expanded, by
the computer. The computer has made it possible to record, arrange, and rearrange
voluminous information quickly and analytically. Today, enormous amounts of data and
other resources are available to anyone with a computer or other access to the Internet.
With the expansion of social data and the enormous increase in computing power, it
is increasingly possible for social scientists to look for relationships in the data alone,
rather than to be guided in that search by theories. Using highly sophisticated statistical
techniques, social scientists analyze data, looking for patterns. After they find a pattern,
they fit that pattern to a theory. For example, social scientists Stephen Levitt and John
Donohue searched the data and found a relationship between the passage of the abor-
tion rights law in the United States and a decrease in crime in later periods. Based on
this evidence, they argued that because abortion reduced the number of unwanted chil-
dren, those children who were born had more guidance, and that it was the law making
abortion legal, not any change in law enforcement or increase in the number of inmates
20 chapter 1 Social Science and Its Methods

1. Do you worry very much about the AIDS problem?


50.4 No
49.6 Yes

If yes, what are your specific worries?


1.8 Past-oriented (did past partners tell truth)
26.6 Present-oriented (e.g., general fear, casual

relationships uncertain, others apathetic)


20.5 Future-oriented (e.g., rapid spread, new
means of spreading may be found)

Directions: For the following items, indicate the degree of your agreement
or disagreement by placing an “X” in the appropriate column.

Strongly Strongly
Agree Agree Disagree Disagree

2. Fraternities are the center 32.5% 47.9% 18.8% 0.9%


of social life on campus.
9.4% 28.2% 53.0% 9.4%
3. Fraternities encourage
too much illegal drinking.
6.9% 15.5% 59.5% 18.1%
4. Fraternities encourage
too much vandalism.
3.5% 33.9% 47.8% 14.8%
5. The fraternity system encourages
the development of positive
female– male relationships.

6. I worry about contracting AIDS. 4.3% 41.9% 39.3% 14.5%


5.2% 19.0% 50.0% 25.9%
7. There is too much fear of AIDS today.
13.8% 46.6% 33.6% 6.0%
8. There should be much more
mandatory testing for AIDS.

9. Regarding AIDS, the best statement


to describe my concern is “It won’t
happen to me.”
1.7% 25.6% 45.3% 27.4%

Figure 1.2
One of the best ways by which social scientists collect information is through a survey.
This is one page of a fourteen-page survey conducted by college students for their
sociology class. Because of time pressures in that particular survey, they were unable to
perform an extensive analysis of their data. For this reason, they urged caution in the
use and interpretation of the information.
Social Science and Society 21

jailed, that was mostly responsible for the decrease in crime rates that the United States
experienced in the 1990s.
Whenever making such claims, social scientists should be very careful not to
confuse correlation—the simultaneous movement of two variables—with causation—
in which change in one variable brings about change in the other variable. The
difference can be seen in the following example. When it is expected to rain, more
people carry umbrellas, so um-brella usage and rain are correlated. But the fact that
people carry umbrellas does not cause it to rain, or so most of us believe.

The Interdisciplinary Approach


Modern industrial societies and their problems are becoming increasingly complex, and because
no one person today can master all the social sciences, growing emphasis is placed on the
interdisciplinary approach to many social problems. The interdisciplinary approach means that
a group of social scientists with different specialties will work together on a cer-tain problem, not
all of whose aspects any one of the group fully understands. For some problems, such as those
surrounding pollution, it may be necessary to call in, say, a physical scientist, a geologist, and an
engineer. But in facing all of these problems, the need for edu-cated people who have a broad
sense of problems and interrelationships—who understand the need for a unified social science—
is also becoming more and more evident.
Though few social relationships can be reduced to exact and invariable laws, human
beings in large groups everywhere show great likenesses of behavior when conditions
are really similar. Thus, there is reason to believe that we can, through systematic study
and re-search, greatly increase our understanding of the nature and development of
human soci-eties, and to hope that the attitudes fostered by the interdisciplinary
approach itself and the knowledge to which it leads us can ultimately result in greater
tolerance and cooperation among diverse groups and among nations.

Social Science and Society


Some people believe that the social sciences are lagging behind the natural sciences. They
maintain that not only does social science have no exact laws, but that it has also failed to
eliminate great social evils such as racial discrimination, crime, poverty, and war. They im-ply
that social scientists have failed to accomplish what might reasonably have been expected of
them. However, such critics are usually unaware of the real nature of social science and of its
special problems and basic limitations. For example, they forget that the solution to a social
problem requires not only knowledge but also the ability to influence people. Even if social
scientists discover the procedures that should be followed to achieve social improve-ment, they
are seldom in a position to control social action. For that matter, even dictators find that there are
limits to their power to change society.

Agreeing on Policy
One of the great problems in a democracy is getting the majority of people to reach sub-stantial
agreement on the major policies that should be followed to create a better society. Social
scientists can aid in bringing about this agreement by helping people to understand the issues, the
difficulties involved, and the possible steps to a solution. If we express social objectives in
sufficiently general terms, agreement is not so hard to obtain. Most people would like to have a
heaven on earth characterized by peace and goodwill, with freedom, justice, security, health, and
happiness for all. But when it comes to drawing up a blueprint
22 chapter 1 Social Science and Its Methods

for reaching these objectives, disagreements and obstacles become apparent. Social
scien-tists themselves are not always in complete agreement on what our specific social
goals should be or on how we can best work toward them.
In any case, the function of social science and of those who practice it is not
primarily to determine social objectives. Its major function is to discover how our
objectives can be achieved. The determination of the goals themselves—our social
values—is not a scientific problem but one having to do with our likes and dislikes, our
esthetic concepts, our moral standards, and our philosophical and religious beliefs. (We
will have more to say about social values in the following chapter.)

Values,Terminology,and Rhetoric
This chapter began with a quotation from Albert Einstein who said that “theories should
be as simple as possible, but not more so.” The same thing could be said about ideas and
the expression of those ideas. Unfortunately, specialists have an incentive to develop a
terminol-ogy that is anything but simple and that often obscures rather than clarifies.
One of the many social science teachers who has written us about this book (and in
doing so, these teachers have played an important role in its development) described a
history conference she attended where “we were treated to such goodies” as
The sociopolitical internecine amortizations of agronomous proletarization, if solely
counter-productive of Jurassic multi-dimensional interstitial extrapolated Augustinian and
Aristotelian epistemological diagrammetric middle-sector dichotomies, as measured in the
context of para-digmatic vestigiae (though challenged none too effectively, if I am not
remiss in saying so, by Freylinghausen’s hypothesis delivered at the University of Bordeaux
in April 1896) are existen-tially and polaristically categorized by Nordlinger’s Metternichian
thermodynamics as tangen-tially interrelated with studies promulgated by
Darffenstangenovich on a scale of one to twenty factored to the 24th power.
Although she may have used a bit of literary license in transcribing the conference
pro-ceedings, her point is well taken. She was attending a conference on her specialty,
yet she didn’t understand what was being said. It happens all the time, not only to
students, but to teachers as well. Although there may be valuable ideas in what many
specialists have to say, we can’t profit from them if we can’t understand them, or if we
must spend hours translat-ing them.
In his wonderful book, The Sociological Imagination, C. W. Mills made precisely this point.
He argued that in many social sciences, “high theory” is top-heavy with jargon. As an example,
he interpreted sociologist Talcott Parsons’s terminology: He reduced it by 80 to 90 percent and at
the same time made it more intelligible. Mills wasn’t making the point that Parsons’s insights
weren’t good ones; to the contrary, Mills believed that Parsons was a brilliant sociologist. But
Parsons’s language obscured his brilliant ideas.
Another characteristic of language is that it embodies value judgments and pre-
serves ways of looking at problems. A good social scientist recognizes this and is
always open to dealing with reality by alternative modes of expression and new ways of
looking at issues.

Conclusion If this chapter has succeeded in its intended purpose, it should have given you a sense of what it
means to be a social scientist. As you saw, the social sciences are evolving: They in-teract and
they move among the humanities, the natural sciences, and the individual social sciences
depending on who is working with them. They are fluid, not static, and that fluid-ity will present
problems to anyone who attempts too fixed a definition of any of them.
Questions for Review and Discussion 23

The ability to handle the fluid definitions, to recognize the shadows as well as the objects
without flinching, is an important characteristic that good social scientists exhibit— one which, if
learned, will serve you well as you study this book and play the game of life.

Key Points
• Social science is the name given to our framework and formulate a hypothesis, choose
knowledge about the nature, growth, and the research design, collect the necessary data,
functioning of human society. analyze the results, and draw conclusions.
• The scientific method is a set of rules about how • Three typical methods in social science are the
to establish rules. historical method, the case method, and the
• A good social scientist generally takes a wait- com-parative method.
and-see position about claims until they are • It is important to use educated common sense in
tested and retested. the social sciences.
• A reasonable approach to a problem in social sci- • A good social scientist is always open to new
ence is to observe, define the problem, review the ways of looking at issues.
literature, observe some more, develop a theoretical

Some ImportantTerms
anthropology (1) experimental method (9) psychology (1)
biological science (2) functionalist theory research program (11)
case method (17) approach (16) scientific knowledge (1)
causation (21) geography (1) scientific method (8)
cognitive science (4) historical method (17) social science (1)
comparative method (17) history (1) sociology (1)
conflict theory approach (16) humanities (2) statistical analysis (19)
correlation (21) interdisciplinary approach (21) survey (19)
cross-cultural method (17) natural experiments (10) symbolic interaction theory
economics (1) natural science (2) approach (16)
educated common sense (18) paradigm (11)
exchange theory approach (16) political science (1)

Questions forReview and Discussion


1. What is scientific knowledge? How does it differ 6. What basic assumption underlies the use of the
from knowledge acquired “unconsciously”? scientific method?
2. Distinguish among the three major fields of human 7. What is the experimental method?
knowledge. What is the emphasis of each? 8. Why is it difficult to formulate precise laws in
3. Name the principal social sciences and define the field of social science?
the field with which each deals. 9. Are there any advantages to having competing
4. Why would it have been difficult to carry on research programs?
sci-entific investigation in primitive societies or 10. In what sense is social science scientific?
even in the Middle Ages? 11. Why is it often impossible to study social prob-
5. What is the scientific method? lems by means of the experimental method?
24 chapter 1 Social Science and Its Methods

12. Explain the ways in which the problems of 14. Social science has been broken down into
social science differ from those of the exact special-ties. Why is it a problem to put them
natural sciences. back to-gether through a unified theory?
13. What are the advantages of the interdiscipli- 15. What new social science fields do you think
nary approach to the study of many social will be important ten years from now? Why do
problems? you think so?

Internet Questions
1. Using an Internet search engine directory (for the subdisciplines or branches listed under your
example, http://dir.google.com or http://dir. choice?
yahoo.com), look at the lists of topics included 4. Take the survey about alcohol use at
under Social Science or Society. How many www.alcoholscreening.org. After taking the sur-
fields are listed? What fields would you add (or vey, look at the feedback you are given based
delete) in a list of your own? on your answers. What can the results for this
2. The website www.buildfreedom.com/content/ survey be used for?
scientific_method.shtml uses an abbreviated 5. Go to www.ncpa.org/pi/crime/pd08599g.html
ver-sion of the scientific method to solve the and read the discussion about Donohue and
social problem of dating. Use this process to Levitt’s study of abortion and crime rates men-
“solve” another everyday problem. tioned in the text. What are some of the alterna-
3. Go to www.wikipedia.org and choose one of the tive arguments that critics use to explain why
branches listed under Social Sciences. What are the crime rate has decreased?

For Further Study


Greene, Brian, The Fabric of the Cosmos, New York: Knopf, WWW Anthropology Resources on the Internet www.
2004. anthropologie.net
Hecht, Jennifer Michael, Doubt: A History: The Great WWW Economic History Services http://eh.net/
Doubters and Their Legacy of Innovation, San Francisco: WWW Encyclopedia of Psychology www.psychology.org
Harper, 2004. (accessed June 16, 2009)
Mills, C. Wright, The Sociological Imagination, New York: WWW Political Resources on the Net www.politicalresources.
Oxford University Press, 1959. net
Repcheck, Jack, Copernicus’ Secret: How the Scientific WWW Social Science Research Council www.ssrc.org
Revolution Began, New York: Simon & Schuster, 2009. WWW Sociological Resources on the Internet www.socioweb.
Slater, Lauren, Opening Skinner’s Box: Great Psychological com
Experiments of the Twentieth Century, New York: WWW Virtual Library in Economics www.helsinki.fi/WebEc
Norton, 2004. WWW Virtual Library in Sociology http://socserv.
Tilly, Charles, Why, Princeton, NJ: Princeton University mcmaster.ca/w3virtsoclib
Press, 2006.
Wilson, Edward O., Consilience: The Unity of Knowledge, New
York: Knopf, 1998.
Historical Roots appendix
ofSocial Science

Natural scientists tell us that the world has been around East during the Crusades (the religious wars from 1095
for some 6 billion years and that living things have been to 1272 in which Christians in Europe attempted to
around for at least 3 billion. We will go back, however, capture Christianity’s traditional territory in the Middle
only about 2,600 years, when Western philosophy began East), Europeans became reacquainted with the learn-ing
on the fringes of ancient Greece (some theorists hold that of the ancient Greeks, and they brought back the body of
the Greeks responded to ideas from Eastern civilizations, ancient Greek learning to Europe, where it was generally
but there are limits to even our broad sweep). The Greeks available by the twelfth century. These ideas spread
came to realize that their ancient account of how the slowly throughout Europe over the next three hundred
world was cre-ated and administered—by an enormous years, and by the middle of the fifteenth cen-tury,
collection of gods, or pantheon—was not the only rediscovery of Greek civilization in Europe was
possible expla-nation. They are credited with being the widespread. Because the period from about 1453 (the fall
first to estab-lish rational theory, independent of of Constantinople) to the end of the seventeenth century
theological creed; to grasp rational concepts and use was characterized by the rebirth and prolifera-tion of
them as a way of looking at reality and seeing logical ancient knowledge, it became known as the Renaissance
connections; and to be empirical and antimystical. Two (a French word meaning “rebirth”).
great Greek thinkers of the third and fourth centuries B.C., The Renaissance must have been a wonderful
Plato and Aristotle, are responsible for establishing a time for scholars. The totality of knowledge was
basis for knowledge as we know it and deal with it today. still comprehensible by the human mind. An ideal
in the Renaissance was that an educated person
The philosophical debates of the Greek period were could know everything and exercise all skills and
in many ways the same ones that go on today, explaining social graces. A true Renaissance man was willing
how, when all things change, things must also be to take on all com-ers on any issue.
simultaneously unchanging; otherwise, some-thing As the store of knowledge grew, it became harder
would have to be created out of nothing—a log-ical and harder to know everything, and so people began to
impossibility. These ideas would later develop into specialize. A natural division opened, one between the
modern physics, including the laws of thermody-namics humanities (the study of literature, music, and art) and
and the proposition that matter can neither be created nor physics. The physics part of this division was not refined
destroyed—merely transformed. The Greeks also enough, and soon physics was broken up into empirical
considered many of the issues that later became the studies (which developed into the vari-ous natural
social sciences; for example, they consid-ered the role of sciences) and metaphysics (nonempirical studies that
the state (political science), the way minds interact with developed into philosophy).
society (psychology), and indi-viduals’ interaction within The Renaissance was preceded by the Middle Ages
the market (economics). Thus, the history of the social (a period from roughly A.D. 476, and the end of the
sciences begins with the Greeks. The history, however, is Roman Empire, to A.D. 1453, the defeat of Christian re-
not continuous. ligious armies in Constantinople by the Islamic Turks).
Much of the Greek contribution to knowledge would In the Middle Ages, religion was so central to life that
have been lost (who knows what other contribu-tions the study of religion was taken for granted, and it tied
actually have been lost?) were it not for its preser-vation together all the other fields of study. For example,
by Eastern civilizations. On their forays into the painters painted religious pictures, musicians wrote

25
26 chapter 1 Social Science and Its Methods

religious music, and the study of literature was the study between A.D. 1650 and A.D. 1700 and continued for
of the Bible and its commentators. Questions that today about one hundred years. It is in this period that the
seem the obvious ones, such as, Why are people divided development of the social sciences took hold and
into classes? and Why are the poor poor? were simply flourished.
not asked. Things were the way they were because that By the time of the Enlightenment, it had become
was God’s will. Once one knew God’s will, the issue evident that to know everything—to be a Renaissance
was how to carry it out. For exam-ple, medieval scholars scholar—was impossible. Not only was it impossible to
believed in a “just” price and that collecting interest on know everything, but it was also impossible to know
savings was immoral. They taught those principles and everything about just one subject—say, all of physics or
condemned those who did not follow their teachings. all of philosophy. Individuals began to spe-cialize their
study. For instance, chemistry and astron-omy were
As the Renaissance dawned and continued, that separated from physics.
religious tie provoked tension as scholars in the vari-ous As philosophers delved into their subject, they
fields of study came to conclusions different from the further divided philosophy into parts. One part was
church’s doctrines, beginning a long conflict be-tween metaphilosophy, the study of issues that most schol-ars
religious learning and beliefs and so-called rationalist agreed were not empirically testable. One such is-sue
learning and beliefs. was: Because God is all-powerful, can he create a rock
The tension between religious explanations and so heavy he cannot move it? The other division of
rationalist explanations was (and still is) inevitable. The philosophy dealt with issues that could, in princi-ple at
rationalist approach places human reason above faith. In least, be empirically tested. For instance: What type of
a rationalist approach, one looks for logical connections political organization of society is preferable? It is from
and is continually asking the question, Can you prove it? the second division that the social sciences evolved.
This meant that somehow the ra-tionalists had to figure (They were called sciences because they were in
out what it meant to prove something. A religious principle meant to be empirically testable.)
approach places faith above reason. A religious The Enlightenment spawned social science be-cause
explanation had no need to prove anything: Explanations the Enlightenment rejected the assumption that the
were accepted on faith. classical world of the Greeks and the Romans was
Throughout the Renaissance, rationalism more and perfect. In the Enlightenment (roughly the whole of the
more replaced religion as the organizing principle of eighteenth century), there was a general belief that
knowledge, and as it did, the various fields of knowledge civilization had improved and so too should the think-ing
became divided along rationalist lines. The humanities about civilization. Moreover, in the seventeenth century,
still reflected religious issues; the rational-ist revolution just preceding the Enlightenment, there was continual
came much later to the humanities. To the degree that turmoil—a long drawn-out war between France and
they were considered, most of the is-sues we now England and a religious conflict between Catholics and
classify under social science were studied as part of Protestants about how to interpret God’s will. That fight
history. History was part of literature and the humanities. broke down the religious explanations and made people
It was simply a documentation of what had happened—it very much aware of social problems. Which of the two
never asked why something happened. To ask why meant explanations, Catholic or Protestant, was right? Why
failure to accept God’s will. Thus, it was primarily from were they fighting? What could be done about it? The
philosophy, not his-tory, that most of the social sciences social sciences developed as indi-viduals attempted to
emerged. explain those social problems and suggest what could be
The natural sciences and philosophy divided along done to solve them.
modes of inquiry and answers to the question, Can you Although the existence of social problems that
prove it? The study of philosophy itself evolved into a require solutions may seem obvious to you, it was
variety of fields, such as logic, morals, and epistemology not always so obvious. This view is the product of
(the study of knowledge). the Enlightenment, which established the “three
humilia-tions” of human beings. These are:
The Enlightenment 1. The earth is not the center of the universe.
The Enlightenment is the period in which rational- 2. Humans are creatures of nature like other animals.
ism definitely replaced religion as the organizing 3. Our reasoning ability is subject to passions and
principle of knowledge. The Enlightenment began subconscious desires.
Historical Roots of Social Science 27

Frontispiece from Diderot’s Encyclopédie, written during the Enlightenment.

Before we experienced these humiliations, thinkers Jean d’Alembert, which appeared over a span of several
could rely on an order they believed was es-tablished by years in the mid-1700s. The full title of this encyclope-
God. Social problems were set up by God and were to be dia proclaimed it to be a rational dictionary of science,
accepted or endured. Only after the beginning of the art, and industry. Unlike earlier compilations, it con-
Enlightenment did people begin to believe that society tained systematic articles on man, society, and method,
and culture are themselves prod-ucts of history and the and a number of the first definitions of the social sci-
evolution of culture—that they had changed and would ences can be traced to this mammoth work.
continue to change. There are many ways to look at social problems, and
As is often the case, the change in viewpoint as scholars began considering human beings in reference
had a paradoxical counterpoint, and human beings’ to their social environment, the diversity soon became
“humiliation” was accompanied by a belief in apparent. The history of each of the so-cial sciences
human beings’ power. If society could change, then becomes hopelessly tangled with that of each of the
the change could be, at least to some extent, guided others at this point. In the Enlightenment, scholars were
and directed by human beings. debating one another and ideas were quickly evolving.
Since its conception, social science has To capture even a flavor of the inter-action and debate
entwined these two aspects. Sometimes it is simply leads to a formidable morass, hardly conducive to a
trying to understand, and it accepts our limited social science course. So we will stop our consideration
powers and our place in the cosmos, and at other here.
times it is trying to change society.

From Philosophy to Social Science Some ImportantTerms


The evolution of philosophy into the social sciences Enlightenment (26)
can be seen in France, where philosophers joined to Middle Ages (25)
produce an encyclopedia, edited by Denis Diderot and Renaissance (25)

You might also like