276-4106-1-pb
276-4106-1-pb
PERSPECTIVE/OPINION
Trait self-control is often defined as the ability to inhibit dominant responses including thoughts,
emotions, and behavioral impulses. Despite the pivotal role of inhibition for trait self-control, a growing
body of evidence found small-to-zero correlations between self-report measures of trait self-control and
behavioral inhibition tasks. These observations seem puzzling considering that both types of measures
Keywords: self-control; self-report measures; Self-Control Scale; inhibition tasks; executive function;
typical versus maximum performance; impulses
Consider the following scenario: A researcher employs Randles, & Inzlicht, 2018). For example, Saunders et al.
two different measurement instruments designed to (2018) published a series of studies suggesting that trait
measure the same construct. To her surprise, she ends self-control and inhibitory control are not meaningfully
up with a low correlation between the two measures. A associated. Trait self-control was assessed with the most
different researcher does the same thing with slightly prominent self-report measure in the field (i.e., the Self-
different measures – again, almost no correlation Control Scale, Tangney, Baumeister, & Boone, 2004);
between the measures. How is this possible? These inhibitory control was assessed with two established
(non-)correlations require an explanation. executive function tasks (i.e., the Stroop task, Stroop,
In recent years, a situation akin to the one described 1935, and the Flanker task, Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974) that
in the previous paragraph has emerged in the field of are commonly used to assess inhibitory control (e.g.,
self-control. A growing body of evidence found small- Diamond, 2013). An internal Bayesian meta-analysis
to-zero correlations between self-report measures of across 5 studies (overall N > 2,600) revealed a null
trait self-control and behavioral inhibition tasks (Allom, relationship between the two types of measures. Similar
Panetta, Mullan, & Hagger, 2016; Duckworth & Kern, results emerged for other constructs closely related to
2011; Eisenberg et al., 2019; Nęcka, Gruszka, Orzechowski, trait self-control, for example, conscientiousness and
Nowak, & Wójcik, 2018; Saunders, Milyavskaya, Etz, impulsivity (Edmonds, Bogg, & Roberts, 2009; Eisenberg
et al., 2019; Enkavi et al., 2019; Fleming, Heintzelmann,
& Bartholow, 2016; Frey, Pedroni, Mata, Rieskamp, &
Department of Psychology, Saarland University, Saarbruecken, DE Hertwig, 2017; Stahl et al., 2014).
Corresponding author: Lasse Wennerhold These empirical observations seem puzzling.
([email protected]) After all, self-report measures of trait self-control
Art. 9, page 2 of 8 Wennerhold and Friese: Self-Report and Inhibition Tasks Measuring Self-Control
and behavioral inhibition tasks are often seen as self-control. While most self-report measures of trait self-
operationalizations of the same or at least closely related control are designed to assess trait-like typical inhibitory
theoretical constructs. They are designed with the performance that is repeatedly shown across a broad
intention to capture the ability of a person to inhibit range of impulses from different domains, behavioral
dominant responses including thoughts, emotions, and inhibition tasks are designed to measure ability-like
behavioral impulses (Tangney et al., 2004). This ability maximum inhibitory performance shown on single
is classically considered the central definitional aspect occasions for specific kinds of impulses.
of the construct of self-control (e.g., Baumeister, 2014; Note that we use the wording “self-report measures
Duckworth & Kern, 2011; Hofmann, Schmeichel, & of trait self-control” and “behavioral inhibition tasks”
Baddeley, 2012). Thus, it would seem appropriate to expect to refer to methodological categories of operationali
moderate or even strong correlations between these zations: self-report questionnaires on the one hand
measures. When we talk about self-control in this article, and performance tasks on the other hand. We do
we also refer to this conceptualization of the construct. this to stress the point that, although we refer to the
Saunders et al. (2018) discuss three factors that might Self-Control Scale and the Stroop/Flanker tasks as
contribute to this seeming conundrum. First, the Self- prominent examples of their respective methodological
Control Scale (Tangney et al., 2004) might capture other categories following the example of Saunders et al.
things than inhibition alone (e.g., items like “I am able (2018), most issues raised in this article do not only
growing evidence suggests that the plentiful associations Taken together, the distinction between typical and
of the Self-Control Scale with desirable life outcomes are maximum performance helps to explain low correlations
to a large extent due to beneficial stable habits, not due to between self-report measures of trait self-control and
effortful inhibition in particular situations (e.g., de Ridder behavioral inhibition tasks because (1) self-report
et al., 2012; Galla & Duckworth, 2015; Grund & Carstens, measures such as the Self-Control Scale (Tangney et al.,
2019; Hofmann, Baumeister, Förster, & Vohs, 2012). In 2004) are designed to assess typical performance, and
other words, it is not mainly individual differences in behavioral inhibition tasks are designed to assess
maximum performance, but in typical performance that maximum performance, (2) typical and maximum
differentiates between respondents on this scale. performance are conceptually different and empirically
If we assume a continuum between typical performance only modestly related, and (3) maximum performance
on one end and maximum performance on the other measures might show comparatively little between-
end, behavioral inhibition tasks are clearly designed to person variability, limiting their ability to correlate
assess behavior more on the maximum performance end with other individual differences. Note that we are
of the continuum compared to self-report instruments not claiming that the distinction between typical and
such as the Self-Control Scale. In behavioral inhibition maximum performance is the factor that explains low
tasks, respondents are instructed to avoid making errors correlations between self-report measures of trait
and/or to try being fast in the task that is to follow. self-control and behavioral inhibition tasks. In fact,
in single situations. In other words, what differentiates tendency of this person’s behavior is relatively stable at
people with high versus low trait self-control and helps a moderate level of self-control (Figure 1C), compared to
them to achieve a multitude of desirable life outcomes that of others. For example, the average self-control level
over months, years, and decades is that people with of a person during a certain time period (e.g., one week)
high trait self-control tend to typically act in a more self- tends to be similar to this person’s average self-control
controlled manner. level during another time period (e.g., the following
week). This led Fleeson to conclude that traits are density
Single versus repeated performance distributions of states such as those depicted in Figure 1.
The observation that relative to self-report instruments Thus, there is strong variability within persons, but also
such as the Self-Control Scale behavioral inhibition tasks high stability of the central tendencies between persons.
assess something more akin to maximum performance This intriguing insight effectively resolved a good part
can partly explain the low correspondence between these of the person-situation debate that kept social and
variables. However, even if one would assume that, on personality psychologists busy for decades (Fleeson &
average, participants in scientific studies show so little Jayawickreme, 2015).
motivation to perform well on behavioral inhibition tasks We regard inhibition as a personality trait. Figure 1B
that these tasks do not work as measures of maximum illustrates that a one-shot behavioral measurement of
performance at all, they would likely make relatively poor inhibition is unlikely to reflect the central tendency of a
A B C
Average self−control time period 2
Frequency acted that way
How much self−control the person showed How much self−control the person showed Average self−control time period 1
Figure 1: Each graph depicts the number of times a hypothetical person acted at each level of self-control. The graph on
the left (A) depicts the density distribution of self-controlled behavior with on average moderate level of self-control
and relatively low intraindividual variability (i.e, this fictitious person almost always behaved in a moderately self-
controlled way). The graph in the middle (B) also depicts the density distribution of a self-controlled behavior with
on average moderate level of self-control, this time with relatively high intraindividual variability (i.e., this fictitious
person often behaves considerably more or less self-controlled than on a moderate level). Fleeson (2001) found that
actual distributions more resemble Figure 1B than Figure 1A. In Figure 1C, each point in this graph represents one
person’s average level of self-control in two different time periods (e.g., one week). The work by Fleeson (2001, 2004)
suggests that how self-controlled a person acts on average in one time period is highly similar to how self-controlled
the person acts on average in another time period. Figure adapted from Fleeson (2004).
Wennerhold and Friese: Self-Report and Inhibition Tasks Measuring Self-Control Art. 9, page 5 of 8
measure general, cross-domain inhibition, whereas inhibition. Similarly, the validity of behavioral inhibition
behavioral inhibition tasks measure also narrower, tasks as pure measures of inhibition can be questioned.
domain-specific inhibition to a substantial degree. Whether performance on these tasks is determined by
The role of potential qualitative differences of impulses inhibitory control alone or other processes as well, for
in different domains is rarely discussed in the self- example, selective attention, is not entirely clear (Cohen,
control literature (e.g., an impulse to eat unhealthy food Dunbar, & McClelland, 1990; Egner & Hirsch, 2005). This
versus an impulse to insult someone in an argument). notwithstanding, these tasks arguably measure inhibition
As a consequence, a naive reader of the literature might to a substantial degree and are commonly used as
assume that there are few qualitative differences between measures of inhibition. Thus, one should expect at least
impulses across domains and that people are typically a moderate correlation of these measures with a self-
able to inhibit impulses in different domains to roughly report scale that measures inhibition if both approaches
the same extent. Domain-general measures of trait self- captured similar inhibitory processes.
control – like the Self-Control Scale (Tangney et al., 2004) Behavioral inhibition tasks are meant to assess domain-
– predict a wide range of desirable life outcomes across general inhibition as well. It is not clear, however, to what
different domains (e.g., de Ridder et al., 2012; Tangney et extent they actually achieve this goal. We argue that these
al., 2004). It is therefore indeed plausible that there are tasks substantially measure the inhibition of rather specific
causes of self-control performance that might be common kinds of impulses as well. For instance, in the Stroop task
increase measurement correspondence between these by contrast, are typically designed to measure ability-
scales and (the latent variable of) inhibition. Measurement like maximum inhibitory performance shown on single
correspondence has been widely discussed in other fields. occasions for more specific kinds of impulses. Future
For example, the seminal correspondence principle by theoretical and empirical research should examine more
Ajzen and Fishbein (1977) suggests that the relation closely which of the discussed (and possibly additional)
between measures of attitudes and behavior increases with factors contribute to which extent to the degree of
increasing correspondence between the measurement empirical (non-)correspondence of different measures
instruments across different entities (e.g., target, action, of self-control, helping the field to understand both the
context, time). Strong attitude-behavior relations can only theoretical nature and suitable measurement approaches
be expected when there is high correspondence between of the construct better.
at least some of these entities. Applied to the present
context, a higher correspondence between the types of Note
inhibition – general versus domain-specific – assessed 1
Fleeson (2001) did not examine this idea with trait
by self-report measures and the indicator of inhibition self-control specifically, but with other traits like
derived from behavioral inhibition tasks should increase agreeableness, extraversion, or conscientiousness
their empirical correlation. (the latter being conceptually closely related to trait
Taken together, we argue that domain-general self- self-control).
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16, 76–99. conscientiousness and executive functioning:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868311418749 Mental set shifting, not prepotent response
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual inhibition or working memory updating. Journal
Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168. DOI: https://doi. of Personality, 84, 1011–1027. DOI: https://doi.
org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750 org/10.1037//0022-3514.80.6.1011
Duckworth, A. L., & Kern, M. L. (2011). A meta-analysis Freudenthaler, H. H., & Neubauer, A. C. (2007).
of the convergent validity of self-control measures. Measuring emotional management abilities: Further
Journal of Research in Personality, 45, 259–268. DOI: evidence of the importance to distinguish between
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2011.02.004 typical and maximum performance. Personality and
Duckworth, A. L., & Tsukayama, E. (2015). Domain Individual Differences, 42, 1561–1572. DOI: https://
specificity in self-control. In C. B. Miller, R. M. doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.10.031
Furr, A. Knobel, & W. Fleeson (Eds.), Character: Frey, R., Pedroni, A., Mata, R., Rieskamp, J., &
New directions from philosophy, psychology, Hertwig, R. (2017). Risk preference shares the
and theology (pp. 393–411). New York: Oxford psychometric structure of major psychological traits.
University Press. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/ Science Advances, 3, e1701381. DOI: https://doi.
acprof:oso/9780190204600.003.0019 org/10.1126/sciadv.1701381
Edmonds, G. W., Bogg, T., & Roberts, B. W. (2009). Galla, B. M., & Duckworth, A. L. (2015). More than
How to cite this article: Wennerhold, L., & Friese, M. (2020). Why Self-Report Measures of Self-Control and Inhibition Tasks Do
Not Substantially Correlate. Collabra: Psychology, 6(1): 9. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.276
Copyright: © 2020 The Author(s). This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons
Attribution 4.0 International License (CC-BY 4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium,
provided the original author and source are credited. See http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.