The Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology and Romantic Relationships Justin K Mogilski Editor Ebook Full Chapter
The Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology and Romantic Relationships Justin K Mogilski Editor Ebook Full Chapter
The Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology and Romantic Relationships Justin K Mogilski Editor Ebook Full Chapter
DOI: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780197524718.001.0001
9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
Printed by Marquis, Canada
A B O U T T H E E D I TO R S
Contributors xi
Introduction 1
Justin K. Mogilski and Todd K. Shackelford
viii Ta b l e of C on ten ts
25. Affective reactions to divorce or spousal death 675
Jeannette Brodbeck and Hans Joerg Znoj
26. Affective self-regulation after relationship dissolution 698
Leah E. LeFebvre and Ryan D. Rasner
27. Post-relationship romance 730
Michael R. Langlais and He Xiao
28. Evolutionary perspectives on post-separation parenting 748
Lawrence J. Moloney and Bruce M. Smyth
29. Dissolution of LGBTQ+relationships 779
Madeleine Redlick Holland and Pamela J. Lannutti
30. Relationship dissolution among adults 797
Dimitri Mortelmans
Limitations and future directions in the evolutionary study
of romantic relationships 826
Justin K. Mogilski and Todd K. Shackelford
Index 831
xii C o n tr ibutor s
Norman P. Li Ryan D. Rasner
School of Social Sciences, Singapore Department of Communication
Management University Studies, Louisiana State University
Severi Luoto S. Craig Roberts
School of Population Health, Division of Psychology, University of
University of Auckland Stirling
Anastasia Makhanova David L. Rodrigues
Psychological Science, University of Centre for Social Research and
Arkansas Intervention, Iscte-Instituto
Evita March Universitário de Lisboa
Department of Psychology, Joshua Everett Ryan
Federation University Australia Department of Psychology University
Emma M. Marshall of Maryland, College Park
School of Psychology Deakin Simona Sciara
University UniSR-Social.Lab, Faculty of Psychology,
Karlijn Massar Vita-Salute San Raffaele University
Work and Social Psychology, Bruce M. Smyth
Maastricht University Centre for Social Research and
Laureon A. Merrie Methods, Australian National University
Department of Psychology, Valerie G. Starratt
Oklahoma Center for Evolutionary Department of Psychology and
Analysis (OCEAN), Oklahoma State Neuroscience, Nova Southeastern
University University
Justin K. Mogilski Ian D. Stephen
Department of Psychology, NTU Psychology, Nottingham
University of South Carolina Trent University
Salkehatchie Jaroslava Varella Valentova
Lawrence J. Moloney Department of Experimental
School of Psychology and Public Psychology, University of Sao Paulo
Health, La Trobe University Marco Antonio Correa Varella
James B. Moran Department of Experimental
Department of Psychology, Tulane Psychology, University of São Paulo
University T. Joel Wade
Dimitri Mortelmans Department of Psychology,
Department of Sociology, Antwerp Psychological Adaptations Research
University Consortium, Bucknell University
Giuseppe Pantaleo Elizabeth M. Westrupp
UniSR-Social.Lab, Faculty of School of Psychology, The Centre
Psychology, Vita-Salute San Raffaele for Social and Early Emotional
University Development, Deakin University
Contributors xiii
He Xiao Hans Joerg Znoj
Department of Educational Department of Psychology,
Psychology, University of University of Bern
North Texas
xiv C o n tr ibutor s
Introduction
Abstract
Evolutionary social science is having a renaissance. What started with a collection of
estimates by Charles Darwin and colleagues about biological change and diversity has
manifested a theoretical powerhouse incorporating varied disciplinary perspectives
and methodologies. In this chapter, we introduce these advancements and provide an
overview of how evolutionary perspectives have enriched the scientific study of intimate
relationship initiation, maintenance, and dissolution. We highlight several areas of
research that feature prominently throughout this handbook, including studies of gender
and sexual diversity, social neuroendocrinology, personality science, and international
trends in human mating. We conclude with a brief editorial note to current and future
relationship scientists, to whom this volume is dedicated.
Psychologists who adopt the evolutionary paradigm (see Nettle & Scott-Phillips, 2021)
seek to document how information processing of the mind has been engineered to address
the unremitting challenges of survival and reproduction. Its predictive framework has
been broadly adopted within the social sciences, including anthropology (Fessler et al.,
2015; Gibson & Lawson, 2015), sociology (Tanskanen & Danielsbacka, 2018), con-
sumer research (Otterbring, 2021; Saad, 2017), decision science (Morris et al., 2021),
animal behavior and cognition (Vonk, 2021), political science (McDermott & Hatemi,
2018; Petersen, 2020), and law and social policy (James et al., 2020; Palomo-Vélez &
van Vugt, 2021). Natural selection of genes is a popular level of analysis at which to pro-
pose and test evolutionary hypotheses because DNA is the fundamental unit of inheri-
tance in sexually reproducing organisms (see Williams, 1966). Biological evolution occurs
when populations undergo cross-generational change in heritable trait frequency. These
traits are pitted against the reproductive and survival demands of life, and those which
better promote self-replication compared to competing alternatives become more preva-
lent. However, evolutionary change occurs in any cyclic system where modification by
competitive replacement occurs over time, such as in neural network modeling (Badcock
et al., 2019; Hasson et al., 2020; Stanley et al., 2019), cellular growth (Aktipis, 2020),
decision-making (Morris et al., 2021), or multilevel selection (Hertler et al., 2020; Wilson
& Coan, 2021). Each application shares the common premise that if you study how evo-
lution has engineered a system, you will discover that system’s functional design.
The evolutionary study of romantic relationships has accordingly uncovered the adapted
psychology underlying intimate relationships (Bode & Kushnick, 2021; Buss & Schmitt,
2019; Durante et al., 2016). For example, it is well documented that reproduction entails
unique adaptive challenges for men and women (e.g., paternal uncertainty and minimum
investment in gestation), which create unique mating optima for each sex (Trivers, 1972;
see Mogilski et al., 2021). These optima can conflict (see Kennair et al., this volume)
causing sexual selection for different ideal mating strategies for men and women (Buss &
Schmitt, 1993; Luoto 2019; Puts, 2016). Understanding how attraction and competition
occurs between the sexes thus becomes a useful framework for predicting how people
initiate, maintain, and dissolve their relationships. That is, the collaboration and conflict
that people experience within their relationship(s) may follow computationally adaptive
scripts that—at least across deep evolutionary time—alleviated the exigencies of men’s
and women’s unique reproductive challenges. These foundations and their empirical sup-
port are reviewed in Chapter 1 and referenced throughout this volume.
Of course, good theory reliably and expansively predicts the phenomena that it
explains. Relationship researchers who use the evolutionary paradigm have rapidly inte-
grated it with gender and women’s studies (Fisher et al., 2020; van Anders, 2013), sexual-
ity (Diamond, 2021; Sommer & Vasey, 2006), marriage and family studies (Aspara et al.,
2018), neuroendocrinology (Welling & Shackelford, 2019), mating cognition (Joel &
MacDonald, 2021; Lenton & Stewart, 2008; Miller & Todd, 1998), intelligence (Baur
et al., 2019; Miller, 2000), and comparative psychology (Fraley et al., 2005). This well-
spring of novelty has matured to create robust, replicable models of mate choice (Conroy-
Beam et al., 2019, 2021; Walter et al., 2020), same-sex competition (see Krems et al.,
in this volume; also Ayers, 2021; Bradshaw & DelPriore, 2022; Reynolds et al., 2018),
friendship (Seyfarth & Cheney, 2012; Williams et al., 2022), jealousy (Buss, 2018;
Edlund et al., 2018), face and body perception (Antar & Stephen, 2021; Brown et al.,
2021; Fink et al., 2018), and interpersonal deception (Desrochers et al., 2021; Redlick
& Vangelisti, 2018; Trivers, 1991). Research once dominated by self-report and forced-
choice paradigms has developed multivariate solutions for describing the logic of partner
choice (Brandner et al., 2020; Csajbók and Berkics, 2022 Li et al., 2002; Mogilski et al.
2014, 2017, 2018; Jones, 2018; Stephen et al., 2017), relationship maintenance (Vowels
et al., 2021). Behavioral genetics (e.g., twin studies) have disentangled the contributions
of genes and environment to variation in human development (Kupfer et al., 2022), and
applied sciences, such as medicine and mental health (Nesse, 2019; Giosan et al., 2020;
Hollon et al., 2021) have advanced how knowledge of evolutionary design can improve
personal and relational outcomes.
This diaspora has developed alongside larger trends within relationship science, such
as the rising utility and rigor of personality measurement (Del Giudice, 2017; Durkee
Int roduction 3
Studies of sexually and gender diverse people (e.g., those who are lesbian, gay, bisexual,
transgender, queer, etc.; i.e., LGBTQ+) have revealed unique variation in human bonding
and competition (see Holland & Lannuti, this volume; Pachankis et al., 2020; Semenyna
et al., 2021; Valentova et al., this volume). For example, Diamond and Alley (this
volume) argue that safety concerns are salient among LGBTQ+relationships because
same-sex attraction and gender nonconformity are targeted more often for condemnation
and violence. Eliminating prejudice and wrongful discrimination against LGBTQ indi-
viduals (Blair & Hoskin, 2019) may be aided by a technical knowledge of which adaptive
concerns these beliefs and attitudes have historically addressed—and whether they still
do. Computation that ancestrally enhanced reproduction may be mismatched to modern
circumstances (Li et al., 2020). Similarly, studying how people form and maintain mul-
tiple, concurrent intimate relationships (i.e., consensual nonmonogamy) (Mogilski et al.,
this volume) may reveal novel strategies for managing extra-pair romance (also see Brady
& Baker, 2022; Hunter & Stockwell, 2022).
By harnessing the insights of interdisciplinary collaboration, evolutionary relationship
scientists have identified novel features of human mating, have expanded durable theories
and perspectives of human development, and strengthened the methodological robustness
of its core predictions. The editors assembled this handbook to showcase the empirical
and theoretical progress of the evolutionary study of intimate relationships. We dedicate
this volume to future generations of relationship scientists. It is our intent that this collec-
tion will be a primer for those seeking to incorporate contemporary evolutionary reason-
ing and methodology into their research program. Many of its contributors self-identify
as evolutionary psychologists. Others do not but are familiar with the evolutionary sci-
ences and have successfully incorporated its reasoning into their work. All have challenged
orthodoxy to improve how evolutionary psychology studies intimacy. The authors’ words
are their own, but the editors offered feedback for improving the interdisciplinary scope
of their writing. Our reflections on each chapter precede each of the three major sections
of this handbook: relationship initiation, maintenance, and dissolution.
We hope that readers of this volume walk away feeling that their views on intimacy and
interpersonal relationships have been enriched.
References
Aktipis, A. (2020). The cheating cell. Princeton University Press.
Antar, J. C., & Stephen, I. D. (2021). Facial shape provides a valid cue to sociosexuality in men but not
women. Evolution and Human Behavior, 42(4), 361–370.
Aspara, J., Wittkowski, K., & Luo, X. (2018). Types of intelligence predict likelihood to get married and
stay married: Large-scale empirical evidence for evolutionary theory. Personality and Individual Differences,
122, 1–6.
Ayers, J. D. (2021). Competitive scenarios increase competition in women: A meta-analysis. Evolutionary
Behavioral Sciences. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/ebs0000278
Badcock, P. B., Friston, K. J., Ramstead, M. J., Ploeger, A., & Hohwy, J. (2019). The hierarchically mechanistic
mind: an evolutionary systems theory of the human brain, cognition, and behavior. Cognitive, Affective, &
Behavioral Neuroscience, 19(6), 1319–1351.
Int roduction 5
Edlund, J. E., Heider, J. D., Nichols, A. L., McCarthy, R. J., Wood, S. E., Scherer, C. R., Hartnett, J. L., &
Walker, R. (2018). Sex differences in jealousy: The (lack of ) influence of researcher theoretical perspective.
The Journal of Social Psychology, 158(5), 515–520.
Fenneman, J., & Frankenhuis, W. E. (2020). Is impulsive behavior adaptive in harsh and unpredictable envi-
ronments? A formal model. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(4), 261–273.
Fennis, B. M., Gineikiene, J., Barauskaite, D., & van Koningsbruggen, G. M. (2022). Acute stress can boost
and buffer hedonic consumption: The role of individual differences in consumer life history strategies.
Personality and Individual Differences, 185, Article 111261.
Fessler, D. M. T., Clark, J. A., & Clint, E. K. (2015). Evolutionary psychology and evolutionary anthropology.
In D. M. Buss (Ed.), the handbook of evolutionary psychology, volume 2: integrations (pp. 1029–1046). John
Wiley & Sons.
Fink, B., Liebner, K., Müller, A. K., Hirn, T., McKelvey, G., & Lankhof, J. (2018). Hair colour and skin
colour together influence perceptions of age, health and attractiveness in lightly pigmented young women.
International Journal of Cosmetic Science, 40(3), 303–312.
Fisher, M. L., Garcia, J. R., & Burch, R. L. (2020). Evolutionary psychology: Thoughts on integrating feminist
perspectives. In L. Workman, W. Reader, & J. H. Barkow (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of evolution-
ary perspectives on human behavior (pp. 378–391). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/
9781108131797.032
Fraley, R. C., Brumbaugh, C. C., & Marks, M. J. (2005). The evolution and function of adult attachment: a
comparative and phylogenetic analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(5), 731.
Frankenhuis, W. E., Panchanathan, K., & Nettle, D. (2016). Cognition in harsh and unpredictable environ-
ments. Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 76–80.
Gibson, M. A., & Lawson, D. W. (2015). Applying evolutionary anthropology. Evolutionary Anthropology:
Issues, News, and Reviews, 24(1), 3–14.
Giosan, C., Cobeanu, O., Wyka, K., Muresan, V., Mogoase, C., Szentagotai, A., Malta, L. S., & Moldovan, R.
(2020). Cognitive evolutionary therapy versus standard cognitive therapy for depression: A single-blinded
randomized clinical trial. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 76(10), 1818–1831.
Gurvich, C., Hoy, K., Thomas, N., & Kulkarni, J. (2018). Sex differences and the influence of sex hormones
on cognition through adulthood and the aging process. Brain Sciences, 8(9), 163.
Hasson, U., Nastase, S. A., & Goldstein, A. (2020). Direct fit to nature: An evolutionary perspective on bio-
logical and artificial neural networks. Neuron, 105(3), 416–434.
Henrich, J., & Muthukrishna, M. (2021). The origins and psychology of human cooperation. Annual Review
of Psychology, 72, 207–240.
Hertler, S. C., Figueredo, A. J., & Peñaherrera-Aguirre, M. (2020). Multilevel selection: theoretical foundations,
historical examples, and empirical evidence. Springer Nature.
Hill, S. (2019). This is your brain on birth control: The surprising science of women, hormones, and the law of
unintended consequences. Penguin.
Hollon, S. D., Andrews, P. W., & Thomson Jr, J. A. (2021). Cognitive behavior therapy for depression from an
evolutionary perspective. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, Article 667592.
Hunter, G., & Stockwell, A. (2022). Toward a behavior-analytic understanding of jealousy and compersion in
romantic and sexual relationships. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 23(1), 78–108.
James, L., Todak, N., & Savage, J. (2020). Unnecessary force by police: Insights from evolutionary psychology.
Policing: A Journal of Policy and Practice, 14(1), 278–291.
Joel, S., & MacDonald, G. (2021). We’re not that choosy: Emerging evidence of a progression bias in romantic
relationships. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 25, 317–343.
Jonason, P. K., Zeigler-Hill, V., & Baldacchino, J. (2017). Before and after: Personality pathology, childhood
conditions, and life history outcomes. Personality and Individual Differences, 116, 38–43.
Jones, A. L. (2018). The influence of shape and colour cue classes on facial health perception. Evolution and
Human Behavior, 39(1), 19–29.
Jones, A. L., & Jaeger, B. (2019). Biological bases of beauty revisited: The effect of symmetry, averageness, and
sexual dimorphism on female facial attractiveness. Symmetry, 11(2), 279.
Kline, M. A., Shamsudheen, R., & Broesch, T. (2018). Variation is the universal: Making cultural evolu-
tion work in developmental psychology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,
373(1743), 20170059.
Int roduction 7
Pan, X., Gelfand, M., & Nau, D. (2021). Integrating evolutionary game theory and cross-cultural psychology
to understand cultural dynamics. American Psychologist, 76(6), 1054.
Pelham, B. W. (2021). The husband-older age gap in marriage is associated with selective fitness. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 121(3), 601.
Petersen, M. B. (2020). The evolutionary psychology of mass mobilization: How disinformation and dema-
gogues coordinate rather than manipulate. Current Opinion in Psychology, 35, 71–75.
Puts, D. (2016). Human sexual selection. Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 28–32.
Redlick, M. H., & Vangelisti, A. L. (2018). Affection, deception, and evolution: Deceptive affectionate mes-
sages as mate retention behaviors. Evolutionary Psychology, 16(1), Article 1474704917753857.
Rehbein, E., Hornung, J., Poromaa, I. S., & Derntl, B. (2021). Shaping of the female human brain by sex
hormones: A review. Neuroendocrinology, 111(3), 183–206.
Reynolds, T., Baumeister, R. F., & Maner, J. K. (2018). Competitive reputation manipulation: Women strategi-
cally transmit social information about romantic rivals. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 78, 195–209.
Roney, J. R., & Simmons, Z. L. (2018). Ovarian hormone fluctuations predict within-cycle shifts in women’s
food intake. Hormones and Behavior, 90, 8–14.
Saad, G. (2017). On the method of evolutionary psychology and its applicability to consumer research. Journal
of Marketing Research, 54(3), 464–477.
Schulz, J. F., Bahrami-Rad, D., Beauchamp, J. P., & Henrich, J. (2019). The church, intensive kinship, and
global psychological variation. Science, 366(6466), Article eaau5141.
Segall, M. H., Dasen, P. R., Berry, J. W., & Poortinga, Y. H. (1990). Human behavior in global perspective: An
introduction to cross-cultural psychology. Pergamon Press.
Semenyna, S. W., Gómez Jiménez, F. R., & Vasey, P. L. (2021). Women’s reaction to opposite-and same-sex
infidelity in three cultures. Human Nature, 32(2), 450–469.
Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2012). The evolutionary origins of friendship. Annual Review of Psychology,
63(1), 153–177.
Shirazi, T. N., Self, H., Dawood, K., Welling, L. L., Cárdenas, R., Rosenfield, K. A., Bailey, J. M.,
Balasubramanian, R., Delaney, A., Breedlove, S. M., & Puts, D. A. (2021). Evidence that perinatal ovarian
hormones promote women’s sexual attraction to men. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 134, 105431.
Silan, M., Adetula, A., Basnight-Brown, D. M., Forscher, P. S., Dutra, N., & IJzerman, H. (2021). Psychological
science needs the entire globe, part 2. APS Observer, 34(6).
Simpson, J. A., Griskevicius, V., Szepsenwol, O., & Young, E. (2017). An evolutionary life history perspective on
personality and mating strategies. Praeger/ABC-CLIO.
Sommer, V., & Vasey, P. L. (Eds.). (2006). Homosexual behaviour in animals: An evolutionary perspective.
Cambridge University Press.
Stanley, K. O., Clune, J., Lehman, J., & Miikkulainen, R. (2019). Designing neural networks through neuro-
evolution. Nature Machine Intelligence, 1(1), 24–35.
Stephen, I. D., Hiew, V., Coetzee, V., Tiddeman, B. P., & Perrett, D. I. (2017). Facial shape analysis identi-
fies valid cues to aspects of physiological health in Caucasian, Asian, and African populations. Frontiers in
Psychology, 8, 1883.
Szepsenwol, O., Griskevicius, V., Simpson, J. A., Young, E. S., Fleck, C., & Jones, R. E. (2017). The effect
of predictable early childhood environments on sociosexuality in early adulthood. Evolutionary Behavioral
Sciences, 11(2), 131.
Szepsenwol, O., Simpson, J., Griskevicius, V., Zamir, O., Young, E. S., Shoshani, A., & Doron, G. (2021).
The effects of childhood unpredictability and harshness on emotional control and relationship quality:
A life history perspective. Development and Psychopathology. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/
10.1017/S0954579421001371
Tanskanen, A. O., & Danielsbacka, M. (2018). Intergenerational family relations: An evolutionary social science
approach. Taylor & Francis.
Trillmich, F., Müller, T., & Müller, C. (2018). Understanding the evolution of personality requires the study
of mechanisms behind the development and life history of personality traits. Biology Letters, 14(2), Article
20170740.
Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B. Campbell (Ed.), Sexual selection and the
descent of man (pp. 136–179). Aldine.
Trivers, R. (1991). Deceit and self-deception: The relationship between communication and consciousness.
Man and Beast Revisited, 907, 175–191.
Int roduction 9
PART
I
Relationship
Initiation
12 R e l ation sh ip In itiation
hormone-mediated relationship formation and identifies methodological advancements
that are yet needed within this field of study.
In chapter 6, Caton, Lewis, Al-Shawaf, and Evans catalogue the diverse courtship sig-
nals and behaviors that humans and other animals employ to attract partners and promote
their reproductive success. Discussion is organized around the five senses to showcase
how courtship is guided by sensory input and the adapted cognition that processes each
sensory mode (i.e., visual, auditory, tactile, olfactory, and gustatory). Throughout, they
maintain focus on cutting-edge research on psychophysical cues of mate quality, including
neck musculature, lumbar curvature, and sebum detection. They conclude by discussing
future directions for studying how humans use each sensory modality to assess potential
partners.
In chapter 7, Krems, Bradshaw, and Merrie provide an overview of the major theories
and evidence underlying research on intrasexual competition in humans. They discuss
the contributions of parental investment theory and sexual strategies theory as well as the
influence of biological markets and ecological factors (e.g., sex ratios and income inequal-
ity) that shape same-sex competition within men and women. They bridge historical and
recent work to identify unresolved gaps in the empirical study of contest competition and
spend considerable time identifying future directions in the study competition among
women, sexual and gender minorities, and parents and older adults.
In chapter 8, Valentova, Amaral, and Varella give an authoritative summary of mate
preference, mate choice, and relationship initiation research among hetero-and non-
heterosexuals and evaluates how evolutionary scientists have approached the study of
relationship initiation among LGBTQ+individuals. They note which sex differences
appear to be robust with LGBTQ+samples and which features of attraction and courtship
diverge from findings among heterosexual individuals. They identify several current limi-
tations, including a largely restrictive focus on Western samples, insufficiently nuanced
measures of sexual orientation, and interactions between sexual attraction and gender (a)
typical developmental process.
In chapter 9, Koren and Ayalon consider how relationship initiation varies across age
and the unique challenges that individuals must resolve to form partnerships later in life.
They consider the roles of individualistic versus collectivistic cultural values, the influence
that children and grandchildren exert on mate choice and repartnering, and the barri-
ers that later-life health and living conditions can introduce. Throughout, they reference
prominent models of socioemotional functioning and successful aging to highlight the
importance of studying the diversity of relationships among older adults, the intersect-
ing effects of culture and parent–offspring interactions and expectations, and the roles of
physical and emotional togetherness/apartness. They conclude with research on LGBTQ+
populations, extreme old age, and emerging technologies for helping older individuals
find and achieve relational satisfaction.
14 R e l ation sh ip In itiation
Another random document with
no related content on Scribd:
imperfect contrivance. The ancient monarchy never seems to have
made use of flat signets. The impression of one has been sought for
in vain on those contracts of the time of Hammourabi, where so
many cylinders have left their mark. The oldest document on which
the trace of a circular seal has been recognized belongs to the
northern kingdom, and dates from the reign of Bin-Nirari, who
occupied the throne of Assyria towards the end of the ninth century
B.C. From this moment the use of the cone becomes rapidly
common. Under the Sargonids, and still more during the second
Chaldee monarchy and under the Achæmenids, it superseded the
cylinder. The dates inscribed on the tablets prove their age; the
space on the cones themselves was too narrow, as a rule, for a
legend. On a few specimens we find one or two characters
engraved, generally a divine monogram or the traditional emblems of
the sidereal powers. A few cones have inscriptions in Aramaic
characters (see Fig. 157); on the example figured we again
encounter the strange composite beast we have already seen upon
a stone tablet and a cylinder (Figs. 87 and 141). In spite of the
alphabet employed, this cone must have been engraved either in
Nineveh or its neighbourhood.
The narrowness of the field explains the want of variety in the
subjects. In a small circle like this there was no room for more than a
single figure with a few accessories, or, at most, for two figures. We
cannot expect to find scenes as varied and complicated as those
upon the cylinders. A very small number of the simplest themes
formed the stock-in-trade of the engraver.
There are about four hundred specimens in the British Museum,
and as many more in Paris, in the Louvre and the Cabinet des
Antiques. In the presence of them all we can only confess to a
feeling of embarrassment. They are never arranged in chronological
order; Assyrian intaglios are mixed up with those from Chaldæa,
from Phœnicia and Persia. Certain types were reproduced and
copied in this region even as late as the Arsacids and Sassanids.
We shall choose a few, however, which we may with some certainty
attribute to Assyria. There is in the first place one on which two
winged figures seem to be adorning the sacred tree (Fig. 158). We
find the impression of an almost exactly similar cone on a contract
dated 650 B.C. The only differences lie in the more careful execution
of the latter seal and in the substitution of the radiant disk of the sun
for the crescent moon.[331] In another impression we find the radiant
disk changed into the winged globe.[332] The shape and fringe of the
Assyrian robe may be recognized in the intaglio in which a man with
long hair and beard does homage to a winged genius (Fig. 159). The
worshipper is standing, but behind him appears a kneeling figure.
This posture is rare, but it is met with in a few instances on
monuments from this period, and is always used to suggest the
profound respect with which a man does obeisance either to his god
or his king.[333]
We need not hesitate to ascribe to the second Chaldæan
monarchy a cone with a bearded individual standing before an altar
on which lies a fantastic animal (Fig. 160); above his head appear
the sun, the moon, and a star. We have already mentioned two
examples of this theme, which begins to appear in the time of
Nebuchadnezzar and remains in fashion until the Macedonian
conquest.[334]
We have now reached the end of our inquiry into the history of
Mesopotamian sculpture—an inquiry that we have endeavoured to
make as complete as the existing remains would allow. So far as
Chaldæa is concerned, these are very few in number. On the other
hand, the three centuries over which the Assyrian power extended
are pictured in such a vast number of reliefs that we are
embarrassed by their number as much as by their want of variety.
Our difficulty in the case of Assyria has been to make a selection
from a vast quantity of objects that tell us the same thing again and
again, while, in the case of Chaldæa, it has been to insure that none
of the scanty salvage from so great a wreck should be lost. We have
more than once had to make induction and conjecture take the place
of examination and assertion before we could complete even a
rough sketch of the development of Chaldæan art.
There is one question that must have been asked by many of our
readers before these pages came in their way, but is now, we
venture to hope, fully answered, and that is, whether the Semites of
Chaldæa drew their first inspiration from a foreign source, or whether
it was an original result from the natural aptitudes of the race.
Ancient as civilization may have been in the Euphrates valley, it was
still more ancient, to all appearance, in the valley of the Nile. And yet
all who have examined the figures we have placed before them must
acknowledge the originality and independence of Chaldæan art. No;
the sculptors of Memphis and Thebes were not the masters of those
of Babylon and Nineveh; they preceded them indeed, but they left
them no teaching and no models to copy.
This is proved in the first place by the difference, we might say
the opposition, between the two styles. The Egyptian sculptor
simplifies, abridges, and summarizes form; the Assyrian amplifies it
and accents its details. The former seems to see the human body
through a veil of gauze, which hides the accidents of the surface and
the secondary forms, allowing nothing to be clearly grasped but the
contour and the great leading lines. One would say that the second
studied nature through a magnifying-glass; he insists upon what the
first slurs over.
This is not the only difference between the two methods and the
two interpretations. The Egyptian artist can seize the character of a
movement with much justice and vivacity, but he endeavours to
ennoble it by giving it a general and typical value. This he does, for
example, in the gesture of the king who brandishes his mace or
sword over the head of his conquered enemy while he holds him by
the hair with his other hand.[341]
He thinks more about elegance in arranging the posture of his
figures; look, for instance, at the men and women carrying offerings,
at the dancers and musicians who abound in the reliefs and pictures.
His favourite attitude, however, is one expressive of force in repose.
We cannot deny that in his figures in the round the Mesopotamian
sculptor showed the same predilection, but his choice was
suggested, or rather imposed, by the resistance of the materials he
employed and the necessity of avoiding certain executive difficulties
over which he could not triumph. We can hardly see how he could
have given his figures more animation or have better expressed the
freedom of their limbs and the swing of their bodies; the stones he
used were either too hard or too soft, and he was without the needful
skill in the management of his tools.
It is in the reliefs, where he is more at his ease, that he allows us
to see whither his natural inclinations would lead him. They contain
hardly any seated figures. Man is there always on his feet and in
action. Movement, to interest the Mesopotamian artist, need not be
the expression of an idea, or the cause of graceful lines. It pleases
him for its own sake by its freedom and unexpectedness, I am
almost tempted to say, by its violence.
This feeling is visible chiefly in the battle pictures and hunting
scenes. In these, no doubt, the drawing of limbs, &c., often leaves
much to be desired. The hand has been unable to render all that the
eye has seen. The unveiled human body has not been displayed
often enough to the sculptor for him to know thoroughly the
construction of its framework and the mode of attachment of its
limbs. On the other hand, when animals have to be treated, with
what singular power and complete success the same artist has often
represented the tension of the contracting muscles, the speed of the
horse as he stretches himself in the gallop, the spring of the lion as
he throws himself upon the spear (see Fig. 161), and, finally, the
trembling of the flesh in the last struggle against suffering and death!
It is in the Assyrian monuments that these things are treated with the
greatest success. A people of soldiers and hunters, whose truculent
energy gave them the empire of all western Asia, they had neither
the mild humour nor the fine taste of the Egyptians, they were less
easily moved, and we find ourselves wondering that they never hit
upon the fights of gladiators as a national pastime. They were
touched and interested by force passing from repose into action, by
force putting forth all its energies in contempt of danger and in spite
of the most determined resistance.