SECQ
SECQ
SECQ
net/publication/261872642
CITATIONS READS
105 21,955
2 authors:
Mingming Zhou J. Ee
University of Macau National Institute of Education (NIE), Singapore
59 PUBLICATIONS 2,776 CITATIONS 47 PUBLICATIONS 750 CITATIONS
Keywords: Social and Emotional Learning (SEL); Social and Emotional Competence
(SEC); assessment; Social and Emotional Competence Questionnaire
(SECQ)
First submission on 10th April, 2012; accepted for publication on 23rd September 2012.
In recent years, social emotional learning (SEL) has become an important element in basic
education that has drawn the attention of educators and researchers. SEL is a process whereby
children and adults acquire the knowledge and skills required to function effectively in various social
contexts. It is related to five core competencies, namely the acquisition of skills to recognise and
manage emotions, develop care and concern for others, make responsible decisions, establish positive
relationships, and handle challenging situations effectively (CASEL, 2003). The development of
social emotional competence (SEC) as an outcome of SEL is believed to enable learners to acquire
the ability to self-monitor their behaviors and self-regulate their learning (Wilson, Gottfredson, &
Najaka, 2001; Zins, Weissberg, Wang, & Walberg, 2004). Consequently, curriculum designers and
researchers are keen to devise intervention programs with the purpose of enhancing SEC. A recent
meta-analysis of over 200 SEL evaluations involving more than 270,000 students showed that, in
addition to a wide range of positive social and emotional benefits, the academic achievement of
students who participated in SEL programs improved significantly (Durlak et al., 2011). Students who
are more self-aware and confident about their learning abilities are likely to persevere and tend to
perform better, as they are likely to manage their stress and organize themselves better in their work.
Likewise, students who make responsible decisions about their learning are able to use their
relationship skills to communicate better to overcome their obstacles in problem-solving (Zins et al.,
2004). Hence, the capability of self-understanding and greater emotional management, coupled with
the ability to deal effectively with others and different situations, positively impact the capacity to
negotiate one’s both academic and personal lives.
1
Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]
ISSN 2073-7629
© 2012 EDRES/ENSEC Volume 4, Number 2, November 2012 pp 28
emotions are likely to be reflective and hence recognize their own state of being, and be clear about
the reasons for their emotional responses. Froming, Nasby, and McManus (1998) found that children
(especially boys) appeared to regulate their behavior using prosocial self-schemas when they were
self-aware. As Carver and Scheier (1981) argued, the state of self-awareness is necessary to engage
one into the self-regulating mechanism. If students can be nurtured to be metacognitively aware of
their emotions, they are likely to develop better self-control of their emotions and thus make more
responsible decision-making in life.
Self-management relates to the ability to manage one’s own impulses and emotions. Self-
regulation of one’s emotions is important for developing close relationships, succeeding at work and
maintaining physical health. Empirical data shows that children who can manage their emotional
experiences in an emotionally-arousing play situation are more successful in their peer relationships
(Hubbard and Coie, 1994). In contrast, children who routinely experience high intensity emotions
without constructive ways of managing such experiences, often engage in socially inappropriate
behaviors and are at risk for low peer status (Eisenberg et al., 1995). In school contexts, students who
cannot control their emotions are unlikely to think clearly and perform well (Weissberg & Elias,
1993).
Social awareness is the ability to read other persons’ cues and to understand, and
appropriately respond to their feelings (Frey, Hirschstein, & Guzzo, 2000). This is closely linked to
empathy, the capacity to share the emotional state of another person and thus relate better with them
(Eisenberg, 1986). Empathy is concerned with the ability to understand another person’s perspective
in interpreting thoughts and feelings, demonstrating an awareness of the sensitivity of complex issues,
and attempting to clarify ambiguities that leads to harmonious functioning between individuals.
Research has shown that empathetic children tend to show greater attentional focus, perceptual
sensitivity, and inhibitory control (Miller & Jansen op de Haar, 1997), and more altruistic (Ukegawa,
1996) and pro-social behaviors (Litvack-Miller, McDougall, & Romney, 1997).
Relationship management: several lines of research suggest that peers play an essential role in
children’s school engagement at school (e.g., Ladd, 1999). Studies show that children who are
rejected by their peers, who experience more loneliness and social isolation, and who affiliate with
more disaffected peers are themselves more likely to become disengaged from academic activities and
eventually leave school (Sage & Kindermann, 1999; Wentzel, 1999). On the other hand, Ryan and
colleagues (1994) found that seventh and eighth graders who felt more secure with their peers,
reported higher identity integration and general self-esteem. It should be noted, however, findings are
mixed with regards to peer relations’ direct effect on academic outcomes.
Responsible decision-making refers to the ability to consider ethical, safety, and societal
factors in making decisions, such that individuals can deal responsibly with daily academic and social
situations and contribute to the well-being of one’s school and community (CASEL, 2003).
Significant correlations were found between metacognition, decision-making style and performance
ISSN 2073-7629
© 2012 EDRES/ENSEC Volume 4, Number 2, November 2012 pp 29
on a decision-making task (Ormond et al., 1991). Responsible decision-making also requires training
in decision-making that emphasizes the building of a sense of social responsibility towards others
(CASEL, 2003).
ISSN 2073-7629
© 2012 EDRES/ENSEC Volume 4, Number 2, November 2012 pp 30
emotional development in students, especially those who have undergone any training. Lastly, we
expect this measure to be a useful tool for both researchers and practitioners in the field to gauge
children’s and adolescent’s social emotional status and assess the effectiveness of SEL programs that
aim to promote the five SEC components. The purpose of the present study was to develop such a
measure.
ISSN 2073-7629
© 2012 EDRES/ENSEC Volume 4, Number 2, November 2012 pp 31
school in Singapore. Study 1 was a confirmatory factor analysis with fourth-graders, to provide
evidence to the construct validity. Study 2 tested whether the best-fitting model for the primary
students’ sample could be tested with data from the secondary school sample. Study 3 examined
whether the reliability could be replicated with a different sample, and Study 4 sought to provide
evidence for the measure’s predictive validity by linking SEC measures to academic achievement
scores.
Results
The validity of a five-factor structure for primary school students was tested using
Confirmatory Factor Analysis. The analyses were conducted on covariance matrices, and the
solutions were generated on the basis of maximum-likelihood estimation. Specifically, the model
hypothesized a priori that: (a) responses to the SECQ could be explained by the five factors labeled
self-awareness, social awareness, self-management, relationship management and responsible
decision-making; (b) each item would have a nonzero loading on each factor that it was designed to
measure and zero or extremely low loadings on all other factors; and (c) the five factors were
correlated.
Multiple criteria were used in determining the goodness of fit to the data for this hypothesized
structure, including the chi-square degree of freedom ratio (χ2/df), the comparative fit index (CFI), the
incremental fit index (IFI), and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). χ2/df less than
3.0 indicates a good model fit; CFI and IFI values near 1.0 are optimal, with values greater than .90
indicating acceptable model fit (Kline, 2005). Meanwhile, the RMSEA values less than .05 indicate
good fit, with values as high as .08 representing reasonable errors of approximation in the population
(Browne & Cudeck, 1993; Byrne, 2008).
ISSN 2073-7629
© 2012 EDRES/ENSEC Volume 4, Number 2, November 2012 pp 32
factor and most were acceptable (see Figure 1, with all loadings being above .45 except for 2 items: “I
know when I am moody” [SA4]; and “I try not to criticise my friend when we quarrel” [RM3]). All of
the subscales demonstrated acceptable levels of internal consistency. For self-awareness, social
awareness, self-management, relationship management and responsible decision-making, the
Cronbach’s alpha were .62, .72, .68, .62 and .72, respectively.
ISSN 2073-7629
© 2012 EDRES/ENSEC Volume 4, Number 2, November 2012 pp 33
been suggested that a decreased χ2/df ratio denotes an improved model fit (Hoelter, 1983). However,
the χ2 statistic is known to be sensitive to multivariate non-normality and sample size, which may
cause uncertainty concerning the overall appropriateness of a study’s model based on this particular
measure (Cheung & Rensvold, 2002; Hu & Bentler, 1995). Thus this fit index was reported, but not
given much weight in terms of the final model selection decision, especially when the χ2/df values in
the three models all showed acceptable model fit. Further, none of the CFI and IFI exceeded .90, yet
only the original model showed a highly close value in these two indices, indicating a reasonable
model fit (Kline, 1998; Schumacker & Lomax, 1996). Finally, ECVI was used as a single sample
estimate of cross-validation to assess how well the three models could be generalized to other
samples. “The model with the smallest ECVI indicates the model with the best fit.” (Hoekstra et al.,
2008, p.1558). For the above reasons, the original model was deemed the best-fitting model (see
Table 1).
Note. CFI = comparative fit index; IFI= incremental fit index; RMSEA = root-mean-square error
of approximation; ECVI = expected cross-validation index.
Results
The best-fitting model for the primary school sample was tested with data from the secondary
school sample. The CFA results again supported the hypothesized model, as not only were all factors
loaded on their respective latent factors with acceptable loadings (see Figure 2, all loadings above .50
except for 3 items with loadings above .45: “I know when I am moody.” [SA4]; “I can read people’s
faces when they are angry.” [SA5]; and “I will always apologise when I hurt my friend
unintentionally”. [RM1]), but also most fit statistics nearly met the criteria for an acceptable fitting
ISSN 2073-7629
© 2012 EDRES/ENSEC Volume 4, Number 2, November 2012 pp 34
model: χ2= 712.20 (df = 265, p < .001), χ2/df = 2.69, RMSEA = .069, CFI = .86, IFI = .86. It should
be noted that although the CFI and IFI indices fell into the “marginal” ranges (above .85 but below
.90), “it is especially important to consider the consistency of model fit expressed by the various types
of fit indices in tandem with the particular aspects of the analytic situation” (Brown, 2006, p. 87). In
addition, all of the subscales demonstrated good levels of internal consistency with Cronbach Alpha
being .71, .78, .76, .73 and .79, for self-awareness, social awareness, self-management, relationship
management and responsible decision-making, respectively.
ISSN 2073-7629
© 2012 EDRES/ENSEC Volume 4, Number 2, November 2012 pp 35
secondary school in Singapore to respond to the scale in English with teachers’ instructions given as
before. The crosscheck of internal consistency revealed Cronbach’s alphas of .72, .77, 73, .71, and .76
for self-awareness, social awareness, relationship management, self-management, and responsible
decision-making, respectively.
Table 2 Mean, SDs, and zero-order correlations among SEC variables and achievement in Study 4
(N = 132)
Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5
1. Self-awareness 4.74 1.05 -
2. Self-Management 4.05 1.18 .60** -
3. Social Awareness 4.17 1.17 .65** .60** -
4. Relationship Management 4.27 1.07 .49** .50** .44** -
5. Responsible Decision Making 4.25 1.06 .65** .62** .65** .52** -
6. English Score 56.48 20.37 .19* .16 .19* .27** .26**
7. Math Score 62.75 22.62 .24** .16 .20* .27** .30**
8. Science Score 68.84 19.69 .27** .20* .22* .26** .28**
ISSN 2073-7629
© 2012 EDRES/ENSEC Volume 4, Number 2, November 2012 pp 36
known SEL model developed by CASEL (2008). We used both primary and secondary school
samples to provide evidence for an adequate model fit for the scale. CFAs results generally showed an
acceptable model fit for both samples. However, the factor loadings of each item to the five factors in
the model were not great, especially in the primary school sample (Study1, with a majority of
loadings between .55 and .60), compared to the secondary school sample (Study 2, with a majority of
loadings between .60 and .70). Similar findings were also observed in the internal consistency
coefficients. Cronbach’s alpha estimates used for scale development purposes should be .70 or higher
(Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994). The measures with the two secondary school samples (Study 2 and 3)
met the cut-off score requirement, suggesting homogeneity of items for the respective measures
(Henson, 2001). However, with the primary school sample (Study 1), three subscales (self-awareness,
self-management and relationship management) fell below the cutoff score. We argue that the low
reliability in these subscales might not be attributable to item generation and scale construction
problems; rather, they could reveal that children could have some difficulties in answering questions
related to the degree of how they deal with emotional and academic situations by differentiating self
from others. This speculation could be confirmed by the more mature sample (secondary school
students).
The data also provides some evidence for the predictive validity of the scale. School
children’s scores on different SEC domains were positively related to their performance in school
subjects, although they did not appear to be very strong. Further regression analyses showed that
relationship management appeared to be a stronger predictor of achievement than the other SEC
constructs for girls. This observation is more hopeful than those studies which found no significant
correlations (e.g., Newsome, Day, & Catano, 2000) and also raises a number of interesting questions
for future research: Why did relationship management predict academic achievement? Why was the
prediction only observed in girls? It is possible that girls who are better able to manage their
relationships with peers/teachers are more effective at developing harmonious relationships with peers
and enjoying their school experience in general, which helps them achieve higher grades? Clearly,
future research needs to sharpen our understanding of the relationship between one’s social-emotional
level and academic success using psychometrically sound instruments.
Caution should be used, however, in drawing these conclusions from our studies. Firstly,
there are alternative operationalization of SEC besides the one provided by CASEL (2008).
Constructs with the same name could be conceptualized differently in different models. We believe,
however, that SECQ could be a useful addition to the spectrum of measures available to assess
children’s social emotional behaviour in the school years. Secondly, we assessed SEC with a limited
set of self-report items from students, instead of using more objective measures from parents, peers,
or teachers. Alternative assessment techniques such as interview, observation or behavioral measures,
might reveal other specific factors, to substantiate the results of this study. Multiple-instrument,
multiple-source, multiple-construct and multiple-context assessment would contribute to the
ISSN 2073-7629
© 2012 EDRES/ENSEC Volume 4, Number 2, November 2012 pp 37
reliability of judgments about children’s and adolescents’ social emotional functioning (Bracken,
Keith, & Walker, 1998). Lastly, since the measure was only validated with primary and secondary
school students in Singapore, it will be necessary to replicate these findings in a more heterogeneous
sample of students in other cultures. For example, it will be important to know whether the 5-factor
model can be replicated and whether SEC correlates with academic achievement with students with
different ethnic and socioeconomic backgrounds, such as western samples which have quite different
cultural blends.
In sum, the findings indicate that the scale holds promise as a reliable, valid measure of SEC
as conceptualized by CASEL (2008). Potential use of the scale in theoretical research entails
exploring the nature of SEC, the determinants of SEC, the effects of SEC on other variables, and the
development of SEC over time. Despite the limitations outlined, the instrument would seem to have
value in assessing individuals who need a valid appraisal of their SEC, such that they can obtain a
better understanding of self or others, for identifying problems in areas related to social emotional
development, to collect baseline data for future assessment of progress and later interventions, or to
seek possible indicators of poor academic performance. Assessing students with regards to these
aspects will help identify their social-emotional strengths and weaknesses, which will facilitate the
delivery of appropriate programming or curricula to improve their social-emotional competence
(Coryn et al., 2009). By strengthening social-emotional educational opportunities, we will increase
children's and adolescents’ capacity to learn, give them the tools to make personal and learning
achievements, and enable them to experience personal satisfaction.
References
Asendorpf, J. B., & Baudonnière, P. (1993). Self-awareness and other-awareness: Mirror self-
recognition and synchronic imitation among unfamiliar peers. Developmental Psychology, 29,
88-95.
Bar-On, R. (1997). Development of the Bar-On EQ-I: A measure of emotional and social intelligence.
Paper presented at the 105th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association in
Chicago.
Beland, K. (2007). Boosting social and emotional competence. Educational Leadership, 64, 68-71.
Bracken, B. A., Keith, L. K., & Walker, K. C. (1998). Assessment of preschool behavior and social–
emotional functioning: A review of thirteen third-party instruments. Journal of
Psychoeducational Assessment, 16, 153–169.
Brown, T. A. (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. NY: Guilford.
Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen & J.
S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136–162). Beverley Hills, CA: Sage.
Byrne, B. M. (1994). Structural equation modeling with EQS and EQS/Windows. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Publications.
ISSN 2073-7629
© 2012 EDRES/ENSEC Volume 4, Number 2, November 2012 pp 38
Byrne, B. M. (2008). Testing for multigroup equivalence of a measuring instrument: A walk through the
process. Psicothema, 20, 872-882.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self-regulation: A control-theory approach to human
behavior. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing
measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 9, 233-255.
Collaborative for Academic, Social and Emotional Learning [CASEL]. (2003). Safe and sound: An
educational leader’s guide to evidence-based social and emotional learning (SEL) programs.
Chicago, Illinois: Collaborative for Academic, Social, and Emotional Learning.
Collaborative for Academic, Social, and Emotional Learning [CASEL]. (2008). SEL assessment,
tools, needs and outcome assessments. Chicago, Illinois: Collaborative for Academic, Social,
and Emotional Learning.
Coryn, C., Spybrook, J., Evergreen, S., & Blinkiewicz, M. (2009). Development and evaluation of the
social-emotional learning scale. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 27, 283-295.
Durlak, J. A., Weissberg, R. P., Dymnicki, A. B., Taylor, R. D., & Schellinger, K. B., (2011). The
impact of enhancing students’ social and emotional learning: A meta-analysis of school-based
universal interventions. Child Development, 82, 405-432.
Eisenberg, N. (1986). Altruistic emotion, cognition, and behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Murphy, M., Maszk, P., Smith, M., & Karbon, M. (1995). The role of emotionality
and regulation in children's social functioning: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 66, 1239-1261.
Elias, M. J., Zins, J. E., Weissberg, R. P., Frey, K. S., Greenberg, M. T., Haynes, N. M., Kessler, R., Schwab-
Stone, M. E., & Shriver, T. P. (1997). Promoting social and emotional learning: Guidelines for educators.
Alexandria, VA: Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development.
Frey, K. S., Hirschstein, M. K., & Guzzo, B. A. (2000). Second step: Preventing aggression by promoting social
competence. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 8, 102–112.
Froming, W. J., Nasby, W., McManus, J. (1998). Prosocial self-schemas, self-awareness, and children's
prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 766-777.
Fu, H. Y. G. (2008). The Asian Child: Resiliency in the Face of Evolving Challenges. Speech at the
Opening Ceremony of the 5th Congress of the Asian Society for Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry and Allied Professions (ASCAPAP). Retrieved on March 28, 2012 from
http://www.moe.gov.sg/media/speeches/2008/08/29/speech-by-ms-grace-fu-at-the-o-3.php
Henson, R. K. (2001). Understanding internal consistency reliability estimates: A conceptual primer
on coefficient alpha. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 34, 177-
189.
Hoekstra, R. A., Bartels, M., Cath, D. C., & Boomsma, D. I. (2008). Factor structure, reliability and
criterion validity of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): A study in Dutch population and
patient groups. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1555-1566.
ISSN 2073-7629
© 2012 EDRES/ENSEC Volume 4, Number 2, November 2012 pp 39
Hoelter, J. W. (1983). The analysis of covariance structures: Goodness of fit indices. Sociological
Methods and Research, 11, 325-344.
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1995). Evaluating model fit. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation
modeling: Concepts, issues, and applications (pp. 76-99). London, England: Sage.
Hubbard, J. A., & Coie, J. D. (1994). Emotional correlates of social competence in children’s peer-relationships.
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40, 1–20.
Humphrey, N., Kalambouka, A., Lendrum, A., & Wigelsworth, M. (2009). A systematic review of social and
emotional skills measures for children and young people, DCSF Research Report 2008/056B. Nottingham,
England: DCSF Publications.
Humphrey, N., Kalambouka, A., Wigelsworth, M., Lendrum, A., Deighton, J., & Wolpert, M. (2011). Measures
of social and emotional skills for children and young people: A systematic review. Educational and
Psychological Measurement, 71, 617-637.
Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York: Guilford
Press.
Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York:
Guilford.
Ladd, G. W. (1999). Peer relationships and social competence during early and middle childhood.
Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 333-359.
Litvack-Miller, W., McDougall, D., & Romney, D. M. (1997). The structure of empathy during
middle childhood and its relationship to prosocial behavior. Genetic, Social, and General
Psychology Monographs, 123, 303–324.
Miller, P., & Jansen Op de Haar, M. (1997). Emotional, cognitive, behavioral, and temperament characteristics
of high empathy children. Motivation and Emotion, 21, 109-125.
Newsome, S., Day, A. L., & Catano, V. M. (2000). Assessing the predictive validity of emotional
intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 1005-1016.
Nunnally, J. C., & Bernstein, I. H. (1994). Psychometric theory (3rd ed.). NY: McGraw-Hill.
Ormond, C., Luszcz, M. A., Mann, L., & Beswick, G. (1991). A metacognitive analysis of decision
making in adolescence. Journal of Adolescence, 14, 275-291.
Park, J., Haddon, A., & Goodman, H. (2003). The emotional literacy handbook: Processes, practices
and resources to promote emotional literacy. London: David Fulton.
Ryan, R. M., Stiller, J. D., & Lynch, J. H. (1994). Representations of relationships to teachers, parents, and
friends as predictors of academic motivation and self-esteem. Journal of Early Adolescence, 14, 226 –249.
Sage, N. A., & Kindermann, T. A. (1999). Peer networks, behavior contingencies, and children’s engagement in
the classroom. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45, 143–171.
Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition and Personality,
9, 185-211.
Schumacker, R. E., & Lomax, R. G. (1996). A beginner’s guide to structural equation modeling.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
ISSN 2073-7629
© 2012 EDRES/ENSEC Volume 4, Number 2, November 2012 pp 40
Strayer, J., & Schroeder, M. (1989). Children’s helping strategies: Influences of emotion, empathy and age. In
N. Eisenberg (Ed.), New directions for child development, No. 44: Empathy and related emotional
responses (pp.85–105). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Ukegawa, S. (1996). Empathy and altruistic behavior among preschoolers: An attempt to measure empathy of
children based on teacher's report. Research and Clinical Center for Child Development Annual Report,
18, 53-61.
Weissberg, R. P., & Elias, M. J. (1993). Enhancing young people's social competence and health behavior: An
important challenge for educators, scientists, policymakers, and funders. Applied and Preventive
Psychology, 2, 179-190.
Wentzel, K. R. (1999). Social-motivational processes and interpersonal relationships: Implications for
understanding motivation at school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 76–97.
Wilson, D. B., Gottfredson, D. C., Najaka, S. S. (2001). School-based prevention of problem
behaviors: A meta-analysis. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 17, 247–272.
Zins, J. E., & Elias, M. J. (2006). Social and emotional learning. In G. G. Bear & K. M. Minke (Eds.),
Children’s needs III (pp. 1–13). Bethesda, MD: National Association of School Psychologists.
Zins, J. E., Weissberg, R. P., Wang, M. C., & Walberg, H. J. (2004). Building academic success on
social and emotional learning: What does the research say? NY: Teachers College Press.
ISSN 2073-7629
© 2012 EDRES/ENSEC Volume 4, Number 2, November 2012 pp 41
Appendix: The Social Emotional Competence Questionnaire (SECQ)
Construct Items
Self-awareness [SA1] I know what I am thinking and doing.
[SA2] I understand why I do what I do.
[SA3] I understand my moods and feelings.
[SA4] I know when I am moody.
[SA5] I can read people’s faces when they are angry.
Social [SoA1] I recognise how people feel by looking at their facial expressions.
Awareness
[SoA2] It is easy for me to understand why people feel the way they do.
[SoA3] If someone is sad, angry or happy, I believe I know what they are thinking.
[SoA4] I understand why people react the way they do.
[SoA5] If a friend is upset, I have a pretty good idea why.
Self- [SM1] I can stay calm in stressful situations.
management
[SM2] I stay calm and overcome anxiety in new or changing situations.
[SM3] I stay calm when things go wrong.
[SM4] I can control the way I feel when something bad happens.
[SM5] When I am upset with someone, I will wait till I have calmed down before
discussing the issue.
Relationship [RM1] I will always apologise when I hurt my friend unintentionally.
Management
[RM2] I always try and comfort my friends when they are sad.
[RM3] I try not to criticise my friend when we quarrel.
[RM4] I am tolerant of my friend’s mistakes.
[RM5] I stand up for myself without putting others down.
Responsible [RDM1] When making decisions, I take into account the consequences of my actions.
Decision-Making
[RDM2] I ensure that there are more positive outcomes when making a choice.
[RDM3] I weigh the strengths of the situation before deciding on my action.
[RDM4] I consider the criteria chosen before making a recommendation.
[RDM5] I consider the strengths and weaknesses of the strategy before deciding to use it.
ISSN 2073-7629
© 2012 EDRES/ENSEC Volume 4, Number 2, November 2012 pp 42