Case Study
Case Study
Case Study
CASE
Study
Sara Miller McCune founded SAGE Publishing in 1965 to support
the dissemination of usable knowledge and educate a global
community. SAGE publishes more than 1000 journals and over
800 new books each year, spanning a wide range of subject areas.
Our growing selection of library products includes archives, data,
case studies and video. SAGE remains majority owned by our
founder and after her lifetime will become owned by a charitable
trust that secures the company’s continued independence.
HOW TO DO YOUR
CASE
Study
Gary
Thomas
SAGE Publications Ltd © Gary Thomas 2021
1 Oliver’s Yard
55 City Road This edition first published 2021
London EC1Y 1SP
Apart from any fair dealing for the purposes of research,
SAGE Publications Inc. private study, or criticism or review, as permitted under the
2455 Teller Road Copyright, Designs and Patents Act, 1988, this publication
Thousand Oaks, California 91320 may not be reproduced, stored or transmitted in any form,
or by any means, without the prior permission in writing of
SAGE Publications India Pvt Ltd the publisher, or in the case of reprographic reproduction,
B 1/I 1 Mohan Cooperative Industrial Area in accordance with the terms of licences issued by the
Mathura Road Copyright Licensing Agency. Enquiries concerning
New Delhi 110 044 reproduction outside those terms should be sent to the
publisher.
SAGE Publications Asia-Pacific Pte Ltd
3 Church Street
#10-04 Samsung Hub
Singapore 049483
ISBN 978-1-5297-0496-9
ISBN 978-1-5297-0495-2 (pbk)
At SAGE we take sustainability seriously. Most of our products are printed in the UK using responsibly sourced
papers and boards. When we print overseas we ensure sustainable papers are used as measured by the PREPS
grading system. We undertake an annual audit to monitor our sustainability.
CONTENTS
About the author x
Preface xi
Online resources xiii
A roadmap for conducting a case study, using this book xiv
5 Ethics 85
Your participants 85
Vulnerable groups 86
Participants’ activity 87
Deception or concealment 88
Confidentiality and anonymity 88
Data security and stewardship 90
Consent90
Risk92
Contacting participants 98
Ethics and social media 98
Care for your participants – and yourself 99
Where do I put discussion about ethics in my case study write-up? 99
Getting clearance – ethical review 99
If you take only one thing from this chapter, take this … 102
Reflective activity 103
Further reading 103
CONTENTS vii
9 Your process: the shape, style and manner of your case study 174
The single case 175
Time as a dimension of the case study 178
The multiple or collective or comparative case (or cross-case analysis) 186
viii HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
10 Out in the field: some ways to collect data and evidence 203
Interviews205
Accounts206
Diaries207
Group interviews and focus groups 210
Interrogating documents 211
Questionnaires212
Observation215
Image-based methods 217
Measurements and tests 218
Official statistics and other numerical data 219
Using social media 220
If you take only one thing from this chapter, take this … 220
Reflective activity 221
Further reading 222
References 284
Index 294
ABOUT THE AUTHOR
Gary Thomas began his working life as a teacher, then he
became a teacher, then an educational psychologist, then
a professor of education at the University of Birmingham,
where his teaching, research and writing now focus on
inclusive education and the methods used in social science
research.
He has led a wide range of research projects and has
received awards from the AHRC, the ESRC, the Nuffield
Foundation, the Leverhulme Trust, the Department for
Education, charities such as Barnardo’s and the Cadmean
Trust, local authorities and a range of other organisations. He has written or edited
20 books and lots of boring academic articles.
He has two grown-up daughters, one small daughter and two grandchildren. He
likes, in alphabetical order, cats, chess, cycling, dogs, gardening and writing. He
dislikes Chelsea tractors, pomposity and people who try to make things sound more
complicated than they are (in that order).
Despite supporting Aston Villa football club, he maintains an optimistic outlook
on life.
PREFACE
I’ve been delighted with the reception of the first and second editions of How to Do
Your Case Study. This third edition contains much more material on ethics, risk and
data collection, new examples, and – among other changes – has new sections on
critical thinking, reflective journals, social media, coding and discourse analysis. As
in the previous two editions, my main aim has been to offer clear advice to students
on how best to conduct a case study inquiry, with lots of examples of how it can be
used most fruitfully.
In my examples I have drawn on instances of real or possible case studies that
could be done by real students doing real inquiries. I have done this rather than
use high-profile, well-known case studies carried out by teams of sociologists, each
taking several years to complete. These iconic examples can provide pointers and
flashes of insight that can inspire and give ideas, but, if I read too many of these,
I find I can become daunted rather than inspired, thinking, ‘How on earth could
I ever do anything like this?’ So, the examples I have given are, in the main, real,
adapted (and many thanks to the students from whom I have stolen them) or
sometimes imagined.
I’ve assumed people will be reading this book because they want to do a case
study, not because they want to find out about the case study method per se. There
must, I suppose, be a very limited number of research methodologists who are
interested in the case study in and of itself, but my assumption is that these are not
the principal audience for this book. Looking at the books available on the topic,
though, it is almost as if it were the other way round. It is almost as though authors
are writing books on case studies as if most people want to pursue an academic
interest in the case study method rather than want to find out about it so that they
can do one better.
I’ve also wanted in this book to provide not just a primer on the case study but
also something rather more eclectic than is to be found in most of the books avail-
able on the topic. In my experience, nearly all of the academic writing on the case
study has been done by sociologists seeing it as a form of inquiry for sociology
alone. While the case study has a long and fascinating tradition in sociology, it
is not owned by sociology. Indeed, as I’ve noted, the method can be used almost
anywhere, and I try to make this point in the book by taking examples from differ-
ent areas of inquiry. I also try to show how different insights about holistic inquiry
from different academic domains can have wider application and how ideas from
across a spectrum of disciplines have commonality.
xii HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
I should note that parts of Chapter 4 in this book have been published in the jour-
nals Qualitative Inquiry and the Oxford Review of Education (Thomas, 2011a, 2011b).
There are too many colleagues, friends and students to thank by name, but I
do really want to thank them all for their ideas and time. Thanks especially to
Jai Seaman at SAGE, who commissioned this third edition and coordinated read-
ers’ comments to provide such an enormously helpful plan of action for the new
volume. And thanks to those anonymous reviewers for their valuable comments.
Many thanks to all of Jai’s colleagues at SAGE for their help in putting the new
book together, especially Katherine Haw, Neville Hankins and their colleagues in
the production department at SAGE.
ONLINE RESOURCES
Discover your textbook’s online resources!
The third edition of How to Do Your Case Study offers a range of online resources
to help you confidently conduct a case study. Find them at: https://study.sage
pub.com/thomascasestudy3e
Videos from author Gary Thomas give you friendly guidance on important
considerations like how to structure your case study.
Checklists help you track your progress throughout the research process and make
sure you don’t miss a step.
Template consent forms that you can tailor to your own case study give you a
strong starting point for doing ethically sound research.
Weblinks showcase handy resources on a range of topics, from doing case study
research with social media to thinking critically and conducting ethical research.
Selected journal articles discussing key topics such as critical thinking help you
expand your knowledge and reinforce your learning.
A ROADMAP FOR
CONDUCTING A CASE
STUDY, USING THIS
BOOK
There are three stages to conducting a case study.
1 F
irst you need to know why you are doing a case
study. Is it really the best way of addressing your
research questions? What can it offer?
2 S
econd, you need to know how case studies are
typically structured and how best to choose your
case study subject.
3 T
hird, you need to know about processes for
collecting data and analysing them.
• w
hat the case study is, and the kinds of inquiries
for which it may be useful
• w
hat we are trying to do with the case study as a
form of inquiry – that is, look at a situation in its
completeness and all its complexity
• w
hy, throughout the history of inquiry, people
have felt that it is a good idea to examine
something in its completeness
method the ‘case method’ or ‘case study’. Jean Marc Gaspard Itard and his study
of Victor, the ‘wild boy of Aveyron’, at the beginning of the nineteenth century
could equally take the accolade, as could Frédéric Le Play in his celebrated stud-
ies of French working people in the middle of that same century. (Le Play used
a case method wherein he would invite himself to reside with ordinary working
families for extended periods until he got to know them intimately. Whether
or not his uninvited sojourns in the cottages of poor coalminers of the Jura
were exuberantly welcomed by his hosts is unrecorded – one can imagine some
Pythonesque tableaux – but his rich analyses were lauded in both political and
literary circles.)
If you use case study as the design frame for your research you will be concen-
trating on one thing: looking at it in detail. When you do a case study, you are
interested in that thing in itself, as a whole. I’m using the word ‘thing’ advisedly
since the thing may be a person, a group, an institution, a country, an event, a
period in time or whatever. You may be looking at the process of a medical diag-
nosis with one patient or you may be looking at relationships among a gang of
teenagers or you may be looking at one student’s learning in class. You may be
looking at a family or at IKEA’s development as a business or at the USA’s politi-
cal stance at the time of the Cuban Missile Crisis. Any of these ‘things’, these
phenomena, could form the subject of a case study. What is of interest is the
uniqueness of the thing and the thing in its completeness.
The main feature of your choice of case will
A case study is about the particular be the interest that you have in the subject of
rather than the general. the study. Why are you interested in it? It could
be interesting because …
• you are intimately connected with it – perhaps it is a child in your class, a patient
in your hospital or a project in your company
• it is a conspicuously good example of something in which you are interested – for
example, IKEA as an example of a successful business
• it is different from what is typical – it is an ‘outlier’ that shows something remark-
able by virtue of its difference from what seems to be the norm, such as, perhaps,
Kerala as a state in India where, despite widespread poverty, people have a far
longer life expectancy than they do elsewhere in India.
evidence from different experiences. Social science is about getting beyond what is
merely anecdotal, some would say.
Certainly, when we want to generalise – to say that this phenomenon has
occurred in this sample of people and we can generalise this finding to the wider
population – clearly we cannot use just one person’s experience or a single set of
uncorroborated observations as the basis for this. That is why such trouble has to
be taken in many kinds of research to establish the sample as being representative
of the wider population. If we want to generalise we need to make the basis of our
generalisation clear.
What we are talking about with a case study, though, is a different kind of inquiry
from those where generalisation is expected to follow. Here we are talking about
understanding how and why something might have happened or why it might be
the case. The assumption in a case study is that, with a great deal of intricate study
of one case, looking at your subject from many and varied angles, you can get closer
to the ‘why’ and the ‘how’. As Becker (2014: 3) puts it: ‘Everything present in or
connected to a situation I want to understand should be taken account of and made
use of. If it’s there, it’s doing something, however unimportant that thing seems, no
matter how unobtrusive it is.’
You will notice that although the assumptions
about valid processes of inquiry and analysis dif- Two important things about a case
study:
fer from those in many kinds of social research
(for we are not working from samples that enable 1. you drill down further
us to generalise), assumptions about the use of 2. you create a three-dimensional
picture – or what Foucault
evidence do not change. With a case study there called a ‘polyhedron of
is still the assumption that we must collect good intelligibility’.
evidence and lots of it. In the modern phrase, we
must ‘drill down’ as deep as we possibly can to get evidence, penetrating into every
nook and cranny, and squeezing out every little bit that can be found.
To mix our metaphors even more, we must look at our subject from many and
varied angles, to develop what the great historian–philosopher Michel Foucault
(1981: 4) called ‘a polyhedron of intelligibility’. By this he meant that inquiries
in the humanities and social sciences are too often one-dimensional, as if we are
looking at our subject just from one direction. In looking from several directions, a
more rounded, richer, more balanced picture of our subject is developed – we get a
three-dimensional view.
It is not just in the social sciences and the humanities that this multifaceted view
is valuable. In what are taken to be ‘harder’ sciences, such as biology, astronomy
and geology, and in many applied sciences, such as palaeoanthropology (the study
of humanoid fossils), the case study is one of the principal methods used.
6 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
A lot can be achieved by recourse to the concrete. We escape from a tendency too
often found in academic writing to obfuscate with abstractions rather than clarify
with specificity – to bring a fog over the topic in hand with abstract words and the
seeking of generalisation where none is possible and none is helpful.
The great writer Harold Evans (2000: 32) said that the abstract should be ‘chased
out’ in favour of the specific. He was talking of journalism rather than academic
inquiry and writing, but I think his point is just as relevant in the academy, if not
more so. It is a point that is especially germane to the use of the case study, where
the reason-for-being concerns the validity of the concrete and the specific. Evans
(2000: 33) quotes from C.E. Montague: ‘The great escape should be from “mere
intellectualism, with its universals and essences, to concrete particulars, the smell
of human breath, the sound of voices, the stir of living”.’
You should, in a case study, be able to smell human breath and hear the sound
of voices. Nothing is lost in their refraction through our own understanding as
interpreting inquirers. In fact, much is gained as we add a separate viewpoint – one
that moulds and melds the experiences of others through our own understandings.
The long and the short of this is that there is no one way to be scientific and the
case study is just as scientific as the next way to carry out an inquiry. As Einstein
said (see above), science is not about a method, but about intuition or thinking – it
is about supplying answers to questions with good evidence and good reasoning,
which can be done in a variety of ways, with the principal feature of importance
being the thought and analysis that go into providing those answers.
We sometimes get too hung up, I think, on the shibboleths of science – that
is, the things taken to be core to the enterprise of science, such as causation and
generalisation. Einstein’s point is that it is not these that are at the core of good
scientific thinking. Rather, it is about making connections and having insights
and testing these out, in whatever way. The great sociologist Howard Becker
(1998: 41) put it this way when he wrote about how the connections we seek
are multistranded and multidirectional, such that causality is a less than helpful
concept in social science:
there are many modes of connection, for which we use words like ‘influ-
ence’ or ‘causality’ or ‘dependence’. All these words point to variation.
Something will vary and something else, dependent on what happens
to the first thing, will undergo some change as well. The things that
so vary will often influence each other in complicated ways, so that
‘causality’ is not really an appropriate way to talk about what we want
to emphasize.
Some definitions
The case study is not a method in itself. Rather, it is a focus and the focus is on one
thing, looked at in depth and from many angles. Bob Stake (2005: 443, empahsis in
original) puts it this way:
So, you have the focus for the case and you choose methods to help you inquire
into the subject. I’ll discuss some of those methods in Chapters 10 and 11.
10 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Stake goes on to emphasise the importance of the singular in the case. So, a doc-
tor may be a case to be studied, but it is difficult to see how methods of ‘doctoring’
could be a case. Nor could the reasons for child neglect be considered a case, says
Stake (2005: 444), since ‘We think of those topics as generalities rather than specifi-
cities. The case is a specific One’ – note the upper-case ‘O’.
Helen Simons (2009: 21) sums this up well in her definition of the case study:
Stake (1995: xi) makes a similar point: ‘Case study is the study of the particularity
and complexity of a single case, coming to understand its activity within important
circumstances.’
Both of these experts stress particularity and complexity and the real-life cir-
cumstances within which the research occurs. Looking at a number of definitions
of the case study, Simons says that what unites them is a commitment to study-
ing the complexity that is involved in real situations and defining the case study
other than by methods.
Still on the question of definition, Hammersley and Gomm (2000: 2) make the
point that all research is, in a sense, case study because ‘there is always some unit,
or set of units, in relation to which data are collected and/or analysed’. They suggest
that the important distinction between the case study and other kinds of research
is the number of cases investigated and the amount of detailed information which
can therefore be collected about each one. Whereas with social surveys, for exam-
ple, you find a little bit – perhaps just one set of questionnaire responses – from
each of the many people participating, with a case study you are finding much,
much more, but about a very limited number.
A summary of Hammersley and Gomm’s table outlining the differences between
the case study and two other forms of research (experiment and survey) is given in
Table 1.1.
The choice of one case (or a small number) is made with a trade-off in mind,
they say. You choose your very restricted sample so that you can gain greater
detail, but this is at the expense of being able to make generalisations about your
findings.
WHAT IS A CASE STUDY? 11
Table 1.1 A comparison of the case study with other forms of inquiry (liberally adapted
from Hammersley and Gomm, 2000)
Like Hammersley and Gomm, Ragin (1992: 5) also offers a definition by present-
ing a contrast with what we might call ‘variable-led research’:
Ragin’s definition does two things. It contrasts the emphasis on cases with an
emphasis on variables in other kinds of research, but it also introduces the impor-
tant idea of the boundary.
An important feature of the case study is drawn by Wieviorka (1992), who says
that straightforward examination of the case itself – the child, or the institution or
the period in time, or whatever – is not, at least as far as research inquiry is con-
cerned, enough on its own to constitute a case study. A case study has to have an
additional ‘angle’ to it: the case has to be a case of something – it has to illuminate
and offer a degree of explanation to some wider theme. Wieviorka puts it thus:
12 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
***
What is a case?
When I was writing this book, a moment of delightful serendipity occurred when, by
mistake, I pressed the thesaurus button on my word processor while the cursor was
stationed over ‘case’. I give a summary of the meanings it came up with in Table 1.2.
I had thought that I knew what ‘case’ meant, but this serendipitous opening
led me to some personal research into the meanings of ‘case’. It turns out (if
my less-than-scrupulously-thorough research is correct) that the rather different
meanings of ‘case’ arise because the word is taken from two similar-sounding
WHAT IS A CASE STUDY? 13
Latin roots that have arrived, after corruption, at the same word, ‘case’, in English.
The two roots are:
Kelly (1993: 35) notes that the Oxford Latin Dictionary lists 11 different meanings for
the word ‘case’, suggesting that ‘the term was just as confusing in Latin as in English’.
Anyway, this personal etymological quest led me to realise that the use of ‘case’ in
‘case study’ ticked two important boxes – by good luck, probably, rather than good
planning – about the essence of this kind of inquiry. Quite fortuitously, therefore, it
is an entirely appropriate term for what is done in a case inquiry. Let’s look at these
two meanings, plus the other one identified by my computer’s thesaurus.
even the doctor would not be a case – at least, not a very interesting or informative
one. To be a case – an interesting one – we would have to be able to say that the
doctor was a case of something (you should have an idea of what he or she is a case
of at the outset). It will then be this analytical focus that crystallises, thickens or
develops as the study proceeds. Indeed, it is the way this emerges, grows and devel-
ops that is at the heart of the study.
So, the Korean War would not be a case unless it was a case of something. Is it
a case of a war? If so, can you say that it is a case of an especially remarkable or
unusual kind of war? Perhaps it may be, by contrast, essentially a case of a border
dispute, but one that has grown out of proportion to its original significance. Is
it a case of US resistance to what was assumed to be communist expansion? Just
chronicling the story of the Korean War would not be a case study. An exploration
of any one of these ‘ofs’ – any of these analytical backdrops – would, however, make
a fascinating case study.
1. what he calls a ‘practical, historical unity’ (the Korean War in my example) – this is,
in essence, the subject
2. what he calls the ‘theoretical, scientific basis’ of the case (such as US resistance
to communist expansion, in my example) – this is the analytical frame or what we
could call the object.
So, just extending the war theme for a moment, we could not have a case study
entitled ‘World War II: a case study’. Here, World War II is not a case of anything.
However, ‘World War II: a case study of a “just
war”’ would contain both of the elements Case study research comprises two
parts:
outlined by Wieviorka. It would make an inter-
esting piece of work since it could examine in 1. a ‘subject’ – the case itself
detail the notion of a just war, played out and 2. an analytical frame, or ‘object’.
exemplified through the case of World War II.
Alternatively, take the example of a hospital ward. Would a description of this
ward constitute a case study? In my opinion, it would not – it is not a case of any-
thing. It becomes a case of something when you can explain the analytical frame
through which you might be viewing it. It might:
16 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
As I indicate in Table 1.3, the analytical focus must extend beyond mere description.
So, a case study is like one of those capsules with two halves: each half, each
ingredient, is necessary in order for the other half to work, as shown in Figure 1.1.
It has to contain the ‘practical, historical unity’, as Wieviorka (1992: 159) puts
it (or ‘the subject’ in plain English), and it has to contain the analytical frame
(or ‘object’). It is not complete without both parts in place: one will not work
without the other.
The subject:
the person, The analytical
place ... frame or object
Figure 1.1 The two parts of a case study, each part needing the other
WHAT IS A CASE STUDY? 17
Let’s take another example and put it in the two-part capsule. Let’s take as our
subject the 1923 hung parliament in the UK. A narrative of this on its own is not a
case study. How can we put it in the context of an analytical frame?
We first of all have to remember that we are looking at this one hung
parliament – the 1923 one. We are looking at it as one of a set of hung parlia-
ments. Why are we looking at this particular one? Perhaps it is an especially
interesting one or an unusual one concerning the negotiations and the ‘horse-
trading’ that occur among party leaders at the beginning of a hung parliament,
just after the election result has been finalised and announced. This, then,
would be our analytical frame. Clearly, a range of analytical frames could poten-
tially accompany the subject, but we would need to choose one (or more) of
these (see Figure 1.2).
Analytical frame:
Subject: negotiation
the 1923 hung process at the
parliament beginning of the
parliament
In the way that Wieviorka sees the case study, as subject and object, the subject is
almost less important than the object. What I mean by this is that the subject, the
case itself, is the lens through which you are looking at the object. Let’s unpack one
of the examples from Table 1.3 to illustrate what I mean …
(Continued)
18 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
group. It is hard to get them to talk or to relate with either adults or other
children.
If ‘quiet children’ were your topic of interest, they would be your ‘object’.
Given this object – this topic of interest that you wanted to research – it
would be useful to use as a case study subject a child who showed the kinds
of behaviours I’ve just described (and of course it may be this particular child
who has led you to this topic of interest).
How would a case study proceed with this child – let’s call her Amelia. With
all ethical considerations taken into account and procedures completed
(see Chapter 5), you would get a rich picture of Amelia, her life, her relation-
ships with adults and children, how she spends her day, what she does and
does not like doing. You would observe her in class and at home, you would
interview her parents and her teachers, you might ask the classteacher to get
the children in Amelia’s class to indicate their favoured partners for a socio-
gram (see page 236). You might volunteer to be a playground helper and/
or a classroom assistant for a day a week for a term, to write an ethnography
(see page 159) of the ‘class world’.
And then you would analyse all the information, the data, that you
had collected about Amelia – your case study subject – but remem-
bering always that you would be analysing those data with the ‘object’
in mind. In other words, you would be analysing your data in trying to
The subject
(Amelia, the
‘quiet child’,
in this
example) is
the lens
through which
we view the
object
With this focus on growth, this case study will be focusing on change
over time, beginning perhaps with a reconnoitre of the available statistics, to
establish the grounds for your case. You will want to establish that IKEA really
is a highly successful retailer and to show how it has grown over time, exam-
ining its turnover and profit figures since its creation by Ingvar Kamprad in
Sweden in 1943. What does the graph of turnover figures look like over the
years? Alternatively, you could show, on a series of maps, each representing
a 10-year period, the countries in which IKEA stores have existed. You may
be able to find out the figures you need quite easily or you may need to go
to some quite obscure sources.
This gathering and plotting of basic data on turnover and profit over the
years since 1943 provides the boundary for your case study. It says, ‘This has
happened: IKEA really is the biggest and it has become the biggest in a
period of less than 50 years.’
This, though, is not enough for a case study. Simply to show the growth
over time is not sufficient. You have the boundaries defined by the growth, but
now you want to know how and why the growth has happened and, while you
will not be able to provide a definitive answer, the case study should provide
you with enough evidence to make a convincing argument. You will need to
click together bits of information like pieces in a jigsaw, bits of evidence that
you collect in support of any one of a number of tentative ideas – sometimes
called hypotheses or theories – about the growth of the company.
If you are thinking about the growth of the company, what might be plau-
sible ideas to explain its growth? Let’s have a look at a few.
The company may have grown because its founder had one big idea or a
series of good ideas, such as realising the:
If any of these were important, where did Kamprad’s ideas come from? Did
he work in a sawmill and notice the abundance of sawdust? Did he work in
retailing and realise the wastage involved in ‘transporting air’ in assembled
furniture? Was it, instead, down to the retailing technique employed in IKEA
stores, wherein consumers are encouraged to walk along a certain route
through the store, being encouraged to buy as they make their extended
and unexpected walk?
WHAT IS A CASE STUDY? 21
How could you go about finding out which of these potential ideas was
important? Clearly the ideal would be an interview with Ingvar Kamprad,
to ask him about your tentative theories, but I doubt if he gave many inter-
views. You will almost certainly have to rely on printed biographies of him.
The Wikipedia entry for him, for example, tells us that in 1976 he wrote a
manifesto detailing the IKEA concept of frugality and enthusiasm. We learn
that he also worked with Swedish journalist Bertil Torekull on the book
Leading by Design: The IKEA Story (Kamprad, 2000). In the autobiographi-
cal account, he further describes his philosophies and the trials and triumphs
of the founding of IKEA. Also, that he began to make a lot of money when
he was a child by buying matches cheaply in bulk and then reselling them at
a profit. He expanded to fish, ballpoint pens and pencils.
Already we can see that he seems to be one of those people who almost
cannot help but make money. It is not as if he had an especially good idea
about furniture or an especially wonderful insight into how it could be mass-
produced. Rather, the seed seems to have come from his dynamism in
finding ways to sell. Alongside this, he apparently drove a 15-year-old car,
flew only economy class and encouraged IKEA employees always to write
on both sides of a piece of paper. So, an obsession with economy, paring
expenses down to the very last little bit, also comes through.
This is the kind of emerging information on which the narrative in a case
study is built. The analysis begins to ‘thicken’ in this way around one theme.
Remember, though, that the topic of theoretical interest that took you to
this case study subject – the analytical frame – is ‘growth in highly successful
companies’. The case study you are conducting is intended to throw light on
this topic and your discussion will be orientated to its illumination.
comprises some elements I have touched upon already and some which I have not
yet discussed in detail, but will explore further in Chapters 6 to 9. The important
feature of this table is that subject, object, purpose, approach, methods and process
are separated, for the purposes of understanding how case studies may be structured.
Methods
Object
methods to be used
case •
Choices about the
• Explanatory
• Outlier case • Illustrative Nested
• Exploratory
analysis
Parallel
Sequential
Throughout this book I shall be drawing on this system for thinking about the
classification of case studies. I hope this will be useful in helping you to think about
the different forms of case study, what their purposes are, and how they might be
constructed. As I say, I go through this in more detail in Chapters 6 to 9 but I’ll give
you a brief introduction to it now so that you can think about how the various case
study examples I offer as the book progresses differ in aim and structure.
The classification system – or ‘typology’ – I have drawn up splits thinking about
case studies into six elements, which translate into six steps in the construction of
a case study:
1. The subject of the study – what is the case you are actually looking at? Is it a class-
room, a child, an institution, a war, a country, a political event, or what? Where
does this subject come from? It may be one that you know a lot about from your
own experience – a local knowledge case. Or it may be a key case in the literature.
Or it may be one that is interesting because it is an outlier – different from the
norm. Whichever, this is the lens through which you are examining the topic of
theoretical interest that you are focusing on, namely the object of the study.
2. The object of the study – what is the theoretical topic at the heart of your question
that the subject of your study is allowing you to explore in detail?
3. The purpose of the study – why are you doing it? Is it to make an evaluation? Is it
out of curiosity, or is it with a particular understanding in mind?
WHAT IS A CASE STUDY? 23
4. The approach of the study – is it to test a theory that you or someone else has
developed about a topic? Or is it to develop a theory from new about that topic?
5. The methods you will use to do the case study. What data collection tools might
you use, and how might you analyse the data that you gather?
6. The process you will use to conduct the case study. Will it be a single or multiple
study, and will you look at the case during one particular moment in time, or over
a longer period?
The choices you make about your subject, object, purpose, approach, methodology
and process will interact in various ways to blend into one unique study out of an
array of possible permutations.
Let’s now use the typology to offer an organisational scaffold for the case study
of Amelia that I gave earlier in this chapter. What is happening here? After noticing
something worth investigating, namely the strange quietness of certain students at
school, the teacher read around the area and wanted to develop insights into the
psychology of these quiet children. Using the typology, the six steps, as a mental
scaffold, she …
1. Identified as a case study subject a student whom she knew well – Amelia. Amelia
exemplified the topic in which she was interested, namely quiet children. This would
be a case study subject that emerged out of her local knowledge and experience.
2. Developed and explicated her interest in quiet children and embarked on a full
literature review of the topic – offering detail on the analytical frame, or object, of
her study.
3. Made clear her purpose in undertaking the study – this would be an instrumental
study with the aim of gaining insights and understanding of Amelia’s quietness
with a view to illuminating the topic more generally.
4. Decided on the approach she would take in undertaking the study. Would she be
testing theories from the literature about quiet children? There was not, she discov-
ered, very much in the way of theory to test. She concluded that her approach was
theory building: trying to formulate ideas about Amelia’s context and behaviour
that offered insights and understandings – theory – about her quietness, and pos-
sibly that of other children.
5. Decided on means and methods of data collection and analysis – her methodology.
This would comprise observation, interviews, a sociogram and analysis involving
theme mapping and thick description.
6. Decided that this would be a single case study spanning a particular period in
time – a snapshot of Amelia’s behaviour, context, interests, relationships, over half
a term.
24 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Mapping these to Table 1.4, we can show the path for Amelia’s case study (Figure 1.4).
Methods
Object
methods to be used
case
Choices about the
analysis
Sequential
study, or one involving several subjects, and you may choose to look at the case
(or cases) over a short or a more extended period.
• Following the six steps from the typology will help you to scaffold your thinking
about how to construct a study depending on your own circumstances, questions
and expectations.
REFLECTIVE ACTIVITY
At the end of this chapter I used the example of Amelia to exemplify mapping
that case study to the typology shown in Table 1.4. In other words, I broke down
the Amelia case study into subject, object, purpose, etc.
Try analysing another case study – perhaps one of the other examples from
Table 1.3, or the boxed example of IKEA – according to the typology. Or use a case
study in your own area of interest – one that you know about, or one that you
might conduct yourself. Draw arrows, as I have, between the different elements.
Explain why the case study is taking this path through the typology. Tip: There’s
no single right answer – you can justify the choices that you make.
Further Reading
Cohen, M.F. (2007) An Introduction to Logic and Scientific Method. New York:
Harcourt, Brace.
Originally published (with Ernest Nagel) in 1934, this is a magnificent, if slightly
dated, outline of what constitutes science. It is good on the duty of doubt in sci-
ence. See especially Chapters 2 and 12.
Gomm, R., Hammersley, M. and Foster, P. (eds) (2000) Case Study Method. London:
Sage.
This book offers, as the editors put it, the most influential and important articles
on the case study. See especially the introduction by Hammersley and Gomm for
an excellent overview.
26 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Ragin, C.C. and Becker, H.S. (eds) (1992) What Is a Case? Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Many excellent articles are included here. See especially Ragin’s introduction.
Stake, R.E. (1995) The Art of Case Study Research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Stake, R.E. (2005) ‘Qualitative case studies’, in N.K. Denzin and Y.S. Lincoln (eds),
The Sage Handbook of Qualitative Research (3rd edn). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Stake is one of the doyens of the case study method and his work is well worth reading.
Thomas, G. and Myers, K. (2015) The Anatomy of the Case Study. London: Sage.
Here, Kevin Myers and I outline in more detail the ways in which case studies work,
and we give several examples of case study in action.
Tight, M. (2017) Understanding Case Study Research: Small-scale Research with Meaning.
London: Sage.
A well-organised, practical primer to doing case study research.
2
THE CASE STUDY AND
RESEARCH DESIGN
First things first: your purpose
Research begins with a purpose and a question, not a research design. You have a
reason for needing to find something out (your purpose) and this leads to a ques-
tion. It is this question that will be at the heart of your research. The design follows
on from that, rather than the other way round.
Investing time and thought in your research question will yield a design solution.
But because of all the initial uncertainty about where the research question might
lead, sometimes people will be tempted to start by thinking of a design route before
they have given their research questions more thought. This means that they will
approach their research problem from entirely the wrong direction – looking for a
method first or going in with a favoured technique – while giving relatively little
attention to the research issue.
You must do the opposite: you must put the question before the method and let
the question determine the method you use to answer it. You cannot say, ‘Here’s a
great method. I’m sure I can use this in my research.’ So, you should not, if you are
sensible, begin by saying, ‘I want to do a case study.’
This mistake of putting the cart before the horse in research design reminds me
of the famous story of a man who lost his keys. He was desperately searching the
gutter under a street lamp. A friend came along and, seeing him hunting around,
asked, ‘Have you lost something?’
‘I’ve lost my car keys. I’ve been looking for them for half an hour’, the man
answered.
The friend joined in to help him, but after some time she couldn’t see any sign of
the keys. ‘Are you positive that it was here that you lost them?’ she asked.
‘No,’ he replied. ‘I lost them over there in that dark alleyway.’
28 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
‘Well, why on earth don’t you look over there, then?’ the friend asked
incredulously.
‘Because this is where the light is, of course!’ the man answered.
The moral of this story is that we should not concentrate our research and all of
our analytical effort on where we have good research instruments, good light, as
they will tell us nothing on their own. The main issue must be ‘What do I want to
find out?’ (‘What am I trying to find? Oh yes, my keys!’) The issue cannot be ‘What
is the best instrument for finding things out?’ until we have decided on that first
question.
Kinds of questions
So, it is the research issue – your question – that is the starting point for your design
journey. It is the pivot for your research: everything rests on it. Your first port of
call in doing your research is not to say boldly, ‘I’m going to do a case study!’, but,
rather, ‘What do I want to know?’ or ‘What do I want to find out?’ It is the research
question that leads you in the direction you need to go.
30 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Your question does not need to be fixed in concrete right at the beginning. It
proceeds from a prima facie question to one that is a more refined, final question.
When I talk about your ‘prima facie’ question I mean your first not-very-well-
thought-out question. This will nearly always change in the course of your inquiry.
It is quite difficult to get your question right, but you have to start somewhere
when you are doing your research, which is where your prima facie question comes
in. It lets you make a start without unnecessary dithering about whether or not you
have got your question right. It allows you to
A question is the starting point for get going. Go boldly with it; be unafraid, on
your research. Begin with a question, the understanding that it will change. It will
not a presupposition that you are
going to do a case study. A case get better as you think more about the subject
study should follow logically from and do more reading.
your question, or else you should
In Figure 2.1, let’s see where your prima facie
not do one.
and final questions lie in the design trail.
Literature review
I think the best policy is to state your prima facie questions at the outset, in the
introduction to your dissertation or thesis, then refine these in the light of your
literature review and state them again, rethought out and reformulated, at the end
of the literature review.
In any research, the literature review is a crucial element, enabling you to see
what has been done in the area of your interest, and allowing you to focus your
ideas based on others’ work. You can look at the agreements, disagreements,
CASE STUDY AND RESEARCH DESIGN 31
dilemmas and questions in an area and adapt or redirect your own focus on the
basis of what you have found. You can also look at the methods used by other
researchers and the trials and tribulations they encountered in their work to see
if you can garner any lessons about method and approach for your own research.
However, in a case study, the literature review assumes a particularly important
role. The ideas you encounter and consider while reading the literature will help you
to formulate your own ideas about the object of the study, which I discussed in the
previous chapter. If you remember, I talked about the subject and the object of a case
study, with the object being the theme, the topic of analytical interest that you hope
to illuminate and elucidate by looking through the lens of the subject, the case itself.
So, here, in the literature review you are exploring the topic of analytical interest, the
object, to see what others have thought – to develop a theoretical understanding of
the area which you can test, refine or otherwise cultivate in your own research work,
that is your own practical work of collecting evidence and analysing it.
What, then, should you be concentrating on when you are doing a literature
review? How should you be cultivating this topic of analytical interest? The key, I
think, is to search for what the great psychologist Jerome Bruner (1997: 142) calls
‘the trouble’ in your area of interest. What, in other words, does not make sense?
What itches? What arouses your interest, and why? Elsewhere, Bruner (1991) talks
about what he calls hermeneutic composability: in plain English this means how
does it all fit together? What appears to depend on what? What contradicts? Where
are there paradoxes? (I explore Bruner’s ideas in more detail in Chapter 11.)
As you do this, you should be developing a storyline with lots of connections
between one part and another. It should have a beginning, a middle and an end:
you outline the issues at the beginning; you provide the analysis and synthesis in
the middle, and you conclude by summarising the issues, differences, paradoxes,
dilemmas and questions yet to be resolved.
The aim is to find themes – or, by contrast, discontinuities, breaks, disagreements –
that run through the literature. When you have done a reasonable amount of searching
you will be able to see these emerging and it is useful at this stage to draw a storyboard –
a plan that sums up and brings together the ideas that are emerging from your litera-
ture review. So, let’s look at a storyboard …
ideas and proceed with those that promise most fruit. You can add to your story-
board as you get further into your literature review and gain information in your
initial inquiries.
In the following example, I try to give a flavour of the kind of thinking that needs
to go on as part of developing your research ideas out of the literature – the general
discourse in your area of interest – and how the storyboard can help in this.
Division
and hatred
• the interest of the USA and the West in the survival of West Berlin over
decades
• the bankruptcy of the Soviet Union after four decades of the arms race
• the conspicuous differences in standards of living in West and East
Germany
• the assumption of power by Gorbachev and his liberalisation of politics
• the damage to the economy caused by the black market in Levi Strauss
jeans
• the Soviet political hierarchy’s refusal to support Erich Honecker, the
East German leader at the time
• and so on.
For each of these possible starting points for an answer – each of these ana-
lytical frames – there are many potential ways to proceed to examine things
further. You could look at statistics and official records, examine newspaper
reports, interview those who lived through it, and so on.
For a piece of small-scale research of the kind done as part of an under-
graduate or postgraduate programme of work, these potential avenues will
be narrowed by the options that are open to you. You may have limited time,
money and access to information. More importantly, though, as far as this
book is concerned, the questions will fan out into a broad range of potential
design frames – and the one that we are interested in here is the case study.
So, what marks out the avenue of inquiry that leads to a case study, as
distinct from some other design frame? It will be a question that:
Thinking about it, reading about it (for your literature review) and talking with
tutors and fellow students, you may realise that, from the various avenues of
(Continued)
34 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
inquiry open to you, what you are really interested in is the role of popular
culture in Eastern Europe at that time and how far this contributed to the fall
of the Wall. This still does not tell you whether or not what you need to do is
a case study, though. What will tell you this?
Remember that the defining characteristic of a case study is the
uniqueness of your subject. So, if you are choosing to do a case study
of the Berlin Wall, you are in some way identifying the Wall as special
(see Figure 2.3). Clearly, we have to be sensible here. It is not unique in
the sense that it is different from your back garden wall or the wall of
your house. (Well, it is, I guess, but not in any interesting way – unless
you are a builder or a bricklayer, in which event you might take a special
interest in its straightness or the quality of the mortar.) It is interesting
perhaps as:
With any of these phenomena you could take the Berlin Wall as a case. You
would then be looking at the Wall as an especially interesting example of
one of these phenomena – one of these analytical frames.
Taking a slightly different slant, you could choose to look at the fall of the
Berlin Wall as an especially interesting example of a rather different set of
phenomena, such as:
Given your interest in popular culture, you decide to take the last of these.
You may be interested in how the fall of the Wall in some way exemplifies
CASE STUDY AND RESEARCH DESIGN 35
Figure 2.3 The Berlin Wall: a case study of what? Photo by Luis Diego Hernández
on Unsplash.
Having established your grounds for using the fall of the Wall as an exam-
ple of the influence of popular culture, you need to keep in mind the whole
time that it is the fall of the Wall that is the event on which you are concen-
trating. In other words, don’t slip into a discussion merely of the influence
of culture on the decline of Soviet influence on Eastern Europe. What is
iconic in the fall of this huge symbol of division? How can its fall in some way
exemplify the power of popular culture? You need to make this connection.
Let’s imagine that you brainstorm again and think of connections with
culture and how those connections can be linked with the Wall. How can
(Continued)
36 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
a story be made? I hope you can see something clearly emerging from the
brainstorm diagram in Figure 2.4.
Newspapers: human
interest stories about Novels: good
separated families backdrop for
scaling the Wall storylines
Clothes:
black market Painting: the Wall Pop music: machine-
smuggling of provides bold, like, without soul; lends
fashionable iconic statement of itself to particular kinds
clothes over separation for of sounds or lyrics about
the Wall works of art freedom
What I’m hoping you can see is that, from the forms of popular cul-
ture I have identified here – media, painting, graffiti, TV, newspapers,
pop music, novels, intellectuals, clothes, celebrities – there are many ele-
ments to the structure of the Wall, culturally speaking. With a spark of
insight, you may see that these make it especially fertile ground for these
various cultural forms. It’s big; it can’t be ignored; it is screaming out to
be painted on; it stands as a conspicuous symbol of brutality in its scale
by comparison with the buildings on either side. It is bounded by stark,
empty space; it stands also as a symbol of the seeming hatred of human
scale and humanity held by the regime that built it – it’s an icon of a rejec-
tion of individualism.
It is in all of these areas that you can make a special distinction of the Wall –
a reason for it to become a case study that makes it stand out as different from
or similar to other borders.
CASE STUDY AND RESEARCH DESIGN 37
Now you can draw a storyboard (see Figure 2.5) in which you ‘join the
dots’ about all of this thinking. You connect the relevant and important bits
of your brainstorming.
In the middle of
the city, seen
every day
Features of the Wall
that make it susceptible
Big – can’t be
to cultural influence
ignored
Can be painted on –
looks like an advert for
Characteristics of the evils of oppression
the Wall that make
it different from Scale and concrete
other borders – its solidity makes it a
Exciting stories,
uniqueness symbol of brutality
e.g. about
smuggling jeans,
can be written Politicians and
celebrities can use it
Other border as a photo-op
emplacements
The Berlin Wall
The Wall is a symbol of separation and oppression. Here, in Figure 2.5, the
storyboard concentrates on its unique features as far as culture is concerned.
It points to the Wall’s special features vis-à-vis others where separation and/or
oppression may be involved – think of the separation of North and South Korea
or of Lebanon from Israel. What makes them less (or more) iconic cultural sym-
bols than the Berlin Wall? For a start, those barriers are of barbed wire rather
than concrete and are separated from ordinary folk by demilitarised zones that
effectively sterilise them from contact with people and, necessarily, culture.
These latter features automatically make other border emplacements
inert as far as their cultural impact is concerned. By their very nature, they are
insulated from people, whereas the Wall was very much ‘in your face’. People
saw it every minute of every day and it was ripe for use as a cultural icon,
eventually working against the very purpose for which it was constructed. It
was, we might say, its own worst enemy.
The storyboard shown in Figure 2.5 enables you, I hope, to see how, with the
literature, you can go from a loose collection of ideas to a starting point for sin-
gularity and specialness to, eventually, an exploration of that specialness, that
38 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
uniqueness, as a case study. I describe the nuts and bolts of drawing a storyboard
in Chapter 11.
Single
Multiple
Outlier Intrinsic Testing a theory • Nested
Key Instrumental Building a theory • Parallel
Local Evaluative Drawing a picture • Sequential
Explanatory Descriptive • Retrospective
Exploratory Interpretative • Snapshot
• Diachronic
Figure 2.6 Mapping out the design for the case study ‘The role of popular culture in the
fall of the Berlin Wall’
CASE STUDY AND RESEARCH DESIGN 39
(Continued)
40 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
This assumption about normal distribution is all very well for height and
weight, which are unproblematically measured, but – as I’ve been at pains
CASE STUDY AND RESEARCH DESIGN 41
(Continued)
42 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Omaar’s work was multilayered, serving different purposes and taking differ-
ent approaches. Its route through the typology I outlined in Chapter 1 is given in
Figure 2.8. Given his starting point – about the provenance and place of the notion
of intelligence – he had to conduct contextualisation via the relevant literature to
build a potential theory which he would then test using his case study. The theo-
retical position which he built, on the basis of the work of Flynn and others, was
that the idea of intelligence had been misleading: it had led to the belief that it was
genetically fixed and that there was little that schools (or other cultural institutions)
could do to counter its effects. His object was to throw light on this. His identifica-
tion of a subject therefore rested on the ability of that subject to contradict standard
explanations resting in intelligence. That identification came in the discovery of
Hostos-Lincoln Academy, an outlier to the general rule that schools succeed in rela-
tion to their student intake. Not only was a theory developed and tested by the case
study, but it also served as a powerful illustration for Omaar’s purposes as a researcher
and a journalist. The study was a single one, taking a snapshot of the Academy as
44 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Omaar visited it and also looking at its history, its mission and its records and at the
official statistics of its environs and the other local schools – looking at it in other
words retrospectively. But there were multiple and nested elements to this in the
particular focus on certain of the school’s students. The methods Omaar chose to
use included interview, observation and examination of local, national and school
records. All of this is summarised in Figure 2.8.
Methodological
Intrinsic Testing a theory
choices
Object
Figure 2.8 The organisational structure of Omaar’s case study of the Hostos-Lincoln
Academy
Your question, and the analytical frame associated with it, will evolve, as Omaar’s
did. It will progress from a prima facie question to a final question. On the way from
A to B it may become transformed as you read, conduct pilot work and develop new
ideas. This process is not unique to case study – it is there in all investigative work in
any field. Einstein famously did it using his ‘thought experiments’ (Norton, 2004).
Becker et al. (1961/1980: 33) describe the way that one relatively simple question
led to another, more complex and analytical one in their classic case study on the
training of medical students, Boys in White:
We had first the descriptive question: How much effort did students put
forth and in what directions? Then we had the analytic question: Why
were the students’ level and direction of effort what they were and not
any of the other things they might have been?
to different kinds of research. I like to think of these questions being of four basic
kinds:
• action research
• case study
• comparative research
• evaluation
• experiment.
46 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Let’s also consider what they are good for, because they all have their advantages
and disadvantages. Each is better or worse when focusing on different questions. For
example, if you are concerned with one of the ‘What happens when …?’ questions –
that is, if you are looking to see whether or not X causes Y – you will probably want
to use some kind of experiment. If you want to discover whether or not some new
method of management has had an impact on industrial relations in a particular
factory, you will embark on an evaluation. If you want to improve your practice,
doing this through research, you will set off on a piece of action research. If you
want to compare phenomena in different countries, you will embark on comparative
research. It’s horses for courses. Let’s match up some of the design frames to kinds of
questions and kinds of study.
You will note from Table 2.1 that certain kinds of design frame, such as the experi-
ment, are limited to one kind of question (does X cause Y?), while others, such as the
evaluation, are appropriate for more than one kind of question. Others, such as the case
study and action research, may cover all of the kinds of questions that I have outlined.
This is because a case study is defined by ‘singleness’ rather than procedure. There
are no procedures that are associated exclusively with the case study, for, in its
examination of the singular, the one thing, it is ‘allowed’ to use whatever proce-
dures and methods it wishes in doing this. The emphasis when doing a case study
is on the singleness. In other words, the case study is not a method, nor is it a set of
procedures. Rather, it is a focus.
CASE STUDY AND RESEARCH DESIGN 47
• Remember that case study is not a method. Rather it is a design frame – a scaf-
fold for your research. Within this frame, this scaffold, you can use a wide range of
research methods.
• When building your case study, always keep in mind the need to establish a storyline.
The key is to draw rich, interconnected information from your singular focus and derive
unique insights from the analysis that follows. You can use storyboards to help you do
this – to move from what might be a loose collection of ideas to a clear narrative.
REFLECTIVE ACTIVITY
Read again the boxed case study in this chapter of race and school achievement
conducted by Rageh Omaar. Omaar conducted his case study by examining the
outlier case of an outstanding school that bucked the trend of worse school per-
formance with black and Hispanic students.
Starting with the same prima facie question as Omaar about the worse
school performance of children from some ethnic minorities, outline a different
case study design that might effectively address the same research question.
Alternatively, outline a case study design about social mobility more generally.
Use the typology from Chapter 1 to help you think about structure. Tips: You
might choose to look at a child, class or school that you know. You might look
at the work of a research organisation such as the Sutton Trust, which focuses
on social mobility, and identify a subject such as the composition of the current
government in terms of the educational histories of its members.
Further Reading
Ball, S. (1981) Beachside Comprehensive: A Case-Study of Secondary Schooling.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lacey, C. (1970) Hightown Grammar. Manchester: Manchester University Press.
These are classic examples of the case study. Each gives a detailed analysis of what
goes on in one school over a period of time. They exemplify the kinds of very
diverse data gathering that can be done in a case study and the ways in which the
case study work can be linked intelligently to national policy.
Becker, H.S., Geer, B., Hughes, E.C. and Strauss, A.L. (1961/1980) Boys in White.
Edison, NJ: Transaction Publishers.
Another classic. This is about American medical students and the way that they
work. It is a good read and tells you as much about the process of the case study as
it does about the training of the medical students.
3
THE WHOLE IS MORE
THAN THE SUM OF
THE PARTS: SEEING A
COMPLETE PICTURE
This chapter offers an intellectual history of the case study – a pedigree, showing
its scholarly heritage. This may not help much with the nuts and bolts of doing
your case study research, but it will help, I hope, in understanding where the case
study has come from and where it currently stands as an approach to inquiry. You
need to know this if you want to understand why the case study is appropriate and
meaningful for addressing a research question.
Looking at the whole represents something of a new – or at least a reawakened –
worldview. It is one that emerged during the twentieth century with the purpose of
understanding phenomena not as so many disconnected parts but, rather, as intercon-
nected elements. Anyone wanting to do a case study implicitly shares this worldview,
though it should be said that the case study does not boast an intellectual allegiance
to any one strand of thought but many. I shall review some of these in this chapter.
The wish to see things more as wholes does present a number of challenges,
however, since the thrust of most scientific inquiry since the Enlightenment (that
is, since the beginnings of rational and scientific thought in the mid-seventeenth
century) has been about breaking things down. It has been about reducing them
and understanding them in terms of their constituent parts. It has, in other words,
been about reduction, so is known to philosophers as reductionism.
Reductionism has done wonders in
the natural sciences and the technol-
A H E A V Y W E I G H T C O N T E S T ogy that springs from it – people on
REDUCTIONISM
RONNIE ‘THE REDUCER’ the Moon, refrigeration, antibiotics,
computers, to name but … er … four.
Viewing the world from this position,
V phenomena are nothing more than
HO M
HARRY ‘WHOLESOME’ the sum of their parts.
The issue about whether or not reductionism is of value is as old as the hills –
the Athenian hills. Plato, Socrates and Aristotle had major disagreements about it.
Plato thought that universal truths developed from generalisation were what we
should all be searching for if we want true knowledge. He says, ‘I am looking for
the simile in multis.’ He is looking, in other words for the essence that captures the
truth evinced by many cases – for example, I’ve only ever seen white swans, so all
swans must be white.
On the other side, Aristotle averred that we progress only by using our practical
reasoning, craft knowledge or tacit knowing – the stuff we know because of our
experience. It is the stuff we learn ‘on the job’ or by simply being alive and it is
explicable only in terms of the particular – the case. It cannot be communicated by
reducing it to general principles. ‘You are wrong, Socrates and Plato,’ says Aristotle,
‘to dismiss the value of cases in the production of knowledge.’
The arguments on this theme continue to the present day. They proceeded
through Base Camp 1 of the Renaissance, with Francis Bacon – the first scientist,
some would say – averring that the generalisers and regularisers, the followers of
Plato, ‘hasted to their theories and dogmaticals, and were imperious and scornful
toward particulars’. Today, the argument has unfortunately come to be known as
the ‘paradigm wars’ (Oakley, 1999) and shows little sign of abating. Any argument
that goes on for 2500 years has clearly got legs and we are not going to resolve it
any time soon.
It was Plato whose ideas won (broadly speaking) and whose thinking took hold
so successfully for the natural sciences. This was a big defeat and a big loss for
our ways of understanding, asserts an astute commentator on these issues, Bent
Flyvbjerg (2001). Flyvbjerg says that the mistake led humankind on two millennia
of false starts in understanding social phenomena. The mistake was in the failure
to distinguish between different kinds of inquiry for different purposes and it leads
us, in extremis, to the absurd position that it is inappropriate to argue or learn from
particular examples for fear that this might be thought ‘anecdotal’ and, therefore,
unscientific.
The distinction, then, has been on a long journey intellectually. In essence, it is
about the relevance of particular individual events to a larger picture and the extent
to which we can use separate events to establish laws or theories by which we can
explain and predict. Some say that it is legitimate to generalise in the natural sciences,
but not in the social sciences. In the former, that is physics and chemistry, the stabil-
ity of the events studied is such that laws and theories can be worked out in a way
that they will prove useful for explanation and prediction. In the human sciences,
however, events follow such random twists and turns that any attempt at establish-
ing stable laws and theories that will reliably explain and predict is meaningless.
52 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
The classic argument here has been posed by the philosopher Alasdair MacIntyre
(1985) in his book After Virtue. If you are interested, read Chapter 8 in that book,
‘The character of generalizations in social sci-
Holism is seeing things as a whole. ence and their lack of predictive power’. It is
(Why hasn’t it got a ‘w’? Don’t ask as entertaining as it is enlightening.
me.) There is an assumption that
the whole is more than the sum of
I’ve said that this argument about reduction-
its parts. It is about seeing things in ism versus holism has been going on for 2500
their entirety, their totality. years and reductionism has certainly won as
far as the main method used for the natural
sciences is concerned. In fact, it is only in the last 100 years or so that protagonists of
Aristotle’s views have come to the fore again in thinking about the human sciences.
Earlier, right at the beginning of the book, I mentioned the idiograph – a draw-
ing or figure that stands not for a sound, as do symbols in the Roman alphabet, but
for an idea. It was the philosopher Wilhelm Windelband who, at the cusp of the
nineteenth and twentieth centuries, drew on the notion of the idiograph to sepa-
rate what he called the ‘idiographic’ and the ‘nomothetic’ (from the Greek nomos,
meaning ‘law’) in social inquiry.
I personally think that it would have made life easier if Windelband had called
the ‘ideographic’ the ‘pictographic’ since ‘pictograph’ is a perfectly acceptable alter-
native to ‘idiograph’ and it more straightforwardly explains what he was trying to
get at. A pictogram is a small picture that has become stylised, so that it represents
the thing in written form. The Chinese pictogram for ‘person’, for example, looks
a little like a matchstick person, with a trunk and two ‘legs’. (Interestingly, these
picture representations eventually take on sound-like representations as well, devel-
oping into our modern system of alphabetic writing.)
With the idiographic, the approach is to specify and study individual phenomena
in detail – we have a ‘picture’ in front of us. With the nomothetic, by contrast, the
approach is to generalise from many cases and derive laws from such generalisation.
Windelband was not the first to notice this difference but, as is so often the case
in the history of ideas, he encapsulated something nicely in his separation of the idi-
ographic from the nomothetic and the distinction he drew has stuck. It has proved
enormously helpful in summarising the differences between these kinds of inquiry.
Gestalt psychology
Not long after Windelband, one interesting avenue down which this discussion
travelled was that of Gestalt psychology early in the twentieth century. This was
key for the case study since the essence of Gestalt thinking is that things should be
SEEING A COMPLETE PICTURE 53
seen in their totality, as we try to do in a case study. The key idea to emerge here
was that the mind works not by perceiving things in isolation, separately, but,
rather, as wholes – as integrated units. We humans, alone among animals, invest
meaning in what appear to be unrelated phenomena and make patterns, make
sense, out of these. It is almost as if our minds are patternmaking or sensemaking
machines.
The key figures here – psychologists such as Max Wertheimer and Wolfgang
Köhler (and, later, the great social psychologist Kurt Lewin, the inventor of action
research) – suggested that the methods psychologists use to study psychological
phenomena distort our understanding of them. By always reducing, atomising the
subject matter of psychology into constituent variables, the end result is a mis-
understanding of the way the mind works, they said. The mind works not in the
convenient building blocks of psychological science – variables such as gender, age,
reaction speed, time, class – but wholes.
It is certainly true that Homo sapi-
ens is a pattern-finding animal. The
fertility of our brains in divining
patterns is boundless – it requires
only the mildest encouragement
for shape and form to crystallise
out of what may seem at first to be
chaotic stimuli. Look at Figure 3.2.
After looking for a while, you will
see a Dalmatian dog. (Can’t see it?
Its nose is in the middle of the pic-
ture and its backside towards the
top right.) In fact, perhaps danger-
Figure 3.2 We have a wonderful capacity for
ously for certain kinds of inquiry, seeing shapes and making meaning
we even seem to be predisposed to
preferring to see form where there is none rather than not see any form at all.
The philosopher Karl Popper suggested that this facility may be inborn. It may be
due, he proposed (1977: 270), to ‘mechanisms which make us search for regulari-
ties’ and be responsible for what he called ‘the dogmatic [ouch] way of thinking’. In
a similar vein, Barrow (1997) points out that the human brain has an evolutionary
imperative to over-see patterns – that is, occasionally see patterns where they do not
exist (as ‘false positives’, for instance ‘seeing’ slender shadows as tiger stripes) – as
this has historically given us an evolutionary advantage over the opposite state of
not recognising patterns where they in fact exist (where the slender shadows are
actually the stripes of a tiger).
54 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
The bottom line is that we must be cautious about this precious ability to see
pattern since, today, harbouring a predilection to make shape and theory out of
that which is shapeless may not be quite so useful. We may overdo generalisation,
tending to generalise from insufficient information. It is this tendency that has to
be guarded against in all of the sampling procedures that have been developed with
what has been called probabilistic research.
Case study’s potential for offering broad generalisations may be limited. However,
in a case study you can use your ability to put things together, to draw from expe-
rience, make informed judgements about cause and effect in this particular case.
It is what the human brain is good at. It is what got us out of the trees. It is what
Newton, Darwin and Einstein did. We should not be ashamed of it. It is about con-
necting ideas, like the ganglia between neurons.
There are a number of superstructures that have been used to accommodate
wholeness, intellectually speaking, and I shall look at these in the sections that
follow.
Shakespeare sums up here several streams of thought about how the world – the
natural as well as the social world – should be seen. So, it is worth saying a little
more about the stage analogy.
In fact, the As You Like It quotation could perhaps be the motto of the case
researcher because the ways of thinking about social situations that I am going to
review in this section are all characterised by seeing the person in context, where
the action is defined by interactions between people and situations, which, of
course, is the sine qua non of case study research. Each person is affected by the
environment around them: no one is an island.
Interpretivists say that there is no There is no better place to begin to explore
‘objective’ social world ‘out there’. this idea than with the work of sociologist
Rather, it is constructed differently
Erving Goffman, who actually called his view
by each person in each situation they
face, so it is useful sometimes to see of social life dramaturgy. In the dictionary defi-
the world as a stage on which we nition, ‘dramaturgy’ is the art of writing and
play out characters.
producing plays and, for Goffman, life is a set
SEEING A COMPLETE PICTURE 55
of dramatic performances in which people take on roles and change the way they
behave depending on their interactions with other actors. I am a different person
with different folk. I take on a different character with my sister Adrienne from the
one I take on with my friend Peter, from the one I take on with my boss Edward.
The core perhaps is the same, but if ever I were to be faced with the three of them
in one room (heaven forbid) I would suffer from an acute case of triple personal-
ity implosion syndrome – or PIS3, as it is known in the psychiatric literature (that’s
meant to be a joke, by the way).
Goffman differentiated between the kinds of situations in which such performances
are necessary, noting that there is a ‘front stage’, where the ‘act’ is always used in order
to ‘impression-manage’, and a ‘backstage’, where people can be more ‘themselves’.
Goffman’s dramaturgical perspective was itself an offshoot, an important one, of a
branch of sociology known as symbolic interactionism. Here is how one of symbolic
interactionism’s main proponents, Herbert Blumer (1992: 82), presents what he calls
the ‘basic premises’ and ‘methodological consequences’ of this school of sociology:
Dilthey came up with the idea of Verstehen (meaning ‘understanding’), more ade-
quately to demarcate between the stuff of the natural and the human sciences and
our ways of inquiring into them. We cannot, he said, ignore the historical context
and the context of meaning in all that human beings do and say.
Ecological psychology
Another manifestation of the need for wholeness is seen in ecological psychology.
In its ‘green’ sense, ecology refers to the relationships between living things in the
environments that they inhabit. It is the study of the complex interrelationships
that take place in those environments.
A starting point here is that the lives of organisms inhabiting these environments
are inextricably intertwined with the nature and quality of those environments.
Changes in any small aspect of the environment will have knock-on effects that will
affect the lives of the organisms populating it. An important aspect of this is that
there will be an equilibrium in this environment and it will be maintained. If foxes
become blighted, their main meal, rabbits will breed like … er … rabbits, with few
predators, so the remaining foxes do well, eating the superfluity of rabbits, so then
rabbits decline, causing foxes, in turn, to decline, then rabbits breed … and so the
cycle goes on (see Figure 3.3). Thus, the equilibrium is maintained, and the same hap-
pens in the environments (family, school, factory) that we inhabit as people.
So, ecological psychologists would say, people – that is, humans, persons, Homo
sapiens – live in a multicoloured world. It is an ecology where an increase in one
stimulus invokes not an automatic reflex or a tropism, but a thought-out reaction
or an overreaction.
By using the ecological metaphor you can see the relevance for inquiry in social
science: it is not as if you could set up a meaningful research study to assess the
effect of turning up the volume of music on the volume of people’s conversation.
If you got some volunteers conversing in a room where rock music is playing qui-
etly and start turning up the volume, you could measure the effect on the volume
of the volunteers’ voices. I guess that you would find that the volunteers’ voice
levels rose (much psychological experimentation is of this kind). An ecological
psychologist would say, however, that the real world is not like this. In the real
world, people would just leave the room to talk or someone would go and turn the
volume down. Then someone would go and turn it up again and perhaps there
would be an argument, then maybe a fight, then someone would be taken to hos-
pital, and so on.
SEEING A COMPLETE PICTURE 57
Numbers
Time
Rabbits Foxes
The real world, then, is complex, and it is the real world in which we are inter-
ested. The understanding that ecological psychology offers is that the way people
behave in a psychological laboratory, with its tight control of a small number of
variables, does not remotely represent the way people behave in the real world.
The basic idea is the same as the one behind symbolic interactionism, which is
that no behaviour occurs in a vacuum; it occurs in the context of others’ activity, in
the context of the family, place of work, school or community and in the context
of the linguistic, cultural, legal and physical environments. Because of the way that
psychology and its inquiries typically proceed – by isolating variables, controlling
important ones and manipulating one or two – we can get into a rut of thinking
that they, using these controls and manipulations, are valid representations of what
happens in our everyday lives. We come to think that the world in which we live
is really like this – like a psychological laboratory with a simple set of variables that
are all more or less inert in their relationships with one another.
The traditional focus on the individual in psychological endeavour permeates
the applied field, where interventions also often take place in a vacuum, with scant
consideration of the physical and social environments within which individuals
learn and behave. The ecological metaphor used by some psychologists since the
mid-twentieth century enables an understanding that things are not that simple.
Behaviours may emerge in an environment that are a consequence of conditions
outside the narrow ambit of the individual. Changing one or two features of this
system may have unanticipated consequences or may have no effect at all.
58 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
In the current context, he came up with an idea that he called ‘field theory’,
which is linked as much to Gestalt psychology as it is to ecological psychology, but
I’ll mention it here. Lewin (1951; 1997) suggested that the ‘field’ of an individual’s
psyche comprises a range of social and psychological factors and changes with the
person’s experience. An alternative term he uses for fields is ‘lifespaces’ – home, job,
school, sports club – that we occupy at different times and in different ways. All are
joined by ‘vectors’ that operate at different levels of strength.
Lewin, with his mercurial (and maybe slightly manic) mind, perhaps tried to
make things too mathematically precise, but with field theory he offered another
stratum to the developing bundle of ideas about holism.
One of the main proponents of ecological thinking was Urie Bronfenbrenner,
but since he developed his model from a hybrid of ecological and systems thinking
(covered next), I shall deal with the latter before going on to talk about his ideas in
more detail.
Systems thinking
Systems thinking is a way of viewing the world that lends itself naturally to the case
study since it aims to see the system as a whole, in much the same way that you
aim to do in a case inquiry. The difference is that systems theory and its various
branches are bounded by some quite firm frameworks about viewing the interrela-
tionships that exist.
With the case study, there are no methodological prescriptions. By contrast, sys-
tems theory proffers methodological tools and route maps to assist analysis. It is a
particular way of studying the whole, unlike the case study. That is not to say you
cannot use the methods of systems thinking in a case study – you can, and they
may prove to be very useful for structuring your thinking. For this reason, I go into
some detail on a branch of systems thinking in Chapter 11.
Systems theory was developed in a variety of contexts, from biology to engineer-
ing, and thus can be relevant for a range of foci and adapted for use in each of these.
If your interests are in engineering or biology or an applied science, or if you are
interested in the interrelationship between one of these and the social world, it may
well be worth your exploring aspects of systems theory.
The models for understanding that have been advanced in systems thinking
were developed specifically to address the need for holism in getting to grips with
complex dynamics. As with the parallel trains of thought in psychology and sociol-
ogy that I have just looked at, the finger is pointed at the barrenness of attempts
to understand complex behaviour by reductionist methods. Our understanding,
60 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
systems theorists say, has to push that complexity to the front: we have to try to
understand the ability of the complex system to adapt, change and ‘learn’ from
experience. Systems theory used ideas from biology, such as negative entropy (the
ability of a system to make sense, re-form itself and maintain an equilibrium), and
from mechanics, such as feedback (the effect of a signal going back into a loop and
magnifying itself each time it returns). These are characteristics of systems that are
not seen in individual parts of the system.
One of the first proponents of systems theory was Ludwig von Bertalanffy (1950) with
his general systems theory. At the risk of getting repetitive, the same theme emerges – one
of complex interactions – though von Bertalanffy did not leave it at that.
He made an important distinction between what he called open systems and
closed systems. A closed system is one such as an engineering system (think of a
diesel engine) that has very little in the way of a change to its movement once it is
working, aside perhaps from a movement of the throttle by the operator. By con-
trast, an open system is one such as a biological system (think of any living thing,
even a bacterium) that seeks to maintain itself in equilibrium by responding to its
environment.
Apart from features such as feedback and negative entropy, von Bertalanffy spoke
about features such as isomorphism (that is, being the same shape) between biologi-
cal and social systems. So, a beehive could be seen (from some points of view) as
being a bit like a town.
His thinking led naturally to interdisciplinarity, with insights from one field pro-
viding clues for thinking about how things work in another. In fact, von Bertalanffy
expected to see more of an integration of the work of science – natural and social.
Sadly, there is not a huge amount of evidence that this is occurring today, despite
attempts to promote it by all and sundry. He said that the human sciences were too
often atomistic, they were a kind of ‘social physics’ of the worst kind.
Beyond all of this, systems theory has been taken forward by others in some
interesting ways that may be quite useful to the case researcher. One of these is the
development of soft systems theory, expounded by Peter Checkland (1981) and his
colleagues at Lancaster University.
Checkland tried to incorporate the basic tenets of systems thinking while offer-
ing a framework for seeing the ways that the elements interconnect. He thus
advanced a model that was simpler than the one promoted by Barker (see page 58),
but still offered a framework of a more holistic understanding of social environ-
ments. Developing his ‘soft systems’ within the context of industrial psychology,
he suggested that the matters with which we are concerned are complex wholes
that maintain themselves in a changing environment. They do this by means of
adaptation and control action.
SEEING A COMPLETE PICTURE 61
Although the concepts used in Checkland’s model are borrowed from biology
and engineering, he does not explicitly use the ecological metaphor, but his aims
are congruent with those of the ecological psychologists. He suggests a model that
draws a distinction between the ‘real world’ and ‘systems thinking’ and forces an
analysis of both. He asserts that the ‘soft, messy problems of human beings in their
everyday lives’ are not amenable to the methods of the ‘hard’ systems tradition of
engineering. He suggests that the hard systems tradition can handle natural (that is,
biological) systems and designed systems (such as bicycles, computers, mathemat-
ics), but cannot cope in situations dominated by human perceptions. He calls the
latter ‘human activity systems’.
His soft systems method suggests a sequence of events and processes for ana-
lysing social settings. I describe this in detail in Chapter 11, which is on forms
of analysis.
Community Workplace
Individual
Microsystem
Mesosystem
Exosystem
Macrosystem
and led to ‘the science of strange behaviour of children in strange situations with
strange adults for the briefest possible periods of time’ (1979: 19). The links with
symbolic interactionism are manifest. There is nothing new in the world.
Present day
Holistic views
Ecological
Dramaturgy systems theory
(Goffman) (Bronfenbrenner)
Systems 1950
Field theory thinking (von
Symbolic (Lewin) Bertalanffy)
interactionism Ecological psychology
(Blumer) (Barker, J.J. Gibson)
Phenomenology 1900
(Husserl)
Separating idiographic
Verstehen, or from nomothetic
understanding (Windelband)
(Dilthey)
• At the heart of the rationale for case study is the assumption that holism provides
an effective and fruitful way of addressing many of the questions that are posed in
social inquiry.
• Several streams of thought have emerged from a holist understanding: Gestalt
psychology, dramaturgy, ecological psychology, systems thinking. Throughout the
twentieth century such perspectives gained respectability as it was recognised
that they provided the rationale for highly productive methods of social and psy-
chological inquiry.
• Figure 3.5 summarises some of the schools of thought that have led to today’s
more eclectic attitude to inquiry. It is an attitude that accepts the relevance and
utility of the idiographic and provides ways of addressing wholeness by emphasis-
ing rather than denying the interconnectedness of the strands of psychological
and social life.
REFLECTIVE ACTIVITY
In the discussion of methodological holism, I have given as an example in this
chapter the insights taken by psychology from ecology. I noted that one of the
features of the ecological insight is that in a complex system nothing remains
constant: as one thing increases, others decline. How might this insight prove
useful in examining and analysing …
Further Reading
Barker, R.G. (1968) Ecological Psychology. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press.
One of the most important pioneers in the movement to make psychology more
responsive to the whole, Barker insists that behaviour is always situated – in other
words, it does not exist in a vacuum. A useful platform for the case study.
SEEING A COMPLETE PICTURE 65
Becker, H. (2014) What About Mozart? What About Murder? Reasoning from Cases.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
An excellent discussion of what case study is, and what it can do.
Burgess, E.W. (1927) ‘Statistics and case studies as methods of sociological research’,
Sociology and Social Research, 12: 103–20.
A classic article that presages many of today’s debates about the distinctions between
research involving small and large numbers.
Flyvbjerg, B. (2001) Making Social Science Matter: Why Social Inquiry Fails and How It
Can Succeed Again. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
See especially Chapter 6, ‘The power of example’, for a comparison of case study
with methods that seek to establish generalisation.
of all the people in whom you might be interested is called the population, and the
sample is a slice or selection drawn from this population).
You might, for example, be interested in alcohol consumption among students
nationally. Clearly, it would be impracticable to issue a questionnaire to each and
every student in the country (the population) so you have to find a way of getting
a representative sample of the students to whom you are able to gain access. This
smaller sample from the whole population is intended to represent this population
and, clearly, the bigger the sample, the better the chance of representing it accurately.
You can, of course, imagine all of the things that can go wrong in sampling of
this kind. A sample from one university is unlikely to be representative of all uni-
versities; a sample from one group, such as your lecture class, even less likely; and
11 students in the bar on Tuesday night even more unlikely accurately to provide
a representative sample of the wider population – especially if you are interested in
students’ more usual drinking habits.
I said ‘the bigger the better’ for a sample, but there are ways of sacrificing size by
doing clever things such as ‘stratifying’, which means making sure that you get the
important parts of the population (by sex, age, type of institution attended, and so
on) properly represented in your small sample.
Now, none of this is relevant to a case study. The reason why I have devoted
several precious paragraphs to it is to try and hammer home the point that those
‘samples’ and all of the assumptions behind them are not necessary for a case study.
In fact, ‘sample’ is the wrong word for what is wanted in a case study. In textbooks,
to separate out the representative kind of sample (of which I have just given an
example concerning alcohol and students) from all others, the term ‘sample’ is
sometimes divided into probabilistic samples and non-probabilistic samples.
I personally think, however, that to make such a distinction is confusing. In a case
study, the choice that you make regarding your subject is nothing like a sample,
probabilistic or non-probabilistic. In fact, with any research, not just a case study, I
would go so far as to say that there is no such thing as a non-probabilistic sample –
or, at least, there might be, but we could just as easily and with less confusion call
it a ‘bad sample’ or, more generously, a ‘quick and dirty sample’. A sample has to be
‘a portion that shows the quality of the whole’,
in the dictionary definition. If it is not that – if
it does not accurately show the quality of the A sample should show the quality of
the whole. A ‘sample’ is not what is
whole to you as a researcher – it is at best a con- wanted in a case study. (Yes, ‘not’.)
venience, purposive or pilot sample.
The point of a case study is not to find a portion that shows the quality of the
whole. You are looking at your selection of subject – a marriage, country or what-
ever, with one, two or a few being focused on – without any expectation that it
represents a wider population. So, it is not a sample; it is a choice, a selection. It is
68 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
this selection that is vitally important for your study. In Chapter 6 I examine the way
in which you can make that selection, based on whether it is …
achieve (and would never want to achieve) findings that are consistent from one
time to another or one researcher to another.
Validity is similar to reliability when it comes to making judgements about its
value in case study research. Everything that I have said about reliability applies
also to validity. In many kinds of research – in those, for example, where probability
samples are used – validity is about the extent to which a piece of research is find-
ing out what the researcher intends it to find out. (It is not quite as simple as this,
and I tease apart some of the ways in which the word validity is used in How to Do
Your Research Project.) In the kind of research we are looking at here, however, where
there is no probability sample and we may have no idea at all about what we expect
to find out from the research, the idea of validity is less meaningful.
This is generally recognised in discussion about the quality of some kinds of
interpretative research, but, despite the concerns about the limited appropriateness
of validity, it is still felt by some people to be terribly important. There is almost a
cult of what Schwandt (1996) calls ‘criteriology’ – in other words, an obsession with
finding criteria. A treatise on methodology is felt to be naked without a discussion
of validity and criteria that can then be ticked off as having been (or not having
been) achieved. Validity is thus wrenched out of its home in normative research
with its samples, variables and statistics and bent and twisted into something quite
different for the purposes of interpretative research. The result is that we end up
bashing our square peg, validity, into the round hole of case study research.
For example, Whittemore et al. (2001), after having reviewed a range of authors
who have written about validity in interpretative research, emerge with a panoply
of criteria for assessing it. These include plausibility, relevance, credibility, com-
prehensiveness, significance, confirmability, positionality, canons of evidence,
generalisability, fittingness and auditability.
Now, I know that people read books like this one having got to the relevant sec-
tion via the index and do not contextualise what they have found. So, if this is you
and you are tempted to copy these words down (plausibility, relevance and so on)
and offer them in your methodology chapters as criteria for validity in case study
research, let me repeat that I do not agree with them. Validity is about that which
makes the research valid. It is not about plausibility, credibility and all the others.
Looking at the criteria with which Whittemore et al. emerge from their review, I
could say that some of the most fascinating and ground-breaking pieces of research
would have failed all of the tests of validity above. Let’s look at one – Einstein’s
work leading up to his theory of relativity. Would it have met many of the criteria
collated by Whittemore et al.? No. Let’s look at some of those criteria. With the
criteria for validity outlined in Table 4.1, an iconic piece of inquiry, key to the
development of late twentieth-century science, fails on nearly all counts. We have
70 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
to be wary of assessing work on the basis of these crude, post hoc constructions.
They are the tools of an audit society, not of good research.
I have deliberately chosen Einstein’s ground-breaking work in the natural sci-
ences, because it is a clear case of highly significant work – done, interestingly, via
‘thought experiments’ and case studies rather than the conventional experimenta-
tion of natural science – that conforms to none of the maxims of methodological
correctness. I could have turned in the other direction and chosen work that does
conform to the criteria of Whittemore et al., but which was … well, worthless. I
could quote work such as that of the educational psychologist Cyril Burt, who has
been shown to have fabricated his results (Hearnshaw, 1979: 245–7), but which, at
the time of publication, seemed to conform to all of the conventional measures of
validity.
Table 4.1 Did Einstein’s work have validity? Or 10 ways of not assessing validity
In fact, Burt’s work amply meets the expectations of plausibility and credibility that
appear in the list from Whittemore et al. It is plausibility and credibility (the first
two criteria in Table 4.1) that are usually to the fore when validity is discussed in the
RIGOUR AND QUALITY 71
social sciences in the context of interpretative work. For me, however, these raise
particular problems as indices of any kind of validity, whether we are thinking in a
technical or even an everyday sense of the word.
Plausibility (in fact, surely much the same as credibility) seems to me a strange way
to assess validity. It is placing responsibility in the hearts and minds of the scholarly
community (you and me) to make that assessment of plausibility, but our views of
what is plausible will depend on the worldviews that have shaped our own thinking.
Our thoughts and ideas are slaves to what we already know and believe (well, mine
are anyway). The case of Burt is especially instructive because it is clear that he lied
and fabricated evidence to match with ideas about the heritability of intelligence,
which he ‘knew’ to be true. Here is the important point: his conclusions were ulti-
mately embraced because they were plausible. That is why they were accepted.
Researchers’ audiences (you and me again) – as judges of plausibility – cannot
be insulated from the prevailing academic, cultural and scientific orthodoxies
in making their judgements. I wonder what is supposed to inoculate us against
employing, in any assessment we make of plausibility, the very prejudices and
expectations that give shape to plausibility. It was this plausibility that made
Burt’s views acceptable to his intellectual peers. Plausibility was the culprit, not
the knight in shining armour.
That was a long way of saying that I don’t think you need to spend a great deal
of time worrying about validity and reliability in case study research as a form of
inquiry. This is partly also because the case study is not one single kind of research,
for it encompasses component methods. In certain of those component methods you
will certainly want to ensure that you pay attention to reliability and validity, but
remember that there is always a choice involved. You have to look at the constituent
bits of your case study and ask yourself whether or not reliability and validity, in fact,
are appropriate issues. Overall, though, the case study, as a study of one thing, is not
the kind of inquiry in which considerations about validity and reliability should be
to the fore since it is the singleness of the subject and the singleness – the peculiarity,
even – of the interpretation and analysis of the evidence that is significant.
Triangulation
The term ‘triangulation’ is used in a metaphorical way in the social sciences, based
on its original use in geometry and surveying. Triangles do not actually need to
be involved, nor do things need to be done in threes. What the term means here
is that viewing from several points is better than viewing from one. Given the
critical awareness that should be the hallmark of good social science researchers,
72 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Interviews
Observation
Case study
subject Focus group
Shadowing
Diaries
Research methodologist Norman Denzin (2017) would call this use of different
methods in research methodological triangulation. Denzin argued, though, that tri-
angulation goes beyond the use of different methods, and he outlined several other
types of triangulation, including investigator triangulation where more than one per-
son is involved in interpretation and analysis, and theory triangulation where more
than one theoretical framework might be involved in its interpretation. I would
add to Denzin’s categories design frame triangulation. In other words, you would be
triangulating if you used both a case study and a longitudinal study together in the
same piece of research.
I discuss triangulation further in How to Do Your Research Project (Thomas, 2017).
RIGOUR AND QUALITY 73
Positionality
Oftentimes in case study you will be immersed in the subject and the situation of
your research. You become an active, not passive, agent in acquiring knowledge of
the processes, histories, events, language and biographies of the research context.
You yourself will take a central role in interpretation. In doing this, it is important
to recognise that you have an undeniable position, and this position affects the
nature of the observations and the interpretations you make.
Recognition of this involves an inversion of some of the shibboleths of tradi-
tional methodology, such as the supposed imperative to ‘objectivity’. There is no
attempt to be ‘objective’ when working interpretatively, but rather an acceptance
of the importance of researchers themselves – their likes and dislikes, their back-
grounds and their pastimes, their vested interests and expectations – in making
interpretations. It is subjectivity, in other words, rather than objectivity, that comes
to the fore. Because of the importance of the nature of the relation between the
researcher and research participants, the researcher’s biography – including class,
gender, ethnicity, ideas and commitments – needs to be made explicit.
In presenting an interpretative case study you should accept your subjectivity
and not be ashamed or afraid of it. Given that it is central, your case study should
be written up in a way that makes clear your understanding of the underpinning
principles that guide the conduct of this kind of research. An interpretative case
study will therefore be reported in an entirely different way from an experimental
study. You should begin – right at the beginning, in the introduction – with a full
discussion of positionality: of yourself, why you are interested in this topic, what
your personal circumstances are, and so on. You will always write in the first person,
saying, for example, ‘I believe that …’ rather than ‘The researcher believes that …’.
This may seem obvious, but it is a common mistake for students conducting inter-
pretative research – research that demands that they put themselves at the centre of
the analysis – to report their work as if they have just conducted an experiment and
to omit any information about themselves. If you do this kind of research, readers
need to know who you are and where you stand, metaphorically as well as literally.
Generalisation
The case study has often been taken to be deficient in the generalisation department,
as I discussed in Chapter 1. But its ‘poor relation’ status exists, I think, only because
it is conspicuously deficient in its potential for generalisation. Its weaknesses in terms
of generalisation, in other words, are not disguised. Other design frames in social
74 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
science seek ways to calibrate and enable generalisation, but these attempts rest on
something of a sleight of hand in the presentation of what generalisation can be.
If, as many have argued (and I shall rehearse the classic case in a moment), little
significant generalisation at all is possible in the human sciences, arguments about
the case study’s deficiencies in this department lose force. My point is that, when
thinking about inference, generalisation in social inquiry is constrained to remain
at the level of what Peirce (Houser et al., 1992) calls ‘abduction’ rather than induc-
tion. I shall explain what abduction is in a moment.
If my argument has validity, then there are forms of interpretation that can
come from case studies which owe their legitimacy and power to the exemplary
knowledge of these studies rather than to their generalisability. For me, seeking
generalisability – seeing generalisation as the first and most important aim of social
science – can inhibit or even extinguish the curiosity and interpretation that should
be at the heart of inquiry.
must be something more than a mere list of instances’. According to MacIntyre, then,
generalisation is possible in the natural sciences, but its status in the social sciences
can never be any better than that of the generalisation of a layperson. Its value will
always be limited by the sheer variability of social life and human agency in all of its
unpredictability.
The problem here is with expectations of watertight (or almost watertight) induc-
tion following generalisation. Perhaps, though, following Peirce (Houser et al.,
1992), our expectations should be moderated concerning the generalisations that
can emerge from social studies. A number of commentators have queried the reli-
ance on a naïve model of scientific induction (for example, Haig, 1995) and others,
such as Miller and Fredericks (1999), point to the lack of ‘specialness’ of induc-
tive reasoning. The thrust of their argument is that common, everyday induction
is better described as ‘inference to the best explanation’, or Peirce’s ‘abduction’.
Abduction is making a judgement concerning the best explanation for the facts you
are collecting. It is what we do in case studies (Thomas, 2010).
Hammersley (2005: 5) suggests that ‘what is good evidence for abduction is differ-
ent from what is good evidence for induction’, proceeding to describe abduction as
‘the development of an explanatory or theoretical idea, this often resulting from close
examination of particular cases’ (see also Hammersley, 2007). It could, perhaps sim-
plistically, be thought of as ‘conclusions drawn from everyday generalisation’, whereas
induction concerns conclusions drawn from a special kind of generalisation – a kind,
if MacIntyre is right, that is impossible in the social sciences.
Thinking about all of this, it seems to me that any argument about the weak-
ness of the case study resting on its lack of generalisability fails to recognise the
limits of induction in the social sciences generally. It also fails, simultaneously,
to acknowledge the significance of abduction. It fails, in other words, to rec-
ognise the offer that can be made in local circumstances by particular kinds of
looser generalisation, whatever one calls these. I have chosen to use Peirce’s term
‘abduction’ for these to foreground the distinctions to be made with induction in
discussing the supposed weaknesses of the case study, but they have been framed
in many other ways – from the ‘common interpretive act’ (Schatzman, 1991),
to ‘bounded rationality’ (Simon, 1983), Bourdieu’s ‘thinking tool’ (in Wacquant,
1989: 50) or Althusser’s Generalities II (Althusser, 1979: 183–90) – all of which
describe much the same kind of process. That process is a fluid understanding
explicitly or tacitly recognising the complexity and frailty of the generalisations
we can make about human interrelationships.
They describe our processes of garnering and organising information to ana-
lyse and deal with our social worlds. Abduction connects all of these, providing
heuristics – that is, ways to analyse complexity that may not provide watertight
76 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Note here that Stenhouse draws upon the idea of cumulation – we draw conclusions
based upon the cumulated evidence available to us … and we surely do this on the
basis of evidence in many and varied forms.
For Stenhouse, then, we build knowledge from the collation of different forms of
case evidence. We build from case evidence an archive of knowledge from what Berger
and Luckmann (1979: 21) have famously called ‘multiple realities’. Researchers build
exemplary knowledge, making connections between another’s experience and their
own, seeing links, having insights from the noticed connections. We create what
Abbott (1992: 12) calls ‘colligations of occurrences’. In discussing the logic of this,
Mitchell (2006) concludes that a good deal of the confusion about case study has arisen
because of a failure to appreciate that the rationale of extrapolation from a statistical
sample to a parent universe involves two different and even unconnected inferential
processes: (i) statistical inference (that which is used in variable-led research) and (ii)
logical or scientific inference, the latter being about the confidence we may have that
the logical connection among the features observed in the case relates also to the par-
ent population. It is this latter that we draw upon in case-based research.
Theory
Developing or testing theory can be thought of as being at the centre of case study,
and I examine how you can develop theory in Chapter 11. Here, though (and in
RIGOUR AND QUALITY 77
the context of rigour and quality), I want to take a sideways, critical look at theory
and what it is.
In the traditional account of the process of science, the end point of the inductive
process results in theory. Seen in the way that Peirce presents it above, abduction
generates ideas, tentative ‘theories’, which serve as hypothetical explanatory con-
cepts. This abduction is then followed by deduction (the connecting of ideas),
which is followed in turn by systematic data collection in pursuit of verification of
the initial explanatory concepts. This last is induction, via which the speculative
theory is or is not verified.
The assumption that this is the course things invariably take seems to me
to lead always to the dead-end identified by MacIntyre. In other words, the
assumption of theoretical validation from inductive inference necessarily
implies a firmness to the structure and reliability of theory that is unachievable
in the inquiries of social science. Because of both the contingency, the way-
wardness, of social life and the necessary limitations on the kind and quantity
of confirmatory evidence that can be disclosed, theory, in any kind of technical
sense, is surely unattainable.
There is an elision of expectations here in the discussion of theory. As I have
noted elsewhere (Thomas, 2007; Thomas and James, 2006), a reading of interpreta-
tive inquiry scholarship from Glaser and Strauss (1967: 1) onwards (Mouzelis, 1995)
reveals an insistent theme in the expectations of theoretical explanations and pre-
dictions from induction.
Of course, something is attainable, but the question is, ‘Is it really theory?’ It can
be theory only in the sense that everything is theory. Fish (1989: 14–15) suggests that
much discourse about theory in the academy generally is not really about theory
at all, but, rather, ‘theory-talk’ – that is, ‘any form of talk that has acquired cachet
and prestige’. He also notes:
In other words, ‘theory’ has come to mean almost anything – any generalisation,
any thought, any structured reflection (or, indeed, any unstructured reflection) may
78 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
in the practical (or tacit) knowledge of phronesis, there are none of these expec-
tations as to consistency – phronesis is about understanding and behaviour in
particular situations.
Fish’s (1989: 317) explication of practical learning analyses the contrast well,
for he explains how practical learning varies with the context of a practice – that
is, as circumstances change, so ‘the very meaning of the rule (the instruc-
tions it is understood to give) changes too’. The phronesis that we acquire and
accumulate is always malleable and corrigible. So there is, in talking about
phronesis, a recognition of the provisional, the tentative in interpretation and
analysis.
There is, in other words, a need to move away from the expectations of generalis-
able knowledge that go alongside the inductive process. Correspondingly, there is a
need a move towards the ‘exemplary knowledge’ of abduction and phronesis. With
the discussion of ‘exemplary knowledge’, I am talking about an example viewed
and heard in the context of another’s experience, but used in the context of one’s
own. The example is taken to be neither representative nor typical, nor is it exem-
plary in the sense of being a model to follow. Rather, it is taken to be a particular
representation given in context and understood in that context. It is interpretable,
however, only in the context of one’s own experience – in the context, in other
words, of one’s phronesis, rather than theory.
If the case study is concerned with phronesis rather than theory – if it is decou-
pled from the inductive frame of theoretical analysis – what are the consequences?
We are left with a view of a case study’s validation coming no longer from refer-
ence to a body of theory or generalised knowledge. Rather, its validation comes
from the connections and insights it offers between another’s experience and your
own. The essence comes in understandability emerging from phronesis – in other
words, from the connection to your own situation.
This is based in what Abbott (1992) calls colligations of occurrences, involving
the making of narrative – the development of stories with which one can connect.
This is a characteristic, he notes, of all social science, intelligently done.
Finding or regularising
Having had this discussion about induction versus abduction and theory ver-
sus phronesis, I think it is worth noting again that this discussion is not new. I
outlined in Chapter 3 the difference between Plato and Aristotle on the desire
(or not) to establish laws. That debate influences even now our attitude to the
80 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
ways in which inquiries should be conducted. You could say that the contrast is
between those who could be called the finders and those who could be called the
regularisers.
Another way of putting it would be that it is an intellectual battle between
the narrators and the theorists, and it exists not just in the social sciences. The
great historian R.G. Collingwood (1946/1994: 30), in The Idea of History, com-
pared the approaches to history of Herodotus and Thucydides and concluded that
Herodotus contributed so much more, because ‘what chiefly interests Herodotus
is the events themselves; what chiefly interests Thucydides is the laws according
to which they happen’. Thucydides’ problem, Collingwood (1946/1994: 31) sug-
gests, is that he ‘is constantly being drawn away from the events to some lesson
that lurks behind them’. Collingwood favours Herodotus’s critical questioning
of narrative as a way of doing history. He notes (1946/1994: 29): ‘In reading
Thucydides I ask myself, What is the matter with the man, that he writes like
that?’ He answers (1946/1994: 29) that Thucydides abandoned history for a kind
of pseudo-psychology: ‘It is not history at all, but natural science of a special
kind. It does not narrate facts for the sake of narrating facts. Its chief purpose is
to affirm laws.’
Collingwood seems to be speaking straight to those undertaking case studies
here since he seems to be saying ‘be more like Herodotus’. Herodotus asks us to
look with him at the facts. He may tell us what he believes, but he also invites
readers to make their own judgements. The lawyer Thomas Geoghegan (2007)
puts it this way: ‘we should also spend more funds to get our young people out
of the library where they’re reading Thucydides and get them to start living like
Herodotus – going out and seeing the world’. Again, a motto for the case inquirer.
Quality
I have used the term ‘structural characteristics of research’ for many of the concepts
I have discussed in this chapter, and, as I hope I have indicated, these concepts
(reliability, validity, generalisation and so on) take on different meanings in case
study from those they might assume in other kinds of research. It is difficult, in
other words, to judge the quality of a case study via the criteria that are often used
in judging other kinds of research.
There are characteristics of research – though very general ones – by which we
can unequivocally judge quality. Assuming for a moment that we are concerned
principally with the interpretative aspects of a case study, Hammersley (2005:
3–4) has offered some useful indicators as to this quality, which I have adapted
as follows:
RIGOUR AND QUALITY 81
Hammersley (2005: 6), however, proceeds to give a health warning about the criteria
he has offered, and this chimes with the ‘criteriology’ warning that Schwandt (1996)
issued. Hammersley (2005, emphasis in original) says of the points that he makes
about quality:
these are lists of what we might call issues for threshold judgment: they
remind the reader what to take account of, what they need to make a
judgment about. However, they are not operationalised in such a way
that anyone who did not already know how to make the judgments
concerned would be able to apply them, nor do they tell the reader what
would be sufficient in each case. And they do not tell us whether all of
the thresholds have to be met for a positive conclusion to be reached,
whether high scoring on one can counterbalance a lower ‘score’ on
another, and so on. Moreover, I do not believe that they can be opera-
tionalised in this way, or that attempting to do this would be desirable.
82 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Put differently, how could you know that you have met these criteria? How could
readers apply such criteria if they were not aware of them implicitly in the first
place? Further, it would be impossible for researchers or readers to weigh up the
importance of one criterion versus another if they were not already steeped in the
discourse of the field of study.
For me, the issues boil down to the choice of your case, the choices made con-
cerning analysis and the robustness of your argument. In other words, your answers
to the following questions:
1. How well has the case been chosen? Decisions should have been made appropriately
about the approach and the processes to be adopted in data collection and analysis.
2. How well has the context for the study been explained and justified? Including
justification for decisions about how analysis has been undertaken.
3. How well have arguments been made? Have rival explanations for the same
kind of observation been explored? This is where the storyboards that I have
drawn for some of the examples in this book can help – providing space to think
about alternative storylines for the same plot. Is it justifiable that, on this evi-
dence and with this reasoning, these conclusions have been drawn?
The case study offers the opportunity to bring evidence together from many and
varied sources to support arguments in ways that would not be possible using
other forms of inquiry that are fenced in by different considerations. Concerns
about how far we can generalise from a case study are neutralised when we real-
ise how tentative any generalisation might be in social research. Conclusions
drawn from case study research become less pronounced when we realise that, to
RIGOUR AND QUALITY 83
a greater or lesser extent, all forms of inquiry, especially social inquiry, produce
knowledge that is provisional – in other words, good only until we find out some-
thing else which explains things better.
REFLECTIVE ACTIVITY
Quality in social research is assessed differently depending on the kind of
research being undertaken. Quality is sometimes measured using particular cri-
teria. Reliability and positionality are two of these criteria. In this chapter I have
discussed the use of these criteria and how their importance varies depending on
the kind of research being undertaken. In variable-led research, such as experi-
ment, certain criteria are more important than they are in case study research.
In case study research other criteria are more important. With these considera-
tions in mind, rate the importance of reliability and positionality in variable-led
research and in case study research in the table below, stating your reasons for
the rating you have given.
Rate the importance of reliability and Why do you give your rating?
positionality in variable-led research and in
case study (5, very important; 1 not important
at all)
Variable-led research 54321
Reliability
Case study 54321
Further Reading
ESRC/TLRP Seminar Series: Quality in Educational Research (available at: www.
education.bham.ac.uk/research/seminars1/esrc_4/index.shtml).
This is a series of seminar inputs on quality in educational research, giving tran-
scripts and articles from the series. The seminars cover issues to do with quality of
evidence and how quality differs in quantitative and qualitative research.
84 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Geertz, C. (1977) ‘Deep play: notes on the Balinese cockfight’, in C. Geertz, The
Interpretation of Cultures. New York: Basic Books (available at: www.si.umich.
edu/~rfrost/courses/Matcult/content/Geertz.pdf).
A classic case study that reads more like a story than a piece of ethnography, and
this is a tribute to the quality of Geertz’s writing.
Matthews, B. and Ross, L. (2010) Research Methods: A Practical Guide for the Social
Sciences. Harlow: Pearson Education.
Matthews and Ross have an excellent section on sampling, and go into some very
useful detail also on other aspects of methodology.
Ragin, C.C. and Becker, H.S. (eds) (1992) What Is a Case? Exploring the Foundations
of Social Inquiry. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Thomas, G. (2010) ‘Doing case study: abduction not induction; phronesis not
theory’, Qualitative Inquiry, 16 (7): 575–82.
Thomas, G. (2011) ‘The case: generalisation, theory and phronesis in case study’,
Oxford Review of Education, 37 (1): 21–35.
Thomas, G. (2017) How to Do Your Research Project (3rd edn). London: Sage.
In Chapter 6, I go into more detail about issues such as reliability, validity and
samples.
Thomas, G. and Myers, K. (2015) The Anatomy of the Case Study. London: Sage.
Your participants
Your case study may not involve your dealing with people. It may be a paper-based
study, looking at one historical case. However, in most of the studies in which
86 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
we become involved we will be working with people. In the bad old days, when
social scientists did research with people, they called those people ‘subjects’. Now,
especially in applied social science, we think of the people with whom we research
more as ‘participants’ or even ‘partners’, rather than ‘subjects’. Thinking with ethics
to the fore, we have to see the people with whom we are involved as participants
in our research and part of it, rather than simply objects from whom data can be
extracted.
In working with our research participants, we have to recognise that these partici-
pants have rights and they should have a stake in the process of research: it should
not be a question of simply ‘using’ people and then walking away. To be genuine
participants they have to be involved to some extent in the planning, execution
and write-up of your work. You will need to think about their particular kinds of
contribution and their particular needs. Especially, you will need to think about the
needs of people who are vulnerable for any reason.
Vulnerable groups
For those who may not understand the ins and outs of consent or who may be sus-
ceptible to pressure to cooperate because of their social or economic position there
will need to be special considerations. These will include:
Some people are especially prone to become drawn into research projects because of
their ‘ready availability in settings where research is conducted’, as the US govern-
ment’s Belmont Report (1979) put it. Careful thought should be given to questions
about why and how they are being involved,
Children, patients, employees and
and they should be protected against involve-
people with special needs are all
vulnerable and you will need to ment solely for the sake of convenience, or
consider carefully the ethics of because they may be easy to persuade or even
working with them.
manipulate.
ETHICS 87
Lewis and Porter (2004) urge that the following questions should always be asked with
any kind of vulnerable group, including children and those with learning difficulties:
• Has the participant’s ability to give fully informed consent been assessed and dis-
cussed with others such as parents, caregivers or teachers?
• Have ways of checking for understanding of confidentiality/research purposes
been explored?
• Will participants, at appropriate intervals, be reminded of their right to withdraw?
• Have all possible steps been taken to ensure anonymity, given that this may be
particularly difficult to achieve with minority populations?
• In giving feedback, have steps been taken to ensure the intelligibility of the infor-
mation? This can be done through, for example, asking a person who is well known
to the individuals to talk with them, or offering pictures with simplified text or case
study material.
• How is the end of the research relationship with participants to be managed? It is
easy for close relationships to be forged during research, and these should not be
terminated abruptly.
Participants’ activity
When relating with your participants – and we are now thinking about all par-
ticipants, rather than specifically those who may be vulnerable in some way – you
should think about issues such as:
Deception or concealment
The default position on doing research is that you should be honest and open
in all your dealings with research participants. Sometimes, though, as in the
Milgram experiment I mentioned earlier, you will need to be less than 100 per
cent open because you may be researching something where the participants’
knowledge of your aims would invalidate any findings that you may make. If
this is the case, you must be prepared to argue convincingly for the need for
concealment, and you must build into your plans a debriefing session with your
participants after the research has finished wherein you explain what you were
doing in your study and why you could not be fully open at the outset.
The codes of conduct from professional asso-
ciations usually recognise that an element of
Always put respect for your
participants centre-stage. As far as concealment or even deception may sometimes
possible be open about your aims, be necessary. Indeed, the US government’s
and avoid subjecting people to any
kind of discomfort. landmark Belmont Report (1979) recognises it,
but says that:
conversations with others, in your storage of data and your reporting. Anonymity
can be ensured by changing participants’ names as well as the name of any institu-
tions (such as schools) to which they are affiliated and the regions in which they
are situated. You can either give pseudonyms or code numbers to achieve this. You
will need to be especially careful in working with participants whose identity may
be hard to disguise, for example those in minority groups. Where appropriate, you
should make it clear to participants that your commitment to confidentiality as a
researcher may be overridden given your legal or moral duty to report incidents of
harm. Circumstances such as these will be most unusual, but if you do come across
them you should discuss them with your supervisor and/or the appropriate services
(such as child protection services) immediately.
Confidentiality is not secured simply by paying attention to anonymisa-
tion, important though the latter is. More broadly, it is about a commitment
to respecting the wishes of informants, who may not want the information
which they have given you to become public. While engagement in research
should involve, of course, an understanding that findings will be published,
your informants may expect that you will disguise or hide information which
they take to be confidential – perhaps using it in drawing your conclusions, but
keeping the details off the record.
You should always be sensitive to this possibility and even proactive in asking
about it, moving on to negotiate with your research participants how much – and
how – you intend to use, analyse and disclose your findings. They may ultimately
decide that they do not wish you to publish certain facts or opinions, and this is
a wish you must respect. In cases where it is unclear whether you have secreted or
masked data to the satisfaction of your informants you will need to give them the
chance to read through your final report to allow them to make suggestions about
how to edit it. You can find an excellent discussion of all these issues in Wiles
et al. (2008).
Occasionally, research participants may ask
As a default position, all data should
to be identified and not anonymised in research be treated as confidential and names
outputs (see Grinyer, 2002). You should respect coded or otherwise anonymised.
Data should be kept secure and
any such requests while ensuring that you dis- deleted after analysis.
cuss fully with all those involved, including
caregivers, the potential consequences of disclosure. And remember that if one
person’s name is revealed, this increases the risk of a breach of confidentiality
for other participants who wish to remain anonymous. Having carefully weighed
up all the pros and cons, you should, if you decide to agree to any request to
disclosure, obtain written consent that an individual wishes to waive the right to
confidentiality.
90 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
• Only use data for the purposes for which the data were collected – not for other
purposes.
• Keep data for an appropriate length of time. What counts as ‘appropriate’ var-
ies according to the circumstances: for most undergraduate research it will
be appropriate to destroy raw data immediately your project is completed and
marked; however, for some research (for example, that which is part of a clinical
programme, which a postgraduate project may be part of), professional codes of
conduct specify that data must be kept for several years following the close of a
programme. It is worth noting that the UK Information Commissioner’s Office (ICO)
says that, with justification, data can be stored for research indefinitely.
• Keep data secure. You should use passwords on any files that contain pre-
anonymised names. For Microsoft Office documents, this means clicking the Office
button (top left of the screen), clicking Prepare, then clicking Encrypt document.
(If this does not work, click the F1 button for help, and enter ‘password’.) You will be
asked for a password, which you must remember. If you do not remember it, there
is no way to get back your data.
• Do not pass the data on to anyone else – obviously, since the data would not then
be secure.
• Keep the data anonymous (see previous section).
If you are in any doubt about any of this, you should search for the most recent
advice from the ICO on the EU General Data Protection Regulation (GDPR) (2016)
and the UK Data Protection Act 2018, which govern the processing (acquiring,
holding, using, etc.) of personal data in the UK.
Consent
Because of the harm that might be caused during research involving people, an
important issue to think about when considering the ethics of research is that of
consent. Consent is about the agreement of people to take part in the study. More
ETHICS 91
than simple agreement, however, is needed, given the issues I have already con-
sidered. Informed consent is needed. In other words, potential participants should
understand what they are agreeing to. Informed consent is centred around the fol-
lowing points:
• With opt-in consent, participants have to make an active choice about becoming
involved and signal their willingness to take part in the research. You would have
to offer an invitation to participants to become involved, preferably in writing,
requesting that they return a form indicating willingness to participate. This could
be done directly and orally if written communication is not possible. Alternatively,
some other active choice of the participant would have to be involved, as would be
the case in returning a questionnaire. It must be made clear that the participant’s
consent may be withdrawn at any time and that if the participant chooses to do
this there will be no further collection of additional data, no further analysis of
data initially collected and removal of existing data from your records.
• With implied (or opt-out) consent, you tell participants about the research and
assume that they give their consent unless they tell you otherwise. Information
about the research can be distributed by a range of means, for example by a letter
from a school to the parents or guardians of all children who would be personally
involved. The letter would include the information you would give as for opt-in
consent, but would be accompanied by a phrase such as ‘I am assuming that unless
I hear from you, you have no objections to Ellie’s participation’. Depending on the
92 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Risk
Risk … low risk or high risk
‘Risk’ refers to the chance that someone could be harmed, psychologically or physi-
cally, by involvement in your research. Making judgements about the harm that
could occur involves identifying the potential hazards that might confront the
research participant. Whether a hazard presents a low risk or high risk is a matter of
judgement and if you are unsure about what might constitute high or low risk you
should discuss the matter with your supervisor.
Potential hazards include:
If there is judged to be any significant risk concerning any of these matters, you
should consider its extent and duration. You may need to consider also the extent
to which any risk can be mitigated or ‘undone’, either naturally or through steps
taken by you. You will also need to weigh any risks against benefits or scholarly
merit, which may include particular advantages to participants or others or any
potential advance in knowledge. You should be prepared to expand on all of these
and make a good case for your research when you seek ethics clearance.
As a guide, Table 5.1 offers a selection of different kinds of study so that you
can get an idea of the dimensions on which ethics judgements may be assessed
and the conclusions that could be drawn about whether these are high risk or
low risk. Table 5.1 reveals, I hope, that judgements about risk are difficult to
make and that the conclusions are never definitive. For example, while I have
suggested that unintrusive observation in a teacher’s own class may be low risk,
others may disagree, based on the potential obtrusiveness of observation and
the dangers of class students realising what the teacher is doing. However one
chooses to categorise it, it would be necessary in this example to inform parents
and children in broad terms of the intended research, with the opportunity given
to veto the activity.
If, in seeking ethical clearance (see ‘Getting clearance – ethical review’, below),
you are asked about risks and benefits, you should spell out:
• the physical, psychological, social, legal or economic risks that may be associated
with the research and outline the arrangements that you have put in place to man-
age that risk
• if there are likely to be any immediate benefits to participants
• any arrangements that you have put in place for support after the study.
Issues of consent are particularly important if you are working with children. There
is an unequal power relationship between any adult and any child, and your posi-
tion as a researcher heightens the perception of authority held by the child. Good
discussions of the ethics of working with children are provided in Alderson (2004),
Kellett (2005), Lewis and Lindsay (2000) and Simons (1989, 2009).
Table 5.1 Ethics: low risk or high risk?
Risk level Case study: subject and object Methods used Why this risk level?
No risk Subject: The Cuban Missile Crisis Documentary analysis of contemporary There is no direct involvement with
Object: Understanding the role of media commentary, government records people (that is to say, research
militarism in US foreign policy, 1960–70 and interviews with key individuals participants) in the data collection
Low risk Subject: A child who ‘freezes’ when Observation over a term of the While this could be high risk if any direct
asked to take a test in class circumstances of each occurrence of involvement with the child were to be
Object: Insight into the consequences for ‘freezing’ in the teacher’s own class planned, risk is much reduced providing
school students of a test-led curriculum observation and note-taking are discreet.
This is quite possible, given that the study
is to be done in the teacher’s own class
Medium risk Subject: A week in the life of an art Observing; interviewing staff, guides, Museums and galleries are unlikely to
gallery volunteers, groups and members of the give rise to much embarrassment or
Object: Understanding responsiveness to public attending the gallery discomfort in respondents. However,
the public in museums and galleries with this particular case study
delicate issues may arise because of
emerging information about tensions or
disagreements among staff about the
running of this gallery
High risk Subject: An intensive study of a young A three-day intensive stay with one While much good could come from this
woman with anorexia nervosa young woman in the young people’s unit study, there are serious potential dangers
Object: Understanding the phenomenon of a psychiatric hospital, ‘shadowing’ her, here for a vulnerable young person.
of ‘taking control’ in anorexia nervosa talking with her and talking with staff Handled insensitively, the young woman
and other patients could become upset or disturbed and
treatment could be disrupted
ETHICS 95
Background checking
If you are involved in any prolonged or regular contact with children or vulner-
able people as part of your research, in the UK you may need to undergo a check
under the Disclosure and Barring Scheme (DBS). (For various background screening
checks in countries other than the UK, see https://cbscreening.co.uk/news/post/uk-
vs-world-background-screening-compare/).
Situations in which an individual has incidental, irregular or indirect contact do
not normally give rise to the need for DBS checks. Decisions about whether you
need a DBS disclosure should happen as part of a risk assessment process (see
Table 5.2), taking into account the exact nature of your contact with individuals
and, for example, the nature of any participant vulnerability. If you are in any
doubt, discuss with your supervisor and any stakeholders in the research such as
a child’s teachers. More details on the DBS scheme can be found at www.gov.uk/
request-copy-criminal-record.
Risk assessment
A risk assessment is a formal examination of the potential hazards that could cause
harm to your participants in your research. It is accompanied by an indication of
how serious that harm could be, considering all of the points I have just discussed,
and how you might manage the risk. Normally, given the brief interactions that
researchers often have with participants, a formal risk assessment should not be
necessary, but with certain participants, particularly those who are vulnerable in
any way, it will be worth systematically undertaking a risk assessment, following
the numbered points I give in Table 5.2.
• the title of project and the name(s) of researcher(s) and their institution
• an explanation of what the research is about
96 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
• confirmation that involvement in the project is voluntary and that participants are
free to withdraw at any time and/or withdraw data supplied
• an explanation about what involvement will be asked of participants and whether,
for example, audio or visual recording will be involved
• details of arrangements being made to provide for confidentiality, such as anonymisation
• arrangements for data security including when the data will be destroyed
• arrangements for debriefing and feedback.
Dear Parent
We know that teaching assistants provide a great deal of help to teachers. In my research I want to find out Briefly
explain
how teaching assistants work with teachers and children. I hope this will contribute to our understanding of
your project
the ways in which teaching assistants work, and I hope that I will emerge with suggestions about how they
may work more effectively.
I will be observing the work of the teaching assistant in your child’s class and I may be asking children
about their work.
If you would prefer that I avoid observation of your child and that I do not talk to him/her, Say what the
no questions will be asked and there will, of course, be no consequence for participants are
your child. being asked
to do
I will not be sharing any personal information I gather with anyone, even school staff. No names will
be recorded: any information about individuals will have a code on it instead of a name. After my
write-up, all information I have collected will be destroyed.
If you are happy for me to undertake my project as outlined in this letter, you don’t need Explain
to do anything. However, if you would prefer that I do not observe or talk to your son/daughter as anonymity,
part of my research, please complete the tear-off slip below and return it with your daughter or son data security
to give to their teacher. Or, if you have any questions at all, please complete the sheet as and feedback
appropriate.
Many thanks
Lucy Bloggs
Student in educational studies
-------------------------------------------------------------------------------
If you do not wish your son/daughter to be involved in my project or if you have any Consent is
questions at all, please complete the following and return to your child’s teacher: implied
My name _________________________________________________________
phone me on ____________________________________________________
text me on ______________________________________________________
email me on ____________________________________________________
Figure 5.1 An example of a letter seeking participants’ involvement via implied consent
98 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Where involvement is less complicated, a simple letter may be all that is needed, as
long as it covers these issues in an intelligible way. Figure 5.1 shows an example of
a letter which doubles up as an information sheet and a seeking of implied consent.
Other examples are given in the online resources, see: https://study.sagepub.com/
thomascasestudy3e.
Contacting participants
Remember that each situation is different. You will need to assess how best to make
contact with potential participants and how to explain to them the nuts and bolts
of your research. In written communications with participants, always use straight-
forward language. Imagine that you are in a spoken conversation with them. So, for
example, write ‘my study starts on …’ rather than ‘my study commences on …’, or
‘Please read through this’ instead of ‘Please peruse this’. (When has anyone actually
said the words ‘commence’ or ‘peruse’? I’ve no idea why people who devise forms are
so fond of words like this.) Use everyday terms such as ‘information’ rather than ‘data’.
A very good discussion on ways of seeking and gaining consent is given by the World
Health Organization at www.who.int/ethics/review-committee/informed-decision-
making.pdf?ua=1. This gives templates of letters, forms and information sheets.
• Consider listing a privacy policy on your webpages, as well as a page that describes
what data you are collecting and how they are used.
ethical review or institutional review. You will have to write an outline of your pro-
posed research which will be looked at by a group known as the ‘ethics committee’
or, more commonly in the USA, the ‘institutional review board’ (IRB).
The use of these formal procedures by universities for ensuring ethical practice
in social science research projects is relatively recent and still regarded by many
researchers, both student and professional, as a bit of a chore. However, this is
not the right attitude, and it stems – I speak only for myself – from a less than
self-critical belief that I could not possibly be doing anything that would harm any-
one with whom I researched. It stems from a belief that my own judgement about
the balance of rights and wrongs is naturally the best judgement and that it is an
attack on my integrity to question this. But the passage of time and experience have
shown me how delicately balanced these matters can be and how necessary it is to
have ethical concerns explicitly articulated and systematically checked. In fact, the
systematic checking of these concerns with another person often will add insights
beyond ethics to the design and conduct of the research.
Your university will have a code of conduct for research, and it will have a helpful
webpage outlining procedures for gaining ethical clearance. The relevant profes-
sional organisations also have policies and guidelines on ethical research. Those
for the British Educational Research Association (BERA), for example, can be found
by entering ‘BERA ethical guidelines’ into your favourite search engine. Others are
given in the box below. All are very helpful, though some (for example, that of
the American Psychological Association) are
You will need to go through your
so long that you may lose the will to live if
institution’s ethics clearance process
before you start work. Professional you embark on them. A very good, well-
bodies have codes of conduct that written one is the Social Policy Association’s
may be helpful in doing this.
Guidelines on Research Ethics.
The key thing to remember in looking at any of these protocols is that it is the
consideration of ethics that is important – not the protocols themselves, helpful
though these can be. A reflection on ethics should be integral, and not seen as a
bolt-on or a chore to be squeezed into your timeframe.
However, if you have not been able to answer ‘no’ to all of the questions you
will have to proceed to the second stage, which involves scrutiny of your plans by
a university-wide committee. The process of ethical review must happen before the
start of any active empirical work, including any pilot studies.
For your own university’s exact procedure, take guidance from your department,
or just enter ‘University of [name of your university] ethical review’ into your search
engine, or, in the USA, ‘University of [name of your university] IRB review’.
At this level, ethical approval may need to be sought from multiple stakehold-
ers (for example, your university and a health service such as the NHS), certain of
whom may require annual updates, reporting of any adverse events, etc.
Ethics are a specialised and increasingly important topic, and all universi-
ties will have detailed procedures for ensuring that you consider ethical issues
appropriately (my university’s procedures – accessible to anyone – are at: www.
birmingham.ac.uk/Documents/university/legal/research.pdf). If, in your planned
work, ethical issues seem to loom large and you wish to think about them in more
depth, then you may wish to look at the further reading listed at the end of the
chapter.
• Your participants. Make sure that they know what is involved and that you give
them ample opportunity to participate or withdraw, as they wish. Consider espe-
cially the needs of vulnerable groups – in other words, those (such as children)
who may not fully understand what is being asked of them, or those who feel that
they have to accede to a request because of their social position (for example,
employees).
• Gaining consent from participants. In doing this, you will need to provide as much
information as possible about the project.
• Openness and sharing of information. Some withholding of full details may be nec-
essary in some circumstances, but this will shift a project into ‘high risk’ and will
need to be explained fully to an ethics committee.
• Confidentiality. There may be exceptions, but the default position is to treat all
data as confidential and anonymise names.
ETHICS 103
• Data security. Keep data secure and destroy after your analysis.
• Getting ethics clearance. All universities and large organisations will have proce-
dures to be gone through. Ensure you do this before your case study starts.
REFLECTIVE ACTIVITY
In Table 5.1 I offered a number of research scenarios and suggested the level of
risk that might be associated with each, from low risk to high risk.
I reproduce the empty shell of the table below. Think of a case study topic, or
take one from the examples I have given already (such as Amelia in Chapter 1),
and outline the methods that might be used in pursuing that study. Then ‘fit’ it to
one of the risk levels in the table and account for why you have decided on this
risk level in the final column of the table.
Further Reading
Alderson, P. and Morrow, V. (2020) The Ethics of Research with Children and Young
People. London: Sage.
Good on the ethics of working with children and young people.
BERA (2018) Ethical Guidelines for Educational Research (4th edition). London: BERA
(available at: www.bera.ac.uk/publication/ethical-guidelines-for-educational-
research-2018-online).
These are the ethics guidelines for educational researchers from the professional
association of educational researchers. Similar guidelines can be found for research-
ers in other areas of applied science from the websites of those associations.
Butterwick, S., Head, G., Madalinska-Michalak, J., Raykov, M., Taylor, A., Taylor-
Neu, R. and Zgaga, P. (2020) ‘Introduction: ethical issues in educational research’,
European Educational Research Journal, 19 (1): 3–9.
104 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
This is the introduction from a special issue of the European Educational Research
Journal (EERJ), which addresses the issues surrounding current concerns about the
ethical conduct of research in education.
Honan, E., Hamid, M.O., Alhamdan, B., Phommalangsy, P. and Lingard, B. (2013)
‘Ethical issues in cross-cultural research’, International Journal of Research & Method
in Education, 36 (4): 386–99.
Looks at some important issues in cross-cultural research and negotiating ethics
committees with them.
Shaw, I. (2003) ‘Ethics in qualitative research and evaluation’, Journal of Social Work,
3 (1): 9–29.
Good on ethics and social justice.
Sikes, P. and Piper, H. (2010) ‘Ethical research, academic freedom and the role of
ethics committees and review procedures in educational research’, International
Journal of Research and Method in Education, 33 (3): 205–13.
This is one article in a special issue of IJRME edited by Sikes and Piper which
addressed a range of issues concerning ethics in educational research and which
looked in particular at the role of the university ethics committee.
Simons, H. and Usher, R. (2000) (eds) Situated Ethics in Educational Research. London:
Routledge.
See especially Glen’s paper discussing ‘complex integrity’.
ETHICS 105
Wiles, R., Crow, G., Heath, S. and Charles, V. (2008) ‘The management of confiden-
tiality and anonymity in social research’, International Journal of Social Research
Methodology, 11 (5): 417–28.
An excellent discussion of issues around confidentiality and anonymity.
STAGE 2
CHOOSING A CASE AND
STRUCTURING YOUR STUDY
• h
ow cases can be categorised
• h
ow you can identify a case
• a
nd how all of these interrelate to create your
unique study.
6
DIFFERENT KINDS
OF CASE STUDIES:
SELECTING A SUBJECT
FOR YOUR CASE STUDY
If you remember, at the end of Chapter 1 I introduced a framework for thinking
about the structure of case studies. It is a classification system – or ‘typology’ –
which splits thinking about case studies into six elements, which translate to
six steps in the construction of a case study. I repeat that structure, those steps,
here …
1. The subject of the study – what is the case you are actually looking at? Is it a
classroom, a child, an institution, a war, a country, a political event, or what? Why
have you identified this subject? It may be one that you know a lot about from your
own local knowledge and experience. Or it may be a key case in the literature. Or
it may be one that is interesting because it is an outlier – different from the norm.
Whichever, this is the lens through which you are examining the topic of theoretical
interest that you are focusing on, namely the object of the study.
2. The object of the study – what is the theoretical topic at the heart of your ques-
tion that the subject of your study is allowing you to explore in detail?
3. The purpose of the study – why are you doing it? Is it to make an evaluation? Is it
out of curiosity, or is it with a particular understanding in mind?
4. The approach of the study – is it to test a theory that you or someone else has
developed about a topic? Or is it to develop a theory from new about that topic?
5. The methods you will use to do the case study. What data collection tools might
you use, and how might you analyse the data that you gather?
110 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
6. The process you will use to conduct the case study. Will it be a single or multiple
study, and will you look at the case during one particular moment in time, or over
a longer period?
The choices you make about your subject, object, purpose, approach, methodology
and process will interact in various ways to produce one unique study out of an
array of possible permutations.
In this chapter, then, let’s begin at the beginning with step number 1 and focus
on how you go about choosing your case study.
A local
Yes knowledge
Is it your own case
special
knowledge
that leads you
to this case? Is it a case that reveals
something by virtue of A special or
No Yes outlier case
its ‘specialness’, its
‘differentness’?
No
Is it a classic or
A key case
exemplary case that
Yes
reveals something
from in-depth study?
The first route may be taken because of your familiarity with it. I have thus called
this a local knowledge case. Is it, for example, your own work or domestic situation
or another situation with which you are familiar? Here, in your own place of work,
your placement or even your home, you have intimate knowledge. You know and
can ‘read’ the people who inhabit the arena. This is a ready-made strength for con-
ducting a case study.
Another aspect of this is that you may be obliged to undertake a research
project as part of your university studies and have limited access to anything
other than your own situation – or, to put this more positively, it may be that, in
your own situation, you can gain access to the richness and depth that would
be unavailable to you otherwise. This is a perfectly valid reason for choosing
your own situation in which to do your research and a good one for doing a
case study. Do consider, though, if you are in this situation, that there are other
forms of research also well suited to small-scale inquiry (I outline some of these
in How to Do Your Research Project, Thomas, 2017).
The second route is taken if you are not choosing your study because of your own
local or special knowledge. Are you focusing on this subject because of its inherent
interest – is it a key case? In the examples that follow in this chapter (summarised
in Table 6.1), Hurricane Katrina is a particularly interesting key case by virtue of
its size, the political context when it hit, and the extraordinary amount of damage
that it wreaked on New Orleans and its environs.
Table 6.1 The origin of your case: a summary of the boxed examples in this chapter
Possible choice –
Kinds of case examples used in
study subject … which means this chapter How to justify your choice
Alternatively, are you examining the case because it is in some way special – in
other words, because it is an outlier that shows something by virtue of its difference
from other examples? An outlier may occur because it is different from the rest, as
in the case of Kerala, the state in India that has extreme poverty yet unusually low
infant mortality rates.
Let us look at examples of a local knowledge case, a key case and an outlier case,
remembering that a key issue in each case will be to identify an analytical frame
that the case is a case of.
comparisons between Ioan and his peers in your neighbourhood. What you
can do, however, with your special knowledge and the knowledge you have
from your reading and wider research, is construct a case study that con-
nects what you know about Ioan – he seems to be a happy, fulfilled young
man – with some of the findings of the research: that is, some of the adopted
children proved to have serious emotional difficulties.
How can you construct a study here? You can try to interconnect your own
information with that from your reading to emerge with a topic that focuses
on Ioan’s success, noting that success has not always been the picture for
some of these young people.
How has success been achieved? In answering this question, you could
look at the age at which Ioan was adopted, the nature of the family into
which he was absorbed (the number of siblings and so on), Ioan’s parents’
reflections and Ioan’s own reflections, including his experiences at school.
You will be using all kinds of data for evidence and analysing and inter-
preting the data in a variety of ways – see Chapters 9 and 10.
The point is that the case study will enable you to ‘drill down’ into this
one case – it is as if it were one of Rutter’s sample of 111. Where Rutter, in
his huge piece of research, was able to offer some generalisable findings
about the progress of children adopted from Romanian orphanages, he was
not able to do the kind of study that yours promises to be with your local
knowledge.
(Continued)
114 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
‘Consequence end’ • Gangs were roaming the city, stealing from homes and looting
from shops
• Around 3 million people were left for over a week without
electricity
• Much of the country’s oil-refining capacity was disrupted
• Many people were made refugees by the disaster, having to
move to temporary homes elsewhere in the country. Some of
these would never return
(Continued)
116 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
This is the basis for a thorough case study. Some pilot exploration, per-
haps by examining online local planning records in the local papers or
discussions with relatives, could confirm this as a basis for a plausible prima
facie hypothesis. If it does indeed seem to be a reasonable narrative that
could be explored in depth, then further data can be collected – from news
footage, interviews, books, original documents and archives about building
plans locally and objections made to them.
Alternatively, Jake could focus on the ‘consequence end’ and look at
the huge upheaval caused by Hurricane Katrina – the looting, lawlessness,
displaced people and disrupted power supplies – locally and nationally.
He may wish to examine the extent to which this was the consequence
particularly of a hurricane or how likely it would be for it to happen else-
where and with different environmental events. Here he could look at the
particular features that mark out Hurricane Katrina and New Orleans to
see if they interacted in some peculiar way that made for this collection
of social consequences. A different storyboard would follow, like that in
Figure 6.3.
Was it
Was the outcome
‘head in
worse or better?
the sand’
mentality?
The two storyboards show how different narratives are possible from very
similar starting points.
118 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
An outlier case
An outlier case is a puzzle. It is a case that is conspicuously different from the norm,
and the question is why. You may have an idea as to a reason (or reasons) and this
will offer a ‘way in’. It may also offer a possible analytical frame (see page 115).
2000
1800
1600 Most states in the
1400 scattergram conform
Mean income
(Continued)
120 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
The link with female education is striking, notes Caldwell (1986: 177): ‘For
most countries in both lists the 1982 infant mortality rankings are very close
to the 1960 female primary school enrolment rankings’ – as education of
females goes up, infant mortality goes down.
He concludes that there are some remarkable parallels between these
states. These parallels include a substantial degree of female autonomy,
dedication to education, open political system, largely civilian society
without a rigid class structure, history of egalitarianism and radicalism and
national consensus arising from political contest.
In a fascinating and wide-ranging analysis, Caldwell suggests reasons for
the importance of the position of women in society, from the likelihood of
girls becoming nurses to the position of children in society being higher
where that of women is higher.
He reports that an early anthropologist, John Davy, noted the Sinhalese
treated their children with ‘extraordinary affection’, attending to them when
sick in the way that other groups he had studied did not.
Caldwell’s conclusion was that women’s education may be important not
just because women may know more about, say, nutrition and development
to help their babies, but also because then women are valued and included
– something more intangible but nonetheless seemingly just as significant.
Thus, the provision of education for all seems to be operating at two levels: it is
providing for individual knowledge but is also symbolic of society’s affirmation
of once-marginalised groups and its endorsement of those groups’ partici-
pation and inclusion. So, in some way, this provision seems to be endowing
added value. Perhaps this added value comes in the form of a boost for iden-
tity, status, belonging and self-belief. Inclusion, and the status it brings, breeds
not only health but also the conditions for learning and growth to occur.
He looks also at historical and cultural investment in health and hygiene,
noting that the high performers had a record of valuing these matters and
investing powerfully in them since the nineteenth century. Education is
important, too, but not just as education per se. It is linked with an active
and participatory political system that in some way enables the hearing of
the people’s ‘voice’.
He compares this with what happens elsewhere in India: ‘The ineffi-
ciency of much of the health infrastructure in rural India is explained by
both the low educational levels and the political passivity of the poor’
(Caldwell, 1986: 203). There appears to be a symbiosis between education
and health.
Although Caldwell at no point explicitly calls his work a case study, it
emphatically is one – of this limited set of countries. In doing work with such
a small number, he cannot definitively assert that factor X or factor Y is, in
fact, the cause of the good life expectancy figures in these outlier countries,
SELECTING A SUBJECT 121
but he can make a good, informed deduction. His initial speculations and
conjectures led to conclusions informed by evidence from historical records,
other statistics, anthropological studies, and reasoning about cultural differ-
ence. In fact, while Caldwell’s study starts off as an epidemiological inquiry
based largely on statistics, it ends more as a comparative history. The inclu-
sion of all the forms of data, argument, reasoning and analysis is a form of
triangulation that should not be downplayed in its importance.
Caldwell’s informed hypothesising is the foundation stone of good anal-
ysis. As long as it involves the intelligent putting together of evidence, it
provides the basis for finding solutions and solving problems.
Always, though, we should have at the front of our minds the importance
of the need to be critical in this or any other kind of thinking when conduct-
ing a research inquiry. We should keep to the fore the need to be our own
most strident critics of the explanations we propose.
If you were doing Caldwell’s study today, you would be able to draw on
a wider range of studies that have used Kerala as an example of something
extraordinary. For example, the Nobel-Prize-winning economist Amartya
Sen (1999: 45–8) describes the process operating in Kerala as a ‘support-
led’ approach to improvement in life conditions: ‘The support-led process
does not operate through fast economic growth, but works through a pro-
gram of skilful social support of health care, education and other relevant
social arrangements.’ You could draw on Sen’s notion of ‘capability’ among
a nation’s citizenry, which he takes to be more important than imposed top-
down measures designed for the good of the people. In other words, this is
about giving the people the opportunity to be involved – a concept similar
to the one Caldwell identified in the participatory political system.
Nowadays, if doing this kind of study, you can draw on some extraordi-
nary banks of data that are bundled together with software that enables
you easily to draw comparisons of the kind that Caldwell would have had to
spend weeks putting together. For example, Gapminder (http://gapminder.
org) can instantly draw a sophisticated set of international comparisons of
everything from national per capita income to life expectancy, oil reserves
and quality of teeth. It is not a problem getting data nowadays – it just needs
leavening with your imagination.
If I were doing a ‘Caldwell-type’ study now and looking at Gapminder, I
might choose to focus on the ‘best’ and the ‘worst’ African countries using
the same measures as Caldwell. These turn out to be Cape Verde and
Swaziland, respectively: Cape Verde has a life expectancy of 72 years with
a per capita income of $3283, while Swaziland’s life expectancy is 40 and its
per capita income $4595. There will be different forces at play for these two
(Continued)
122 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
countries, but ones that will reveal fascinating outlier case studies for com-
parison. A storyboard might include factors such as:
Single
Multiple
Outlier Intrinsic Testing a theory • Nested
Key Instrumental Building a theory • Parallel
Local Evaluative Drawing a picture • Sequential
Explanatory Descriptive • Retrospective
Exploratory Interpretative • Snapshot
• Diachronic
Figure 6.5 Mapping out the design for the case study ‘Kerala: puzzling longevity’
124 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
In this chapter, we have primarily looked at your subject, as indicated in Figure 6.6.
As you proceed you have to make a number of further decisions here and these
define the kind of case study that you undertake. These choices about purpose,
approach and process interact variously to produce an array of permutations and,
ultimately, each unique study.
• you know a great deal about the case in question and you want to understand
some aspect of it – this is a local knowledge case
• it provides a particularly good example of something – this is a key case
• it reveals something interesting because it is different from the norm – this is an
outlier case.
Once you have established your reason for doing a case study, there are a number
of kinds of case studies for you to choose from. The classification in Figure 6.6
enables you to think about structure in such a way that you can review and select
from different design routes for case studies – routes that involve consideration of
purposes, approaches and processes. So, in the next three chapters I shall offer ideas
on how to think about:
REFLECTIVE ACTIVITY
In Figure 6.5 I mapped out the design for the case study ‘Kerala: puzzling longev-
ity’, using the typology to show how Caldwell developed this case study from its
outlier beginnings.
Choose one of the local knowledge cases – Ioan or Brandon – and trace a
path through the typology as I did in Figure 6.5 for the Kerala case study. Then
explain in a paragraph or two how the person conducting the case study is able
to use their intimate knowledge of the situation in question to offer insights from
various different angles and with different kinds of knowledge, in one of these
local knowledge cases. You may choose to inform your reflections by reading
Lawrence Stenhouse’s advice on the use of case study in research at https://
tinyurl.com/yca9y37m
Further Reading
Bassey, M. (1999) Case Study Research in Educational Settings. Maidenhead: Open
University Press.
Bassey discusses the case study in the context, principally, of educational action
research in classrooms. He expounds his idea of the case study producing a kind of
generalisation that he calls ‘fuzzy’. We can, in other words, be pointed in the right
direction by the findings of a case study without being certain about whether or not
a generalisation will apply in another setting.
de Vaus, D.A. (2001) Research Design in Social Research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Useful for putting the case study as a design frame in the context of many other
design frames.
Elman, C., Gerring, J. and Mahoney, J. (2016) ‘Case study research: putting the
quant into the qual’, Sociological Methods & Research, 45 (3): 375–91. https://doi.
org/10.1177/0049124116644273
A useful ‘left field’ view of case study and in particular study identification, recog-
nising that the selection of cases may be viewed through ‘a quantitative template’.
126 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
George, A.L. and Bennett, A. (2005) Case Studies and Theory Development in the Social
Sciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
A good combination of theoretical and hands-on advice on the construction of a
case study.
Why are you doing a case study? What is your object? Is it intrinsic interest or
is it as a means to another end – perhaps a way of getting a more rounded picture
of something, such as a medical procedure? Are you doing it to evaluate a process?
Maybe you want to conduct the study to explore the field before doing a different
kind of study. Maybe you are trying to explain what is going on in a situation.
Stake (2005) uses the words intrinsic and instrumental to separate the different pur-
poses you may have in your mind at the beginning of a study. Let’s look at these first.
Intrinsic
In an intrinsic case study, the subject is being studied not with a secondary purpose in
mind but out of interest, pure and simple. Of course, if we went on to try and analyse
our interest, it could come from any one of a number of angles, but we will put that to
one side for a moment. Sometimes, when a study is undertaken simply for its interest,
it is called ‘blue sky’ research or ‘curiosity-driven’ research – blue sky, because you are
free to think just of the sky; there are no obsta-
With an intrinsic study, you are
cles or barriers to block the view because there is
inquiring for the purpose of
inquiring. It is ‘blue sky’ research. no ulterior motive – that is, you are finding out
for the sake of finding out.
Stake (2005: 445) suggests that the case study is intrinsic
if the study is undertaken because, first and last, one wants better under-
standing of this particular case. It is not undertaken primarily because
the case represents other cases or because it illustrates a particular trait
or problem, but instead because, in all its particularity and ordinariness,
the case itself is of interest.
The intrinsic label is used to separate a study such as this from an instrumental study.
Instrumental
An instrument is a tool and an instrumental study is one that is done with a purpose
in mind. You are using the study as a tool; you are not doing it purely for the love
of knowledge. Is your research aimed at evaluating something? (More on this in a
moment.) Do you have a purpose in trying to understand with a view to making
things better? For example, if your case study is about Joshua’s persistent difficulty
with reading, your ultimate aim is probably going to be something to do with help-
ing Joshua. The case study in this sense is instrumental; it is a means to an end.
YOUR PURPOSE 129
Stake (2005: 445) makes the distinction between the intrinsic and the instrumen-
tal to emphasise the point that some case studies are
You may notice Stake’s use of the word ‘generalisation’ in this quotation. If so,
you may be saying to yourself, ‘Hang on. Didn’t the bloke who wrote this book
say on the first page that you can’t generalise from a case study?’ Well, yes, I did
say that and this is where it begins to become a little tricky (strokes beard mean-
ingfully), thinking about what the case study can and cannot offer. There may be
generalisations that each of us holds in our heads – not scientific generalisations,
but everyday generalisations – that a case study
With an instrumental case study,
may help to confirm or refute. You may remem-
the inquiry is serving a particular
ber that I discussed this property of case studies purpose. So, the case study is acting
in Chapter 4, where I noted their importance as an instrument – a tool.
Evaluative
Evaluation is framed by the expectation that you are doing the research to see how
well something is working or has worked. Something has been changed or a new
idea introduced, and evaluative research is carried out to find out what the change
has led to. Have things got better or worse – or just stayed the same?
There are, of course, many ways to answer questions such as these, and a case
study provides just one avenue into evaluative research. The main thrust of evalu-
ative research will usually involve counting something before the change has been
introduced, introducing the innovation, then counting again at the end. Whatever
the consequence – an improvement, a steady state or a deterioration – we have
grounds for believing that it may have been caused by the innovation. Mind you,
I should stress that the key words here are ‘we have grounds for believing’ since
there are many reasons for such changes to have happened. Indeed, much discus-
sion concerning social science method is about the confidence we can have in the
beliefs that we hold after research has taken place.
130 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
It is in these subtle clues that you may be able to build a story about what
has happened in this case. The key advantage of doing a case study is that you
are empowered to do this building job – building from varied sources and using
your own knowledge of the world, its intricacies, meanings and webs of under-
standings. With these, you construct an explanation, a story that knits all of the
threads together. The philosopher–educator Jerome Bruner (1991: 2) calls this
‘The narrative construction of reality’. In his famous article in Critical Inquiry,
Bruner (1991: 2) says:
This is important for understanding what the case study offers. Bruner is making
the point that there are different ways of understanding what people do and, if we
are assuming that we can use only in a limited way the methods of natural science
in social science, we must treat as different the knowledge that comes via building
the narrative.
Explanatory
Explaining is probably the most common purpose of a case study. Remember that,
in a case study, you are trading breadth of coverage for depth of understanding,
and potential explanations based on depth of understanding are what a case study
does best.
These explanations may be tentative or context-specific, but it is in the mul-
tifaceted nature of a case study that you get the opportunity to relate one bit to
another and offer explanations based on the interrelationships between these bits.
In being able unselfconsciously to look at these interrelationships, a case study
is, thus, the most powerful engine of potential explanations. We must remember,
however, that the explanations it offers may be limited to the background pro-
vided by the case study’s circumstances. Always be aware that you are looking at
one among many.
132 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
How can you start on this? Imagine that you are throwing a rope, a lasso,
around one situation that in some way exemplifies the area of your interest.
By defining the area of interest in this way, you give yourself the opportu-
nity to explain in this small situation. Here, you are thinking of classrooms
with extra people working alongside the class teacher, so choose one such
classroom. In this classroom, your attention is focused single-mindedly on
the problem: why is it the case that research shows additional adults do not
seem to help student attainment?
Once the question has been framed and the rope thrown round the situa-
tion, you can then begin to think around the issue, using a storyboard of the
kind shown in Figure 7.2. The boundary of the study here is this class – this
teacher with her 29 children in this room in this school. It is not, as would be
the case in other kinds of research, this variable isolated and manipulated in
this way, judging its effects on that variable. Your aim is explanation – albeit
tentative explanation, but explanation all the same – and this involves taking
a holistic view.
To get this holistic view with explanation in mind, you will be looking from
a number of different directions, and those directions will be determined by
the ideas that have emerged from your first storyboard.
For the first view, going back to Figure 7.2 and looking at the left-hand side
of the diagram, you might decide to look not at attainment but instead at
something that seemed not to be examined in the literature on this subject –
the children’s attention.
Figure 7.2 Storyboard of questions about the effects of having extra help in the
classroom
(Continued)
134 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
So, you set up two conditions for observing the children: first, on Monday
morning when Mrs Speed, the teaching assistant, is present in the class; and,
second, on Tuesday morning when the children are doing the same kind of
work but Mrs Speed is not present. You look at each child in each situation,
video recording their behaviour. Then you compare the two situations. From
this, you are able to say whether or not there was a significant difference in
the children’s behaviour in the two situations. This on its own, though, is not
enough.
For a second view, going back to the left-hand side of the diagram again,
moving up, we come to happiness. Even if attainment cannot be shown to
change, might the children be happier with an aide or assistant present?
How could you examine this? Could you look at how much the children are
smiling? Probably not a good idea since, even if you could find a good way to
measure a smile (impossible, surely), you could not be sure that smiling was
an accurate representation of happiness. Maybe children who appeared to
be smiling were merely trying to contain excess wind. Maybe they were smil-
ing because they were laughing and joking in a classroom with little control.
Would this be a measure of happiness? We know that children do not like
being in uncontrolled classrooms, so, even though they may be laughing,
they may not be happy. (Social research – it’s not easy, is it?)
Perhaps it would be better to use some other indicator of happiness, such
as a self-report. You could, in other words, simply ask the children, using
charts with smileys (), how happy they are in the different situations.
Moving to the right-hand side of Figure 7.2, we come to a different set
of issues entirely. Here are some quite profound questions about the ways
in which people work together, and some interesting ideas have emerged
from your storyboarding about the effects that putting two people together
in a working environment can have. In fact, academics in economics and
management science know these effects well, summarising them in ‘the law
of diminishing returns’. This law is that each unit of extra resource – whether
it be people or machines – will produce less than the one before it. It may
not be 100 per cent appropriate to this situation, but it is a clue as to what
might be happening when there are extra people in the classroom. More
people equals more confusion if the extra people do not know what they
are doing. (They may not know what they are doing since teachers are unfa-
miliar with having to manage and work with other people in their territory.)
So, how could you, in your case study, try to examine whether or not this
law of diminishing returns might be operating? You could do it in a number
of ways – perhaps actually observing to see if there are any signs of confusion
or irritation. You might ask the teaching assistants themselves, by interview or
questionnaire, how they feel about being in this situation. Do they feel unsure?
YOUR PURPOSE 135
Do they feel directionless? You might ask the teacher if she feels unconfident
about how to handle and manage the additional staff. You might watch the
children for signs of them taking advantage of confusion between the teacher
and the assistant. Are they capitalising on ambiguity?
So, with the case study you would be doing the ‘drilling down’ into this situation
to provide the explanation for the kind of paradoxical finding about attainment.
Exploratory
An exploratory case study will be done where you are faced with a problem or an
issue that perplexes you. You need to know more: what is happening and why?
You may have little preliminary knowledge of the issue – or, at least, little rounded
knowledge of it. You may have some familiarity with it, but what you know may be
one-dimensional since you only ever see the situation as one person – in your role
as a teacher or as a visiting parent or the school’s headteacher or a midday supervi-
sor who helps out as a volunteer during lessons.
Let’s take an example.
Suppose that the first part of your study is to ask a range of different people
about their impressions of Joel. You ask each of them – his teacher, parents,
headteacher, the midday supervisor – why they think that Joel freezes when
the time for SATs is approaching. Given that the study is exploratory, you will
principally be listening for ideas, rather than presenting ideas to test against
your respondents’ views. It would be inappropriate to say, for example, ‘Do
you think that Joel has dyslexia?’ This would be very much framing the initial
ideas of the respondents according to constructs you have suggested –
putting words into their mouths.
You might set aside an hour each morning for a week to watch Joel in
class at a time when he and his peers are doing more formal work. You would
likely observe in an unstructured way, trying to pick up any clues about Joel’s
behaviour from the contextual antecedents (that is, the things that hap-
pened before) and consequences (the things that happened after) of formal
work. Did he seem to use avoidance strategies? Did he look anxious? Did he
try to remove himself from situations with other students? Did he start mess-
ing around when structured work was imminent? Did his teacher or teaching
assistant seem to be expressing any anxiety through their behaviour, and
could this be transferring itself to Joel?
It is with such exploratory questions, followed by observations and open-
ended interviews, that you would begin to conduct an exploratory case
study for Joel’s situation. As ‘leads’ appear, you follow them up. Some might
seem to have substance, and it is these that you then test further.
Here is another example, from real life. It is a much more complex one, in the
arena of public health. It shows, I hope, how useful the case study can be when you
have a seemingly amorphous problem. In fact, it shows not just how the case study
can be valuable in an exploratory sense, but also how these explorations can be
taken further, narrowing down the area of search as more data are gathered.
(Continued)
138 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
More Other
infected sources of
cattle infection,
The start here? such as
Local meat-
of a national from
epidemic? handling
dentistry?
practices?
Figure 7.3 Exploring and generating ideas is a haphazard process, so follow the
ones that seem most likely first
The first rule is to think. The investigators had to think about these alterna-
tives and start with the most likely. They started by looking into cattle-rearing
practices locally. They found that:
YOUR PURPOSE 139
Local beef cattle were raised alongside dairy cattle. This meant that
beef cattle were fed meat and bonemeal supplements from the age
of 6 days rather than 6 months, which is the case for pure beef herds.
(Bryant and Monk, 2001: 2)
The upshot was that the cattle raised here had a higher chance of incubating
BSE than did cattle elsewhere, having had a longer exposure to the prion in
bonemeal than in many places elsewhere, and this area of Leicestershire did
indeed prove to have a high incidence of BSE.
A strong additional candidate for culpability was the nature of the meat
eaten in this particular place, which led the investigative trail to butchers’
shops. At this point the story gets rather gory, so, if you are squeamish, it is
best to avoid the next few paragraphs.
Butchers in the area obtained their carcasses in one of three ways:
So, the investigators also looked in more detail at the differing cattle-slaugh-
tering practices in each place.
They discovered that the local practice in small slaughterhouses included
the insertion of a ‘pithing rod’ after the cattle were killed with a bolt. The pith-
ing rod is pushed through the bolt-hole in the brain of the cow to prevent the
animal kicking, which can occur after the use of the bolt. This practice is not
used in large slaughterhouses (and the practice has now been made illegal).
There were two other major differences:
Local killing and butchering practices, then, provided a likely candidate for
infection. They could actually be responsible for spilling the infective agent
(Continued)
140 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
out of the brains of the cattle and on to the raw meat. Using the traditional
butchers’ methods, this material would not be hosed away. In fact, the use of
cloths to wipe the carcasses could actually make matters worse, spreading
the infective agent around.
Following their initial investigations, Bryant and Monk refined their origi-
nal ‘thought bubble’ ideas (see Figure 7.3) to arrive at a hypothesis that:
Bryant and Monk tested the second part of this hypothesis by comparing
the source of meat consumed by vCJD patients with that of non-patients.
Was there a pattern, with the families of patients with vCJD sourcing their
meat from traditional local butchers? Using a structured questionnaire, they
first of all asked relatives of the infected patients about diet and purchasing
patterns for meat during the 1980s. The responses here were compared with
those of a set of ‘controls’, these being comparable people who had not
contracted vCJD.
The findings were, in short, that four of the five vCJD casualties had regu-
larly consumed meat from one of two local butchers, who had sourced their
meat from cattle killed using traditional slaughtering methods. The meat
bought by the controls was from a far wider range of retailers (supermarkets,
freezer stores and other butchers). The latter – the ‘safe’ retailers – were
questioned about the sources of their meat, which turned out, as expected,
to have come from further afield and not butchered using the traditional
methods.
Using simple statistics, it was then possible to show that the likelihood
of this pattern occurring by chance was very slim indeed. The point I keep
making, about the impossibility of generalising from case study, however, is
still valid and Bryant and Monk (2001: 5) were wisely cautious in their conclu-
sions. They say that the study:
This case study is from the field of public health, but the steps taken by these inves-
tigators when doing this exploratory work are common to all such investigations,
in any field. They involve:
• evaluative – where the aim is to find out how well something is working or whether
it has worked as expected
142 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
• explanatory – where the phenomenon in which you are interested needs ‘unpack-
ing’, the connections between different parts of the issue need unravelling and the
case study offers a route to explanation
• exploratory – where little is known and the principal purpose is to establish the
‘shape’ of the problem or issue.
The issue is about the way in which the analytical frame for your study will take
form.
REFLECTIVE ACTIVITY
In two of the boxed examples of case studies in this chapter, I have used story-
boards (I outline how to use them in Chapter 2) to help identify an object or
purpose for the case study.
Now look at the boxed case study under the ‘Evaluative’ subheading in this
chapter. I have based it around the paradoxical finding that a new office rota
scheme has resulted in a fall-off in productivity in the office, and have moved
straight into a theory-building case study route which allows ideas to emerge as
data collection proceeds. In other words, I have not used a storyboard: I wanted
ideas to emerge from research participants. An alternative to this would have
been to begin with a set of ideas about why productivity might fall in a sce-
nario such as this. How would you gather such a set of ideas? A good way to
start would be to draw a storyboard. Draw a storyboard that outlines the various
hypotheses and directions you might follow in this inquiry.
Further Reading
Acemoglu, D. and Robinson, J.A. (2012) Why Nations Fail. London: Profile Books.
The authors of this book ask why some nations are more prosperous than others.
They set out a series of key cases which are instrumental in that they aim to demon-
strate the validity of an already-developed thesis.
that different kinds of research find common cause in being empirical – in other
words, in finding out from (our own) experience and analysis, as distinct from what
some very important authority tells us.
Booth, W.C., Colomb, G.C. and Williams, J.M. (2016) The Craft of Research (4th
edn). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Booth and his colleagues give one of the best overviews I have come across of why it
is important to look at your purpose. Chapter 14 is especially helpful for establish-
ing the rationale for your work. The book looks at research as a holistic, integrated
process that, although it is integrated, does not just ‘happen’. You have to think
about your purpose, what has been done before and what you have to offer to the
area with your proposed research. Although they are not looking at the case study
in particular, the stance they take on research is very relevant to the case study,
stressing, as it does, the integrity of the complete work. You have to work at making
the integrity real for the reader, though, and this is what the book helps you to do.
Thomas, G. (ed.) (2013) Case Study Methods in Education: Volume 4: The Case Study in
Practice – General Issues and Specific Examples. London: Sage.
Certain examples are given in this chapter (that is, in this chapter of this book), but
the volume I refer to here contains many examples of published case studies con-
ducted for different purposes in different contexts.
Thomas, G. and Myers, K. (2015) The Anatomy of the Case Study. London: Sage.
Here, Kevin Myers and I give detailed examples of different kinds of case study and
how they can be analysed according to the typology I have outlined.
Tight, M. (2017) Understanding Case Study Research: Small-Scale Research with Meaning.
London: Sage.
A well-organised book on the nature and use of case study research, with much
good, practical advice.
8
YOUR APPROACH:
THEORY TESTING OR
THEORY BUILDING;
INTERPRETATION
OR ILLUSTRATION
Subject Purpose Approach Process
Retrospective
Snapshot
Outlier Intrinsic Testing a theory Diachronic
Key Instrumental Building a theory Single
Local Evaluative Drawing a picture Multiple
Explanatory Experimental Nested
Exploratory Interpretative Parallel
Sequential
Having looked at your purpose in Chapter 7, you can think now about the approach
you are going to take in your study (see Figure 8.1). Are you, for your analytical
YOUR APPROACH 145
frame, or object, going to try to build your own set of ideas from scratch (build-
ing a theory) or are you going to test ideas that you have come across elsewhere
(testing a theory)? Are you going to do both of these, as in the vCJD case study in
Chapter 7? Is your approach in the case study to illustrate something, perhaps a
process that you need to explicate in more detail for some purpose? Will you be
seeking to uncover and interpret what is going on in the area of your searchlight
beam?
At the other end of the continuum, the shape is already there, like a template,
and the job you set yourself is to see whether or not the template, the theory, in
fact provides satisfactory explanations for the data you are collecting. Remember,
though, that all research – a case study or otherwise – begins with an inspiration or
a hunch and it is this that leads to the journey of your research. You cannot begin
without the hunch stage, whether you are building or testing a theory.
Building a theory
‘Theory’ is a tricky word in the world of research since it has so many meanings. I’ll
come to this and discuss it a little more in Chapter 11, but, for now, let’s assume that
theory means an explanatory model. Building a theory is therefore about develop-
ing, almost from scratch, a framework of ideas, a model, that somehow explains the
subject you are researching.
So, in a theory-building case study, you are aiming to develop ideas, starting off
in much the same way that you did for an exploratory case study, but taking the
146 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
process much further. This is much more than exploration. You build a framework
of ideas that has no overt connection to preformulated notions about what is
important here. Perhaps that is putting it too strongly, since it is impossible not to
connect to pre-existing ideas. What I mean is, you have no allegiance to pre-existing
ideas and no presuppositions. Your pre-existing ideas will always be there – you
cannot erase them from your mind, after all – but you must have no commitment
to them as potential answers. Your mind should be open to new interpretations
suggested by your data.
• the station
• a group of people in a station
• one person who has expressed a fear of a terrorist incident?
Whichever you choose, your focus must be the site of something that is of
interest. Remember again Wieviorka’s (1992: 160) comment that a case ‘is
significant only if an observer … can refer it to an analytical category’. It can-
not simply be plucked out of the air. You cannot simply think, ‘I’ll do a case
study on this station’, with no special reason for choosing that station. If you
do choose to focus on a station, then there must be a reason for focusing
on the ‘station-ness’ of this particular station. So, for the case study, there
are two issues:
How, then, can you proceed? The answer is to make a broad assumption
at the outset about the importance of your chosen focus – about the station
and its ‘station-ness’ – then follow the usual steps that I have been talking
about until now, getting as many different sides of the situation as far as this
focus is concerned. These might include starting points about noise, prox-
imity of people, movement, places to sit, presence of concealed entrances,
presence of bins, and so on. You can then find ways to explore these issues
and let the information that you discover speak for itself. You do not go
into a theory-building study expecting to confirm the significance of these
features of the situation – rather, you explore their importance.
So, our theory-building case study might be called ‘The station: a site of fear
about global terrorism’. The weight is immediately being thrown on the station
as the core of your inquiry. You are making it clear in the first two words that it
is the station that is the focus of your interest. It is this that provides the kernel
for your inquiry into terror and why people should feel it. Does the station have
any features that exacerbate (or perhaps lessen) people’s fears? Your methods
need to be geared towards finding an answer to this basic question; then the
theory will grow around the data you collect as a result of your inquiries.
Presence of
Noise concealed
entrances
Fear in stations
themes that are developing, the theory building, as in Figure 8.3. Here, in
this redrawn figure, you can see that the explanation, the theory, is building
around the people who are present in stations, rather than the physical struc-
tures that are there (such as bins and seats).
Proximity of
people
Presence of
bins
Presence of
concealed Fear in stations
entrances
So, in the redrawn storyboard, I have moved the boxes around so that
the important features are closer together and represented by heavier lines.
There are, moreover, connections between the boxes.
Testing a theory
The assumption here is that there is already some sort of explanatory framework
available for the phenomenon or situation on which you are focusing. Your case
study, then, is being undertaken to test this explanatory framework, this ‘theory’.
(Continued)
150 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
COVID-19 virus has been particularly high. You want to examine why this
might be the case.
There will already be a great deal of existing ‘theory’ to explain why lev-
els of the COVID-19 infection are higher in particular places. It is known,
for example, that rates are higher in hospitals that have more vulnerable
patients and undertake more invasive specialist care, and lower in paediatric
units. It is known also that the frequency of handwashing by staff is nega-
tively correlated with rates of other infection – such as MRSA infection – that
is, the more handwashing, the better. Furthermore, figures can be affected
by the number of patients transferring from one hospital to another, and
between care homes and hospitals.
All of this pre-existing knowledge and theory provides a substantial back-
drop for the construction of your own theoretical formulation about the high
level of infection by COVID-19 in your hospital. In fact, it is important to stress
that all of this contextual information is essential to the conduct of a case
study and the ‘shape’ of your own emerging theory. The fact that you are
doing a case study does not absolve you from the task of setting the study
in context and finding all that there is to know about this particular situation.
In doing your case study, your first port of call will be this pre-existing
theory. Does your hospital fall into one of the categories which make it par-
ticularly susceptible to infection (for example, more vulnerable patients,
such as those who are elderly)? If not, you can move on and assume that
there are some other features of the situation that are causing your high
rates of infection, and it is these features that may form the basis for your
theory-testing case study.
So, the theory that you test will be based on existing knowledge and your
own knowledge of the situation. As I noted earlier, all research starts with a
hunch, and your own hunch will be based on some key observations that you
have made yourself as a member of the hospital staff. Imagine that you have
heard comments such as:
• ‘Everyone knows it’s there and everyone in the know realises that we
can’t get rid of Covid.’
• ‘The politicians, public and media are ignorant – we know that Covid
can’t be got rid of.’
• ‘It’s about more than washing your hands.’
• ‘It’s an excuse, a smokescreen, for too little money being spent on the
health service on better PPE equipment, better cleaning. They are trying
to blame frontline service providers for lack of equipment and shortcuts
on cleaning.’
YOUR APPROACH 151
Let’s assume that your experience includes working in several hospitals and,
alongside these comments, your casual observations suggest that there
seems to be less concern about handwashing in this one. You may feel that
there is a culture of complacency about this aspect of hygiene, which goes
right the way through the system – from the cleaners and ward orderlies
right up to senior management.
At the heart of your ‘theory’ here is an implied comparison with other
places and their culture of hygiene, but you have nothing on which to base
this comparison other than your own experience. To test this comparison
out empirically, you would have to visit other hospitals and collect data to
compare with data from your own place of employment. This might involve
a comparative case study, but time precludes this.
Initially, you might like to look at the kinds of features of this situation
that constitute the culture in general, and a culture of complacency about
hygiene in particular.
The first question that you need to ask here is about the supposed culture
of complacency and resistance. Your focus would be on the presence of this
culture in a wider culture – a highly visible and highly audible one – among
politicians, health service managers, the media and the public about the
importance of hygiene. No one could claim not to have heard about the
need for not just hygiene but much better hygiene – all the time, the mes-
sage is coming through loud and clear.
What kind of narrative could be built around this mismatch between
knowledge and action? In other words, what can we infer from the fact that
there might be a culture of complacency in an environment of excellent
knowledge about the danger of this superbug? What does it tell us?
Surely, it says that there is something else going on in the social environ-
ment of the hospital that is subverting the message about the danger of lack
of hygiene. No one could claim that the message is unimportant, so, if it is
being undermined, what is going on?
Your theory – the theory you want to test – is that the ‘undermining factor’
concerns a resentment about management and new initiatives. This resent-
ment is finding its expression in an oppositional attitude to innovation in this
situation. The storyboard for this might be as shown in Figure 8.4.
For the theory testing that you are doing in this case study, then, you
have made a number of assumptions based on the literature and your own
knowledge, with a twist of your own analysis. From here, you can go on to
test the theory in a number of different settings, as shown in Figure 8.4 –
that is, by discussing the matter with staff, watching people’s behaviour,
(Continued)
152 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
listening to them talking and hearing the official and unofficial messages
that are articulated during committee, working group and other meetings.
Recording
Talking to Observing Listening to comments
staff behaviour conversations made in
meetings
Theory: an undermining
factor – resentment about Internal
External management contextual
contextual information
information
The theory you are testing, in other words, has emerged, has ‘thickened
around’, an idea that you had at the outset, which has been reinforced as
you have looked into the issue further.
The highest levels in the learning process, that is, virtuosity and true
expertise, are reached only via a person’s own experiences as practi-
tioner of the relevant skills. Therefore, beyond using the case method
and other experiential methods for teaching, the best that teachers can
do for students in professional programs is to help them achieve real
practical experience; for example, via placement arrangements, intern-
ships, summer jobs, and the like.
The newly qualified dentists thus have to engage in a whole range of further
training and supervised practice. Each element of this postgraduate work has to
demonstrate that important activities have been covered and shows how profes-
sional development will continue so that the dentists in question can prove that
they are able to keep up to date as their careers progress. The case study has to illus-
trate an aspect of this professional development.
The case study is a form of inquiry that is especially well suited to these needs at
this stage in the dentists’ careers. They have all done the important basics in their
undergraduate work at university and shown their competence in these basics. As
they start real work on real people, their needs become more individual, with a
more individually tailored programme of postgraduate work necessary for each.
So, a starting point was that each case study in the year’s cohort of young den-
tists was always likely to be different from the next one: there is idiosyncrasy in the
work of each one. The study could take one of many forms – the use of a particular
piece of equipment, a week in the working life of the practice receptionist, the
use of X-ray equipment by the practice, a particular patient’s experience, and so
on. The only proviso was that the case study should in some way exemplify and
demonstrate the learning that had happened on the course in the postgraduate pro-
fessional year. Clearly not everything could be covered, so it was specified that the
case study should focus on and illustrate two of the four main areas covered in the
postgraduate work – areas such as business skills, clinical skills, patient psychology
and control of infection. It may not be Goffmanesque in the beauty of its analysis
or in the depth of its insight, but this illustrative case study did lead to better under-
standing and development of the dental practice.
(Continued)
156 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Nav first had to make absolutely sure that his case study of his patient
‘Hannah’ was ethical, with Hannah giving her informed consent to the use
of the information relating to her case (this is discussed more fully on pages
85–103). He planned for the case study both to demonstrate knowledge that
had been acquired and to illustrate how this knowledge had been used with
Hannah. To draw the maximum benefit from this (and to gain the highest
marks) Nav would attempt to draw out problematic aspects of the case to
exemplify how the problem had been overcome or indicate where there was
a systemic problem – that is to say, a problem to do with the system that had
been set up in the practice.
Nav started by looking for ways in which he could gain purchase on
Hannah’s experience. Available to him were the practice records of appoint-
ments, letters and emails and his own records as a new dentist. He planned
to supplement these records with two sets of interviews – one with Hannah
herself and another with his mentor, Jayne, who happened to be a senior
partner in the practice. As an important part of patient communication, Nav
also planned to look at the practice leaflets and other publicity and how
these were dovetailed (or were not) with phone calls and emails. With all
of these Nav hoped to secure leverage on Hannah’s experience and ‘drill
down’ (no pun intended) into this. With the information, he would be able
not only to illustrate his own connection with and understanding of the prac-
tice, but also, if possible, to offer signposts for improvement in the practice’s
work and his own.
Nav started by drawing a storyboard, as shown in Figure 8.6. On the
left-hand side of the storyboard are some points Nav planned to address
concerning his business planning module, and on the right are some fea-
tures relating to patient psychology.
Given that he was new to the practice and Hannah was also new, he felt it
appropriate to look at the practice’s ‘front of house’ habits, as well as its meth-
ods for acquiring new patients and finding out about their needs. Much of this
questioning could be done in a structured interview. Richer information – for
example, about the demeanour of staff and their willingness to help – could
be gathered in an unstructured rather than a structured interview.
On the right-hand side of the diagram are the issues to do with patient
psychology and whether or not the practice adequately handles this, offering
help and support where necessary, both in the person-to-person contacts
that happen immediately on coming through the door and in the general
ambience of the place. Are there flowers around? Are there comforting pic-
tures (rather than posters of molars, exposed nerves and adverts for teeth
whitening)? Are there large notices about the dire consequences of not turn-
ing up to appointments? Is Mozart playing calmingly in the background or
YOUR APPROACH 157
can you hear the mosquito-like whine of drills behind doors? That kind of
thing.
For both sides of the diagram, the case study is an ideal form of inquiry,
for it provides, if it is done properly, the kinds of data that will not be gained
from other kinds of inquiry. It provides:
This case study illustrates why it is necessary to take into account the location of
the fracture when decisions are made about the nature of the remedial action that
should be taken to fix the problem. This is, if you like, the analytic frame or object
YOUR APPROACH 159
(see pages 15–19). It demonstrates what can go wrong and why, as well as being
instructive for practitioners, bringing a theoretical set of issues to life.
In medicine, illustrative case studies can also be used to reveal a new or unfamil-
iar set of issues, a new syndrome or a promising new technique, yet to be trialled
for its efficacy with a larger number of people. With any kind of inquiry, it may also
be added to a larger piece of research to exemplify a theme or illustrate an aspect
of the analysis.
Case studies are also often used in the teaching or explication of the law. As
in medicine, they also tend to be illustrative. A good website for viewing some
US legal cases is Stanford Law School’s Case Studies Collection (available at: www.
law.stanford.edu/organizations/programs-and-centers/environmental-and-natural-
resources-law-policy-program-enrlp/case-studies).
Interpretative
Often, when a case study is written about in the academic literature, what is being
discussed is an interpretative case study. This is, if you like, the ‘classic’ approach to
doing a case study. This is not to deny the importance and significance of the other
approaches I have talked about so far, but the aims of a case study and the style of
interpretative inquiry dovetail together very nicely. For this reason, I shall go into
this approach in rather more detail than the previous ones.
Interpretative inquiry is a form of inquiry that employs a particular approach to
answering questions – an approach that assumes an in-depth understanding and
deep immersion in the environment of the subject. You can probably see why,
then, I have said that the case study and interpretative inquiry are natural bedfel-
lows, since each calls for rich, intensive understanding. I should go further: they are
not only natural bedfellows but also obvious marriage partners. They were made for
each other: it is love and marriage – and they go together like a horse and carriage.
Each demands a deep understanding of the multifaceted nature of social situations,
so they complement each other and seem natural with each other.
This kind of approach is often called ethnographic, so a case study using such an
approach may be called an ethnography. The word ‘ethnography’ is from the Greek
ethnos, meaning folk or people, so ethnography literally means ‘study of folk’. It
was used first in a field of social research that emerged in the early part of the twen-
tieth century as a branch of anthropology.
Before the new ethnographers, anthropologists had done their work by treating
people as objects of study, as dispassionately as a biologist might study an insect.
Displacing this style of work, the new ethnography aimed to get to the heart of
160 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
people’s understandings of life by doing fieldwork with them rather than suppos-
edly objective study of them. One could, interestingly in relation to the case study,
say that they aimed to reach what is summed up by the Latin phrase casus anima,
which roughly translates as ‘the soul of the case’ or ‘the heart of the matter’; they
aimed to reach an understanding of what makes the person or people tick.
So the ethnographers aimed to get right into the centre of the cultures with
which they worked by becoming members of those cultures. They would actually
live with the people and try to understand their culture from within. It was as if the
earlier anthropologists were taking callipers and rulers to measure the people they
were looking at, while the ethnographers, by contrast, thought that the best tool
for the study of other human beings was themselves, as fellow human beings.
The ethnographers, therefore – people such as Bronisław Malinowski, who in
1914 went to live among the Trobriand Islanders of the Western Pacific – suggested
that, instead of trying to distance yourself from your subjects of study, you should,
as a social scientist, get as close as possible by becoming a participant in their cul-
tures. Try to understand them as a person yourself, by being a participant observer.
Forget about any pretence at objectiveness; do not deny that you are a person,
pretending that you can see things objectively. This is never possible, said the eth-
nographers. Not only is it impossible, but it simultaneously throws away one of
your ready-made strengths as an inquirer among people. That strength is you –
yourself – which has to be your watchword in doing the kind of case study that I
am talking about here.
The ethnographers influenced, and in turn were influenced by, the thought of a
group of social scientists in the 1920s and 1930s who said that the world in which
we are interested as social scientists is not straightforwardly perceivable in the way
that the world of chemicals is perceived by chemists. We cannot view it like this
because it is constructed by each of us in a different way. It is not simply ‘out there’;
it is different for each of us. It cannot therefore be adequately studied using the
methods of the natural scientists (such as physicists and chemists), with talk of
variables and quantification – a wholly different mindset and set of procedures is
needed for inquiring into it. This view came to be called interpretivism.
This approach to social study caught on like wildfire and spawned a range
of fascinating, in-depth ethnographies in the middle of the twentieth century,
such as Street Corner Society by William Foote Whyte (1955/1993). For more than
three years, Whyte lived in a slum district of Boston, populated largely by Italian
immigrants. He lived among and with the people and documented how young
men became either ‘corner boys’ or ‘college boys’. It is interesting to look at the
way in which Whyte reflected on theory in doing this, because it might be said
that this kind of interpretative case study is inherently about building a theory.
YOUR APPROACH 161
So, the kind of theory that we are looking for is more a temporary ‘conceptual frame-
work’ or ‘thinking tool’ than it is an abstraction in its own right and for its own
purpose. It should not be an intellectual barricade, to be defended against all-comers.
Rather, it should help you to think about and understand the subject in hand. It can
be held, tested and then discarded or retained depending on its usefulness.
It is important to remember that some social scientists, in the bad old days,
wished to develop what has come to be called ‘grand theory’. In the words of the
science fiction comedy The Hitchhiker’s Guide to the Galaxy, these are theories of
‘life, the universe and everything’. It is now generally accepted that that kind of
theory is unattainable and it is not useful to try and establish it. In fact, the attempt
162 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
to establish it has probably done social science a good deal of harm, as the grand
theory that is set up is often wildly misleading.
I can give as an example of an interpretative case study a piece of work that I
conducted while I was a parent governor of the secondary school that my daughters
attended.
that the slower he speaks, the more gravitas he has. Chairing for bor-
ing, inconsequential, dull or plain stupid people is clearly a problem
in this kind of forum (or in most kinds of forum). The trouble is that
we’re all such nice people, we don’t want to offend the feelings of
others – and this is especially so when we are fully aware in this kind of
setting (that is, as governors) that we’re all working voluntarily and out
of the goodness of our hearts. You don’t want to discourage people
or seem to be denying or downplaying the quality or value of their
contribution. It’s doubly a problem, though, when the body is taking on
quasi-management roles and there is the need to be a little more disci-
plined about the machinery that makes things happen. If that discipline
doesn’t exist (or exists badly) there’s the possibility the machinery will
become completely gummed up.
How can this be turned into interpretation and theory? In other words, how
can an analytical frame be established? It is important to note that the diary
I was keeping was not simply ‘this happened … then that happened … then
this happened … and so on’. Rather, it was my reflections on the topic in
hand, with a critical edge to the commentary. It is not, as the great anthro-
pologist Clifford Geertz (1975: 9) put it, as if I was merely a ‘cipher clerk’ in
making these recordings. Although you cannot perhaps appreciate it from
this diary entry on its own, my attempt in doing this case study was to look
at the ability of governors to fulfil a role that the government at the time
had set them, which was to act almost as managerial overseers – with a slice
of ‘voice of the people’ twisted in. In this particular diary entry, I was saying
something about the ability of school governors to fulfil this role. The key
idea coming through in this entry is of amateurism, of not being able to fulfil
this role. This can perhaps be seen better in the next excerpt from the diary:
There are some blatant clues here as to the constructs that I was building.
For a start, I use the phrase ‘well-meaning amateurs’, which is hardly a covert
or disguised expression of my views. It needs little interpretation. There are,
however, more interesting comments about the nature of the enterprise in
which governors are occupied.
First, I make a comment about the sort of language and kind of deliv-
ery that Walter is using – it is of an innocent amateur playing at being a
professional. Then, there is his error in calculating the costs of heating and
lighting. The error is serious, not only for the consequences it might have
had on policy (excluding community use of the school facilities) but also for
the effects it had on the work of senior management in the school. My sub-
sequent thinking – my ‘theorisation’ – about the diary commentary was that
government policy, by mistrusting professionals, had produced a ‘double
whammy’. It had not only displaced the professionals from their proper role
but also wasted the professionals’ time as they then had to intervene and
sort out the mess that the amateurs like Walter were creating.
A stronger narrative around trust, professionalism and the audit culture
could be made of this. It could even be linked with an idea such as that
of Richard Sennett’s (2009) in his book The Craftsman, where he suggests
that much of the lack of purpose, the anomie, of modern life is about the
dismissal of the craft of the tradesperson and the professional – whether the
craft is that of an architect, teacher, X-ray technician or zookeeper. Too many
of today’s crafts are made insipid and trivial, argues Sennett, not only as a
result of the intervention of machines and computers but also, even more
importantly, because of the procedures that remove the care, skill, pride and
dedication from our craft.
Using this insight, I could – for theorising my argument – ‘step sideways’
and look at what enfeebles and belittles the craft of teachers, removes their
YOUR APPROACH 165
The case study in this circumstance could be either building or testing a theory,
depending on how I had framed it at the beginning, but the important point, as
far as this section is concerned, is that interpretation is being used. The germ of the
idea at the outset could be about policy decisions and their journey from idea (the
idea of the politician or whoever) to action. You collect data about this journey and
make interpretations about what is happening along the way.
Experimental
When people talk about case studies, the experiment is probably the last thing that
comes into their minds. We assume that a case study is about singularity and depth,
whereas an experiment is about large numbers, control and comparing this group
with that. This assumption, however, while partly correct when talking of experi-
ments, is only partly so.
Certainly, an experiment is a particular kind of research design where ideas are
being tested under controlled conditions, but, in everyday language, ‘to experiment’
has a much looser meaning. It can just mean to try something out, and so ‘an experi-
ment’ could be simply a little trial of some kind. So, I might say ‘I’m going to do a little
experiment’ if I choose one day to try putting my milk in my tea before rather than
after the hot water to see if the tea tastes better the new way. (I did try it and it doesn’t.)
When approaching certain kinds of questions for case studies, we can take the
systematicity of the experiment and graft it onto our expectations of a case study.
This requires a particular kind of experiment, though. Before going into this – into
the kind of experiment that might be used in case study – let me say a little more
about experiments and how we can think about them.
166 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
potentially relevant variables: the variation in the ages of the children, the colour of
the walls, the direction of the light, the heating in the room, the presence or not of
a certain naughty boy, the hungriness of the teacher (depending on whether or not
she had to skip her breakfast that morning), the kind of work the children are doing,
the educational background of the teaching assistant, and so on.
We can potentially change any of these things and, in a perfect world, control for all
of the others. The trouble is, though, that we are not in a perfect world and we cannot
control for all of them (or even most of them), and we do not even know which ones we
should be controlling. We can guess, certainly, but there is a strong chance that we may
be wrong. There is also the problem, as the psychologist Jacob Kounin (1970) noted,
that the classroom is like an ecology: you cannot change one aspect of it without some
unexpected consequence. So, if you take out the naughty boy – let’s call him Kyle – you
may find that another member of the class takes over Kyle’s role. Playing with variables
in a social situation is a bit like squeezing a balloon – it will bulge somewhere else.
So a preliminary, cautionary note is needed when we are talking about a manip-
ulation of the variables of the classroom – and an experiment demands such
manipulation. It makes the assumption that all of the variables can be manipulated
and, when we manipulate them, we can look to see how this manipulation has had
an effect on other variables.
In its simplest form, an experiment seeks to isolate two variables – the one that you
assume may cause change (such as the science curriculum) and the one you are assum-
ing may change as a result (the understanding of science, in the example that we have
been using) – from the myriad that might be at play in a situation. Beyond this, there
is a vast superstructure of kinds of experiments that are built on this very simple basic
assumption, but we do not need to go into them here as it would be most unusual, if
not impossible, to find some of these designs incorporated into a case study.
What kinds of experiment, then, can be done in a case study? Well, the case
study has to conform to the basic idea of what a case study is, which is a study of
one thing. Interestingly, research specialists who are clever with numbers have a
special way of saying ‘one thing’, which is ‘n = 1’. I can already feel the adrenaline
beginning to flow into the veins of some of my readers as I include an equals sign
in the text. I know that you might be saying to yourself, ‘I thought by doing a case
study that I was going to be able to avoid equations and statistics.’
Well, you should not think that way. The method you choose should be the serv-
ant of your research question and if your research question demands an inquiry
that uses numbers and simple statistics, it is these you should use. People’s fear of
statistics is often completely groundless, and the kinds of statistics that you need
for this kind of study are actually quite simple. I look briefly at these in How to Do
Your Research Project (Thomas, 2017).
168 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
The usual form, the classic experiment (see Table 8.1), in the social science situa-
tion, involves two or more groups being treated in exactly the same way, except for
the manipulation of one variable. So, the procedure is to bring in an extra group,
as alike as possible to the first group, and give them everything that the first group
had so we can eliminate sources of variation between the groups – every source of
variation except the one we are deliberately varying. Any differences that then exist
between the two groups after the manipulation is made to happen by the experi-
menter are taken to exist because of the experimental treatment.
effectively with each other, sometimes getting in the way of each other, and
certainly not communicating effectively.
Her first task is to set down a question and draw a storyboard that might begin
to sketch out some lines of inquiry that could be addressed (see Figure 8.7).
Alyssa has one permanent teaching assistant and two parent volunteers
who come in every morning. The management and organisation of this kind
of assistance were not part of her teacher-training curriculum, so Alyssa has
read the literature and found that there are a number of ways of thinking
about how extra assistance of this kind can be deployed. One method is
called ‘room management’ (Cremin et al., 2005) and involves giving each
adult a set of very specific tasks to do (such as predominantly helping indi-
viduals or groups), those tasks constituting a named role. The roles can be
changed from session to session, but the main thing is that each individual
knows which role he or she is supposed to be fulfilling.
Lack of
communication?
With each other? Not knowing
With me? what to do?
Alyssa thinks that this method of organisation holds some promise and
hopes it will provide some structure for her work with other adults in her class.
Rather than simply trying it out and proceeding according to a gut feeling
about whether or not the new system of organisation seems to work, she does
a small experiment. This involves a repeated measures design where the one
group (Alyssa’s class) is examined under two circumstances, or conditions –
the only change being to the system of organisation in her class. In the first
condition Alyssa and the other adults work normally; in the second, the only
change is that they will use the room management system of organisation.
(Continued)
170 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
To assess the effect of this change, Alyssa decides to measure the chil-
dren’s engagement with their work – in other words, to look at whether or
not they do what they are supposed to do, so certain behaviours, such as
sitting and writing, will count as ‘on-task’, while others, such as wandering
around the class, will count as ‘off-task’. By drawing up a list of on-task and
off-task behaviours and observing for these systematically, Alyssa can meas-
ure what happens in the class under the two conditions.
The experiment requires organisation that will take many features of
the situation into consideration. Alyssa first has to plan when the observa-
tions will take place under the two conditions. Because the two conditions
must be identical apart from the change in the system of organisation, she
has to ensure that the same kind of work is being done by the children and
the same people are there with her class for both periods of observation.
They also have to be at the same time of day and even, if possible, on the
same days of the week, for these are also sources of variation that could
plausibly make a difference. Then she has to train the assistant and parents
in the room-management system and schedule the sessions. She also has
to arrange for observations to take place to test the effect of the system
on the dependent variable – children’s engagement or ‘on-taskness’. She
decides to organise this by asking another parent who regularly assists in
the classroom to video the sessions and subsequently goes through the
video with a checklist, looking at each child in turn for a set period to work
out an on-task percentage figure for each of them.
Each child’s overall engagement (or ‘on-taskness’) is then plotted for the
two occasions. Using some simple statistics, Alyssa is able to show that the
differences between the two occasions are statistically significant. Simply
‘eyeballing’ the chart (that is, just looking at it intelligently) also shows a
number of interesting things. Because Alyssa ranked the children’s engage-
ment from lowest to highest in the first condition, she can see how those
who are at the lower end of the scale – that is to say, those children who find
most difficulty attending – are affected by the introduction of room man-
agement. The chart clearly shows that, for most of them, their engagement
improves markedly over the two conditions.
Added to other sources of information, however, a case study of this kind may well
add an invaluable dimension to that polyhedron.
• If, on the one hand, you start with a firm idea or set of premises, you could be said
to be testing a theory.
• If, on the other hand, you prefer to see what ideas emerge as you immerse yourself
in the situation you are studying, you could be said to be building a theory.
• Alternatively, you may simply be aiming to illustrate something.
Beyond these broad divisions of approaches to case study research, you can pick
and mix to create the approach you want to take. So, you may choose to take:
Remember that the case study is like a wrapper: within it, the shape of the study is
determined by your question and your purposes.
REFLECTIVE ACTIVITY
In Figure 8.4 I indicate how a theory is developed in the case study about infection
in a hospital. Here, the storyboard points to the high infection in this particular
hospital having its roots in staff resentment about top-down management, and
a plethora of new initiatives about which there has been a lack of consultation.
Given what we all know about the spread of infection following the COVID-19 pan-
demic, think of an alternative theory which could be tested by a case study and draw
an alternative storyboard which might offer different routes for testing that theory.
172 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Further Reading
Eisenhardt, K. and Graebner, K.M. (2007) ‘Theory building from cases: opportuni-
ties and challenges’, Academy of Management Journal, 50 (1): 25–32.
This is a highly traditional, but very solid and well-explicated account of theory
and the case study’s contribution to it, written from the point of view of manage-
ment science.
Field, A. (2017) Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS (5th edn). London: Sage.
This really tackles research from the point of view of statistics and, if you do an
experiment as part of your case study, it will be invaluable – not least because it is
presented via the main software package used in the social sciences, SPSS. If you
are doing a study of the kind I have suggested in this chapter, look especially for
non-parametric tests, such as the Mann–Whitney test and the Wilcoxon rank-sum
test.
Luker, K. (2009) Salsa Dancing into the Social Sciences. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Luker discusses well the framing and construction of an interpretative research
project, starting from a hunch to the development of an investigation.
Malinowski, B. (1922) Argonauts of the Western Pacific. London: Routledge & Kegan
Paul.
fellow human beings rather than as objects of study and, in doing this, he helped
to establish the ethnographic tradition. He was a participant observer, keeping a
diary of his observations, and his analyses were pioneering in their use of an inter-
pretative approach.
Thomas, G. and Myers, K. (2015) The Anatomy of the Case Study. London: Sage.
Here Kevin Myers and I give more examples of the kinds of case studies outlined in
this chapter, analysing each of them in terms of the classificatory system used in
this book.
Whyte, W.F. (1985) Learning from the Field: A Guide from Experience. Beverly Hills,
CA: Sage.
This covers just about everything that an interpretative researcher could wish to
know.
9
YOUR PROCESS: THE SHAPE,
STYLE AND MANNER OF
YOUR CASE STUDY
How will you go about structuring your case study? What I am talking about here
is the style and manner of your case study – whether you are doing a case study of
one individual or several and, if the latter, whether they are all done at once or one
after the other, whether you will separate out nested elements of the single case for
special examination or look back at events that have happened in the past or collect
data as time proceeds (or both). We will focus in this chapter on the nuts and bolts
of the study, as summarised in Figure 9.1.
How do terrorists
escape detection?
You draw your storyboard, which expresses your basic question, ‘How do
terrorists escape detection?’, as shown in Figure 9.2. In it, you sketch out
some ideas to follow up.
So, you have a question, but there are still a number of options open to
you concerning the route that would offer the most illuminating avenues for
tackling it. You could, for example, first try to group terrorist acts in some
way to see if they shared any characteristics. Then, using information in the
public domain, you might pick a couple of these groups to focus on, looking
for clues as to how they evaded detection by the security services. While this
would be instructive, it would not be likely to offer much data that would
provide an enlightening analysis. Really, on the question of evading the
authorities, something richer that taps a variety of sources and aims to knit
them together is likely to be more helpful. In other words, a case study.
Once you have come to this conclusion, you are at the stage of explain-
ing why a case study would be valuable in the circumstances of your current
knowledge and the question that you have sketched out. Your expectation is
that a case study of a particular terrorist incident will offer more insight into
this question than a range of other approaches. By enabling you to focus in
depth, a case study will mean that you can draw out the lines of reasoning,
to see the possible strands of causality at play.
You decide to focus on a terrorist incident that will provide a good deal
of detail about the people involved in it. The availability of data on the back-
grounds of the people involved is the key issue here. Once you have chosen
an attack, what might you do to look at how it evaded the gaze of author-
ity? Remembering that the case study is singled out by its emphasis on the
richness of the individual account or narrative, you might choose to focus
on one of the terrorists in a particular terrorist cell. You could focus on all
of them and treat the attack itself as ‘the case’, but this would necessarily
reduce the richness of each terrorist’s history and situation. With the depth
provided by one account, you can look at a great deal of detail: you can look
at the childhood of the terrorist, his education, his early adulthood and his
friends – was he a loner or did he have lots of friends? You can look at the
reasons for his attraction to fringe religious sects and, ultimately, groups of
people who were connected with Al Qaeda.
This initial inquiry might tell you a number of things. It might point, for
example, to his childhood in a wealthy family and his upbringing in a British
private school, where he developed an especially aggravated feeling of dif-
ference and alienation from his host culture. This might give you a clue to his
resentment of authority and Western culture and his willingness to engage
in acts of this kind. More relevantly for the dimension of escaping capture by
the authorities, it might point to a familiarity with isolation and a predilec-
tion for ‘keeping his head down’, avoiding notice by keeping quiet. There
YOUR PROCESS 177
into a project such as this, you could also seek to understand (within the
constraints of your own position, probably as a student inquirer with little
power to find out detailed personal information) what the incipient terrorists
did specifically to avoid observation. Were they known to have used coded
messages? Did they assume disguises? Did they use pseudonyms? These,
then, would be evidence of the more deliberate avoidance strategies.
A conclusion to the study might show how these strategies had broken
down. Perhaps the group had been infiltrated by the security services? Did
those services understand the modus operandi and psychology of groups
such as this, looking for psychological profiles and patterns of behaviour,
seeking out such groups to penetrate them?
All the time, you as an inquirer would be focusing first and foremost on
the individuals involved, building a narrative. Because this is a case study, it
is the narrative structure that is important – the story, in other words, build-
ing a picture of the personalities involved. Remembering, too, that this is a
single case study, the one individual would form the core that would bind
together all of the other elements, around whom the tapestry of your story
would be woven.
The snapshot
Here, the case is being looked at in one period of time. It could be a month, week, day
or even a period as short as an hour. That is the frame – the borders within which the
study is undertaken. Remember that a case study is already a bounded study, and the
boundaries are defined by something – possibly an institution, or the life of the person
in question – but here, in the snapshot, it is defined by a period of time.
If this is so, then time should in some way be important. If the snapshot defines
the study, then you are not getting contextual information arising from a whole
range of ongoing sources of data that you are able to mix and match. Your focus is,
if you like, ‘a day in the life of’ and the skill will lie in being able to draw data into
that narrow frame.
In doing this, the snapshot case study is illustrative, demonstrative and analyti-
cal, like other forms of case study, but the analysis here is aided by the temporal
juxtaposition of events – and that temporal juxtaposition (in other words, things
being next to each other in time) will help you and the reader to understand the
connectedness of events.
By presenting a narrative from one day, whether it is like a continuous video or
more like a scrapbook, you are offering the reader your analysis of the situation.
The object emerges out of this analysis. As Walton (1992: 129) puts it, ‘Cases come
wrapped in theories.’ In other words, the view being presented is a three-dimensional
picture with all of the colours painted in – the contour lines and the colour provided
by your own ‘theoretical’ stances, which are all of the things that make you who
you are and, in turn, your analysis what it is. The case inquirer is the central person
providing the theoretical wrappings, and this is done here in the snapshot especially
with a view to the picture being presented as a Gestalt over a tight timeframe.
What does this mean in practical terms? I’ve mentioned that your focus is like ‘a
day in the life of’, and it’s useful to think of one of the most famous examples of the
genre – Aleksandr Solzhenitsyn’s (1963/2000) One Day in the Life of Ivan Denisovich.
Although it is a work of literature rather than a research report, it is useful as an
example since Solzhenitsyn based the book on his own experience and it illumi-
nates his subject and enlightens his readers. Apart from any literary merit, this was
180 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
an experience worth knowing about since it gave a picture of the author’s reflec-
tions after his time in a Soviet labour camp – an experience he was forced to suffer
because of a derogatory comment he made about Joseph Stalin.
As with all other forms of case study, there should be a purpose for doing it. The
case study cannot be merely illustrative. If I were to provide a snapshot of my day
as One Day in the Life of Gary Thomas, it sadly would be nowhere near as revela-
tory as that of Ivan Denisovich. There would have to be a reason for my writing
it and, while Solzhenitsyn’s experiences in a Soviet labour camp are an immediate
and obvious source of interest, my own in my everyday life are not. Solzhenitsyn’s
experiences, however, can tell us about a whole range of things. We learn of the
desperation brought about by the intense cold and what this does to a human
being, the nature of the discourse permitted about Stalin, camaraderie, its nature
and its purposes. Then we can put all of this into the context that is known about
Stalin’s USSR and all that came after it. Not only does telling the story over one
day serve the narrative purpose of making it more interesting, real and compelling,
but also the timeframe connects the bits of the narrative. By describing the cold
and its effects on the guards (as well as the prisoners), Solzhenitsyn humanises the
guards and helps us to understand their reactions to the prisoners – both sympa-
thetic and unkind. Take this passage from the beginning of the book (Solzhenitsyn,
1963/2000: 7):
As usual, at five o’clock that morning reveille was sounded by the blows
of a hammer on a length of rail hanging up near the staff quarters. The
intermittent sound barely penetrated the window-panes on which the
frost lay two fingers thick, and they ended almost as soon as they’d
begun. It was cold outside and the camp-guard was reluctant to go on
beating out the reveille for long.
The camp guard becomes human – he is dispirited by the cold and we empathise
with him. Not only this, by empathising with him we understand better the other
actions he takes. The timeline, whether a week, a day or an hour, focuses the gaze
of the observer and encourages an analysis of the interconnections between parts
of the narrative. It helps an understanding of the ways in which one element of
the story is bound to another. It helps us to understand the fabric of the day, the
ways in which the inmates avoid or gain the guards’ attention and their facility
in harnessing from them small treats that could be traded and the ways in which
these became converted to a kind of currency. We learn about this in the context of
the intense and all-pervading cold and the prisoners’ single-minded preoccupation
with warmth and survival.
YOUR PROCESS 181
As I said above, A Day in the Life of Gary Thomas would not be as compelling as
that of Ivan Denisovich. Ivan Denisovich has the advantage, as a subject, of all
that we have just talked about. Ultimately, it helped to change the world. My being
woken – me and 6 million others – by my radio alarm competes poorly with the
camp guard’s reveille. My day could be interesting, however, as could anyone’s, if an
angle on it could be found. Here are two possible such angles, which would form
the object of my study.
As I have noted regarding Ivan Denisovich, this could not simply be a record. I
could not simply say, ‘this happened, then this happened, then that happened’.
That would simply be a diary, and a not very interesting one. The key in employ-
ing a snapshot is to find those interconnections over the day, week or whatever. It
is the finding of these interconnections that would constitute the analytic frame.
How did this link to that and what were my reflections on each? This is the stuff of
which analysis is made.
Here is another example of a snapshot, this one occurring over three days:
Consequences
Behaviour
What happens after
Antecedents The eating itself.
the eating? Are
What goes before What is it
these consequences
any eating? characterised by?
in any way contingent
How does it happen?
on the eating?
• the physical context – the workbench, seat, engine parts zinging down
the assembly line
• the social context – the other staff and the group’s supervisor
• time – the key one, with changes happening as time proceeds.
Bev has to remember in doing this case study that it is not governed by the
same principles as an experiment (unless she were to deliberately set up
an experiment as part of the case study, but she is not going to do that).
So, she is not going to be changing the value of a variable (the height of
the seat) while holding the other variables constant in order to assess the
YOUR PROCESS 185
effect of the change. Nor is she going to be formally comparing this group
with another group of workers further down the assembly line who have
not had their seats raised. Rather, she is going to be looking at the case as a
whole from a number of directions and interpreting what she finds. Always,
in analysing her findings, she will have to have at the front of her mind her
purpose for doing the study, which is to understand the Hawthorne effect
in her particular circumstance. Her understanding is informed by all of the
work on the Hawthorne effect that she has discovered during her literature
review. On the basis of the latter and her thinking, she draws up a story-
board about potential explanations, as shown in Figure 9.5.
Psychology of well-being
Doesn’t a certain
and happiness
amount of stress improve
Reduction in performance?
Why should stress?
feeling special
make people work
harder? Why should cohesion
make people work
harder?
So, Bev starts the study and raises the seats of the three staff. She
assesses their output and finds that it does indeed rise in all three cases.
(Suppose it had not risen. Would this have been the end of the inquiry? No.
Bev could have proceeded with the study, either modifying it in such a way
that another change or changes were introduced until an effect was found,
or else she could simply have continued with the study as an exceptional
example, offering reasons for there having been no Hawthorne effect in this
case. It could, for example, lend weight to the findings of Levitt and List
(2009), who suggested that Hawthorne effects are not real, which would then
point to a new direction her inquiry could take.)
On finding a Hawthorne effect – the rise in output of these three mem-
bers of staff – Bev could proceed to the crux of the study, which is an
(Continued)
186 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
In a multiple case study, because there are several subjects, each individual sub-
ject is less important in itself than the comparison each offers with the others.
YOUR PROCESS 187
• local, since one of the cases draws from his own experience, and key
because they all are exemplars
• instrumental, since it serves a purpose
• illustrative, since it is showing something
• multiple or comparative, since the core of the message comes out of the
comparison of cases.
YOUR PROCESS 189
The first chef Sennett describes is Richard Olney. Sennett picks on his
description of how to bone a chicken. The instructions provided by Olney,
says Sennett, tell instead of show: ‘If the reader already knows how to bone,
this description might be a useful review; for the neophyte it is no guide.
Many unfortunate chickens will be hacked to bits if a beginner follows it.’
Here is a sample of the instructions that Sennett is talking about: ‘Sever
the attachment of each shoulder blade at the wing joint and, holding it firmly
between the thumb and forefinger of the left hand, pull it out of the flesh
with the other hand.’ He likens this kind of writing to engineering: ‘Not only
do engineer-writers leave out “dumb things” that “everyone knows”: they
repress simile, metaphor, and adverbial color. The act of unpacking what’s
buried in the vault of tacit knowledge can make use of these imaginative
tools.’
The second part of the multiple case study consists of a word picture
that Sennett draws of chef Julia Child. He gives an account of how Child’s
recipe is like a story that gets inside the chef’s head, expressing forebod-
ings and sympathy. She uses analogy, focusing on the cook rather than
the chicken. She gives clues rather than direct instructions and guides
the reader like an expert with an apprentice, anticipating difficulties and
suggesting ways around them: ‘for a moment Child will imagine holding
the knife awkwardly; the cello master will return to playing wrong notes.
This return to vulnerability is the sign of the sympathy the instructor gives’
(Sennett, 2009: 186).
Part three is a pen portrait of the famous Elizabeth David, who shared
her skills with her readers by giving the cultural context of the food. She
(Continued)
190 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
talks about local cooks in France touching and prodding the bird. She
tells stories about when, how and with whom she ate and gives interest-
ing titbits about how tarragon might be used by Bordeaux cooks while,
for the same recipe, sage was used by the cooks in Perpignan. So, as a
reader you think about the food you are cooking, being encouraged to
consider the process as creative, variable and intuitive rather than a rigid
set of procedures.
The fourth case study offered by Sennett is perhaps the most interest-
ing. It is of Madame Benshaw and her recipe for poulet à la d’Albufera.
Madame Benshaw is different from the others in that she is not famous –
she emerges from Sennett’s own personal knowledge – and because
she talks almost entirely in riddles, devoid of instructions in the formal
sense. Sennett was a student of hers at an evening class and he says that,
because her English was poor, she would teach almost entirely by exam-
ple, ‘coupled with slight smiles and emphatic, frowning contradictions of
her thick eyebrows’. With Sennett’s help, Madame Benshaw wrote down
the recipe:
Your dead child. Prepare him for new life. Fill him with the earth. Be
careful! He should not over-eat. Put on his golden coat. You bathe him.
Warm him but be careful! A child dies from too much sun. Put on his
jewels. This is my recipe. (2009: 190–1)
By the ‘dead child’, she meant the chicken. The ‘preparation for new life’
meant boning. ‘Filling with the earth’ was about stuffing, and ‘not overeat-
ing’ about not overstuffing. The ‘golden coat’ was about browning before
baking. The ‘bathing’ referred to the preparation of a poaching liquor and
the ‘jewels’ the pouring on of this liquor, basting.
This recipe, then, is told entirely via metaphors, and these allow the nov-
ice cook to feel and understand. Calling the chicken a ‘child’ immediately
evokes a sense of intimacy, protection and tenderness, so we understand
that we should place the bird only in a cool oven. As Sennett puts it, meta-
phors ‘roll forward and sideways’, allowing us to gather different meanings
as they do so.
This comparative case study illustrates exactly the point that Sennett
wants to, which is that learning a craft is not about simply following instruc-
tions but a human process. He builds the theory that in doing this, we do
well to follow our noses, to use, if you like, our ‘instinct’ for learning. This is
something that is done by modelling and copying, getting inside another
person’s head. To build the theory, he draws out the key points and synthe-
sises them, as shown in Table 9.1.
YOUR PROCESS 191
This is where many student researchers go wrong in doing a case study of this
kind. There is too much description – and it is just description. Too little actually
comes from looking at the differences between the cases. It is this that is, in fact,
more important than the presentation of the cases. Fascinating though Madame
Benshaw’s case is, it is far less interesting on its own than it is when compared with
the others. The analytical frame is provided by the comparison.
An interesting comparative case in the literature can be found in the work of
Jiménez and Gersten (1999). They made comparisons between the teaching styles
of two Latina/o teachers and the different ways in which they went about teaching
literacy.
is within the principal unit of analysis, such as classrooms within a school. This is
summarised in Figure 9.7.
Nested
Nested
Case 1 Case 2 unit 2
unit 1
Wider
case
Nested Nested
Case 3 unit 3 unit 4
Case 4
A nested study is distinct from a multiple study in that it gains its integrity, its
wholeness, from the wider case.
As with all decisions regarding case studies, there has to be a reason for conduct-
ing the study in a particular way and, with the nested study, that reason comes
out of contrasting the units as part of the wider case. If you were looking at three
classrooms within one school, but the school had no significance other than that
it physically housed these three classrooms, then the cases would not be seen
as nested. They are nested only in the sense that they form an integral part of a
broader picture – integral to something that might be happening within the school.
Let’s flesh out this example a little.
for the dissertation she has to complete for her degree, but she expects that
it will also form the basis for developmental work in the school afterwards.
Lara’s starting point is that the kinds of changes expected to result from
an inclusion policy of this kind are difficult to implement at a practical level
because they depend on the cooperation of individual people, and different
people notoriously behave in different ways.
Lara’s tutor has referred her to a classic paper, ‘The myth of the hero-innovator’,
by Georgiades and Phillimore (1975). The point that Georgiades and Phillimore
make is that implementing change in an organisation is far more difficult than
it might at first sight seem. Because of this, the organisational landscape is lit-
tered with the carcasses of brave people who have tried to change things but
failed. It is almost as if people working within organisations, whoever they are –
teachers, doctors, firefighters, zookeepers – conspire to resist change. Allied
to this is Lara’s knowledge that there are few recipes for success with an inno-
vation such as the implementation of an inclusion policy. If trying to introduce
something of this kind, you have to adapt and modify to suit the organisational
conditions you find in different places.
This is the starting point for Lara’s study – that is, the six classes of her
primary school are all very different since all the teachers in the school have
their own styles which will have to be accommodated in the implementation
of the policy.
She chooses three teachers and their classes as nested case studies within
the school. The core of Lara’s study will focus on the inclusive principles
espoused by the school and the ways in which it is possible to implement
these aspects of the inclusion policy, given the different styles of the teach-
ers working at Franklin Drive. The principles of the inclusion policy include:
The first four of these are whole-school issues that will not be affected by the
idiosyncrasies of the teachers’ styles. Lara is concerned about the last three
(Continued)
194 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
• Jon, a newly qualified teacher, who does things by the book and enjoys
involving parents, but is rather in awe of them
• Meryl, who retires next year and is a formal teacher who firmly declines
offers of help from parents or volunteers
• Jane, a mid-career teacher who is spontaneous, dismissive of govern-
ment directives, popular with parents and works easily with them.
Lara decides to focus in these studies on the ways in which these three teachers
can implement the school’s new policy, given their different styles. She consid-
ers forming a focus group with Jon, Meryl and Jane in which she will present
the policy and discuss views on its implementation. On reflection, however,
she rejects this idea, realising that it may accentuate any differences in style
and make Meryl defensive and alienated from the others. What she needs to
do is study each of these classrooms as environments for the development of
the policy, each working in its own way. In the sense that the study will help to
change practice, this is a case study that is also a piece of action research.
Lara ultimately decides to conduct semi-structured interviews with each of
the teachers, in which she will ask about inclusion strategies, the curriculum
and parental involvement. These will be analysed using the constant com-
parative method (see Chapter 11) for both unique and shared ideas about
implementation in class. The analytical framework that she will develop will
serve as the basis for a summative plenary discussion with the teachers in
which their ideas are shared.
As Lara expected, the interviews reveal quite different attitudes to the
curriculum, learning and the community, as well as education’s place in it.
She uses construct maps (Thomas, 2017: 246–8) to show diagrammatically
the differences in thinking and try to draw strands of commonality from
them. She fails in this last aim, however. While Jon and Jane show some
commonality in their thinking, there is no congruence at all with Meryl.
Lara’s conclusion is that the units of the school operate as quite discrete
elements in the larger structure of the school, and she uses Bronfenbrenner’s
systems theory (see Chapter 3) to frame her analysis here. She speaks in her
write-up of the insularity of the teacher role within the school and of the
resilience of this insularity. She also questions if it would, in fact, be a good
thing to attempt to break this down, coming back to her starting point
about styles – styles that are built on attitude, belief, personality, habit
YOUR PROCESS 195
I hope that you can see how the nested study is different from the simple multi-
ple study, comparing particularly the multiple study of chefs with the study of the
three classrooms. Comparisons are at the heart of each kind of study, but in the
nested study these occur in a wider, connected context.
(Continued)
196 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Time
The first part of the study (A1 and B1) provided confirmation of an increase
in usage and an endorsement of the improvements in access to the park.
The in-depth interview and ‘walk-through’ element (B1 and B2) gave mixed
information about the nature of the changes, with a difference between the
wheelchair users and the autistic adults in the way they responded in their
interviews and walk-throughs. Those using wheelchairs were indeed content
with the primarily access-based changes. The autistic adults, however, were
unhappy with what they felt was an unthinking association of autism with
learning disabilities, and this raised ethical and practical issues that Sarah
and the charity had to pursue. On the substantive subject of the project –
park design – the autistic participants made comments about how green
spaces can provide a sense of security and enable autistic people to self-
regulate from sensory and emotional overload. They talked of the need for
quiet spaces, not too crowded, and a need for planning for noise reduction.
Predictability, they said, needed to be thought about, with uncluttered sig-
nage which used clear and unambiguous language with visual supports.
By doing her case study sequentially in this way, Sarah was able to offer
useful information to aid the rolling out of the programme to other parks.
For example, she had been able to understand how the design that had
been produced with the aid of computer visualisation had not met all expec-
tations. She was then able to make suggestions about future projects. Also,
although the case studies did not draw from a representative sample of disa-
bled people, they enabled a clearer understanding of the needs of these
users, pulling in ideas that would not have been garnered from a study with
a more one-dimensional focus.
• single
• multiple.
198 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Beyond this, time can be thought of as a key component in structuring the case
study. We have thought about:
Each of these employs time in a different way. And if the study is a multiple one,
the multiple subjects may be studied:
As with all questions about design, thinking carefully about what you need will be
repaid in your findings and the analysis you are able to draw. Keep asking yourself,
‘What kind of analysis, what kinds of insights am I seeking?’, and then structure the
case study – thinking about both the subject and the object (pages 16–18) – around
these questions.
REFLECTIVE ACTIVITY
From your own area of interest, outline two case studies (identifying subject and
object in each), one of which would usefully be constructed around …
• a single case
• multiple cases
… and in both case studies say whether the study would be using time by
looking back (that is, retrospectively), or at a particular snapshot in time, or
diachronically. You may also like to draw each case study’s path through the
typology.
YOUR PROCESS 199
Further Reading
Mills, A.J., Durepos, G. and Wiebe, E. (2010) Encyclopedia of Case Study Research
(Vols 1–2). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. doi: 10.4135/9781412957397
These volumes give some good examples of different kinds of case study research
and offer a particularly interesting discussion of multiple case study.
Thomas, G. (ed.) (2013) Case Study Methods in Education (4 vols). London: Sage.
Here, I offer examples of different kinds of case study in many kinds of social
research. Volume 3 gives examples of case studies on a range of topics, and Volume 4
deals specifically with case studies in education.
Yin, R.K. (2018) Case Study Research: Design and Methods (6th edn). Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage.
Yin draws some interesting distinctions between different kinds of case studies.
STAGE 3
COLLECTING EVIDENCE,
ANALYSING AND WRITING IT UP
You may be seeking data, pure and simple, to see where this takes you in develop-
ing your ideas about a topic. Alternatively, you may be seeking evidence in support
of a clearly defined proposition. Which you do depends on the kind of research
you are conducting. If you go into your case study without a tightly constructed
theory or set of propositions to guide your research, you will be seeking data that
will gather around ideas which emerge as the study progresses. If, however, you
begin with a clear hypothesis or a well-defined theory at the outset, then you have
propositions that will be supported (or not) by the data that you collect – you are
looking for evidence.
With this distinction in mind, how can you go about collecting information that
will help you with your case study? I have mentioned many of the methods that
can be used in the examples I have given in Chapters 6–9. Let’s do this more sys-
tematically now, though, and run through some of the most commonly used ways
of collecting evidence as a guide for you when you actually get down to doing your
study. This is only a guide and an aide-memoire, though, and I give more details
about methods for gathering data in How to Do Your Research Project (Thomas, 2017).
I summarise these commonly used methods of collecting evidence in Table 10.1.
Let’s look at each of them in a little more detail.
• accounts
• diaries
• reflective journals
• group interviews
• focus groups
• interrogating documents
• questionnaires use words, images and/or numbers
• observation
{ structured observation
{ unstructured observation
{ participant observation
• image-based methods
• measurements and tests mainly use numbers
• official statistics
• other numerical data
SOME WAYS TO COLLECT DATA AND EVIDENCE 205
Interviews
Structured interviews
In a structured interview, you meet with another person and ask a predetermined list
of questions. The structured interview has a limited range of strengths:
But, beyond these, it does not have very much in its favour. Because there is no
great advantage in giving it in a face-to-face manner, it may as well be given in
written form – in other words, as a questionnaire (see page 212).
Unstructured interviews
Likely to be used in an interpretative case study (see Chapter 8), an unstructured
interview is like a conversation. There is no fixed way to conduct such an interview.
You do not present your interviewee with a list of questions. In fact, the idea really
is for interviewees to set the agenda. They should be the ones who determine the
direction of the interview and the topics that emerge. As the researcher, you go in
with an open mind and just try to listen and facilitate. This is of course what is
wanted in interpretative research: in this kind of research you are looking for your
respondents to set the scene and let them tell you what the issues are.
Beyond this desire to let the interviewee lead the way, there are variations on
the theme of just how unstructured the interview should be. If your respondents
go completely off the topic, then you would wish to bring them back to it in some
way. However, this would need to be done sensitively, since it may be the case that
this is the way that the respondents talk – going ‘off message’ for a while before
they return to the topic. Or they might like to tell a story when they are talking,
and the unstructured interview gives them a chance to do this. If it seems the case
in this sort of circumstance that something of a delicate or sensitive nature is being
disclosed, you should clear with your interviewees after the interview that they are
happy for the specific information to be used and in what guise. You could reveal
it only as a general comment, perhaps, or it may be acceptable to your interviewees
to quote from them in an anonymous way on which you can agree. (See also the
discussion on ethics and confidentiality in Chapter 5.)
The unstructured interview will be more like a conversation than a formal inter-
view. Sometimes, though, you may wish to move the conversation in a particular
206 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
direction and prompt the interviewee. How should you prompt without setting the
agenda yourself? There are degrees of strength of prompt: you may simply say, ‘Can
you tell me more about that?’, or offer something stronger such as ‘How did that
make you feel?’ or ‘What happened next?’ However, if you really are interested in
what the interviewee has to say you would avoid a question such as ‘Does that make
you feel angry?’ This really would be putting words into the interviewee’s mouth.
Semi-structured interviews
In a semi-structured interview you provide the structure with a list of issues (rather
than specific questions) to be covered and you have the freedom to follow up points
as necessary. Because of these advantages, it is the most commonly used kind of
interview arrangement in small-scale social research.
However, for case study, the fact that it is the most commonly used does not
mean that it is necessarily the best. If you really are interested in interpreting your
interviewees’ comments and if you are a participant observer in the situation that
you are researching, an unstructured interview may well be a better choice. I make
this point because too many students (in my opinion) opt for the semi-structured
interview as the most straightforward and seemingly obvious choice, where in fact
it can lead them to do a different kind of research from that which they set out
to do. If you really do intend to do an ethnographic, interpretative case study,
entering your research environment as a participant observer, you are trying to
understand how the ‘players’ in that environment are playing their roles – depend-
ing on the meanings that they construct. If this is the case, if this is what you want
to do, then the semi-structured interview may be too rigid.
If you decide after reflection that you do need semi-structured interviews, though,
you will start with an interview schedule, which is a list of issues that you intend to
cover. You are not obliged to go through these points in order – or in any way keep
to a formal set format for the interview. Rather, it gives you a reminder of what you
want to cover. It reminds you not just of the issues but also of potential questions,
possible follow-up questions and ‘probes’, which may encourage the interviewee to
say more on these follow-ups. Probes may be verbal – for example, ‘Go on …’ – or
non-verbal, such as a nod, smile or tilt of the head.
Accounts
An account comes from a respondent and is the result of a format that allows him
or her to communicate experiences and feelings freely. Accounts are really like the
SOME WAYS TO COLLECT DATA AND EVIDENCE 207
Diaries
A diary may involve you, or a participant in your case study, making a record of
ideas, reflections, thoughts, emotions, actions, reactions, conversations and so on.
Alternatively, it may involve making a structured record of activities. Diaries may
be divided into three types:
If you are completing a diary yourself, it is best done immediately after your session
in the field, recording an assortment of opinions, views, interpretations, remem-
bered conversations and so on. It can take written form or be an audio or even a
video recording.
Some people will be more fulsome in accounts in a private disclosure of the kind
given in a diary than they would be in a face-to-face interview. This is often the case
with children and young people, as long as they have the writing skills. If they do
not have such skills, you could consider the use of an audio diary. You may even
wish to keep a video diary, where expressions, gestures, sighs, grimaces, frowns,
tears even, may be recorded – features of the situation that would be missed in a
written or audio diary.
opinions, appraisals, judgements, feelings and insights. These reflections can then
be used to help with …
1. your own process of learning how to research, helping you to reflect on and
improve your own research practices and skills, and
2. actual data for inclusion in your findings.
Occasion:
Observed in Ms X’s
classroom: 06/06/21
What did I From unstructured observation I discovered that Z [focus child] is highly
discover? active, moving about the classroom a great deal. I discovered that if I am
a ‘participant observer’ I need to participate in all areas of the room, not
getting too involved with one child or group as I am likely to miss things that
Z is doing as he moves around
What went well and I was able to talk to Z on a few occasions and engage him in conversation
why? in a reasonably relaxed way. I think I got more from him in these ‘natural’
meetings than I would have if I had withdrawn him for an interview. In the
unstructured observation I could see Z’s behaviour in context and I could
begin to understand it in the light of what the other children were doing and
how they were responding to him
What could I have I could have asked Ms X if she could have introduced me to the class
done better? with a bit more detail for the children as they kept asking me who I was.
I could have moved around the classroom more, not getting too involved
in the work of one or two groups. I left it too long before writing up notes
and I should have asked Ms X for her views on my observations and
interpretations
The journal does not have to take this form; it could instead be more free-flowing,
recording your reflections day by day on your research work.
You can find ideas for keeping a journal and other ideas for reflective writing
in your university library website. The advice from my own university library can
be found at https://tinyurl.com/ycq78w56 and an adapted version of my univer-
sity’s tips for keeping a reflective journal, with more information, at https://canvas.
bham.ac.uk/courses/11841/pages/keeping-a-reflective-journal
SOME WAYS TO COLLECT DATA AND EVIDENCE 209
As I have noted, the reflective journal is not simply for your own benefit in the
development of your own research skills, but also may be included in your research
findings, either in the main body of the text or in an appendix. Indeed, in a case
study, a reflective journal may form a central element of your data collection
armoury, allowing you to incorporate a form of ‘thick description’, recorded natu-
rally while you do your research.
Quoting from your journal as an interpretative researcher also helps you explain
your own positionality. Here is how one researcher describes how she used a
research journal:
Ortlipp also describes how the reflective journal helped her to reflect on her research
methodology and change from her original plans:
Being open about your reflections on a topic, on your own process of working and
on your own positionality are important facets of case study research, and keeping
a reflective journal will help you in the process of making these explicit.
210 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Focus groups
The term ‘focus group’ has come to be used interchangeably with ‘group inter-
view’, but the two kinds of group are different in important respects. In group
interviews the emphasis is on the researcher taking a lead role, asking questions
and being in control of the discussion – rather in the way that an interviewer
is leading the discussion in a structured or semi-structured interview. So the
researcher asks questions and respondents answer. But in focus groups the
researcher plays the role of facilitator or moderator. If you are running a focus
group your aim is to facilitate or moderate discussion among participants, not
between yourself and the participants. The idea is that you take a marginal rather
than a pivotal role.
SOME WAYS TO COLLECT DATA AND EVIDENCE 211
In the focus group, participants, usually eight or so, who have a relevant charac-
teristic or feature of their lives in common will be brought together in an informal
setting to discuss the topic in question. As in an unstructured interview with an
individual, the idea is to get at the understandings, beliefs and values of the partici-
pants. And in the same way that in an individual unstructured interview the aim
is to let the individual take the lead in setting the direction of the discussion, so in
the focus group the aim is to let the group take the lead. As facilitator, your role is to
stimulate discussion, and you may do this with your comments or you may prepare
a range of focus materials for the group to discuss. These are materials – for example,
objects, artefacts, photographs, drawings, newspaper clippings, short videos, audio
recordings – that help the group focus on the topic of interest.
Given that you need your wits about you to facilitate effectively in a group such as
this, it is common practice in professionally conducted research to use an observer
to record information about context, environment and participants’ behaviour. In
small-scale research it will not usually be possible to get help in this way, so it may
be helpful to make an audio and/or video record of proceedings.
Interrogating documents
Case studies take many and varied forms, and a retrospective case study may depend
entirely upon documentary evidence. It may be the case that there are no living sur-
vivors of the event, or no one who is accessible to offer their thoughts. Even in the
case of an event about which there are potential informants, these people may not
be able or willing to be interviewed.
Gathering data from documents represents an entirely different proposition from
gathering data from people. Essentially, the knack is to find the right documents,
read them and think about them.
There are methods for examining documents with your word-processing pro-
gram for, for example, readability or the existence of certain kinds of words, but
there is no substitute for your own careful reading for meaning and substance in
a document. Fortunately, reading documents has been made easier since most are
nowadays available in Word, as PDFs or online and can be searched and cut-and-
pasted from.
Given the ease with which documents can now be downloaded (particularly gov-
ernment and other policy documents), a key shortcut is in some basic document
interrogation using computer software. This is easy if the document is in Word,
but a bit trickier if it is in PDF. If the latter, you can, after checking copyright, copy
the whole thing and then paste it into Word. (Tip: To copy the whole lot in a PDF
212 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
file, press Ctrl-A, which selects the whole document; then Ctrl-C, which copies it,
then switch to a blank Word document and paste it into that with Ctrl-V.) If you
are not able to download the relevant document in word-processing or PDF format,
the one technical trick that is widely available now is through electronic scanning –
through a smartphone, a printer or a dedicated scanner. Most of these devices now
are provided with optical character recognition (OCR) software which will do the
‘reading’ (but sadly not the thinking or understanding) for you. When you scan
your text your software should give you the option of enabling the text to be ‘read’
as text (as distinct from simply taking a picture of the text). It will then save this into
a word-processing file, and once this is done there are a number of ways in which
your computer can help you to analyse the text; these forms of analysis are covered
in Chapter 11.
Questionnaires
A questionnaire is a written form of questioning and the questions may be closed
(‘Do you think parents should be allowed to smack their children? Yes or No’) or
open (‘What are your feelings about parents smacking their children?’). You may be
collecting facts or, as here, assessing people’s attitudes on a topic.
A questionnaire can be tightly structured, but can also allow the chance for more
open responses if required. It may be read out by interviewers (either face-to-face
or over the phone) or sent to respondents for them to complete themselves. It may
be sent by post or email or presented online. For online questionnaires, there are
various Web-based services available (such as SurveyMonkey, available at: www.sur-
veymonkey.com, which lets you construct your own questionnaire free for a survey
of up to 100 respondents).
Ways of questioning
The first thing to consider is whether you are going to used open or closed questions.
Open questions leave the response open to the respondent, and I will come to these
in a moment at the end of this section. Closed questions limit the response and can
be organised in a number of ways.
Dichotomous questions
‘Dichotomous’ means ‘two-way’, and the dichotomy is usually ‘yes’ or ‘no’. For
example:
SOME WAYS TO COLLECT DATA AND EVIDENCE 213
Yes
No
These can often be screening questions. In other words, you may use the question
to separate respondents into groups, who can then be questioned separately. If they
have not applied for a post of responsibility (in this example), why not? Your sub-
sequent questions to this group will pursue this theme. And if they have applied,
what happened? A separate route of questioning can be used for this subset.
Multiple-choice questions
These contain two or more answers where respondents can be told either to tick
one box or to tick as many boxes as needed.
Rank-order questions
Here, respondents have to rank items (that is, put them in order) on a list according
to some criterion – for example, best to worst, most able to least able, or degree of
difficulty. Within this ranking, you can either ask for a limited number of choices
(for example, first, second and third), or require respondents to rank the whole list.
Rating-scale questions
These require the respondent to rate some experience, attribute, attitude, etc., along
a continuum. You may for example wish to ask adults about their experience of
testing and assessment when they were at school:
Remembering back to when you were at school, would you say that your
experiences of formal assessment and testing were:
Very positive
Positive
Neutral
Negative
Very negative
Importance 1 2 3 4 5
Caring
Efficient
Reliable
Kind
Knowledgeable
Understandable
Helpful
Scales
Scales, that is to say, sets of items and responses, appear in some of the question
formats above. However, the Likert scale is worth mentioning as an easily used tool
for measuring attitudes: respondents indicate their levels of agreement to state-
ments provided by the researcher relating to that attitude, belief or characteristic.
The respondent, rather as in rating-scale questions, responds to each item on a
five-point or seven-point scale, for example corresponding to strongly agree, agree,
neither agree nor disagree, disagree, strongly disagree. To remove the tendency for
some people to over-choose the middle option, this middle option is sometimes
removed, making a four-point scale. The latter is used in the Rosenberg Self-Esteem
Scale, where the first three items are as shown in Table 10.3.
Strongly
Strongly agree Agree Disagree disagree
1. On the whole I am
satisfied with myself
2. At times I think that I
am no good at all
3. I feel that I have a
number of good
qualities
SOME WAYS TO COLLECT DATA AND EVIDENCE 215
Open questions
You can think of open questions in a questionnaire in the same way that you
think about unstructured interviews, diaries and accounts. They are bounded by
the same considerations, in that you are aiming to get at the central concerns of
an issue as your respondents see them. In a written questionnaire, though, where
there is little in the way of stimulus (as there would be in an interview), if you
want to use open questions you will have to give more of a prod to jog the mind
of your respondent.
Open questions may be simply a question such as ‘How would you describe your
manager’s management style, in two or three sentences?’ Or you may structure
them differently, for example:
Observation
Observation is a key way to collect data, though there are quite different ways in
which to go about it. The core difference is between:
• the kind of observation in which you systematically look for particular kinds of
behaviour, and
• the kind of observation in which you watch informally (but methodically), recording
important facets of what is happening.
The first kind of observation, where you watch for particular kinds of behaviour, is
called structured observation. The second kind, where you are in the situation, take
part, record and watch from within, is called unstructured observation.
If you undertake a structured observation, you make assumptions that the
social world is viewable through a prism that enables the breakdown of social
activity into quantifiable elements – bits that you can count. You, as the observer,
have to define what these bits are. They may be individual pieces of action or
language, such as a child physically touching another child or a teacher using
a particular kind of question. The next thing that a structured observer has to
do is devise some way to count these elements. This can be done in a number
of ways.
216 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Structured observation
Duration recording
Here, the observer measures the overall time that a target behaviour (such as ‘child
out of seat’) occurs. You will end up with an overall time, for example 27 minutes
in a session of 60 minutes.
Frequency-count recording
The observer records each time a target behaviour occurs. You will end up with
an overall number, for example four times in a session. (This is also called ‘event
sampling’.)
Interval recording
You decide on:
You will end up with data that can be processed in a number of ways. The most
usual way of processing data is to count up the number of times that the target
individual has scored in, say, the behaviour category of interest and then to express
this as a percentage of the total number of possible observations.
Unstructured observation
By contrast with structured observation, an unstructured observation is undertaken
when you immerse yourself in a social situation, usually as some kind of participant,
in order to understand what is going on there. This is the more likely scenario in a
case study. Often this kind of observation is called participant observation, because it
is associated with researchers becoming participants in the situations that they are
researching. Ethnographers, with whom this kind of observation is usually associ-
ated, often discuss social life as if it were a stage, or a set of stages, on which we act
out roles (see the discussion of Goffman’s work in Chapter 3). Unstructured obser-
vation will be geared to understanding how these roles play themselves out on the
stage of life.
SOME WAYS TO COLLECT DATA AND EVIDENCE 217
As Burgess (1982) points out, the term ‘participant observation’ is a little con-
fusing since it connotes much more than simply observation. It entails talking to
people, watching, reading documents, keeping notes and anything else that ena-
bles you to understand a situation.
Image-based methods
Here, photographs, videos, films, graffiti, drawings, cartoons and so on are used as
data sources. Especially now that digital photographs and videos are more or less
free and disposable, image-based methods provide a powerful extension of obser-
vation and open up a range of possibilities for case study research. Image-based
methods not only slot effortlessly into the illustrative case study but also are a
means of recording observations and eliciting responses from your research par-
ticipants in more or less any kind of case study. There are a number of advantages:
• You can more easily include the person or persons on whom the research focuses.
While observation pure and simple is always from researcher to researched, image-
based methods offer the reciprocal relationship as well – that is, the researched
can set the agenda and the research can be more inclusive. In research with young
people in a secondary school, Cremin et al. (2011) explored the students’ ‘voice’ by
giving them a camera.
• You can capture a social scene far more quickly than you can with notes. The cap-
tured scene will enable you to freeze it in time for your subsequent analysis at
leisure.
• The scene can be captured discreetly, with little input from you, ready for your
subsequent interpretation. Not only is it more subtle than, say, interviewing, but
it can also break the ice and help conversation to start. As Schwartz (1992: 1) put
it, the camera was ‘an excuse to start up a conversation, and the longer I made
photographs, the more people I met’.
• You can adapt your method to your case study ‘process’. For example, in sequential
or diachronic studies (see Chapter 9) you may wish to repeat photographs of the
same situation over a range of times and dates.
• These methods can be blended extraordinarily easily with other methods. Prosser
and Loxley (2008) describe a study about city gentrification that combined pho-
tography, ethnographic fieldwork, grounded theory and analysis of detailed field
notes over a period of 16 years.
• Images can enable the use of the photo-elicitation method – that is, photos
(or other images or videos) are shown in interviews to evoke a response. This
218 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
• a norm-referenced test compares the person being tested with a sample of similar
people
• a criterion-referenced test assesses whether or not someone meets some crite-
rion, irrespective of how well other people perform on the test.
Tests of ability and attainment are usually norm-referenced since they seek to tell
testers how well those tested compare with other people. The driving test is the
SOME WAYS TO COLLECT DATA AND EVIDENCE 219
data. Do try to get a grip of the range and variety of data-gathering instruments and
understand them as well as you can. In this chapter we have reviewed the use of …
• interviews
structured
unstructured
semi-structured
• accounts
• diaries
• group interviews
• focus groups
• document interrogation
• questionnaires
• observation
structured observation
unstructured observation
participant observation
• image-based methods
• measurements and tests
• official statistics
• social media.
Having made use of these tools and techniques to gather together a body of data,
you will want to proceed to analysis. To analyse the data you gather you will need
to think about analytical techniques, and these are introduced in Chapter 11.
REFLECTIVE ACTIVITY
Using the example of Nav, the dentist, and his concerns about patient experi-
ence at his dental practice (see p. 155 and Figure 8.6), identify the forms of data
collection Nav planned to use. Think further about the storyboard in Figure 8.6
and see if it can be developed further and in different directions. Are there other
ways that Nav could collect data, either using methods outlined in this chapter,
or using methods you have seen outlined elsewhere?
222 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Further Reading
Bryman, A. (2015) Social Research Methods (5th edn). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
An excellent compendium on research methods and data-gathering techniques.
Cohen, L., Manion, L. and Morrison, K. (2017) Research Methods in Education (8th
edn). London: Routledge.
Cohen et al. give an excellent overview and explication of a wide range of research
methods used in social research, and in particular in education.
Ortlipp, M. (2008) ‘Keeping and using reflective journals in the qualitative research
process’, Qualitative Report, 13 (4): 695–705. Retrieved from https://nsuworks.
nova.edu/tqr/vol13/iss4/8
This article gives some excellent advice on using a reflective journal.
Salkind, N.J. (2019) Statistics for People Who (Think They) Hate Statistics (7th edn).
London: Sage.
Good on basic statistics, with some information also on not-so-basic statistics.
Smith, E. (2006) Using Secondary Data in Educational and Social Research. Maidenhead:
Open University Press.
An excellent discussion of the use of secondary data.
Thomas, G. (2017) How to Do Your Research Project (3rd edn) London: Sage.
Chapter 7 covers data gathering and goes into more detail than I do here about
interviews, accounts, diaries, questionnaires and observation.
11
ANALYSIS: A TOOLKIT
FOR ANALYSING
AND THINKING IN
CASE STUDY
As I have been noting throughout the book, there are two parts to a case study:
(i) the subject; and (ii) the analytical frame or object. The way in which you develop
the analytical frame takes shape in the context of all of the considerations that I
have already touched upon, and these considerations will require that you think
long and hard about what you wish your case study to be a study of. In Chapter 1 I
gave the example of the choice of World War II as a subject. On its own World War II
would not be a case; only with an analytical frame such as ‘a case study of a “just
war”’ would the subject derive meaning as a case study.
So, the analytical frame – the object – is essential. While the development of the
analytical frame is crucial – it is, after all, at the core of the study – the way in which
you go about actually undertaking the analysis is almost equally important. It is
about how you do the analysis.
There are myriad ways in which you can analyse data. Since we are concentrating
on analysis in a case study here, though, I am going to say a little about methods
that seek a holistic analysis rather than those encouraging a separate analysis of
particular parts. I do hope that you will not feel that these are compulsory or rec-
ommended, since case study, as I have been at pains to point out, is not defined
by an approach, nor by a method, nor even by a set of methods – even if these
conspicuously stress the holistic. Rather, a case study is a container, a wrapper, for
a situation or a set of circumstances and it may contain a range of phenomena to
be analysed. The analysis of those phenomena may, of course, take multiple shapes
224 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
or forms depending on the nature of the phenomena, so you may use statistics or
sociograms or network analysis (Thomas, 2017) or whatever, and these may all pro-
vide perfectly satisfactory means of analysis of these different elements.
You will wish to take a line in your study, however, that explicitly frames your
analysis in a holistic context. It is a case study, after all; it is holistic. So, the ways
of thinking about analysis that I offer in this chapter stress a view of the whole
situation. It is, perhaps, rather an unusual assortment and by no means covers
exhaustively the analytical methods that are available, but I have brought these
together as means of seeing patterns and developing connections. They are, of
course, no substitute for your thinking. In fact, on the contrary, they require your
thinking, but what they do is help your thinking progress.
bells and whistles that are added to the constant comparative method, but, how-
ever elaborate, it will always be defined by the simple principle of going through
data again and again (this is the constant bit), comparing each element – phrase,
sentence or paragraph – with all of the other elements (this is the comparative bit).
That’s all there is to it, though it has been pimped up by the addition of many
and varied ‘improvements’, which you can find detailed in textbooks on research
methods. You may find some of these useful, but most, in my opinion, make things
appear to be more difficult than they really are.
The basic principle governing the process of constant comparison is that you
emerge with themes that capture or summarise the essence (or essences) of your
data. Taylor and Bogdan (1984: 126) sum up the process thus:
1. READ. Examine all of your data – read the interview transcripts, diaries,
notes from unstructured observations and so on, look at videos and lis-
ten to audio recordings.
2. COPY. Make an electronic copy of all of your raw data and mark it ‘RAW’.
You now have two copies: your raw data (which you do not want to lose
or corrupt) and your working data files. Keep them separate. In fact,
employ belt and braces: keep a copy of your raw data on a memory stick
as well as on your hard drive. Now, rename your working files, adding
the suffix ‘WORKING’.
3. CODE. Read through your working files. As you are reading, underline
or highlight parts that seem to be important. This underlining or high-
lighting is called coding. If you have audio or video from which you have
transcribed, look at or listen to this as well. So much more comes from
that context. As you proceed, you will get an impression of important
ideas or subjects that are recurring. These can be called your temporary
constructs or prima facie constructs. Make a list of them.
(Continued)
226 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
4. READ AGAIN. Read through the data a second time, using the list of
temporary constructs from your first reading to check against. Draw up
a grid, with the temporary constructs on the left and page references to
where the constructs are evidenced on the right. Make notes and obser-
vations on the grid as you do this.
5. SORT AND SIFT. Get rid of any temporary constructs that do not seem to
have been reinforced in the rest of the data. Do not delete the actual data
themselves, though – they may form an important counter-example for
the general themes that are emerging. Highlight these counter-examples
in a different colour in your working data records and keep a separate list
of them.
6. RE-CODE. From the second reading, come up with second-order con-
structs that seem to be a good ‘fit’ with your data. These second-order
constructs should do a good job of summarising the important ideas in
your data.
7. EMERGE WITH THEMES. Look through once more, refining these sec-
ond-order constructs now as marker posts for the final organisation of
your data. Once you are satisfied that these capture the essence of your
data, label these as your final themes.
8. THINK ABOUT THE THEMES. How do they seem to be connecting
together? What matches with what? Are there any unanimous areas of
agreement? Are there any contradictions or paradoxes?
9. MAP. Find ways to map your themes (see below).
10. ILLUSTRATE. Select good quotations or sections from your work to illus-
trate the themes.
These themes, or categories, are the essential building blocks of your analysis.
Remember that the aim in using an interpretative approach is to emerge with the
meanings that are being constructed by the participants (including you) in the
situation.
There are various ways in which you can map your themes to show the intercon-
nections between them. This mapping is often the weakest part of students’ use of
interpretative data. While the identification of themes is important, students some-
times go little beyond this and do not manage to explain how ideas are related to
one another.
ANALYSIS 227
More on coding
As I have just noted, when you are using the constant comparative method you
mark up your data with codes – abbreviations, names, marks and/or colours – that
describe important facets of the data. As I outlined in the points above, you can call
these temporary constructs and second-order constructs – different commentators use
different names for this process of sifting and categorisation. Do not worry about
the different words – they are describing the same process. Your main aim in doing
this is to pick out the important bits, and eventually emerge with themes which
capture or summarise the contents of your data. For example:
Teacher: Oh, we’ve got a little girl in here; she looks different and she
acts different from the others, and they’ll make up a name that they
call her. Um, it seems like every year there’s one child that gets picked
on more than somebody else, because they’re different, because
they might look different, they might act different. Say, for instance,
she’ll say something or she gets very excited about something, they’ll
tell her to ‘Sit down’, ‘Be quiet’, ‘Stop doing that’ – they’re like on her
case all the time. Then you’ve got another boy in here who, well he
loses control of himself, and so he blurts stuff out or yells out, and
the other children will turn around and yell back at him. And out on
the playground they do tend to sometimes get physical. Like with
her [pointing to a desk] I’ve watched them actually walk by this little
girl and purposely bump into her or something like that, but then
even though you’re watching them, the child will turn around and say,
‘Well, I didn’t do that’ when you confront him. And it’s not everybody,
it’s just a few, and it’s a few that have behaviour problems that seek
attention, and they don’t know how to get attention any other way
besides a negative way.
(Continued)
228 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
There are no right or wrong answers about how you code: you may choose
to code this passage entirely differently from the way I have done it here: you
will be guided in your coding by the focus of your study.
Ultimately, you need to read into what your codes (and eventually your
themes) seem to be identifying (and a fuller transcript is given in the online
resources). For example:
Theme mapping
While the constant comparative method can identify the themes in a case study, on
its own it does nothing to show the relationships among those themes. You need
to make it clear from your commentary and discussion how the themes interrelate.
Theme mapping makes such interrelationships clearer by presenting an interview
and its themes in picture form.
Theme mapping begins, as does most qualitative analysis, with the constant
comparative method. Once you have established your themes, you go through your
working data files and look for good quotations that illustrate those themes. Then,
in the order that those quotations appeared in the interview, you can put them into
boxes on the page (see Figure 11.1). The page now becomes your ‘map’. You may
also find other quotations that in some way complement or contrast with these
quotations. Put these in boxes on the ‘map’ too. Now, label the boxes with the
names of the themes and draw dashed lines if ideas seem to be connected between
themes, and solid lines with arrows where one theme (the theme towards which
the arrow points) seems in some way to account for or explain the theme at the
other end of the arrow.
1 PERSONAL 2 THREAT/SUSPICION 3 TERRITORY
4 COMMUNICATION 5 PROFESSIONAL 6 7
The example I have given in Figure 11.1 is a theme map following the analysis of
some interviews I conducted with support teachers for children with special educa-
tional needs (see Thomas, 1992). Rather than withdrawing children to work with
on their own, these teachers had changed their way of work to be in the classroom,
working alongside the class teacher. I was interested in their perceptions and feel-
ings about their role in the classroom. Did they feel useful? Were there tensions,
and, if so, of what kind? From my interviews and from my own diary, working as
a support teacher, I emerged with a number of themes relating to these questions.
These were: status and self-esteem, territoriality, threat/suspicion, interpersonal fac-
tors, ideology–professional, communication, organisation, school policy and role
clarity. You will see that seven of these nine themes are flagged in the interview
theme map in Figure 11.1. Aside from anything else, the theme map gives a kind of
mini-representation of the interview, because the illustrative quotations are given
in the order that they have appeared in the interview.
NVivo
Remember when you are doing all of this that it is the twenty-first century and
there are many software tools to help you analyse data in the form of language. You
may, for example, find that packages such as NVivo and ATLAS.ti help if you have
a large amount of data. Your university will probably have an institutional licence
that will give you free access to one or both of these packages.
NVivo enables you to code data – documents, PDFs, audio, video – and sort these
data into themes. The process for examining the data is essentially the same as I
noted above for the constant comparative method. In other words, you explore
the data, come up with tentative ideas and code interesting bits, search the data
for similarities, mark these, group them under ‘nodes’, and eventually emerge with
final themes.
Figure 11.2 gives an example of an NVivo page from a project in which I was
involved with my colleagues Liz Ellis and Liz Hodges (Ellis and Hodges, 2014). This
was a project funded by the deafblind charity Sense, exploring the life experiences of
people with Usher syndrome, which is the most important single cause of deafblind-
ness. Forty-two people with Usher syndrome were interviewed and their interviews
were transcribed into a Word document. This document was imported into NVivo,
the data were coded and nodes identified. Figure 11.2 shows those nodes.
You will see that 17 nodes were identified in total; the ‘Education’ node has
been expanded to show its 8 ‘child nodes’. The ‘Sources’ column shows how many
respondents made reference to each node, and the ‘References’ column shows how
many times the topic (that is, a reference coded by this node) appeared in total. So,
ANALYSIS 231
Figure 11.2 Part of a page from NVivo showing nodes and child nodes from the Usher
project (Ellis and Hodges, 2014)
Grounded theory
Grounded theory (Glaser and Strauss, 1967) is a commonly used term to describe the
process of drawing themes from qualitative data – or, even more broadly, the whole
232 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
process of interpretative research. Many people say that they are using a grounded
theory approach when what they mean, actually, is that they are using the constant
comparative method. Some people speak about grounded theory as if it is synony-
mous with interpretative inquiry – or even as if all of interpretative inquiry depends
on it. It is not, and it does not.
Perhaps I’m being a bit pedantic, but I find this scooping up of all things interpre-
tative and calling them ‘grounded theory’ a little irritating. Interpretative inquiry
is the Big Daddy; grounded theory is a set of techniques for use in interpretative
inquiry. In fact, many of the assumptions behind grounded theory – for example,
about grounded theory enabling prediction – seem inappropriate and past their
sell-by date now, and I have explored this elsewhere (Thomas and James, 2006).
Lincoln and Guba (1985: 339) make a criticism similar to mine, suggesting that
constant comparison is the kernel of grounded theory worth preserving.
What is helpful about grounded theory is that it offers a neat encapsulation of
the essence of interpretative inquiry – in that it puts a heavy emphasis on the way
that the ideas (the ‘theory’) emerge from your immersion in a situation. This is in
contrast, of course, to the notion that you can go into a case study with fixed ideas
(fixed ‘theory’) about what may be happening. Again, though, it is important to
note that many commentators have queried how far one can clear one’s mind of
existing ideas and theory and allow these to condense, untainted, out of the data
(again, see Thomas and James, 2006). One should, in other words, acknowledge the
extent to which already-established ideas and theory may contribute to the illumi-
nations, interpretations and understandings that come through case study inquiry,
and this is why it is so important to discuss one’s positionality in the methodology
chapter of the case study – in order that the reader can have some understanding of
the provenance of those interpretations.
• Open coding is the first stage, going through the data, examining them, comparing
one part with another and beginning to make categorisations. This is, if you like,
the part where you are using your coloured highlighters to mark the text: it is the
identifying of the ‘temporary constructs’ I talked about when discussing the con-
stant comparative method.
ANALYSIS 233
• Axial coding is the second stage, in which you begin to make sense of the open
coding. It is where you ask yourself ‘What goes with what? What can we call
this set of comments? How are these ideas connected to those?’ Here you can
come up with labels for your codes – labels similar to the ‘nodes’ used in NVivo,
or the ‘second-order constructs’ I noted in the constant comparative method
above.
• Selective coding is the final part where the main themes are drawn. As Strauss and
Corbin put it (1990: 116), it is ‘The process of selecting the core category, system-
atically relating it to other categories, validating those relationships, and filling in
categories that need further refinement and development’. It is the stage where
you may be drawing the theme map I discussed above.
Strauss and Corbin also talk about memoing, which is just a posh way of saying tak-
ing notes. As you go through the coding and constant comparative processes, you
are taking notes (memos), in whatever way you choose, about associations, poten-
tial themes, ideas and so on. It may be useful to keep an example of your memos
for use in an appendix to your case study write-up.
Thick description
If you are confident enough, you may wish to forgo the systemisation offered by
the constant comparative method and its derivatives and rely on ‘thick descrip-
tion’. The term ‘thick description’ was popularised in interpretivist circles by the
great anthropologist Clifford Geertz (1975). It refers to understanding a piece of
behaviour – a nod, a word, a pause, for example – in context, and using one’s
‘human knowing’ to interpret it when one describes it.
Geertz (1975: 9) avers that in interpreting meanings you cannot be simply a
‘cipher clerk’. You cannot, in other words, just report things ‘thinly’ without con-
text. The example he uses is the one originally used by philosopher Gilbert Ryle
(from whom he borrowed the idea of thick description), of three boys moving their
eyes. With one, the movement is a twitch, with another a wink, and with the third
a parody of the second one’s wink. What turns the twitch into a wink, and what
makes the mickey-take a parody? As Geertz puts it, ‘a speck of behaviour, a fleck
of culture, and – voilà! – a gesture’. The trick is, in reporting your observations, to
make clear what is going on in a social situation to turn the twitch into the wink, or
into the parody of the wink. How do you know which is which? The interpretative
researcher has to tell the reader.
When you are offering thick description you are also offering an analysis. You are
doing this by intelligently reflecting on the scene, imagining, putting yourself in
234 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
another person’s shoes, and genuinely interpreting what the other person is doing.
You are doing this with the knowledge you have not just of people, but also of life
and of the contexts that people inhabit – the stages on which they act. If you opt
for doing this kind of thick description, it helps, then, to know something of the
situation that you observe.
Good examples of this making use of your ready-made knowledge – the knowl-
edge of being human – are found in the great sociologist Erving Goffman’s (1956)
book The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life. Here, Goffman unpacks a storehouse
of examples of how people behave in social life as if they were on a stage, always
conveying some meaning with their behaviour. In doing this, he is employing thick
description.
Word clouds
Think about using something such as a ‘word cloud’ to represent the verbal data
that you collect. I pasted the first few pages of this chapter into a program called
Wordle (available at: www.wordle.net and see Figure 11.3 – other word cloud gen-
erators are available; search for ‘free online word cloud generator’). While the words
from this chapter produce a rather dull cloud, I have to admit, the program could
produce some very interesting comparisons if you were, say, contrasting the tran-
scripts from long, unstructured interviews in a comparative case study. You can
choose the shape of your word cloud – I rather like the idea of the footprint used
for this one. While a word cloud almost certainly will not suffice as a method of
analysis on its own, it can often be a good starting point to get you thinking – one
can view it as a pilot study of an interview.
Figure 11.3 Using a word cloud can give a visual impression of an interview
ANALYSIS 235
Discourse analysis
Discourse analysis is the study of language in social use and it may be particularly
useful in the study of documents, speeches, essays, dialogue, newspaper articles
and so on. As Arnott and Ozga put it, these often overtly or between the lines ‘carry
definitions of problems, reference particular kinds of evidence and argument, and
produce “knowledge” of particular kinds’ (2010: 339).
The field of critical linguistics has sought to reveal the power of various kinds of lan-
guage, in texts and talk, and how readers or listeners are influenced via the use of particular
words and terms and argumentative strategies. It looks at the way in which choices
are made about people to cite, events to exemplify and words used to describe them.
Rather confusingly, discourse analysis is spoken about in different ways in differ-
ent branches of the social sciences. Psychologists think of discourse as the language
that goes on between people, tending to focus on small units of language such as
the choice of individual words and intonations (‘micro-analysis’). Sociologists, by
contrast, tend to think of discourses as forms of language use that define social
relations, and particularly power relations between and among people (‘macro-
analysis’). You will understand from these very different starting points that there
is no one method to discourse analysis.
Fairclough (1995) describes what he calls a ‘critical discourse analysis’ which is
useful for us in the applied social sciences because it combines both psychological
and sociological traditions. Fairclough puts it this way:
There is no set methodological formula for doing this. For simplicity’s sake, take
it that the general method in ‘gutting’ a document, an interview or some other
sample of language use is broadly the same as in the constant comparative method
outlined in this chapter. The difference is in the focus of the discourse analyst.
Rather than being at the first level on ideas, it is on the use of particular words,
phrases, idioms, similes, metaphors, kinds of rhetoric and so on. How are these
used to construct notions such as ‘education’, ‘health’, ‘order’, etc.? In each case
the discourse analyst will look to see how notions are constructed by the choice of
words and language forms used in a discourse, whether that be an interview tran-
script or a document.
Sociograms
Sociograms are useful for mapping relationships among people. They are often
used in case study as they offer interesting insights into the dynamics of a group.
Notably, they were used by Paul Willis in Learning to Labor (1981). Using a variety of
analytical means, Willis untangled how the students at the ‘Hammertown’ school
in which he was observing developed an antagonism towards school. They devel-
oped what Willis calls a ‘counter school culture’. He intertwined the development
of the theoretical narrative about counter-culture with observations and illustra-
tions from the case study itself. As part of this, he used sociometry.
The sociogram given in Figure 11.4 is adapted from research I undertook for the
children’s charity Barnardo’s on the inclusion of children with physical difficulties
in mainstream schools. The children in one primary school class were asked to say
in confidence who they would like to sit next to or play with.
Sociometry involves asking respondents who they would like to sit next to or
who they would like to work with – or the opposite: who they would not like to sit
next to, and so on. For each choice an individual makes an arrow is drawn to show
the direction of choice. If a choice is returned, the arrow becomes double-headed.
Various features may emerge from the sociogram:
• one-way choices, where someone chooses another but this choice is not reciprocated
• reciprocated choices, where individuals choose each other
• cliques, tight-knit groups of three or more whose choices are all made within the
group
• isolates, those with no choices from other individuals
• clusters, more general groupings
Sophia
Maya Zoe
Oliver
Jacob
Ava
Noah Max
Lily Jack
Madeline
Leo
Charlie
George
Charlotte
Chloe Harry
Joshua
Henry Dylan
Abigail Luke
Grace
Daniel
Layla
Ellie Ben
Adam
Amelia
Emma
Hayley
Systems thinking
I discussed the close ties between systems thinking and the case study in Chapter 3.
Because the aim of systems thinking is to try and avoid breaking up a complex web
of social activity, it fits naturally with the holistic emphasis of the case study. It is
especially useful if there is a problem or puzzle to be solved, which there might be
in an action research project, for example. Given that action research is likely to be
about your own specific situation, the case study will be particularly appropriate.
I have noted that there have been several models developed for using systems
thinking. These have originated in science and technology, but one of the most use-
ful for our purposes has been devised for use in social systems, where the problems
are often taken to be ‘messier’ than those in, say, engineering. It is known as the soft
systems methodology and was conceived by Peter Checkland (1981).
Checkland’s soft systems method is particularly useful in what might be called
‘purposive’ systems – that is, systems with a purpose. You can probably imagine a
broad range of purposive systems in your home or university or work life, such as
a system for taking out the rubbish on bin day, a university system for ensuring
speedy feedback to students on their work, a system for involving parents in their
children’s school. Almost anything where a purpose is involved and people are
expected to do something to achieve the purpose requires a system.
Checkland’s soft systems methodology involves seven stages:
elements and how they fit together and include, for example, relationships and
attitudes – the ‘atmosphere’. Draw stick diagrams of the people, with thought bub-
bles showing their fears and hopes, agreements and conflicts.
3. This is perhaps the most important stage and involves moving into systems thinking –
a bit like going through the back of the wardrobe and into Narnia. Here we have to
write ‘root definitions’, which will say something like ‘a system to do A by means of
B in order to C in the context of D’. The root definition involves six features of the
situation as a system:
a) customers, those who benefit from the system
b) actors, people who transform inputs into outputs
c) transformation from this to that, A to B, inputs to outputs
d) Weltanschauung, the broader context and worldviews
e) owners, people who ‘own’ the problem and want it resolved
f) environment, the constraints that the environment sets up.
Checkland gives this set of considerations the acronym CATWOE, created from the
first letters of the names of the six features listed above. It can be a bit confusing
since, in a simple system, the same person or people can occupy several of the
roles (an actor could be the same as the owner, for example). The root definition
(see above) enables us to think in systems terms about the roles being occupied by
people, the values and objectives that they may hold, who holds power and how it
operates in the system.
4. In this stage, you can think about how things might change. You develop what
Checkland calls ‘conceptual models’. You can be as creative as you like as you do
this, imagining how things would work. You should think about what is a ‘must
have’ in the system and what is simply desirable.
5. You now come back through the wardrobe door and return to the real world. You
compare the ideal, coming from the conceptual models, with the real world. You
ask why the actual and the imagined are not the same.
6. You look to see what changes may be desirable and feasible, on the basis of the
comparison you made in stage 5.
7. Action is proposed to improve the problem situation.
This all perhaps looks a little rigid and mechanical, but I prefer to think of it as
a framework for thinking, using the ‘let’s look at the whole picture’ of systems
thinking. Dick (2002) provides a useful summary of the approach. He says that
Checkland’s method has at its heart:
240 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
a comparison between the world as it is, and some models of the world
as it might be. Out of this comparison arise a better understanding of
the world (‘research’), and some ideas for improvement (‘action’) …
[T]he researchers begin with a real-world problem. They study the sys-
tems which contain the problem. Following this, they develop some
models of how those systems might work better … The ‘ideal’ mod-
els are then compared to the actual situation. Differences between the
models and reality become the basis for planning changes.
Checkland provides examples and it is useful to look at his book if you wish to go
into this method in any further detail. Figure 11.5 gives a summary of the method
and Figure 11.6 gives a worked example.
So root definition is ‘A
family system to empty
bins regularly by relevant
family members in line
with weekly bin collection’
I have given plenty of examples of storyboards earlier in the book, so I will not
give another now. Here are some tips, however, about the nitty-gritty of drawing
storyboards in Word. Do remember, though, it is the ideas that are more important
than the drawings themselves and they do not need to be done in Word – you could
do them just as effectively with sticky notes.
1. Place the cursor where you want the Drawing Canvas for your storyboard to be.
2. Click the Insert tab at the top of the Word page, then look for the Illustrations
section, click the little arrow under Shapes, scroll to the bottom, and click New
Drawing Canvas.
3.
When you put your cursor
inside the Canvas, you’ll
find that the menu at the
top of the page adds ‘Shape
Format’ at the right-hand
side. (If the Shape Format
menu doesn’t immediately
appear, click Shape Format.)
This gives you options about
shapes to insert.
If you feel life is too short for this, you can draw it by hand and/or use sticky notes,
but it’s not as hard as it sounds. If you commit to devoting a couple of hours of your
life to it, you’ll find it quite rewarding.
Glue? Tool? Yes, I am mixing metaphors a bit. I think of theory as both, really, but
let’s start with theory as glue.
Theory as glue
I have said that your case study is like a wrapper containing all the bits of your
inquiry. The inquiry is defined by a moment in time, a person, place or whatever,
and your job in this is usually to drill down to the ‘how’ and the ‘why’. In doing
this, you will amass a treasure trove of data that you need to make sense of. For
all of the data to provide useful pieces of sensemaking, you need to connect them
somehow, which is where theory comes in – it makes the connections between X,
Y and Z and holds them together.
It is the ‘holding together’ bit that is important. Perhaps rather than glue, it is
best to think of theory as the sinews and tendons of your study – holding one part
to another, yet allowing them to articulate with each other.
Theory as a tool
The great sociologist Pierre Bourdieu was the person who encouraged me to think
of theory as a tool. He said of his own work (Bourdieu, in Wacquant, 1989, cited in
Jenkins, 1992: 67, emphasis in original):
For Bourdieu, then, theory exists for a purpose. It is not the aim of the inquiry, it
is not an end in itself, it has to do a job, which is to help explain your findings. It
comes and goes as you need it, so it may be fleeting, like a shooting star, giving you
a flash of inspiration.
This idea of transitoriness is at variance with the idea of theory that is sometimes
held in social science. Sometimes, I think, people assume that theory is the end
point of an inquiry, but Bourdieu’s advice – excellent advice in my opinion – is
that it is disposable. It is the explanation the theory provides that is important, not
the theory itself. When I shave with a disposable razor, the important part of the
process is my silky smooth skin (I wish) at the end, not the razor itself. I don’t, on
completing my ablutions, carry the razor to work with me to show off to my col-
leagues, and, when it’s blunt, I dispose of it.
244 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Here’s an example:
ANALYSIS 245
Already, then – in this review of the literature and her reflections on it,
informed and bolstered by her own informal observations – Kayla is bring-
ing a theoretical element to her original questions. Already the beginnings
of answers are emerging – a little crystal of an idea forming here and there,
ready to be followed up and built on. These are embryonic theories, ready
for further investigation.
Kayla’s prima facie questions did not need much reformulation, but she
modified them more precisely to address her interests thus:
(Continued)
246 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
• Are there differences between the ways that newspapers report general
interest news stories in North America and in the UK?
• If so, how are these manifested in discourse?
• Are differences in discourse likely to impact the nature of journalism and
public discourse more generally?
The first of these three questions cannot be answered by a case study since it
calls for some kind of representative sample being taken of similar stories in
matched newspapers on both sides of the Atlantic. So, such a comparison of
stories in matched newspapers using formal discourse analysis is what Kayla
did to answer this question in the first part of her inquiry. We will not delve
any deeper into this since it is not relevant to our subject of case studies.
The second and third questions, however, are suitable for exploring using
a case inquiry, so Kayla decided to conduct a comparative case study of two
newspapers – the Guardian and the New York Times – over a period of two
months, comparing especially their treatment of similar or broadly equiva-
lent news stories.
Kayla was surprised at the degree to which the actuality bore out her
expectations. Here is one example of different treatments of stories about
something as seemingly innocuous and unadversarial as the weather. The
first is from the New York Times on 6 February 2010, about a particularly
fierce Washington, DC snowstorm:
The hard edges of Washington were softened as the snow recast the
capital of monuments and malls into a postcard town of soft ice cream
shapes that had been statues and aerodynamic blobs that had been
parked cars: the buried machines of a lost civilization.
Contrast this with a report from the Guardian about the unusually snowy and
cold winter in the UK exactly one month before the record of Washington’s
snow:
Ideas
‘Theorisation’ and ‘theory’, then, take a variety of shapes and forms as a study
progresses. There are two stages:
1. setting your analysis in the context of everything that has gone before, especially
your literature review
2. tying findings together – intertwining ideas, seeing patterns – and then knitting all
of this together into a fabric that is called ‘theory’.
In doing all of this, the important thing is to use your brain to try and understand
what is going on – to make sense as you progress.
Elliot Eisner (1991: 238) suggests that theorising is the ‘art of the eclectic’, wherein
we put things together, make multiple interpretations, build bridges and reason by
analogy. He makes a plea for ‘convincing insight rather than Truth’.
Using narrative
Throughout this book I have stressed the importance of the narrative – the
storyline – in a case study.
ANALYSIS 249
In the same way that a story has coherence, integrity and progression, so must a
case study. In looking at the whole you are eschewing the reductionist, fractionat-
ing methods of much social science inquiry – methods that attempt to dissolve the
connecting threads and fibres that hold social phenomena together. In a case study
you must make sense of the whole by retaining the fibres that bind the whole story
together. Those fibres concern time, place, meaning, intention and much more, all
interrelating. The interrelationship makes sense in much the way that a story does.
We cannot take one page of a novel and make very much sense of it. Nor can we
extract and analyse just the sentences that contain a character’s name to work out
his or her personality – we would probably form a very distorted picture of that
character if we did. Rather, each character is understandable only in relation to the
whole story. It is the same in a case study.
Not only this, but the structure of a story itself adds a great deal. In stories there
are assumptions about, for example, motives, intentions, jealousies and kindnesses.
These are things that we all understand from our daily experiences, our understand-
ing of life, and we use this experience and these understandings to deconstruct the
narrative of a case study.
The educator–philosopher Jerome Bruner (1997: 126) goes so far as to say that
narrative is at the heart of all meaningmaking, even if it is scientific meaningmaking:
In his famous article in the journal Critical Inquiry, he calls this the narrative construc-
tion of reality (Bruner, 1991: 1). I give in the box below some key ingredients when
using narrative in case studies. In doing this I have borrowed heavily from Bruner’s
(1991: 3) ‘ten features of narrative’, taking away a few of them and adding a couple
of my own. It is an anatomy, if you like, of storymaking. I offer these ideas as ways to
extract every possible drop of juice from the story you are telling in your case study.
(Continued)
250 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Narrative diachronicity
Narrative diachronicity? Not the most user-friendly term. ‘Diachronic’ means
changing over time. So, narrative diachronicity means ‘a story that changes
over time’.
Someone doing a case study should be acutely aware of change over
time – and not only in a diachronic study (see page 183). They notice
change as it happens and seek its antecedents and consequences. We
have to find the ‘sequence of steps’, as Becker (1992: 209) puts it, and
understand cause in relation to time, with ‘each step understood as pre-
ceding in time the one that follows it’. In doing this, we conjecture not
only about how one thing is related to another, but also about how cause
ANALYSIS 251
and effect change with time as other variables in a situation also change.
Becker (1992: 209) gives an example:
So, time may switch on or switch off the potency of a variable, and it is
important to be aware of this since its significance is one of the ready-made,
built-in advantages of the case study. While a reductionist inquiry shows the
patchy significance of a variable only with great difficulty, a case study ought –
by stressing the importance of the diachronic – to show how a variable such
as race (in Becker’s example) may ebb and flow in its significance.
Particularity
In her novel Under the Net, Iris Murdoch (2002) has one of her characters
come out with this line: ‘All theorising is flight. We must be ruled by the
situation itself and this is unutterably particular.’ This is perhaps at variance
with the advice I have been giving about the need for theorising, and there
is an apparent contradiction here. Certainly, if theory is about generalisation,
there is a tension to be resolved.
Without going into the ins and outs of this now, what Murdoch is getting
at is that there is a uniqueness to a particular situation and we should seek
to understand this without what the philosopher Michael Oakeshott (1967: 2)
calls the ‘irritable search for order’. We should not always judge this situation
and its significance by reference to others but by reference to the particular.
This may call for a special effort of will from the student or professional
researcher, given the ever-present desire to establish, develop and refer to a
certain kind of generalising theory among social scientists.
(Continued)
252 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Analogy
We make sense of the unfamiliar by reference to the familiar, drawing
likenesses between one situation and another. We use our own knowl-
edge to do this – our ‘common sense’. Our understanding is based on
myriad personal interpretations we weave together into meaningful sto-
ries that help us to make sense of similar events and situations, similar
plotlines.
The narrative that a case study lets you draw can be the ideal frame for
enabling such analogy and metaphor. Tavor Bannet (1997: 655) discusses
analogy and, interestingly, says that it is a ‘method of reasoning from the
known to the unknown, and from the visible to the speculative’ by carry-
ing familiar terms and images across into unfamiliar territory. It is like a
form of translation, ‘a way of transporting something from place to place,
from old to new, from original to copy’, and we can therefore move from
one context to another. We bring together, juxtapose and see similarities
across contexts.
In discussing the importance of narrative in the case study, Abbott (1992) suggests
that we should always be seeking what he calls ‘causal narrativity’, but I personally
prefer to suggest that, in social science, we are, as with ‘theory’, making connec-
tions rather than trying to find causes. Becker (1998: 60–1) puts this very nicely:
he suggests that the use of the word ‘cause’ is misleading in social research, given
its complexity, so we should see ourselves as seeking narrative rather than a cause:
Assume that whatever you want to study has, not causes, but a history,
a story, a narrative, a ‘first this happened, then that happened, and
then the other happened, and it ended up like this’. On this view we
understand the occurrence of events by learning the steps in the process
by which they came to happen, rather than by learning the conditions
that made their existence necessary.
It is the case study that enables such creativity – allows you to suggest these bridges
and passages between ideas.
254 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Think drama
In Chapter 3, I looked at the ways in which some sociologists, such as Erving
Goffman, have organised their views of the world around dramaturgy. This is a view
in which life is seen as a set of dramatic performances and has been used by not just
Goffman but also a range of interpretivist social scientists. They talk about actors,
roles and stages in their discussions of the interactions that take place between
people.
If the meat of your case study is about people’s interactions and you decide to
adopt an interpretivist approach (see page 159), how do you take it forward? I think
one of the best ways to make this happen is to extend the theatrical analogy. Think
of the substance of the case study as a play, a drama, complete with:
The likening of a case study to a play is useful, I think – not just because it stresses
the bounded nature of this study (the boundaries in the case of the drama being
provided by the script of the play and the walls of the theatre) but also because it
emphasises the narrative of the study. In a play, a story is told – of a drama, with
a beginning a middle and an end (unless it is Waiting for Godot). There are argu-
ments and links intertwined through the story – a plot, with questions, premises,
intrigues, subterfuges. Characters come to life as they deal with the obstacles that
the plot puts in front of them. The case study is similar on all these counts.
The analogy is not perfect, however, since in a play the work is fictional and in a
case study the focus – the person, event, phenomenon or whatever – is there. It is
real. If it were not real we would not be interested in studying it.
So, there are differences and it is interesting to pursue the dramaturgical meta-
phor for those differences. In a case study, the script is not written by a playwright,
but is ‘written’ by life, by what has occurred. Anyone who has watched a modern-
day production of a Shakespeare play will realise, though, that a script can be inter-
preted in such a way that a familiar play becomes almost unrecognisable. Directors
examine scripts for meanings and impose their own interpretations on them. In the
same way, the script provided by a case study is interpretable. Actually, the ‘facts’
of the case are far more fluid than a play’s script since it will be for you (as director)
ANALYSIS 255
Earth does this sentence/this paragraph/this chapter mean? How does it fit? What’s
it doing here?’ In my long life, I have read many dissertations and this, sadly, has
been an all-too-frequent set of questions in my mind while reading them.
For the readers, reading your report should be like eating a bowl of their favourite
ice-cream. It should be smooth, enjoyable, have nice gooey bits that are particularly
interesting and, when they have finished, it should leave them thinking ‘I enjoyed
that, mother. Is there any more?’
Actually, it should be better than a bowl of their favourite ice-cream because it
should get them thinking – and there is nothing better in life than thinking.
There are various techniques that can be adopted when writing up your case
study that will make it more interesting and more polished – I shall discuss a few
of these in Chapter 12.
In Chapter 1, I noted that Einstein had said of the process of science, ‘There is no
logical path, but only intuition’ and this sentiment – with the obsession of social
science to be methodologically exact, rigorous and innovative – has sometimes
been forgotten. The philosopher Karl Popper (1968: 32) made a comment similar to
Einstein’s: ‘there is no such thing as a logical method of having new ideas, or a log-
ical reconstruction of this process. My view may be expressed by saying that every
discovery contains “an irrational element”, or “a creative intuition”.’ He proceeds
to liken this to Einstein’s comment (in an address on Max Planck’s 60th birthday)
that scientific advance depends on ‘intuition, based upon something like an intel-
lectual love (Einfühlung) of the objects of experience’.
So, it is intuition and intellectual love that we have to nurture. A tough call, per-
haps, since intuition and intellectual love are qualities that do not grow on trees,
nor do they lend themselves to bullet-point breakdowns. Given the elusive nature
of intellectual love, imagination and intuition, how should we consider them and
how should we foster them?
Where to start? Sadly, thinking about how we should foster an intuitive mindset
has not been a central consideration in the social sciences. The social sciences have
instead become obsessed with correctness, reliability and design exactitude, at the
expense of imagination and intuition.
ANALYSIS 257
With the paradigm wars that I spoke about on page 51 it is almost as though a
false opposition has been set up wherein one cluster of ideas – positivist, normative,
experimental, objective – has been set against another – interpretative, subjec-
tive, intuitive. This leads to a regrettable hunkering down and a defensiveness in
each of the positions. It leads ultimately to an inward turn, wherein there is more
concern for process than there is for questions, arguments and conclusions. The
consequence is that curiosity in inquiry is diminished and research becomes less
interesting.
As astute critic of social science, Stanislav Andreski (1972: 108), says that how we
achieve creativity
Is Andreski right about this? Can we really not offer useful advice on creativity and
intuition?
In large part I think he is right. I quoted the first line from Wittgenstein’s Tractatus
right at the beginning of this book, and he said such beautifully gnomic things that
I cannot resist quoting from him again. This time, I shall quote the last line, in the
context of Andreski’s advice (Kenny, 1993: 31): ‘Whereof one cannot speak, thereof
one must be silent.’
How true, though I think Wittgenstein was saying a little more than just
‘Keep your mouth shut if you have nothing to say’ (this is something I have
often wanted to stand up and shout out at conferences, but I have never had
the courage). Indeed, Frank Ramsey – philosopher, genius, admirer and transla-
tor of Wittgenstein – after translating the Tractatus (at the age of 19!) suggested
in a subsequent commentary that what Wittgenstein was getting at here was
‘What we can’t say we can’t say, and we can’t whistle it either.’ Ramsey died
at the age of 26. How tragic that someone so gifted and so funny should die so
young.
I cannot do better than Ramsey’s explanation about whistling, save to offer a
very ungnomic deciphering of what these superbrains were going on about. It
is this: some things are beyond our words; they exist in what the philosopher
Michael Polanyi (1958) has called ‘tacit knowledge’. Included in tacit knowledge
are your knowledge of how to ride a bicycle (if you can) and your intuition about
how to solve difficult puzzles. What the case study offers here, then, is the capac-
ity – or, better, a predilection – to see wholes rather than fractured parts and, with
258 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
All of this is similar to the ‘imagery’ that Becker (1998: 12) speaks about. He suggests
that, in studying society, we start with images and end with them. Our aim ‘is the
production and refinement of an image of the thing we are studying’. The rest of
Becker’s book, Tricks of the Trade, is excellent further reading on this.
Well, I haven’t done very well in advising you how to be intuitive, have I? No.
Perhaps, then, I should sum it up by saying that we all have intuition – it’s part
of the human condition – and when you are doing a case study, treasure it and
don’t let it become suffocated by concern over what you consider to be correct
procedure.
ways in which you can give integrity and coherence to your analysis. To review
some of these ways of making an analysis:
• the constant comparative method offers a means of drawing themes from inter-
pretative data
• theme mapping allows you to present in diagram form the ways in which themes
may interconnect
• packages such as NVivo enable the easier management of coding and theme-
making where large amounts of qualitative data are involved
• ‘grounded theory’ offers a specialised operationalisation of an interpretative
approach
• ‘thick description’ captures the essence of interpretative analysis in its synthesis
of reporting, reflecting and meaningmaking
• systems thinking lets you understand how separate bits of behaviour may be inter-
connected
• the storyboard offers a way of navigating along different potential routes to
develop your argument
• you have always in your analysis to be thinking of developing theory – it is about
seeing links, noticing patterns, abstracting ideas from your data and offering
explanations
• thinking about narrative – or how the events in your study can be seen drama-
turgically – can help to give structure to the events that unfold. In doing the
analysis, it is important to understand how you may be both using and develop-
ing your theory and how you can be intuitive and imaginative in making sense of
your findings.
REFLECTIVE ACTIVITY
In case study we are usually looking at data as words or images – heard, seen,
interpreted and (possibly) understood. Many of the data analysis strategies and
techniques outlined in this chapter involve the summarising of these words and
images and/or their transformation into a different form which makes them eas-
ier to understand, which interprets them, or which emphasises their relevance to
the topic being studied. Some methods are simple, while others are more com-
plex. One of the simplest starting points I have outlined in this chapter is the
word cloud, and while I would not recommend the word cloud as a sole means of
(Continued)
260 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
analysing data, it offers a good starting point for thinking about the use of words,
interpretations and themes.
Try pasting a page or more from some writing you have done – perhaps an
essay or other assignment, or a transcript from an interview – into a free online
word cloud generator. Does this give you any clues about the themes covered in
your writing or in your respondent’s interview response? What clues does the use
of words in the passage give you for further analysis?
Further Reading
Becker, H.S. (1998) Tricks of the Trade. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Becker’s take on research is refreshingly independent of formulae and bullet points.
He encourages readers to use their own resources of imagination and intuition to
make shape and sense out of their data.
Bruner, J. (1991) ‘The narrative construction of reality’, Critical Inquiry, 18 (1): 1–21.
The essence of a scientific approach, says Bruner, is in meaningmaking, while
narrative, with its ‘grammar’, its rules and its structure, provides a vehicle for
meaningmaking.
Eisner, E. (1991) The Enlightened Eye: Qualitative Inquiry and the Enhancement of
Educational Practice. New York: Macmillan.
Eisner has some interesting ideas on what constitutes theory in qualitative inquiry.
George, A.L. and Bennett, A. (2005) Case Studies and Theory Development in the Social
Sciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Taking quite a traditional view on theory, the authors give a perspective on the case
study that is located principally in political science. It thus draws on a different dis-
ciplinary base from most commentary on the case study and offers some interesting
discussion on comparative case studies.
Taylor, S.J., Bogdan, R. and DeVault, M.L. (2016) Introduction to Qualitative Research
Methods: A Guidebook and Resource. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
A key text, and excellent on the constant comparative method, with some fascinat-
ing examples of real-life qualitative inquiries.
Wright, T. (2010) ‘Learning to laugh: a portrait of risk and resilience in early child-
hood’, Harvard Educational Review, 80 (4): 444–63.
An excellent and moving study, told in a narrative form.
12
WRITING YOUR STUDY
Ultimately, your case study is written up as a research project, dissertation or thesis.
So, it needs to conform to what is expected of one of these. How, then, should it
be structured, and is it different from the balance and shape of the write-up of a
project that follows a different research design?
Structure
The following gives a breakdown of a typical dissertation or thesis. Be aware, how-
ever, that there are few ‘typical’ dissertations; all are different, and yours may be
rather different from this. For example, a literature review may comprise not one,
but two or more chapters. The findings and discussion may be conflated into one
chapter or expanded to form several, depending on the nature of your work. A case
study will often be unusual in its structure because of its typically narrative form,
but you should remember that there are essential elements to any project that must
be incorporated in the write-up. Here is a summary.
You need an introduction that explains how you came
to be interested in the topic of the study. Here, you will lay
Chapter 1
out your prima facie questions (see page 30).
Introduction
You need a literature review that looks at what other
inquiries have been done on this or related topics and helps
you to understand their contribution to your own ques-
Chapter 2 tion. As I explained in Chapter 2, this enables you to refine
Literature review
and reformulate your prima facie research question(s). It
may comprise more than one chapter if there are different
elements of the literature that you feel warrant their own
treatment.
WRITING YOUR STUDY 263
Figure 12.1 shows the rough proportions (in terms of numbers of words) given to
each of these sections, though I should warn you that this is only indicative – some
case study write-ups will have a very different balance. As I have noted, your field-
work, findings, analysis and discussion may, in an interpretative case study, all be
merged together because you will be reporting observations almost simultaneously
with discussing them.
264 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Conclusion Introduction
5% 5%
Findings Method
15% 15%
Figure 12.1 Elements of the write-up of a formal, traditional study and their rough
proportions
The methods that may be used inside the case study wrapper may be constrained by
the same kinds of fences, but the case study itself is defined more by your own imagina-
tion than methodological boundaries. You thus have freedom to roam, without fences.
Marvellous. With this freedom, though, the onus is on you, the researcher, to provide
the structure and meaning for the work. With a case study, at its worst, this can lead to
the mushy heap syndrome to which I referred earlier (see page 265). Sometimes, what
goes under the name ‘case study’ can amount to not much more than an undifferenti-
ated collection of thoughts and quotations from interviews with very little in the way
of glue to hold the whole thing together. This is what you must avoid.
The point I am trying to make is that the quality of a case study cannot be guar-
anteed by simply following a procedure correctly. Instead, it is determined by your
imagination, the way in which you construct your narrative and theory, your criti-
cal reasoning and the way in which you develop your argument using evidence.
The whole of this book, really, has been about your imagination, so I will not dwell
on this now and I have discussed narrative and theory in Chapter 11, but it is
important to say a little more about critical reasoning and argument.
• Are there any assumptions, overt or hidden, that underpin the research? Does it
seem to start from a particular position? For example, much research in the area of
WRITING YOUR STUDY 267
special education has started from the position that ‘children with special needs’
are an identifiable group or set of groups. This approach is now taken to be sim-
plistic. If you read around, you will get a rounded picture of the area that you are
studying, taking in the controversies and disagreements.
• Are there any vested interests at play? Was the research funded, for example, by
an organisation that may have had an interest in particular findings being made?
• Might the writers’ objectives in undertaking the research sway their reasoning in
some way?
• Would different methods have yielded different findings?
• What sources of information are being drawn upon – is there evidence of balance,
or are sources being ‘cherry picked’?
• What is the quality of the data being drawn upon – are they from good primary
sources?
• Is the writer’s reasoning sound – so, if you were arguing with the writer, what would
you say? (But ask yourself also how much validity your own criticisms would have,
and whether you yourself are likely to be swayed by tradition, sentiment or vested
interest.)
You will need to be tentative about conclusions that you feel you are able to make.
Try to avoid phrases such as ‘this proves’ and, instead, express yourself in phrases
such as ‘this indicates’ or ‘the evidence suggests’ or ‘points towards’. Try to use
phrases that moderate the strength of an assertion, such as ‘tends to’ or ‘one might
conclude that’. Academic writing is an area where it pays to be tentative: no one
will give you high marks for making unwarranted claims about your own research
or for innocently swallowing the reports of others.
Doubt everyone’s findings, even your own. Many great minds have attested to
the importance of this. René Descartes (1647/1996) said that ‘Doubt is the ori-
gin of wisdom’, summing this up in the aphorism ‘De omnibus dubitandum’
(meaning ‘Doubt everything’), which Karl Marx took as his own motto for life.
Similarly, the great biologist J.B.S. Haldane concluded his famous essay Possible
Worlds (1928: 224) with the words ‘science has owed its wonderful progress very
largely to the habit of doubting all theories’. Jeremy Paxman summed up the
sentiment a little more demotically in his personal rallying cry, ‘Why is this lying
bastard lying to me?’ Actually, Paxman claims that what he meant was that he
always approaches people with ‘a degree of scepticism, asking “why are they say-
ing this?” and “is it likely to be true?” … Scepticism is a necessary and vital part
of the journalist’s toolkit’ (Wells, 2005). Scepticism is a necessary and vital part of
the researcher’s toolkit, too.
268 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
• going beyond surface appearances and digging down, trying to understand the
meaning of an observation or an utterance
• including consideration of the context (what
is happening, what went before, what were
Be critical. Remember: ‘De omnibus the consequences?)
dubitandum’ or ‘Why is this lying
bastard lying to me?’ • uestioning your first assumptions, connect-
q
ing with the ideas of others.
Let us look at these in turn and see what each should contain.
The problem
The problem must be the sandwich filling between two elements: the context and
the response:
WRITING YOUR STUDY 269
The opening context sets out some common ground on which everyone can
agree. The problem or ‘angle’ has two parts:
Beyond this, your literature review will help you to refine and revise the issue that
you wish to research. After doing this, you will begin your research using your case
study and the methods that you have chosen to conduct it. Your case study wrapper
encases all of the methods and instruments you choose to address the issue.
So, in the example of the special school that was closing, my fellow researchers
and I had started with some notions about the ways in which inclusion operated,
and these notions were further informed by our reading of the literature. We then
had ideas – a theory, if you like – about the challenges and opportunities that the
project might face as it proceeded. We made observations, held interviews with staff
and students, kept diaries, conducted documentary analysis and drew sociograms,
and these helped to ‘thicken’ or, alternatively, tone down or soften the various
strands of theory that we had developed.
The argument
The evidence that emerges as you proceed contributes all the time to the question
that you originally posed. Does the question still seem valid in the light of the new
information? Do you want to change course?
Remember that evidence is nothing on its own: it is data in support of a propo-
sition or a claim, so you will be modifying and adapting the claims that you are
making as you continue with your empirical work.
So, the process of argument involves recursion (retracing your footsteps), sum-
mary, synthesis, putting ideas (yours and those of others) next to each other to see
how they shape up together.
This is the way that your argument develops. It involves a revisiting and reas-
sessment of your original ideas and arguments to see which seem more or less
valid. Some ideas may be rejected or put into abeyance. Tell readers about this –
tell them about your process of acceptance, rejection and reformulation and the
conclusions you have come to. Booth et al. (2016) suggest that the process is
like that set out in Figure 12.3. I’ve added the bin to their diagram – I hope they
don’t mind.
Claim
Supports
Reasoning
Explains
Supports
Figure 12.3 The relationship between evidence and reason supporting a claim
(adapted from Booth et al., 2016)
I give several examples of such quality elsewhere (see Thomas and Myers, 2015).
As you explore the methodological literature about the case study, you will repeat-
edly find the same iconic studies used as examples. Again and again, you will find
case studies done as part of the sociological tradition, such as Howard Becker et al.’s
(1961/1980) Boys in White and William Foote Whyte’s (1955/1993) Street Corner
Society. You will probably also find reference to The Polish Peasant in Europe and
America by Thomas and Znaniecki (1927/1958).
I think it is a pity, though, to treat the case study as the sole prerogative of the
interpretative sociologist, so, in seeking examples of good analysis I have tried to
take a broader disciplinary approach to it. In the points that I make in this section,
I should like – for the sake of freshness – to use different examples of the genre.
They are interesting not only in terms of what they have to offer as case studies,
but also because their authors do not flag the fact that they have undertaken case
studies. Perhaps they don’t even care that they’re doing case study research. As I said
272 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
earlier, those who research in disciplines outside the social sciences seem delight-
fully unselfconscious about the methods they use.
I will concentrate on two sets of case studies – those done by a neurologist and a
biologist – for the insights that they have to offer.
The exploration of deeply altered selves and worlds is not one that
can be fully made in a consulting room or office. The French neurolo-
gist François Lhemitte is especially sensitive to this, and instead of just
observing his patients in the clinic, he makes a point of visiting them
at home, taking them to restaurants or theatres, or for rides in his car,
sharing their lives as much as possible. (It is similar, or was similar, with
physicians in general practice. Thus, when my father was reluctantly
considering retirement at ninety, we said, ‘At least drop the house
calls.’ But he answered, ‘No, I’ll keep the house calls – I’ll drop every-
thing else instead.’)
A case study is like keeping the house calls. Sacks spent days and weeks with
people with different kinds of behavioural issues – ones that caused them or
WRITING YOUR STUDY 273
those around them concerns of one kind or another. He made every effort,
however, to put those behaviours in context, not seeing them as disembod-
ied, decontextualised medical or psychiatric conditions but, rather, as valid
ways to deal with the world, sometimes very successfully. For example, an
autistic woman who found great difficulty relating with other people but
who felt great tenderness for animals, combined the latter with her skill in
drawing and planning. She used this to help in the design of more humane
slaughterhouses that minimised, as far as possible, any potential distress
to the animals. Rather than defining a condition or trying to exemplify or
generalise from a condition with a supposedly typical example, Sacks used
the intimate knowledge he was gathering to illustrate the idiosyncrasy, the
particular story, of her case. He showed the individual at work and at home.
He showed her autonomy, strength, individuality and the integrity of her life.
This is not something that could be said of most medical or psychological
writing about autism.
Sacks makes no attempt to generalise from this or from any of his
vignettes, and his method is almost invisible since he lays out no mental
map at the beginning of his work. Rather, readers are left to make up their
own minds; they are left to piece together conclusions for themselves.
The conclusions – conclusions, note, rather than generalisations – are
about the ways in which difference can be respected rather than pigeon-
holed. The pigeonholing comes from the obvious categories of clinical and
administrative convenience and the emotions associated with those stereo-
typical categories leading too easily into hackneyed emotions of pity or
admiration so familiar to those with the conditions concerned. By using the
case study, Sacks expects to move away from the rut of categories and offers
us instead rich analyses of people’s lives.
The second set of case studies is from the work of well-known biologist Jared
Diamond (2005). While Sacks leaves us to make what we will of his case studies,
with each one being a little jewel in and for itself, Diamond develops from the case
studies a complex argument and thesis.
He looks at a range of communities and societies that have collapsed or are cur-
rently in the process of collapsing. Each one – the Montana of today, the Easter Island
of the seventeenth century, Pitcairn, the Mayans, the Vikings, modern Haiti – is very
different from the others, but they are connected by decline and collapse.
Diamond’s thesis is that the decline of a society happens because of a number
of potential factors, ranging from hostile neighbours to environmental change
and the society’s response to that change. His first point, then, was to establish a
274 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
connection between these communities. The second was to look for threads that
might form the basis of the connection.
How did all those Easter Islanders, lacking cranes, succeed in carving,
transporting, and erecting those statues? Of course we don’t know for
sure, because no European ever saw it being done to write about it.
But we can make informed guesses from oral traditions of the island-
ers themselves … from statues in the quarries at successive stages of
completion, and from recent experimental tests of different transport
methods.
WRITING YOUR STUDY 275
For me, two things are interesting about this passage. One is the self-
questioning that I have just mentioned. The other is the humility and
deference with which Diamond approaches knowledge. ‘Of course we
don’t know for sure’ should be the motto of anyone doing a case study. I’ll
go further: it should be the motto of any inquirer. We don’t know anything
for sure – we don’t even know for sure if the sun is going to rise tomorrow
morning.
The second thing is that Diamond goes on to point out that we make
informed guesses, which is a point I have made throughout about the
case study. Guessing – using our imagination on the basis of the available
evidence – is one of the main tools of the researcher, not just someone
working on a case study.
Diamond’s method of informed questioning, reasoning and case con-
struction works powerfully – and intelligent guessing is at the centre of all
this. It happens not just as a result of combining inert hard facts (which are,
of course, never as hard as we might like them to be) but also a chemis-
try, a bubbling cauldron in our brains. Thus, while he is making his case,
Diamond is imagining, linking here, there and everywhere, from prehistory
to Hollywood. He tells a story and develops an argument.
We are not all as imaginative or knowledgeable or energetic as Diamond,
but we can replicate his process to an extent. We can weave a narrative,
using the information that we garner not just as part of our formal inquiries
but also from our everyday lives – as professionals and ordinary people.
Thus, it is about the creative and the pragmatic. The Guardian (2010) asked 20
leading novelists and writers to offer their own ‘rules for writers’ and these, inter-
estingly, split into the creative and the pragmatic. In fact, it was so interesting that
276 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
I have extracted some of the ones most pertinent to our own focus here and taken
the liberty of offering my own comments in Table 12.2.
A last thought about writing up a research report: you do have to consider register.
‘Register’ means the accepted form and style of writing for a particular audience
or forum – to write your dissertation using text-speak, for example, would obvi-
ously be unacceptable. A student of mine told me that she had tried to write like I
do in another of my books. Flattering as this was, I had to remind her that in the
book I was writing for a particular audience – that is, students who wanted to learn
something and who are sometimes intimidated by academic style and jargon – so
my register was as light as I could make it without its actually blowing away. The
register you need for a report is, sadly, more formal.
Table 12.2 Rules for writers, taken from the Guardian (2010)
Author My comment
Diana Athill
• Read it aloud to yourself Then you will hear as the reader will hear. Does it
sound right? Does it make sense? Will the reader
understand?
• Cut (perhaps that should be CUT): only Do cut out words that you think sound impressive.
by having no inessential words can every Avoid – like the plague – pretentious words such as
essential word be made to count ‘hegemony’ and ‘inscribe’
Roddy Doyle
• Chances are the words that come into As above. Ordinary words are usually best, though
your head will do fine, e.g. ‘horse,’ ‘ran,’ you will sometimes need technical ones in a piece of
‘said’ academic work. Think about the possible words you
could use and judge which will be right
Helen Dunmore
• Reread, rewrite, reread, rewrite Excellent advice. You might add … and read it out
loud to someone else
Anne Enright
• Keep putting words on the page One of the ways to make sure you get the thing
finished is just to keep writing. Every day. Set
yourself a target number of words per day. Even if
you write rubbish, you are keeping the momentum
going and you can cut out anything that doesn’t
work later
Jonathan Franzen
• The reader is a friend, not an adversary, Do think of your readers. Your readers are not some
not a spectator inert, bodyless phenomena. They are human beings
who breathe, burp, yawn and make sense of your
work (or don’t), depending on how you write it.
Imagine your readers
WRITING YOUR STUDY 277
Author My comment
Michael Morpurgo
• Record moments, fleeting impressions, Particularly in a case study, you make sense of
overheard dialogue, your own sadnesses others’ intentions, feelings, words and emotions.
and bewilderments and joys Keeping a record of your own may help you to
interpret the emotions and ideas of others
Andrew Motion
• Honour the miraculousness of the Look for what is special in ordinary moments … the
ordinary gaze that is a second too long, the turn of the head,
the uncomfortable moment of silence … What can
you glean from anyone doing a case study?
• Think big and stay particular Could be the motto of anyone doing a case study.
While seeing the big picture – the model you are
developing – always root it in the concrete, the
particular
Will Self
• Always carry a notebook. And I mean Self-explanatory and very true
always. The short-term memory only
retains information for three minutes;
unless it is committed to paper you can
lose an idea for ever
Jeanette Winterson
• Turn up for work. Discipline allows She means ‘get down to work – and regularly’.
creative freedom. No discipline equals no Nothing gets written if you don’t write
freedom
• Never stop when you are stuck As above – it’s the momentum thing again
and admired. George Orwell, in his brilliant essay Politics and the English Language
(1946), makes the point that there is bad writing that finds its roots in imitation of
others (that is, imitating them and therefore not sounding like yourself) and he sug-
gests that this bad, imitative writing actually leads to sloppy thinking.
While your writing may well be published, for most write-ups your audience will be
other students, colleagues at work and, perhaps most importantly, your markers (in
the plural, since there will nearly always be at least two). Try to remember that your
markers are human, and want to read something that engages them, that they find
coherent, logical and interesting. It is good to have an idea in mind of what your mark-
ers are like so you can have them in your mind when you do your writing. Wallace
and Poulson (2005: 8) offer a useful guide, which I have liberally adapted in Table 12.3.
Table 12.3 A sense of audience: what your reader (and marker) looks like. (Liberally
adapted from Wallace and Poulson, 2005)
Age 25–70+
Appearance Anyone’s guess – from smart casual to mad-professor-look with odd socks. (If male
and wearing a tie, ask to see their credentials)
Lifestyle Busy, and likes reading – so appreciates writing with a logical structure and clear focus
Attitudes Fair and respectful
Sceptical
Open-minded
Favourite What you’re writing about. Highly knowledgeable about the area in general, but
subject possibly not au fait with the details of your specific topic
Likes Books, broadsheet newspapers and scholarly periodicals. So … likes carefully
reading … constructed, well-argued, balanced, detailed, reflective writing
Pet hates Waffle
Irrelevance
Avoidable errors … of punctuation, spelling, referencing – or anything that careful
proofreading would have picked up
Most likely How is this relevant?
to say Keep to the point
Be more critical
Read around the subject more
middle and end), quality of argument and style. It also has to be good to read. Do
think about your readers – put yourself in their shoes. Remember:
however, that there is any requirement to split the world into manageable-sized
chunks since, by that splitting, we may force ourselves to sidestep scrutiny of some
key aspect of the whole.
I have cautioned in this book against the ‘mushy heap syndrome’ in case studies – a
syndrome in which a case is loosely focused without care for the elements of design
and conduct that I have stressed. At its worst, a case study can be a bit like an
unmade bed, with an untidy collection of quotes and observations without object,
direction, argument or conclusion. At its best, though, it can provide sparkling
insight and analysis that is unrivalled by any other kind of research. I hope that
in this book you will have found advice that helps you to conduct such research.
Enjoy your case study research, and good luck.
REFLECTIVE ACTIVITY
While a case study done as a piece of research has to conform to certain expecta-
tions, particularly if it is done as a piece of assessed academic work, it is, more
than the write-ups from research structured according to the expectations in
other design frames, a piece of writing. It should be interesting, stimulating,
thought-provoking. It is not just a write-up: it is writing, and the story, the narra-
tive, has to come through. This is why I have included advice from outstanding
writers at the end of this chapter. Try extracting from their advice, in three bullet
points, the key points for you.
Further Reading
Becker, H.S. (2007) Writing for Social Scientists (2nd edn). Chicago: University of
Chicago Press.
Good advice on writing, but it has to be read, like most of Becker’s work, a bit like a
novel. Very good on the idea that there is no single way of writing something well.
You can say things in a variety of ways. Lots of good examples. No bullet points.
Becker, H.S. (2014) What About Mozart? What About Murder? Reasoning from Cases.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
More wisdom from Becker, this specifically on case study.
Booth, W.C., Colomb, G.C. and Williams, J.M. (2016) The Craft of Research (4th
edn). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
WRITING YOUR STUDY 281
I have mentioned this book in relation to its advice about establishing a rationale
(Chapter 7). It is equally good on the question of writing – not just how to con-
struct good sentences but also how to build good argument.
Cohen, M. (2015) Critical Thinking Skills for Dummies. London: Wiley, For Dummies.
Don’t be fooled by the ‘for dummies’ of the title. This is the most thoughtful and
readable book about critical thinking that I know of. It is as entertaining as it is
informative.
Cottrell, S. (2017) Critical Thinking Skills: Effective Analysis, Argument and Reflection.
London: Macmillan.
Good, basic advice on reflecting on your own work.
Diamond, J. (2005) Collapse: How Societies Choose to Fail or Survive. London: Penguin.
This book is a collection of case studies that can be read separately or all together.
About the collapse of societies due to environmental degradation, together the
studies offer an integrated thesis that Diamond unrolls as he proceeds through this
multiple case study.
Goodwyn, A. and Stables, A.W. (eds) (2004) Learning to Read Critically in Language
and Literacy. London: Sage.
While this book is nominally about criticality in language and literacy, the first two
chapters, by Louise Poulson and Mike Wallace, are specifically on criticality gener-
ally and what is meant by ‘thinking critically’ in higher education. They are as good
as anything else I have read on the topic, before or since.
Kamler, B. and Thomson, P. (2014) Helping Doctoral Students Write: Pedagogies for
Supervision. London: Routledge.
Useful advice throughout on writing in academic life, and particularly on the rela-
tionship of writing to research.
Mewburn, I., Firth, K. and Lehmann, S. (2019) How to Fix Your Academic Writing
Trouble. London: Open University Press.
Good practical advice on writing, offering a range of techniques and strategies to
achieve a ‘scholarly voice’. Begins each chapter with examples of where things can
go wrong.
Two examples are given in this chapter of good analysis and writing. This is another
one. The study is of a working-class family in the 1930s which successfully resisted
the pressure of the local authority to send the youngest child of the family to a
residential special school. It raises a range of issues, among them eugenics, the
employment of psychometrics, the use of authority and professional power, and
the changing role of parents. It brings these together to provide a rich analysis of
the ways in which forces interplay to develop, enact or resist policy.
Thomas, G. (2012) ‘Changing our landscape of inquiry for a new science of educa-
tion’, Harvard Educational Review, 82 (1): 26–51.
Thomas, G. (2016) ‘After the gold rush: questioning the “gold standard” and
reappraising the status of experiment and randomized controlled trials in educa-
tion’, Harvard Educational Review, 86 (3): 390–411.
In both of these articles I talk about the power of case study as a form of social inquiry
vis-à-vis other forms of inquiry. They were written in the context that the use of
case study research has waned as policymakers have sought ‘evidence-based’ policy
informed by randomised controlled trials. They seem unable to understand the frailties
of such trials in social research and the potential contribution of case study evidence.
Thomas, G. (ed.) (2013) Case Study in Education. Volumes 1–4. London: Sage.
This collection gives many examples of iconic case studies and less well-known but
equally good ones. Some of these provide excellent examples of good writing.
Thomas, G. and Myers, K. (2015) The Anatomy of the Case Study. London: Sage.
In this book, Kevin Myers and I try to work out what is going on ‘inside’ the case
study. In Chapter 6 we offer ten examples of what we consider to be good analysis.
Dickens, W.T. and Flynn, J.R. (2001) ‘Heritability estimates versus large environ-
mental effects: the IQ paradox resolved’, Psychological Review, 108: 346–69.
Eisenhart, M. (2009) ‘Generalization from qualitative inquiry’, in K. Ercikan and
W.M. Roth (eds), Generalizing from Educational Research. Abingdon: Routledge.
Eisner, E. (1991) The Enlightened Eye: Qualitative Inquiry and the Enhancement of
Educational Practice. New York: Macmillan.
Ellis, L. and Hodges, L. (2014) ‘Life and change with Usher: the experiences of diag-
nosis for people with Usher syndrome’, University of Birmingham (available at:
www.birmingham.ac.uk/Documents/college-social-sciences/education/projects/
final-report-on-life-and-change-with-usher.pdf).
Evans, H. (2000) Essential English for Journalists, Editors and Writers. London:
Pimlico.
Fairclough, N. (1995) Critical Discourse Analysis. Harlow: Longman.
Fairclough, N. (2003) Analysing Discourse: Textual Analysis for Social Research.
London: Routledge.
Ferguson, N. (1999) Virtual History: Alternatives and Counterfactuals. New York: Basic
Books.
Feyerabend, P. (1993) Against Method (3rd edn). London: Verso/New Left Books.
Field, A. (2013) Discovering Statistics Using SPSS (4th edn). London: Sage.
Fish, S. (1989) Doing What Comes Naturally. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Flynn, J.R. (1987) ‘Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: what IQ tests really measure’,
Psychological Bulletin, 101: 171–91.
Flynn, J.R. (1998) ‘IQ gains over time: toward finding the causes’, in U. Neisser (ed.),
The Rising Curve: Long-Term Gains in IQ and Related Measures. Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Flynn, J.R. (1999) ‘Searching for justice: the discovery of IQ gains over time’,
American Psychologist, 54 (1): 5–20.
Flynn, J.R. (2003) ‘Movies about intelligence: the limitations of g’, Current Directions
in Psychological Science, 12 (3): 95–9.
Flyvbjerg, B. (2001) Making Social Science Matter: Why Social Inquiry Fails and How It
Can Succeed Again. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Flyvbjerg, B. (2006) ‘Five misunderstandings about case-study research’, Qualitative
Inquiry, 12 (2): 219–45.
Foucault, M. (1981) ‘Questions of method: an interview with Michel Foucault’,
Ideology and Consciousness, 8 (Spring): 3–14.
Geertz, C. (1975) The Interpretation of Cultures. London: Hutchinson.
Geoghegan, T. (2007) ‘History lessons: for Americans, Herodotus has better ones to
offer than Thucydides’, The American Prospect, 12 March (available online only
at: https://prospect.org/article/history-lessons-1).
REFERENCES 287
Georgiades, N.J. and Phillimore, L. (1975) ‘The myth of the hero-innovator and
alternative strategies for organizational change’, in C.C. Kiernan and F.P.
Woodford (eds), Behaviour Modification with the Severely Retarded. Amsterdam:
Associated Scientific Publishers.
Glaser, B.G. and Strauss, A.L. (1967) The Discovery of Grounded Theory: Strategies for
Qualitative Research. New York: Aldine.
Goffman, E. (1956) The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life. Edinburgh: University of
Edinburgh Press.
Grinyer, A. (2002) ‘The anonymity of research participants: assumptions, ethics and
practicalities’, Social Research Update, 36: 1–6.
Guardian (2010) ‘10 rules for writers’, Guardian, Guardian Review, 20 February: 2–5.
Haig, B.D. (1995) ‘Grounded theory as scientific method’, in A. Neiman (ed.),
Philosophy of Education Yearbook, 1995: Current Issues. Champaign, IL: Philo-
sophy of Education Society, University of Illinois Press at Urbana-Champaign,
pp. 281–90.
Haldane, J.B.S. (1928) Possible Worlds and Other Essays. London: Chatto & Windus.
Hammersley, M. (2005) ‘Assessing quality in qualitative research’, Paper presented
to ESRC TLRP seminar series: Quality in Educational Research, University of
Birmingham, 7 July.
Hammersley, M. (2007) ‘The issue of quality in qualitative research’, International
Journal of Research and Method in Education, 30 (3): 287–306.
Hammersley, M. and Gomm, R. (2000) ‘Introduction’, in R. Gomm, M. Hammersley
and P. Foster (eds), Case Study Method. London: Sage.
Hearnshaw, L.S. (1979) Cyril Burt: Psychologist. London: Hodder & Stoughton.
Holton, G. (1995) ‘The controversy over the end of science’, Scientific American, 273
(4): 168.
Houser, N., Kloesel, C. and the Peirce Edition Project (eds) (1992) The Essential
Peirce, 2 vols. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.
James, W. (1890/1981) The Principles of Psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Jenkins, R. (1992) Pierre Bourdieu. London: Routledge.
Jiménez, R.T. and Gersten, R. (1999) ‘Lessons and dilemmas derived from the liter-
acy instruction of two Latina/o teachers’, American Educational Research Journal,
36 (2): 265–302.
Johnson, S. (1759/1963) ‘The Idler, no. 58, Universal Chronicle’, in W.J. Bate, J.M.
Bullitt and L.F. Powell (eds), Works of Samuel Johnson, Vol. 2. New Haven, CT: Yale
University Press.
Junghans, C., Feder, G., Hemingway, H., Timmis, A. and Jones, M. (2005) ‘Recruiting
patients to medical research: double blind randomised trial of “opt-in” versus
288 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
“opt-out” strategies’, British Medical Journal, 331: 940 (available at: www.bmj.
com/cgi/reprint/331/7522/940.pdf).
Kahneman, D. and Tversky, A. (1973) ‘On the psychology of prediction’, Psychological
Review, 80 (4): 237–51.
Kamprad, I. with Toreleull, B. (2000) Leading by Design: The IKEA Story. London:
HarperCollins.
Kellett, M. (2005) How to Develop Children as Researchers. London: Paul Chapman
Publishing.
Kelly, D.H. (1993) ‘Case: grammar and terminology’, The Classical World, 87 (1):
35–9.
Kenny, A. (ed.) (1993) The Wittgenstein Reader. Oxford: Blackwell.
Kossyvaki, L. and Papoudi, D. (2016) ‘A review of play interventions for chil-
dren with autism at school’, International Journal of Disability, Development and
Education, 63 (1): 45–63. doi: 10.1080/1034912X.2015.1111303
Kounin, J. (1970) Discipline and Group Management in Classrooms. New York: Holt,
Rinehart & Winston.
Kuhn, T.S. (1970) The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago: University of
Chicago Press.
Lebow, R.N. (2007) ‘Counterfactual thought experiments: a necessary teaching
tool’, The History Teacher, 40 (2) (available at: www.historycooperative.org/
journals/ht/40.2/lebow.html).
Levitt, S.D. and List, J.A. (2009) ‘Was there really a Hawthorne effect at the Hawthorne
plant? An analysis of the original illumination experiments’, NBER Working
Paper No. 15016. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research.
Lewin, K. (1951) Field Theory in Social Science: Selected Theoretical Papers. New York:
Harper.
Lewin, K. (1997) Resolving Social Conflicts: Field Theory in Social Science. Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association.
Lewis, A. and Lindsay, G. (2000) Researching Children’s Perspectives. Buckingham:
Open University Press.
Lewis, A. and Porter, J. (2004) ‘Interviewing children and young people with learn-
ing disabilities: guidelines for researchers and multi-professional practice’, British
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32 (4): 191–7.
Lincoln, Y.S. and Guba, E.G. (1985) Naturalistic Inquiry. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
MacIntyre, A. (1985) After Virtue: A Study in Moral Theory. London: Duckworth.
Mandel, D.R., Hilton, D.J. and Catellani, P. (2005) The Psychology of Counterfactual
Thinking. Abingdon: Routledge.
Milgram, S. (1963) ‘Behavioral study of obedience’, Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, 67 (4): 371–8.
REFERENCES 289
Solzhenitsyn, A. (1963/2000) One Day in the Life of Ivan Denisovich. London: Penguin.
Stake, R.E. (1995) The Art of Case Study Research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Stake, R.E. (2005) ‘Qualitative case studies’, in N.K. Denzin and Y.S. Lincoln (eds),
The Sage Handbook of Qualitative Research (3rd edn). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Stenhouse, L. (1979) ‘Can research improve teaching? Summary of a lecture given
by Lawrence Stenhouse at the Scottish National Conference on Curriculum
and Evaluation for P.E. Teachers’, January (available at: www.uea.ac.uk/docu
ments/4059364/4994243/Stenhouse-1979-Can+Research+improve+teaching.
pdf/54a14fb3-6932-40f4-8570-942944ef0d63).
Storr, A. (1997) Feet of Clay: A Study of Gurus. London: HarperCollins.
Strauss, A. and Corbin, J. (1990) Basics of Qualitative Research: Grounded Theory
Procedures and Techniques. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Tavor Bannet, E. (1997) ‘Analogy as translation: Wittgenstein, Derrida, and the law
of language’, New Literary History, 28 (4): 655–72.
Taylor, S.J. and Bogdan, R. (1984) Introduction to Qualitative Research Methods: The
Search for Meanings. New York: Wiley.
Thomas, G. (1992) Effective Classroom Teamwork: Support or Intrusion? London:
Routledge.
Thomas, G. (2007) Education and Theory: Strangers in Paradigms. Maidenhead: Open
University Press.
Thomas, G. (2009) ‘“What works” as a sublinguistic grunt, with lessons from cata-
chresis, asymptote, football and pharma’, Research Intelligence, 106: 20–2.
Thomas, G. (2010) ‘Doing case study: abduction not induction; phronesis not the-
ory’, Qualitative Inquiry, 16 (7): 572–82.
Thomas, G. (2011a) ‘A typology for the case study in social science following a
review of definition, discourse and structure’, Qualitative Inquiry, 17 (6): 511–21.
Thomas, G. (2011b) ‘The case: generalisation, theory and phronesis in case study’,
Oxford Review of Education, 37 (1): 21–35.
Thomas, G. (2012) ‘Changing our landscape of inquiry for a new science of educa-
tion’, Harvard Educational Review, 82 (1): 26–51.
Thomas, G. (2016) ‘After the gold rush: questioning the “gold standard” and reap-
praising the status of experiment and randomized controlled trials in education’,
Harvard Educational Review, 86 (3): 390–411.
Thomas, G. (2017) How to Do Your Research Project (3rd edn). London: Sage.
Thomas, G. (2020) ‘Experiment’s persistent failure in education inquiry, and why it
keeps failing’, British Educational Research Journal, doi:10.1002/berj.3660
Thomas, G. and James, D. (2006) ‘Reinventing grounded theory: some questions
about theory, ground and discovery’, British Educational Research Journal, 32 (6):
767–95.
292 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY
Willis, P.E. (1981) Learning to Labor: How Working Class Kids Get Working Class Jobs.
New York: Columbia University Press.
Wood, R. (2019) Inclusive Education for Autistic Children: Helping Children and Young
People to Learn and Flourish in the Classroom. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Yin, R.K. (2018) Case Study Research: Design and Methods (6th edn). Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage.
INDEX
Abbott, A., 76, 79, 253 bias, 209, 266
ABC of behaviour, 182 biography, 73
abduction, 74–9, 84, 129, 283 Blumer, H., 55
accounts, 206–7, 215 Booth, W., 143, 269, 270, 271, 280
accumulation of evidence, 76 Bourdieu, P., 75, 161, 243
action research, 45–6, 53, 58, 125, 194, Bradshaw, Y., 283
238, 255, 265 brainstorming, 33, 37, 116, 135, 238
adopted Romanian children, 111–3 British Educational Research Association
Alderson P., 93, 103 (BERA), 100, 101
Althusser, L., 75 Bronfenbrenner, U., 59, 61, 62, 194
analogy, 189, 191, 248, 253–4, 258 Bruner, J., 31, 131, 249–53, 260
analysis, 203–35 Bryman, A., 222
analytical category, 12, 147 Burgess R., 65, 172, 217
analytical frame, 12–17, 21–4, 33–4, 39, Burt, C., 70–1
41, 44, 223
Andreski, S., 257 Caldwell, J.C., 118–25
anecdotal evidence, 4 Campbell, D., 283
anomalies, 74, 249 capability, Sen’s notion of, 121
anonymisation, 89, 96 cartoons, 217
anonymity, 87–8, 91, 96, 104–5, 238 causal narrativity, 253
anorexia nervosa, 94, 182 causation, 9, 11, 46
Anthropologist on Mars, 272, 282 cause, and Becker’s ‘sequence of steps’, 250
anthropology, 159, 172 census data, 219
approaches to research, 44 Checkland, P., 60–1, 65, 238–40
archival records, 178 chefs’ recipes, 188, 195
Aristotle, 51, 52, 78, 79 classrooms, 133, 192, 194, 195
artefacts, 211 closed questions, 212
ATLAS.ti, 230 coding, 225–7, 232–3, 259
attitude to knowledge, 266 axial, 232
attitudes, 212–4, 239 open, 232
audiences, of research, 254–5, 265, 276–8 selective, 232
audio, 96, 207, 211, 225, 230 Coffey, A., 104
audit society, 70 Cohen, M.F., 25, 222, 281
authority, problems arguing from, 268 collecting data, 1, 171, 203–222
collective case study. See multiple study
Bacon, F., 51 Collingwood, R., 80, 178
Ball, S., 47, 48 comparative study. See multiple study
Barker, R., 29, 58, 60, 64 comparative research, 45, 46, 219, 269, 283
Barthes, R., 252 concealment, 88, 93
Bassey, M., 125 conceptual model, in soft systems, 239
Bateson, G., 58, 64, 65 confidentiality, 87–91, 96, 105, 205, 238
Beachside Comprehensive, 47, 48 consent, 86, 89, 90–8, 101–2, 112
Becker, H., 5, 9, 26, 44, 48, 65, 84, 142, 244, constant comparative method, 194, 224–5,
250–3, 256, 258, 260, 271, 280 228, 230–3, 236, 259, 261
Belmont Report, 86, 88 contextual antecedents, 136
Bendix, R., 283 control group, 168
BERA, 100, 103 correlation, 118
Berlin Wall, 32–8, 179 craft knowledge, 51, 78, 188
INDEX 295
key case, 22, 38, 68, 109, 111–2, 115, normal distribution curve, 40, 41
124, 142 NVivo, 230–1, 233, 259
kinds of case study. See types of case study
Köhler, W., 53 object of the case study, 15–24, 31, 109,
Korean War, 15 127–41, 145, 181, 223, 256
Kounin, J., 167 observation, 23, 44, 47, 72, 82, 93–4, 96,
Kuhn, T., 252 166, 170, 178, 186, 203, 204, 208, 215–7,
Kurtz, M., 283 221–2, 266, 268
structured, 204, 215–6, 221
Le Play, F., 4, 187 unstructured, 204, 208, 215, 216–7,
legal case studies, 159, 175 221, 225
Lewin, K., 53, 58–9, 238 Office for National Statistics, 219
Lewis, A., 87, 93 official statistics, 44, 204, 221
literature review, 23, 30–3, 41, 47, 185, 203, Omaar, R., 39–44, 48, 203
245, 247–8, 262–3, 269–70 One Day in the Life of Ivan Denisovich.
Loader, D., 261 See Solzhenitsyn, A., 179
local knowledge case, 22, 68, 111, 112, online, 98, 101, 103, 211, 212,
124, 125 optical character recognition (OCR)
longitudinal study, 72 software, 212
Loxley, A., 39, 217–8 opt-in versus implied consent, 91
Luker, K., 172 Orwell G., 278
outlier case, 4, 22, 24, 42, 43, 48, 68, 109,
MacIntyre, A., 52, 65, 74, 75, 77, 78 111, 112, 118–25
macrosystem. See ecological systems theory
Malinowski, B., 160, 172 paradigm shift, 61
Marx, K., 244, 267 paradigm wars, 51, 257
Mead, G.H., 55 paradigms, 83
measurements and tests, 218–9 parallel and sequential studies, 178
memos, as part of coding, 233 Parlett, D., 8
mesosystem. See ecological systems theory participant observation, 204, 208, 216–7, 221
metaphors, use of, 154, 188, 190, 191, 236 Patrick, J., 6, 26
methodology, 23, 73, 84, 99, 110, 232, 263 pattern-finding, 53
methods Paxman, J., 267
of data collection, 203–222 Peirce, C., 74, 75, 77
of analysis, 223–260 personal experience, using, 16, 68, 78, 111, 188
microsystem. See ecological systems theory PGCE qualification, 113
Milgram, S., 85, 88 photo-elicitation, 217
Miller, S., 75 photographs, 148, 178, 186, 203, 211, 217
Mitchell, J., 14, 76 phronesis, 78–9, 84, 129, 153, 283
Mouzelis, N., 77 piloting, 44, 67, 102, 117, 195, 234
multiple study, 186–191, 192, 195, 197 Plato, 51, 79, 274
mushy heap syndrome, 29, 265, 266, 280 plausibility, 69–71
Polanyi, M., 78, 257
narrative, 79, 131, 178–80, 248–50, 253–66 polyhedron of intelligibility, 5, 72
narrative diachronicity, Bruner’s Popper, K., 53, 256
concept of, 250 population, 5, 40, 66–7
natural sciences, 7, 50–2, 70, 74–5, 166 positionality, 66, 69, 73, 83, 209, 232
nested case studies, 38, 42–4, 174, 191–5, potential explanations, 119, 131, 132, 137,
197, 198 141, 185
network analysis, 224 practitioners, 154, 159
newspaper stories, 178, 246 prediction, 52, 76, 232
Newton, I., 3, 54 presentation, 74, 91, 191
Nietzsche, F., 6 prima facie question, 30–2, 39, 41, 44, 48,
nomothetic research, 52 203, 245, 262
298 HOW TO DO YOUR CASE STUDY