Coping and Stress in Psychology
Coping and Stress in Psychology
Coping and Stress in Psychology
Signs of stress
Stress makes it hard for us to relax
and can come with a range of emotions, including
anxiety and irritability. When stressed, we may find
it difficult to concentrate. We may experience
headaches or other body pains, an upset stomach
or trouble sleeping. We may find we lose our
appetite or eat more than usual. Chronic stress can
worsen pre-existing health problems and may
increase our use of alcohol, tobacco and other
substances.
Stressful situations can also cause or exacerbate
mental health conditions, most commonly anxiety
and depression, which require access to health care.
When we suffer from a mental health condition, it
may be because our symptoms of stress have
become persistent and have started affecting our
daily functioning, including at work or school.
Theories of stress
Theories that focus on the specific
relationship between external demands (stressors)
and bodily processes (stress) can be grouped in two
different categories: approaches to systemic stress
based in physiology and psychobiology (among
others, Selye 1976) and approaches to psychological
stress developed within the field of cognitive
psychology (Lazarus 1966, 1991, Lazarus and
Folkman 1984, McGrath 1982).
COPING
Coping refers to conscious
strategies used to reduce unpleasant emotions.
Coping strategies can be cognitions or
behaviors and can be individual or social.
Coping is to deal with and overcome struggles
and difficulties in life.[1] It is a way for us to
maintain our mental and emotional well-
being.[2] Everybody has a way of handling the
hard events that occur in our life and that is
what it means to cope. Coping can be healthy
and productive, or destructive and unhealthy for
you or others. It is recommended that an
individual copes in ways that will be beneficial
and healthy. "Managing your stress well can
help you feel better physically and
psychologically and it can impact your ability to
perform your best."[3]
Classification of Approaches
The Lazarus model outlined
above represents a specific type of coping theory.
These theories may be classified according to two
independent parameters: (a) trait- oriented versus
state oriented, and (b) micro analytic versus macro
analytic approaches (cf. Krohne 1996). Trait
oriented and state-oriented research strategies
have different objectives: The trait-oriented (or
dispositional) strategy aims at early identification of
individuals whose coping resources and tendencies
are inadequate for the demands of a specific
stressful encounter. An early identification of these
persons will offer the opportunity for establishing a
selection (or placement) procedure or a successful
primary prevention program. Research that is state
oriented, i.e., which centers around actual coping,
has a more general objective. This research
investigates the relationships between coping
strategies employed by an individual and outcome
variables such as self-reported or objectively
registered coping efficiency, emotional reactions
accompanying and following certain coping efforts,
or variables of adaptational outcome (e.g., health
status or test performance). This research strategy
intends to lay the foundation for a general
modificatory program to improve coping efficacy.
Micro analytic approaches focus on a large number
of specific coping strategies, whereas macro
analytic analysis operates at a higher level of
abstraction, thus concentrating on more
fundamental constructs.
Macro analytic, Trait-
Oriented Coping Theories
Research on the
processes by which individuals cope with stressful
situations has grown substantially over the past
three decades (cf. Lazarus 1991, Zeidner and
Endler1996). Many trait-oriented approaches in this
field have established two constructs central to an
understanding of cognitive responses to stress:
vigilance, that is, the orientation toward stressful
aspects of an encounter, and cognitive avoidance ,
that is, averting attention from stress-related
information (cf. Janis 1983,Krohne 1978, 1993, Roth
and Cohen 1986). Approaches corresponding to
these conceptions are repression–sensitization
(Byrne 1964), monitoring-blunting (Miller 1980,
1987), or attention-rejection (Mullen and Suls
1982). With regard to the relationship between
these two constructs, Byrne's approach specifies a
unidimensional ,bipolar structure, while Miller as
well as Mullen and Suls leave this question open.
Krohne, however, explicitly postulates an
independent functioning of the dimensions
vigilance and cognitive avoidance.
Repression–sensitization.
The repression–sensitization
construct (cf. Byrne 1964, Eriksen 1966) relates
different forms of dispositional coping to one
bipolar dimension. When confronted with a
stressful encounter, persons located at one pole of
this dimension (repressers) tend to deny or
minimize the existence of stress, fail to verbalize
feelings of distress, and avoid thinking about
possible negative consequences of this encounter.
Persons at the opposite pole (sensitizers) react to
stress-related cues by way of enhanced information
search, rumination, and obsessive worrying. The
concept of repression–sensitization is theoretically
founded in research on perceptual defense (Bruner
and Postman 1947), an approach that combined
psychodynamic ideas with the functionalistic
behavior analysis of Brunswik (1947).
. Future Perspectives
Although the fields of stress and
coping research represent largely explored
territory, there are still fertile perspectives to be
pursued in future research. Among the promising
lines of research, two perspectives will be
mentioned here. 1. Compared to the simplistic
stimulus-response conception of stress inherent in
early approaches on stress, the `psychological' (i.e.,
cognitive transformation) approach of the Lazarus
group clearly represents progress. However, in
advocating a completely `subjective' orientation in
conceptualizing stress, Lazarus overstated the
`cognitive turn' in stress research. In stating that
`we might do better by describing relevant
environments and their psychological meanings
through the lenses of individuals' (Lazarus 1990, p.
8) he took a stand that is at variance with the
multivariate, systems- theory perspective proposed
in his recent publications on stress and emotions
(Lazarus 1990, 1991). First, the stress process
contains variables to be assessed both subjectively
and objectively, such as constraints, temporal
aspects, or social support networks, as well as
responses to be measured at different levels (cf.
Lazarus 1990, Table 1)